Systemic and Central Amygdala Injections of the mGluR2/3 Agonist LY379268 Attenuate the Expression of Incubation of Cocaine Craving Lin Lu, Jamie L. Uejima, Sarah M. Gray, Jennifer M. Bossert, and Yavin Shaham Background: We and others reported time-dependent increases in cue-induced cocaine seeking after withdrawal, suggesting that craving incubates over time. Recently, we found that central amygdala extracellular signal-regulated kinases (ERK) and glutamate are involved in this incubation. Here, we further explored the role of central amygdala glutamate in the incubation of cocaine craving by determining the effect of systemic or central amygdala injections of the mGluR2/3 agonist LY379268 (which decreases glutamate release) on cue-induced cocaine seeking during early and late withdrawal. Methods: Rats were trained to self-administer cocaine for 10 days (6 hours/day); infusions were paired with a tone-light cue. Cocaine seeking and craving after systemic or central amygdala injections of LY379268 were then assessed in extinction tests in the presence of the cocaine-associated cues during early (day 3) or late (day 21) withdrawal. Results: Systemic (1.5 or 3 mg/kg) or central amygdala (.5 or 1.0 g/side) injections of LY379268 attenuated enhanced extinction responding on day 21 but had no effect on lower extinction responding on day 3. Conclusions: Results confirm our previous findings on the role of central amygdala glutamate in the incubation of cocaine craving and together with previous reports suggest that mGluR2/3 agonists should be considered in the treatment of drug relapse. Key Words: Basolateral amygdala, drug seeking, glutamate, reinstatement, relapse
R
elapse to cocaine use in humans can occur after prolonged abstinence and is often precipitated by exposure to cocaine-associated cues that provoke drug craving, a subjective state that often precedes and accompanies cocaine seeking (O’Brien et al 1992). In an attempt to account for this persistent relapse, Gawin and Kleber (1986) hypothesized that cue-induced cocaine craving (and relapse) progressively increases over the first several weeks of abstinence and remains high over extended drug-free periods (see Kosten et al 2005 for tentative empirical support for this hypothesis in a clinical trial). We and others (Grimm et al 2001; Lu et al 2004b; Neisewander et al 2000; Sorge and Stewart 2005) identified an analogous phenomenon in laboratory rats: time-dependent increases in cueinduced cocaine seeking after withdrawal from drug self-administration, suggesting that cocaine craving incubates over time. In addition, we and others (Di Ciano and Everitt 2004a; Grimm et al 2002, 2005; Shalev et al 2001; Shepard et al 2004) found time-dependent increases in cue-induced reward seeking in rats that previously self-administered heroin, methamphetamine, or sucrose. In initial studies (Grimm et al 2001, 2003), we assessed time-dependent increases in cue-induced responding on a lever previously associated with cocaine self-administration (the operational measure from which we inferred that cocaine craving incubates over time) with extinction tests in the presence of contextual cues previously associated with cocaine availability
From the Behavioral Neuroscience Branch, Intramural Research Program/ National Institute on Drug Abuse/National Institutes of Health/Department of Health and Human Services, Baltimore, Maryland. Address reprint requests to Prof. Lin Lu, National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, 38 Yixueyuan Road, Beijing 100083, China; E-mail:
[email protected]. Received March 13, 2006; revised April 13, 2006; accepted April 17, 2006.
0006-3223/07/$32.00 doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.011
and subsequent cue-induced reinstatement tests (Meil and See 1996), during which lever presses led to contingent presentations of a discrete 5-sec tone-light cue previously paired with cocaine injections during self-administration training. We found that lever-responding in the extinction and the cue-induced reinstatement tests followed a similar time course and were highly correlated (Lu et al 2004c). These observations suggest that although different sets of cocaine cues induce drug seeking in these tests, they likely provoke a similar motivational state (craving) that incubates over time. Therefore, in more recent studies (and the present report), we and others have assessed the incubation of craving in extinction tests, during which rats were exposed to the drug contextual cues and lever presses resulted in contingent presentations of a discrete cue; the discrete cue serves as a conditioned reinforcer in the extinction tests (Conrad et al 2005; Lu et al 2004a, 2004b, 2005; Sorge and Stewart 2005). Recently, we explored whether the incubation of cocaine craving involves activation of extracellular signal-regulated kinases (ERK) signaling in the amygdala (Lu et al 2005). Cocaine activates the ERK pathway in mesolimbic dopamine projection areas (Berhow et al 1996; Mattson et al 2005; Valjent et al 2000). In the amygdala and other brain areas, this pathway is involved in learning and memory processes (Adams and Sweatt 2002) that were hypothesized to play a role in drug addiction and relapse (Everitt et al 2001; Jentsch and Taylor 1999; Nestler 2001; White 1996; Wise 1988). We found that exposure to cocaine cues in the extinction tests increases ERK phosphorylation in the central but not basolateral amygdala after 30 days but not 1 day of withdrawal from cocaine self-administration. In the basolateral but not the central amygdala, ERK phosphorylation was increased after 1 day but not 30 days of withdrawal from cocaine; this effect, however, was not associated with exposure to the cocaine cues in the extinction tests. After 30 withdrawal days, inhibition of central amygdala ERK phosphorylation by U0126 or the N-methyl–D aspartate (NMDA) antagonist AP-5 decreased cueinduced cocaine seeking. After 1 day of withdrawal from cocaine, stimulation of central amygdala ERK phosphorylation by BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598 © 2007 Society of Biological Psychiatry
592 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598 NMDA increased cocaine seeking, an effect that was reversed by U0126. We interpreted these data to indicate that glutamatemediated, time-dependent increases in the responsiveness of the central amygdala ERK pathway to cocaine cues are critically involved in the expression of incubation of cocaine craving after prolonged withdrawal from the drug. Here, we further explored the role of glutamate in the expression of incubation of cocaine craving by determining the effect of systemic and central amygdala injections of a group II metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist (LY379268) on cueinduced cocaine seeking, as measured in extinction tests that were performed after 3 or 21 days of withdrawal from cocaine self-administration. Group II metabotropic glutamate receptors consist of the mGluR2 and the mGluR3 subtypes (Schoepp 2001). Subtype 2 metabotropic glutamate receptors are expressed primarily on presynaptic glutamate (and ␥-aminobutyric acid) neurons, and when activated they provide negative feedback to decrease evoked neurotransmitter release (Anwyl 1999; Schoepp 2001). Subtype 3 metabotropic glutamate receptors are expressed on both postsynaptic and presynaptic neurons and on glia, but their functional role is largely unknown (Schoepp 2001; Tamaru et al 2001). We studied the effect of LY379268 on the expression of incubation of cocaine craving, because systemic or intracranial injections of LY379268 were reported to attenuate discriminative, cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking (Baptista et al 2004) and contextual cue-induced relapse to heroin seeking (Bossert et al 2004, Bossert et al, in press), as measured in a reinstatement model (Self and Nestler 1998; Shaham et al 2003; Stewart and de Wit 1987). On the basis of our previous work (Lu et al 2005), we hypothesized that systemic and central amygdala injections of LY379268 would decrease the expression of incubation of craving, as measured in a late withdrawal (day 21) extinction test. As an anatomical control, we also examined the effect of basolateral amygdala injections of LY379268 on extinction responding after 21 days of withdrawal from cocaine.
