Accepted Manuscript Development and validation of the Food Disgust Scale Christina Hartmann, Michael Siegrist PII: DOI: Reference:
S0950-3293(17)30171-4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.07.013 FQAP 3369
To appear in:
Food Quality and Preference
Received Date: Revised Date: Accepted Date:
26 January 2017 25 July 2017 28 July 2017
Please cite this article as: Hartmann, C., Siegrist, M., Development and validation of the Food Disgust Scale, Food Quality and Preference (2017), doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.07.013
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
FOOD DISGUST SCALE
Development and validation of the Food Disgust Scale Christina Hartmann & Michael Siegrist ETH Zurich, Department Health Science and Technology (D-HEST), Consumer Behavior, Switzerland
Address for correspondence: Dr. Christina Hartmann ETH Zurich Department Health Science and Technology (D-HEST), Consumer Behavior Universitaetstrasse 22, CHN H75.3 CH-8092 Zurich Switzerland E-mail:
[email protected] Telephone: +41 (0)44 632 97 47
1
FOOD DISGUST SCALE
Development and validation of the Food Disgust Scale Abstract The function of disgust as a pathogen avoidance promoter in the food domain is not well understood. One problem is that no food-specific disgust scale is available. Thus, we developed and validated the Food Disgust Scale (FDS) through a series of five studies. The FDS is a self-report measure that enables the assessment of an individual’s emotional disposition to react with disgust to certain food-related (offensive) stimuli. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were used to develop eight FDS subscales that represent unique types of food disgust: animal flesh, poor hygiene, human contamination, mold, decaying fruit, fish, decaying vegetables, and living contaminants. The subscales showed good internal consistencies across three different adult samples (N between 170 and 527). Alongside the 32item version, an 8-item composite measure was developed and tested. Validity was supported by correlational analysis between the revised version of the Disgust Scale, germ aversion, food neophobia, picky eating, and individuals’ number of food-borne illnesses in the last five years. In addition, two-week test-retest reliability was very good. Incremental validity was supported in an eating experiment (i.e., willingness to eat insect-based food). The new scale will not only help improve the understanding of how food disgust shapes people’s food behavior in a functional and dysfunctional way, but will also help enhance the understanding of consumer acceptance of new foods and food technologies.
Keywords: disgust sensitivity, food neophobia, food acceptance, food choice, food-borne disease
2
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 1
1. Introduction
2
1.1. Functional domains of disgust
3
When considering evolutionary processes and the drivers of natural selection, one
4
would expect humans to have developed threat-detection systems and responses. Evidence
5
has suggested that a disgust reaction is one such highly functional system that aims to reduce
6
the contact with and thus the likelihood of infection from bacteria, parasites, and viruses.
7
Disgust is a regulatory human emotion thought to be a component of the behavioral immune
8
system because it cognitively triggers disease-preventive behavior to avoid health threats
9
(Terrizzi, Shook, & McDaniel, 2013). Besides the behavioral component, a feeling of disgust
10
is accompanied by specific physiological reactions and a characteristic facial expression.1
11
Food disgust, at its core, is a food-rejection emotion intended to prevent the ingestion
12
of potentially noxious and/or pathogen-laden substances (Chapman & Anderson, 2012; Haidt,
13
McCauley, & Rozin, 1994). Bitter tastes in particular are prototypically a stimulus for an
14
innate oral rejection, leading to spitting out the unpalatable, potentially toxic material
15
(Chapman & Anderson, 2012). Apart from distaste, disgust is triggered by cues that
16
symbolize hazardous items and the presence of pathogens including certain odors (e.g., smell
17
of decayed food), visual cues (e.g., mold), tactile cues (e.g., slime), and auditory input (e.g.,
18
hearing someone clear a throat full of mucus) (Curtis & Biran, 2001). Likewise, objects that
19
contact a disgusting object can become contaminated and a subsequent trigger of disgust.
20
Disgust elicitors can be not only culturally specific, but also the same across cultures
21
(Curtis & Biran, 2001; J. M. Tybur et al., 2013). Some elicitors are directly related to the
22
presence of pathogens, while others do not directly pose a health threat. The variability of
23
disgust elicitors within and across cultures caused researchers to develop a theoretical model
24
to classify disgust elicitors above and beyond the domain of pathogen avoidance – for
25
example disgust elicitors related to moral violations (Haidt et al., 1994; Tybur, Lieberman, &
26
Griskevicius, 2009; Tybur et al., 2013). In food research, the moral domain of disgust might
27
be of importance when it comes to either the acceptance of new food technologies (Scott,
28
Inbar, & Rozin, 2016) or differentiation between appropriate and inappropriate animal-based
29
food products. Most people in Western societies would probably call it disgusting (in terms of
30
morally unacceptable and offensive) to eat cats or dogs, while in certain non-Western
31
countries these animals are part of the countries’ cuisine. Food-related moral disgust is 1
For a comprehensive introduction to the topic of disgust, please see Chapman and Anderson
(2012), Oaten, Stevenson and Case (2009), Rozin, Haidt, and McCauley (2017), Schaller and Park (2011), Tybur, Liebermann, Kurzban and DeScioli (2013).