L. Lu et al planted bilaterally 1 mm above the central or basolateral amygdala. The coordinates (Paxinos and Watson 2005) for the central or basolateral sub-nuclei were: AP ⫺2.5 mm, ML ⫾ 4.5 mm (2° angle), DV ⫺7.5 mm, and AP ⫺2.5 mm, ML ⫾ 5.3 mm (2° angle), DV ⫺7.8 mm, respectively. After cannulae implantation, silastic catheters were inserted into the jugular vein and were passed subcutaneously to the top of the skull, as previously described (Lu et al 2004b; Shaham et al 1997). The catheters were then attached to a modified 22-gauge cannula and mounted to the rat’s skull with dental cement. Buprenorphine (.1 mg/kg, SC) was administered as an analgesic after surgery, and the rats were allowed to recover for 7–10 days. During the recovery and training phases, catheters were flushed every 24 – 48 hours with sterile saline ⫹ Gentamicin (.08 mg/mL). Systemic and Intracranial Injections The LY379268 (a gift from Eli Lilly, Indianapolis, Indiana) was dissolved in 1.2 mol/L NaOH solution (pH adjusted to 7.4). For systemic injections, the rats were injected with vehicle or LY379268 (1.5 or 3.0 mg/kg, IP) 30 min before testing. The doses of LY379268 were based on previous reports (Baptista et al 2004; Bossert et al 2004; Kim and Vezina 2002). Bilateral intracranial injections of LY379268 (0, .5, or 1.0 g/side, injection volume .5 L) were made with 10-L Hamilton syringes (Hamilton Co., Reno, Nevada) that were connected via polyethylene-50 tubing to 30-gauge injectors (Plastics One). The LY379268 or vehicle was infused into each side over 1 min, and the injector was left in place for an additional 1 min to allow for diffusion. The intracranial doses of LY379268 are based on our previous studies (Bossert et al 2004, Bossert et al, in press). At the end of Experiments 2–3 (see subsequent sections), the rats were killed, their brains were removed, and coronal sections (40 m) were sliced on a cryostat and stained with Cresyl Violet (ICN Biomedicals, Aurora, Ohio). Cannulae placements were verified under a microscope, and their anatomical location is depicted in Figure 1C.
Methods and Materials Subjects and Apparatus Male Long-Evans rats (total n ⫽ 183, 300 – 400 g, Charles River, Raleigh, North Carolina) were maintained under a reverse 12-hour light– dark cycle (lights off at 9 AM). Food and water were freely available in the home cage. Experimental procedures followed the guidelines of the “Principles of Laboratory Care” (National Institutes of Health publication No. 86-23, 1996) and were approved by the local Animal Care and Use Committee. Fifty-six rats were excluded because of loss of catheter patency, poor health, or cannulae misplacement. The self-administration chambers were located inside soundattenuating cabinets and were controlled by a Med Associates (Georgia, Vermont) system. Each chamber had two levers located 9 cm above the floor. Presses on one (active, retractable) lever activated the infusion pump; presses on the other (inactive, stationary) lever were also recorded. The rat’s catheter was connected via a modified cannula (Plastics One, Roanoke, Virginia) to a liquid swivel (Instech, Plymouth Meeting, Pennsylvania) via polyethylene-50 tubing that was protected by a metal spring. Intravenous and Intracranial Surgery Rats were anesthetized with a mixture of sodium pentobarbital ⫹ chloral hydrate (60 and 25 mg/kg, respectively, IP), and permanent guide cannulae (23-gauge, Plastics One) were imwww.sobp.org/journal
Procedures The experiments consisted of three phases: self-administration training, withdrawal period, and tests for cocaine seeking under extinction conditions (Figure 1A). Training Phase. The rats were chronically housed and trained in the self-administration chambers. They were trained to selfadminister cocaine-HCl (National Institute on Drug Abuse) during six daily 1-hour sessions, separated by 5 min, over 10 days under a fixed-ratio-1 reinforcement schedule. Cocaine was dissolved in saline and self-administered at a dose of .75 mg/kg/infusion (.10 mL/infusion). Active lever responses activated the infusion pump and led to the delivery of a 5-sec tone-light compound cue (2900 Hz, 20 dB above background; 7.5 W white light located 4 cm above the active lever). Each infusion was followed by a 40-sec timeout period during which presses were recorded but did not result in drug delivery. The self-administration sessions started at the onset of the dark cycle and began with the insertion of the active lever and the illumination of a red house light that remained on for the duration of the session. At the end of each 1-hour session, the house light turned off, and the active lever was retracted. To facilitate acquisition of cocaine self-administration, food was removed from the chambers during the 6-hour sessions of the first 3 training days. A maximum of 20 cocaine infusions/hour was set to minimize overdose. At the end of the training phase, the groups to be tested with the different LY379268 doses were matched for their cocaine intake during training.
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598 593
L. Lu et al
A. Experimental timeline Cocaine Selfadministration training (10 d)
Withdrawal period (3 or 21 d)
Extinction test Day 21
Extinction test Day 3
B. Training
Infusions (6 h)
100
75
50 Exp. 1 Exp. 2
25
Exp. 3 0
1
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Training day
C. Cannulae placements Central amygdala Bregma -2.16 mm
Bregma -2.52 mm
Bregma -2.92 mm
Basolateral amygdala
Bregma -2.16 mm
Withdrawal Phase. After the training phase, rats were returned to the animal facility, where they remained for 3 or 21 withdrawal days; the rats were handled three times per week. Extinction Test. The extinction test in the presence of the cocaine-associated cues consisted of a single 3-hour extinction session, conducted after 3 or 21 withdrawal days. (The extinction tests were conducted over 3 hours rather than 6 hours [the duration of the training sessions], because we initially assessed in several rats the effect of systemic LY379268 injections on cocaine-priming induced reinstatement. These rats were given saline on hour 4 and different doses of cocaine on hours 5– 6. These data, however, are not presented because of differences between the day-3 and day-21 groups in response to saline injections on hour 4; these differences in “baseline” response rates confound the interpretation of the cocaine priming data.) The experimental conditions were the same as those in the training phase except that presses on the previously active lever were not reinforced with cocaine. Tests started at the onset of the dark cycle and began with the insertion of the active lever and the illumination of the red house light, which remained on for the duration of the session. Active lever responses during testing resulted in contingent presentations of the tone-light cue that was previously paired with cocaine infusions. We assessed early withdrawal extinction responding on day 3 rather than on day 1 (as we did in our recent report; Lu et al 2005). The reason for this procedural change is that on withdrawal day 1 extinction responding is very low, which makes it difficult to detect the effects of experimental manipulations on decreases in response rates. In contrast, extinction responding on day 3, although significantly lower than during late withdrawal (Lu et al 2004a), is similar to that typically observed in studies on the effect of pharmacological and neuroanatomical manipulations on reinstatement of lever responding (Bossert et al 2005b; Shaham et al 2003). This is an important methodological consideration, because our aim was to study mechanisms that selectively mediate the enhanced cue-induced cocaine seeking in extinction tests during late withdrawal (what we term the “expression of incubation cocaine craving” in this report) and not the neuronal mechanisms underlying extinction responding in general. Specific Experiments Experiment 1: Systemic Injections of LY379268. Six groups of rats (n ⫽ 9 –10/group) were used in a 2 (withdrawal period: day 3, day 21) ⫻ 3 (LY379268 dose: 0, 1.5, 3.0 mg/kg) factorial design. The LY379268 or its vehicle was injected 30 min before the extinction tests, which were performed after 3 or 21 withdrawal days. Experiment 2: Central Amygdala Injections of LY379268. Six groups of rats (n ⫽ 8 –10/group) were used in a 2 (withdrawal period: day 3, day 21) ⫻ 3 (LY379268 dose: 0, .5, 1.0 g/side) factorial design. The LY379268 or its vehicle was injected 5–10 min before the extinction tests, which were performed after 3 or 21 withdrawal days.
Bregma -2.52 mm
Bregma -2.92 mm
Figure 1. Experimental procedure, training, and cannulae placements. (A) Timeline of the experimental procedure. (B) Training phase: mean ⫾ SEM number of cocaine infusions over the ten 6-hour daily self-administration training sessions in Experiment 1 (n ⫽ 57), Experiment 2 (n ⫽ 56), and Experiment 3 (n ⫽ 14). (C) Cannulae placements: representative pictures of central and basolateral amygdala injector tips and the approximate placements of the injectors’ tips of the rats (Paxinos and Watson 2005).
www.sobp.org/journal
594 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598
Results The rats (total n ⫽ 127) demonstrated reliable cocaine self-administration, as indicated by the significant effect of training day [F (9,1107) ⫽ 24.5, p ⬍ .01] (Figure 1B); there were no significant differences between the rats that participated in Experiments 1, 2, and 3 (p ⬎ .05). Experiment 1: Systemic Injections of LY379268 Systemic injections of LY379268 decreased active lever responding in the extinction test on withdrawal day 21 but not day 3 (Figure 2A); this effect was most pronounced during the 1st hour of testing (data not shown). The LY379268 had no effect on inactive lever responding, which was very low during testing (Figure 2A). Active lever responses were analyzed with a mixed-factor ANOVA, using the between-subjects factors of LY379268 Dose and Withdrawal Day and the within-subjects factor of Hour. This analysis revealed significant effects of Withdrawal Day [F (1,51) ⫽ 12.1, p ⬍ .01], LY379268 Dose [F (2,51) ⫽ 3.7, p ⬍ .05], and Withdrawal Day LY379268 Dose ⫻ Hour [F (4,102) ⫽ 3.1, p ⬍ .05] and an approaching significant effect of Withdrawal Day ⫻ LY379268 Dose [F (2,51) ⫽ 3.1, p ⫽ .06]. No significant effects were found for inactive lever responding (p values ⬎ .05), with the exception of a significant effect of Withdrawal Day [F (1,51) ⫽ 5.3, p ⬍ .05], due to a small increase in responding on day 21 (Figure 2A). Post hoc group differences (Fisher probable least-squares difference test) are indicated in Figure 2A. Experiment 2: Central Amygdala Injections of LY379268 Intracranial injections of LY379268 into the central amygdala decreased active lever responding in the extinction test on withdrawal day 21 but not day 3 (Figure 2B); this effect was most pronounced during the 1st hour of the extinction tests (data not shown). The LY379268 had no effect on inactive lever responding, which was very low during testing (Figure 2B). Active lever responses were analyzed with a mixed-factor ANOVA, using the between-subjects factors of LY379268 Dose and Withdrawal Day and the within-subjects factor of Hour. This analysis revealed significant effects of Withdrawal Day [F (1,50) ⫽ 22.4, p ⬍ www.sobp.org/journal
Lever presses (3 h)
200
Day 21 Day 3 Active Active Inactive Inactive
150 100
*
# #
50 0
0 1.5 3.0 LY379268 dose (mg/kg, i.p.)