3
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 32
especially relevant with regard to culturally determined food appropriateness, though there is
33
presumably a low level of variation between individuals from the general population within a
34
cultural region. Therefore, the moral domain of disgust was not considered in the present
35
research. Rather, the focus was on both cues that might symbolize hazardous items and cues
36
that are not pathogen-related that may evoke a non-morally based disgust reaction. For
37
example, spoilage and decay of animal and non-animal food often coincide with changes in
38
color, texture, smell and taste, which are then recognized as disgust elucidating cues, even
39
though they are not necessarily an indicator of pathogen presence (Martins & Pliner, 2006).
40
Moreover, food contamination with human body fluids and products was identified as a
41
disgust elicitors in previous research (Haidt et al., 1994; Tybur et al., 2009).
42
In fact, people can vary in their sensitivity and reactivity toward such cues and thus
43
experience disgust in various situations, induced by various substances. On the one hand,
44
insensitivity to cues (e.g., mold on cheese) might inhibit the necessary preventive behavior,
45
leading people to expose themselves to higher risks. On the other hand, oversensitivity to non-
46
pathogen-related cues (e.g., black spots on a banana) and overgeneralization based on crudely
47
defined cues trigger false alarms and might lead to the neglect of viable food resources.
48
However, the functional and dysfunctional effects of food disgust sensitivity on eating
49
behavior still need to be explored. To explain the effect of food disgust on behavior, a reliable
50
and valid scale is needed. Therefore, the aim of the studies presented in the following, was to
51
develop and test a tool that can measure individual differences in the reactivity to cues that
52
may evoke food-related disgust.
53 54
1.2. Measuring disgust sensitivity
55
Various scales to measure disgust sensitivity have been proposed (Table 1). Haidt et
56
al. (1994) established the 32-item Disgust Scale (DS), which measures disgust sensitivity in
57
eight domains: food, animals, body products, sex, body envelope violations, death, hygiene,
58
and magic (contact with or visual appearance of a disgust elicitor). Olatunji, Williams, et al.
59
(2007) proposed a revised version of the DS (DS-R) with a reorganized item structure and a
60
reduced number of subscales (core disgust, contamination-based disgust, and animal reminder
61
disgust). Other researchers have also suggested new disgust measures. For example, Tybur,
62
Lieberman, and Griskevicius (2009) proposed the 21-item Three Domains Disgust Scale
63
including pathogen disgust, sexual disgust, and moral disgust. Van Overveld, de Jong, Peters,
64
Cavanagh, and Davey (2006) published the Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale (revised
65
by Fergus & Valentiner, 2009), which is supposed to measure the frequency of experiencing
4
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 66
disgust and the bodily and emotional impact of experienced disgust. These disgust scales
67
consist of three to eight subscales, and factor analyses of disgust sensitivity measures have
68
consistently produced multidimensional factor solutions (e.g., Kleinknecht, Kleinknecht, &
69
Thorndike, 1997; Olatunji, Williams, et al., 2007). Thus, researchers have suggested that
70
disgust sensitivity is domain specific (e.g., Tybur et al., 2009). For a more detailed review of
71
current disgust scales see Rozin et al. (2017).
72
A major deficit in the disgust literature, however, is that the published scales measure
73
overall disgust sensitivity or disgust sensitivity in certain domains; none of these scales is
74
sensitive enough to capture individual differences in disgust sensitivity in the domain of food.
75
Food-related items, if they are included in the disgust scales at all (Fergus & Valentiner,
76
2009), are diffusely spread among the subscales with questionable theoretical justifications
77
for the loadings and low mean factor loadings (Olatunji, Cisler, Deacon, Connolly, & Lohr,
78
2007). Moreover, in various studies using the DS by Haidt et al., low alpha reliabilities below
79
.40 did not allow the formation of a food-related subscale (Haidt et al., 1994; Olatunji, Cisler,
80
et al., 2007; Schienle, Walter, Stark, & Vaitl, 2002; Stark, Walter, Schienle, & Vaitl, 2005;
81
van Overveld, de Jong, Peters, & Schouten, 2011). In the scale by Tybur et al. (2009), the four
82
included food items had low factor loadings on one single factor (pathogen disgust) and were
83
not distinctive enough to provide sufficient information about the different types of food
84
disgust. Likewise, in the disgust scale by Kleinknecht et al. (1997), only disgust related to
85
rotting food is measured. To date, there is no extensive measure that captures disgust
86
sensitivity in the domain of food.
87 88
1.3. Food disgust2
89
Rozin and Fallon (1987) suggested that food rejection can be classified into four
90
types: distaste, danger, inappropriateness and disgust. In the present study, the focus was
91
solely on disgust and the development of a food-specific disgust measure. The measure
92
focuses on the food domain and people’s ability to detect pathogen-related cues (functional
93
cues) that symbolize potentially hazardous items. It also considers sensitivity to certain cues
94
that are actually not pathogen related or that do not indicate a health threat. Within the scope
95
of the new scale proposed here, disgust sensitivity was defined based on the conceptualization
96
by Haidt et al. (1994) and Olatunji et al. (2007). Thus, the new scale measures food disgust as
97
a measure of trait disgust (i.e., a person’s emotional predisposition to be more or less easily 2
The English word “disgust/disgusted” was used to signify being sickened or grossed out in
the presented studies.