B. Central amygdala injections 200 Lever presses (3 h)
Statistical Analyses Data from Experiments 1 and 2 (systemic and central amygdala injections of LY379268) were analyzed separately for total (non-reinforced) active and inactive lever responses with a mixed-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA), using the betweensubjects factors of LY379268 Dose and Withdrawal Day and the within-subjects factor of Hour. Data from Experiment 3 (basolateral amygdala injections of LY379268) were analyzed with a mixed-factor ANOVA, using the between-subjects factor of LY379268 Dose and the within-subjects factor of Hour. Significant interaction effects (p ⬍ .05) were followed post hoc Fisher probable least-squares difference tests (two-tailed).
A. Systemic injections
Day 21 Day 3 Active Active Inactive Inactive
150 100
* #
*
50 0
0 0.5 1.0 LY379268 dose (µg/side)
C. Basolateral amygdala injections 200 Lever presses (3 h)
Experiment 3: Basolateral Amygdala Injections of LY379268. We found that central amygdala injections of LY379268 decreased lever responding in the extinction tests after 21 days but not 3 days of withdrawal from cocaine self-administration. Therefore, in Experiment 3 we determined the anatomical specificity of this effect by injecting LY379268 into the nearby basolateral amygdala. Two groups of rats (n ⫽ 7/group) were used. The LY379268 (1.0 g/side) or its vehicle was injected 5–10 min before the extinction tests, which were performed after 21 withdrawal days.
L. Lu et al
150
Day 21 Active Inactive
100 50 0
1.0 0 LY379268 dose (µg/side)
Figure 2. Extinction tests: total lever presses. Data are mean ⫾ SEM presses on the previously active lever and on the inactive lever during the extinction tests after (A) systemic (n ⫽ 9 –10/group, Experiment 1), (B) central amygdala (n ⫽ 8 –10/group, Experiment 2), or (C) basolateral amygdala (n ⫽ 7/group, Experiment 3) injections of vehicle or LY379268. During the extinction tests, the rats were exposed to contextual cues in the self-administration chambers that were associated with cocaine availability and effects during training, and active lever presses led to contingent presentation of a 5-sec tone-light cue previously paired with each cocaine injection. *Different from Vehicle, p ⬍ .05; #different from Day 21, p ⬍ .05.
.01], LY379268 Dose [F (2,50) ⫽ 10.1, p ⬍ .01], Withdrawal Day ⫻ LY379268 Dose [F (2,50) ⫽ 8.6, p ⬍ .01], and Withdrawal Day ⫻ LY379268 Dose ⫻ Hour [F (4,100) ⫽ 4.4, p ⬍ .01]. No significant
L. Lu et al effects were found for inactive lever responding (p values ⬎ .05). Post hoc group differences are indicated in Figure 2B. Experiment 3: Basolateral Amygdala Injections of LY379268 Intracranial injections of LY379268 into the basolateral amygdala had no effect on active or inactive lever presses during the extinction tests that were performed after 21 days of withdrawal (p values ⬎ .05) (Figure 2C).
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598 595 amygdala. However, LY379268 injections into the nearby basolateral amygdala had no effect on enhanced extinction responding on withdrawal day 21, suggesting that the central amygdala is likely the main target of our intracranial injections. From an anatomical perspective, the lack of effect of basolateral injections of LY379268 on extinction responding is also of significance, because both mGluR2 and mGluR3 are expressed at higher levels in the basolateral amygdala than in the central amygdala (Ohishi et al 1998; Petralia et al 1996; Tamaru et al 2001).
Discussion The main finding in this report was that systemic and central amygdala injections of LY379268 attenuated cue-induced cocaine seeking after 21 days but not 3 days of withdrawal from cocaine. This pattern of results suggests that these LY379268 injections selectively attenuated the enhanced response to the cocaine cues during the late withdrawal extinction test (or the expression of incubation of cocaine craving) rather than inducing a general (time-independent) decrease in the motivational effects of the cocaine cues. These data provide the first demonstration that a pharmacological agent can selectively affect cue-induced drug seeking after prolonged but not short withdrawal period and confirm our recent finding on the role of central amygdala glutamate in the incubation of cocaine craving (Lu et al 2005). Methodological Considerations The selective effect of systemic and central amygdala LY379268 injections on extinction responding on withdrawal day 21 might be due to LY379268-induced motor deficits that were not manifested on withdrawal day 3 because of the lower response rate on this test day (Figures 2A and 2B). The effects of pharmacological manipulations on lever responding are dependent on baseline response rates, and drugs in general are more likely to decrease higher response rates than lower rates (Sanger and Blackman 1976). However, it is unlikely that motor deficits can account for the present results. Systemic injections of LY379268 (1.0 or 3.0 mg/kg) had no effect on the high rate of lever pressing for oral sucrose (about 100 presses/30 min) or condensed milk (about 80 presses/40 min) (Baptista et al 2004; Bossert et al, in press). As for potential non-specific effects of central amygdala injections of LY379268, we found that accumbens core injections of a higher dose of LY379268 (1.5 g/side) than the ones used here had no effect on high response rates for oral sucrose (Bossert et al, in press). In that study, we also found that dorsal striatum injections of LY379268 (1.5 g/side) had no effect on context-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking (Bossert et al, in press). The dorsal striatum and accumbens are critically involved in motor control (Mogenson et al 1980), and anatomical studies report that they express higher levels of mGluR2 and mGluR3 than the central amygdala (Ohishi et al 1998; Tamaru et al 2001). It is also unlikely that systemic or central amygdala LY379268 injections had no effect on extinction responding on withdrawal day 3 because of a floor effect. Systemic and intracranial injections of LY379268 reliably attenuated reinstatement of lever responding by contextual or discriminative drug cues when response rates were similar (Bossert et al 2005a, Bossert et al, in press) or lower (Baptista et al 2004) than the response rates on withdrawal day 3 (Figure 2). Another methodological concern is that the effect of central amygdala injections of LY379268 is due to diffusion of the drug to other amygdala sub-nuclei (Pitkanen 2000). This possibility cannot be ruled out, because it is unlikely that under our experimental procedure LY379268 was confined to the central
Role of Amygdala Glutamate in Cue-Induced Cocaine Seeking The main finding in the present report was that central but not basolateral amygdala LY379268 injections attenuated the enhanced cue-induced cocaine seeking during the late withdrawal (day 21) extinction tests. We interpreted these data to indicate that glutamate transmission in the central amygdala is critical for the expression of incubation of craving when rats are re-exposed to the cocaineassociated cues after prolonged withdrawal from the drug. The present data with LY379268 are in agreement with those from our previous report in which we found that central but not basolateral amygdala injections of the ERK antagonist U0126 attenuates enhanced extinction responding after prolonged withdrawal from cocaine (30 days) (Lu et al 2005). Our data on the lack of effect of basolateral amygdala LY379268 injections on cue-induced cocaine seeking during the late withdrawal extinction tests are in agreement with two previous studies. See et al (2001) found that basolateral amygdala injections of NMDA or amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptor antagonists have no effect on cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Di Ciano and Everitt (2004b) found that basolateral amygdala injections of an AMPA receptor antagonist have no effect on cue-controlled cocaine seeking, as measured in a second-order reinforcement schedule. The authors of these two studies also reported that blockade of basolateral amygdala dopamine receptors attenuate cue-induced cocaine seeking. A framework that might account for our present and previous findings (Lu et al 2005) as well as those of See et al (2001) and Di Ciano and Everitt (2004b) is that basolateral amygdala dopamine transmission mediates cue-induced cocaine seeking but is not critical for the time-dependent enhancement of cue-induced cocaine seeking after withdrawal from the drug. In contrast, central amygdala glutamate selectively mediates the time-dependent enhancement of cue-induced cocaine seeking after withdrawal from the drug or the incubation of cocaine craving. This is a speculative framework, but it is congruent with other reports on the dissociable roles of the central and basolateral amygdala in mediating appetitive and aversive conditioned responses (Holland and Gallagher 2003; Jentsch and Taylor 1999; Killcross et al 1997). For example, lesions of the basolateral amygdala disrupt the ability of cues paired with food or water to serve as conditioned reinforcers (Everitt et al 1999). However, when these lesions were performed after the conditioning phase, they only modestly affected psychostimulant-induced potentiation of the responses to conditioned reinforcers (Burns et al 1993). In contrast, central amygdala lesions have no effect on the cues’ ability to serve as conditioned reinforcers, but they attenuate psychostimulant-induced potentiation of their conditioned responses (Everitt et al 1999; Robledo et al 1996). To our knowledge, our data on the effect of central amygdala injections of LY379368 and the NMDA receptor antagonist AP5 in our previous report (Lu et al 2005) on cue-induced cocaine seeking after prolonged withdrawal from the drug provide the first evidence for a role of amygdala glutamate in cue-controlled www.sobp.org/journal
596 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598 cocaine seeking. These data are potentially in agreement with two other reports on the role of central amygdala in cue-induced cocaine seeking, as measured in the cue-induced reinstatement test (See et al 2003). Kruzich and See (2001) and Alleweireldt et al (2006) reported that central amygdala injections of either the sodium channel blocker tetrodotoxin or the D1-like receptor antagonist SCH 23390 attenuate cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking, respectively. However, unlike the present report in which we found a selective effect of LY379268 in the central amygdala, the authors of these previous reports found that central amygdala tetrodotoxin or SCH 23390 injections were either equally effective (Kruzich and See 2001) or more effective in the basolateral amygdala (Alleweireldt et al 2006). Tetrodotoxin and SCH 23390 are lipophyllic drugs, and at the doses used in these studies (5 ng/side and 1–2 g/side, respectively), these agents likely reach brain areas adjacent to the central amygdala (Caine et al 1995; Zhuravin and Bures 1991). Therefore, the effects on cue-induced reinstatement observed in the above two studies might be due to drug diffusion to the basolateral amygdala (see Alleweireldt et al 2006 for a discussion of issues related to the interpretation of data obtained from studies using intracranial injections of SCH 23390). The glutamatergic projections to the central amygdala that mediate the expression of incubation of cocaine craving after prolonged withdrawal from the drug are unknown. Although not conclusive, our data on the lack of effect of basolateral amygdala LY379268 injections on extinction responding after 21 days of withdrawal from cocaine suggest that the basolateral– central amygdala glutamate projection (Pitkanen 2000) is not involved. There are several excitatory projections from the cortex and the hippocampus to the medial part of the central amygdala (where the output neurons of this brain region are located) (Pitkanen 2000; Swanson and Petrovich 1998). In the context of incubation of cocaine craving, it will be of interest to explore the role of central amygdala glutamate projections from the ventral subiculum (Canteras and Swanson 1992; Cullinan et al 1993) and the agranular insular cortex (Ottersen 1982; Reynolds and Zahm 2005). These brain areas play a role in conditioned responses (Bast et al 2001; Hankins et al 1974; Morgan and LeDoux 1999; Rudy and Matus-Amat 2005). In addition, electrical stimulation of the agranular insular cortex induces neuronal firing of selected central amygdala (medial part) neurons (Pascoe and Kapp 1987; Quirk et al 2003), and stimulation of the ventral subiculum induces long-term potentiation of central amygdala neurons (Henke 1990) and reinstatement of psychostimulant seeking (Taepavarapruk and Phillips 2003; Vorel et al 2001). Implications for Treatment In previous work we found that blockade of central amygdala ERK phosphorylation by U0126 or the NMDA receptor antagonist AP-5 prevents the expression of incubation of cocaine craving after prolonged withdrawal from the drug (30 days). In addition, Valjent et al (2000, 2006) reported that systemic injections of an ERK antagonist (SL327) block cocaine and morphine reward, as measured in the conditioned place preference procedure. These findings are of interest from a neurobiological perspective, but their implications for treatment are not straightforward. ERK signaling is critical for many peripheral and central physiological processes (MacCorkle and Tan 2005; Yoon and Seger 2006), and medications that target fast synaptic transmission mediated by ionotropic glutamate receptors are likely to alter normal processes of learning, memory, and reward (Pennartz et al 2000). Furthermore, systemic injections of an NMDA receptor antagowww.sobp.org/journal
L. Lu et al nist potently reinstate cocaine seeking (De Vries et al 1998). Thus, undesirable effects resulting from systemic administration of ERK or NMDA receptor antagonists will likely preclude their use for the prevention of relapse to abused drugs. In contrast, pharmacological manipulations that target extrasynaptic glutamate transmission such as cysteine prodrugs (Baker et al 2002) or group II metabotropic glutamate receptors are less likely to interfere with normal learning and memory processes that are dependent on fast synaptic glutamate transmission and therefore might provide a new pharmacological approach for the treatment of drug relapse (Kalivas 2004). The present findings on the effect of LY379268 injections in a rat relapse and craving model (Epstein and Preston 2003) extend those from previous studies on the effect of this mGluR2/3 agonist on reinstatement of cocaine seeking by cocaine priming injections (Peters and Kalivas 2006) and discriminative cues (Baptista et al 2004). Systemic injections of LY379268 also attenuate context- and discrete cue-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking (Bossert et al 2004, 2005a) and the enhancement of amphetamine-taking behavior by prior sensitizing exposure to the drug (Kim et al 2005). In addition, Rodd et al (in press) recently found that a related mGluR2/3 agonist (LY404039) reduces cue-induced alcohol seeking and the expression of the alcohol deprivation effect. These results as well as other reports on the attenuation of the behavioral effects of abused drugs by systemic injections of LY379268 and related compounds suggest that pharmacological manipulations that target group II metabotropic glutamate receptors should be considered in the treatment of drug addiction (Heidbreder and Hagan 2005; Kenny and Markou 2004; Weiss 2005). Finally, in the present report and in the previous studies mentioned earlier, the behavioral effects of LY379268 on cueinduced relapse behavior were observed after acute systemic injections. Thus, from a medication development perspective, it is important to confirm these findings in studies in which investigators use chronic dosing regimens. Unfortunately, owing to a limited supply of LY379268 and related mGluR2/3 agonists, to our knowledge, such studies have not yet been performed.
This research was supported (in part) by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Drug Abuse. We thank Deepti Nagarkar and Gabriela Poles for technical support. Adams JP, Sweatt JD (2002): Molecular psychology: Roles for the ERK MAP kinase cascade in memory. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 42:135–163. Alleweireldt AT, Hobbs RJ, Taylor AR, Neisewander JL (2006): Effects of SCH-23390 infused into the amygdala or adjacent cortex and basal ganglia on cocaine seeking and self-administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 31:363–374. Anwyl R (1999): Metabotropic glutamate receptors: Electrophysiological properties and role in plasticity. Brain Res Rev 29:83–120. Baker DA, Xi ZX, Shen H, Swanson CJ, Kalivas PW (2002): The origin and neuronal function of in vivo nonsynaptic glutamate. J Neurosci 22:9134 – 9141. Baptista MA, Martin-Fardon R, Weiss F (2004): Preferential effects of the metabotropic glutamate 2/3 receptor agonist LY379268 on conditioned reinstatement versus primary reinforcement: Comparison between cocaine and a potent conventional reinforcer. J Neurosci 24:4723– 4727. Bast T, Zhang WN, Feldon J (2001): The ventral hippocampus and fear conditioning in rats. Different anterograde amnesias of fear after tetrodotoxin inactivation and infusion of the GABA(A) agonist muscimol. Exp Brain Res 139:39 –52. Berhow MT, Hiroi N, Nestler EJ (1996): Regulation of ERK (extracellular signal regulated kinase), part of the neurotrophin signal transduction cascade,
L. Lu et al in the rat mesolimbic dopamine system by chronic exposure to morphine or cocaine. J Neurosci 16:4707– 4715. Bossert JM, Busch RF, Gray SM (2005a): The novel mGluR2/3 agonist LY379268 attenuates cue-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking. Neuroreport 16:1013–1016. Bossert JM, Ghitza UE, Lu L, Epstein DH, Shaham Y (2005b): Neurobiology of relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking: An update and clinical implications. Eur J Pharmacol 526:36 –50. Bossert JM, Gray SM, Lu L, Shaham Y (2006): Activation of group II metabotropic glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell attenuates context-induced relapse of heroin seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology in press. Bossert JM, Gray SM, Lu L, Shaham Y (in press): Activation of group II metabotropic glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell attenuates context-induced relapse of heroin seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology. Burns LH, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (1993): Differential effects of excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala, ventral subiculum and medial prefrontal cortex on responding with conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity potentiated by intra-accumbens infusions of D-amphetamine. Behav Brain Res 55:167–183. Caine SB, Heinrichs SC, Coffin VL, Koob GF (1995): Effects of the dopamine D-1 antagonist SCH 23390 microinjected into the accumbens, amygdala or striatum on cocaine self-administration in the rat. Brain Res 692:47–56. Canteras NS, Swanson LW (1992): Projections of the ventral subiculum to the amygdala, septum, and hypothalamus: A PHAL anterograde tract-tracing study in the rat. J Comp Neurol 324:180 –194. Conrad KL, Marinelli M, Wolf ME (2005): Cocaine-seeking behavior and AMPA receptor trafficking in the nucleus accumbens Soc Neurosci Abstr Program No. 1030.1. Cullinan WE, Herman JP, Watson SJ (1993): Ventral subicular interaction with the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus: Evidence for a relay in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. J Comp Neurol 332:1–20. De Vries TJ, Schoffelmeer AN, Binnekade R, Mulder AH, Vanderschuren LJ (1998): MK-801 reinstates drug-seeking behaviour in cocaine-trained rats. Neuroreport 9:637– 640. Di Ciano P, Everitt BJ (2004a): Conditioned reinforcing properties of stimuli paired with self-administered cocaine, heroin or sucrose: Implications for the persistence of addictive behaviour. Neuropharmacology 47(suppl 1): 202–213. Di Ciano P, Everitt BJ (2004b): Direct interactions between the basolateral amygdala and nucleus accumbens core underlie cocaine-seeking behavior by rats. J Neurosci 24:7167–7173. Epstein DH, Preston KL (2003): The reinstatement model and relapse prevention: a clinical perspective. Psychopharmacology 168:31– 41. Everitt BJ, Dickinson A, Robbins TW (2001): The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour. Brain Res Rev 36:129 –138. Everitt BJ, Parkinson JA, Olmstead MC, Arroyo M, Robledo P, Robbins TW (1999): Associative processes in addiction and reward. The role of amygdala-ventral striatal subsystems. Ann N Y Acad Sci 877:412– 438. Gawin FH, Kleber HD (1986): Abstinence symptomatology and psychiatric diagnosis in cocaine abusers. Clinical observations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43:107–113. Grimm JW, Fyall AM, Osincup DP (2005): Incubation of sucrose craving: effects of reduced training and sucrose pre-loading. Physiol Behav 84: 73–79. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y (2001): Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature 412:141–142. Grimm JW, Lu L, Hayashi T, Hope BT, Su TP, Shaham Y (2003): Time-dependent increases in brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein levels within the mesolimbic dopamine system after withdrawal from cocaine: Implications for incubation of cocaine craving. J Neurosci 23:742–747. Grimm JW, Shaham Y, Hope BT (2002): Effect of the cocaine and sucrose withdrawal period on extinction behavior, cue-induced reinstatement and protein levels of the dopamine transporter and tyrosine hydroxylase in limbic and cortical areas in rats. Behav Pharmacol 13:379 –388. Hankins WG, Garcia J, Rusiniak KW (1974): Cortical lesions: flavor illness and noise-shock conditioning. Behav Biol 10:173–181. Heidbreder CA, Hagan JJ (2005): Novel pharmacotherapeutic approaches for the treatment of drug addiction and craving. Curr Opin Pharmacol 5:107–118. Henke PG (1990): Hippocampal pathway to the amygdala and stress ulcer development. Brain Res Bull 25:691– 695.
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598 597 Holland PC, Gallagher M (2003): Double dissociation of the effects of lesions of basolateral and central amygdala on conditioned stimulus-potentiated feeding and Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Eur J Neurosci 17: 1680 –1894. Jentsch JD, Taylor JR (1999): Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharamacology 146:373–390. Kalivas PW (2004): Glutamate systems in cocaine addiction. Curr Opin Pharmacol 4:23–29. Kenny PJ, Markou A (2004): The ups and downs of addiction: role of metabotropic glutamate receptors. Trends Pharmacol Sci 25:265–272. Killcross S, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (1997): Different types of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within amygdala. Nature 388: 377–380. Kim JH, Austin JD, Tanabe L, Creekmore E, Vezina p (2005): Activation of group II mGlu receptors blocks the enhanced drug taking induced by previous exposure to amphetamine. Eur J Neurosci 21:295–300. Kim JH, Vezina p (2002): The mGlu2/3 receptor agonist LY379268 blocks the expression of locomotor sensitization by amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochemi Behav 73:333–337. Kosten TR, Kosten TA, Poling J, Oliveto A (2005): “Incubation” of cocaine relapse during a disulfiram clinical trial. CPDD, Annual Meeting Abstracts p. 90. Kruzich PJ, See RE (2001): Differential contributions of the basolateral and central amygdala in the acquisition and expression of conditioned relapse to cocaine-seeking behavior. J Neurosci 21:RC155. Lu L, Dempsey J, Liu SY, Bossert JM, Shaham Y (2004a): A single infusion of brain-derived neurotrophic factor into the ventral tegmental area induces long-lasting potentiation of cocaine seeking after withdrawal. J Neurosci 24:1604 –1611. Lu L, Grimm JW, Dempsey J, Shaham Y (2004b): Cocaine seeking over extended withdrawal periods in rats: Different time courses of responding induced by cocaine cues versus cocaine priming over the first 6 months. Psychopharmacology 176:101–108. Lu L, Grimm JW, Hope BT, Shaham Y (2004c): Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal: A review of preclinical data. Neuropharmacology 47(suppl 1):214 –226. Lu L, Hope BT, Dempsey J, Liu SY, Bossert JM, Shaham Y (2005): Central amygdala ERK signaling pathway is critical to incubation of cocaine craving. Nat Neurosci 8:212–219. MacCorkle RA, Tan TH (2005): Mitogen-activated protein kinases in cell-cycle control. Cell Biochem Biophys 43:451– 461. Mattson BJ, Bossert JM, Simmons DE, Nozaki N, Nagarkar D, Kreuter JD, Hope BT (2005): Cocaine-induced CREB phosphorylation in nucleus accumbens of cocaine-sensitized rats is enabled by enhanced activation of extracellular signal-related kinase, but not protein kinase A. J Neurochem 95:1481–1494. Meil WM, See RE (1996): Conditioned cued recovery of responding following prolonged withdrawal from self-administered cocaine in rats: An animal model of relapse. Behav Pharmacol 7:754 –763. Mogenson GJ, Jones DL, Yim CY (1980): From motivation to action: Functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Prog Neurobiol 14:69 –97. Morgan MA, LeDoux JE (1999): Contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 72:244 –251. Neisewander JL, Baker DA, Fuchs RA, Tran-Nguyen LT, Palmer A, Marshall JF (2000): Fos protein expression and cocaine-seeking behavior in rats after exposure to a cocaine self-administration environment. J Neurosci 20:798 – 805. Nestler EJ (2001): Molecular basis of long-term plasticity underlying addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:119 –128. O‘Brien CP, Childress AR, Mclellan TA, Ehrman R (1992): Classical conditioning in drug dependent humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci 654:400 – 415. Ohishi H, Neki A, Mizuno N (1998): Distribution of a metabotropic glutamate receptor, mGluR2, in the central nervous system of the rat and mouse: An immunohistochemical study with a monoclonal antibody. Neurosci Res 30:65– 82. Ottersen OP (1982): Connections of the amygdala of the rat. IV: Corticoamygdaloid and intraamygdaloid connections as studied with axonal transport of horseradish peroxidase. J Comp Neurol 205:30 – 48. Pascoe JP, Kapp BS (1987): Responses of amygdaloid central nucleus neurons to stimulation of the insular cortex in awake rabbits. Neuroscience 21:471– 485.
www.sobp.org/journal
598 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2007;61:591–598 Paxinos G, Watson C (2005): The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 5 ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic Press. Pennartz CM, McNaughton BL, Mulder AB (2000): The glutamate hypothesis of reinforcement learning. Prog Brain Res 126:231–253. Peters J, Kalivas PW (2006): The group II metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist, LY379268, inhibits both cocaine- and food-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmaocology (Berl) 186:143–149. Petralia RS, Wang YX, Niedzielski AS, Wenthold RJ (1996): The metabotropic glutamate receptors, mGluR2 and mGluR3, show unique postsynaptic, presynaptic and glial localizations. Neuroscience 71:949 –976. Pitkanen A (2000): Connectivity of the rat amygaloid complex. In: Aggleton JP, editor. The Amygdala: A Functional Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 31–115. Quirk GJ, Likhtik E, Pelletier JG, Pare D (2003): Stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex decreases the responsiveness of central amygdala output neurons. J Neurosci 23:8800 – 8807. Reynolds SM, Zahm DS (2005): Specificity in the projections of prefrontal and insular cortex to ventral striatopallidum and the extended amygdala. J Neurosci 25:11757–11767. Robledo P, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (1996): Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the central amygdaloid nucleus on the potentiation of reward-related stimuli by intra-accumbens amphetamine. Behav Neurosci 110:981–990. Rodd ZA, McKinzie DL, Bell RL, McQueen VK, Murphy JM, Schoepp DD, McBride WJ (in press): The metabotropic glutamate 2/3 receptor agonist LY404039 reduces alcohol-seeking but not alcohol self-administration in alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Behav Brain Res. Rudy JW, Matus-Amat p (2005): The ventral hippocampus supports a memory representation of context and contextual fear conditioning: Implications for a unitary function of the hippocampus. Behav Neurosci 119:154 –163. Sanger DJ, Blackman DE (1976): Rate-dependent effects of drugs: A review of the literature. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 4:73– 83. Schoepp DD (2001): Unveiling the functions of presynaptic metabotropic glutamate receptors in the central nervous system. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 299:12–20. See RE, Fuchs RA, Ledford CC, McLaughlin J (2003): Drug addiction, relapse, and the amygdala. Ann N Y Acad Sci 985:294 –307. See RE, Kruzich PJ, Grimm JW (2001): Dopamine, but not glutamate, receptor blockade in the basolateral amygdala attenuates conditioned reward in a rat model of relapse to cocaine-seeking behavior. Psychopharmacology 154:301–310. Self DW, Nestler EJ (1998): Relapse to drug-seeking: neural and molecular mechanisms. Drug Alcohol Depend 51:49 – 69. Shaham Y, Funk D, Erb S, Brown TJ, Walker CD, Stewart J (1997): Corticotropin-releasing factor, but not corticosterone, is involved in stress-induced relapse to heroin-seeking in rats. J Neurosci 17:2605–2614.
www.sobp.org/journal
L. Lu et al Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, De Wit H, Stewart J (2003): The reinstatement model of drug relapse: History, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology 168:3–20. Shalev U, Morales M, Hope B, Yap J, Shaham Y (2001): Time-dependent changes in extinction behavior and stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking following withdrawal from heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology 156:98 –107. Shepard JD, Bossert JM, Liu SY, Shaham Y (2004): The anxiogenic drug yohimbine reinstates methamphetamine seeking in a rat model of drug relapse. Biol Psychiatry 55:1082–1089. Sorge RE, Stewart J (2005): The contribution of drug history and time since termination of drug taking to footshock stress-induced cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology 183:210 –217. Stewart J, de Wit H (1987): Reinstatement of drug-taking behavior as a method of assessing incentive motivational properties of drugs. In: Bozarth MA, editor. Methods of Assessing the Reinforcing Properties of Abused Drugs. New York: Springer-Verlag, 211–227. Swanson LW, Petrovich GD (1998): What is the amygdala? Trends Neurosci 21:323–331. Taepavarapruk P, Phillips AG (2003): Neurochemical correlates of relapse to d-amphetamine self-administration by rats induced by stimulation of the ventral subiculum. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168:99 –108. Tamaru Y, Nomura S, Mizuno N, Shigemoto R (2001): Distribution of metabotropic glutamate receptor mGluR3 in the mouse CNS: Differential location relative to pre- and postsynaptic sites. Neuroscience 106:481–503. Valjent E, Corbille AG, Bertran-Gonzalez J, Herve D, Girault JA (2006): Inhibition of ERK pathway or protein synthesis during reexposure to drugs of abuse erases previously learned place preference. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:2932–2937. Valjent E, Corvol JC, Pages C, Besson MJ, Maldonado R, Caboche J (2000): Involvement of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase cascade for cocaine-rewarding properties. J Neurosci 20:8701– 8709. Vorel SR, Liu X, Hayes RJ, Spector JA, Gardner EL (2001): Relapse to cocaineseeking after hippocampal theta burst stimulation. Science 292:1175– 1178. Weiss F (2005): Neurobiology of craving, conditioned reward and relapse. Curr Opin Pharmacol 5:9 –19. White NM (1996): Addictive drugs as reinforcers: Multiple partial actions on memory systems. Addiction 91:921–949; discussion 951–965. Wise RA (1988): The neurobiology of craving: Implications for the understanding and treatment of addiction. J Abnorm Psychol 97:118 –132. Yoon S, Seger R (2006): The extracellular signal-regulated kinase: Multiple substrates regulate diverse cellular functions. Growth Factors 24:21– 44. Zhuravin IA, Bures J (1991): Extent of the tetrodotoxin induced blockade examined by pupillary paralysis elicited by intracerebral injection of the drug. Exp Brain Res 83:687– 690.