5
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 98
disgusted by certain food-related cues). The focus was on potential disgust elicitors and not
99
on items that are avoided because of a special medical condition such as a food allergy or
100
lactose intolerance. The new scale was also not intended to measure peoples’ reactions
101
towards toxic substances or items (e.g. fly agaric or deadly nightshade) that do not show
102
disgust-elucidating cues (e.g. mold, slime, or a bad smell). In this context, knowledge seems
103
to be a more relevant factor than disgust sensitivity.
104
After an extensive literature review on disgust and food rejection (Curtis & Biran,
105
2001; Haidt et al., 1994; Martins & Pliner, 2006), we aimed to find a representative selection
106
of food-specific disgust cues. We chose cues that are related to the process of decay (e.g.,
107
mold), hygienic aspects of food preparation (e.g. an unclean cook), reminders of animal origin
108
(e.g., blood), visible and invisible contamination (e.g., hair), and hygiene (e.g., dirty
109
silverware). Different food groups were selected as carriers of these cues. Food-cue
110
combinations were chosen that were familiar to most people in Western societies and that
111
worked equally well in English and German language. We asked if participants perceived
112
something as disgusting instead of whether they were willing to eat it. The intention to eat
113
may not be a useful predictor of food disgust sensitivity because of the various reasons that
114
people eat something despite being disgusted by it (e.g. social pressure and/or prevention of
115
food waste).
116
The development of the new Food Disgust Scale (FDS) was processed in three phases.
117
In the first phase, the construct of interest, food disgust, was defined based on previously
118
established theories of disgust and pathogen avoidance. Items were developed and tested.
119
Item performance was analyzed, and the scale was constructed. In the second phase, the new
120
scale was tested in another sample, and various construct validations were accomplished,
121
including test-retest reliability. In the third phase, the scale’s incremental validity was tested
122
within the scope of a study on willingness to eat.
123 124 125
2. Item generation and scale construction (Study 1)
126
Study 1 aimed to generate items that measure food-related disgust sensitivity and to
127
identify an underlying factor structure. No assumptions were made regarding the
128
dimensionality of the new scale.
129 130
2.1. Material and methods
131
6
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 132
2.1.1. Survey participants
133
Data collection for the first study occurred in January 2015 in Switzerland. The study
134
participants were recruited from an Internet panel from a commercial provider of sampling
135
services (Respondi AG). Excluded were respondents who did not complete the survey and
136
whose total survey duration was less than half of the median of the total survey duration (e.g.,
137
Hartmann, Keller, & Siegrist, 2016), which indicates that the respondent did not seriously
138
answer the questions (n = 18). Quota samples were used with the quota variables of gender
139
and age. The sample consisted of 318 respondents (Table 2).
140 141
2.1.2. Item generation
142
We developed a large pool of 63 items. For example, participants were asked how
143
disgusting they perceived “the mold-free part of a partially moldy tomato,” “an apple that
144
dropped on the street,” or “food prepared by unknown neighbors.” As in previous research
145
(Tybur et al., 2009), items were rated on a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (not disgusting at all
146
[in German: überhaupt nicht eklig]) to 6 (totally disgusting [extrem eklig])3. The first set of
147
items was pretested with a small sample of people of different ages and educational
148
backgrounds. Ambiguous and unclear items were revised, and sources of misunderstanding
149
were fixed. Face validity, which indicates to what extent the items are subjectively viewed as
150
to have captured the underlying concept, was assessed by two research assistants.
151 152
2.1.3. Item analysis and scale construction
153
The 63 items were analyzed using exploratory factor analysis (principal component
154
analysis [PCA] with varimax rotation) to examine the underlying factor structure. When
155
selecting the number of factors, the following criteria were used: (a) all factors with
156
eigenvalues greater than 1.0, (b) the point of inflection on the scree plot, and (c) a good
157
interpretability of the factors. In addition, items with cross-loadings and loadings of <.40 were
158
excluded (Stevens, 2012). Reliability analysis (Cronbach’s alpha) was conducted for every
159
subscale. Items with item-total correlations of <.30, items that did not affect reliability when
160
excluded, and items that were redundant with other higher-loading items were excluded as
161
well.
162
Since the option “forced response” (available for online surveys) was used, there were
163
no cases of missing data. All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS Statistics
164
software package version 23 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). 3
For a discussion of the translation of disgust in this context, please see the discussion section.
7
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 165 166
2.2. Results
167
Initial item analysis revealed that participants used the whole 6-point response scale
168
for all the items. Thirty-two items were dropped based on the criteria described above. The
169
reduced item pool was submitted to a final exploratory factor analysis. The Kaiser-Meyer-
170
Olkin (KMO) measure was 0.88, and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was χ2(465) = 5868.81, p
171
< .001), which verified the sampling adequacy of the analysis and indicated that the
172
correlations between items were sufficiently large for a PCA. Seven components had
173
eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and together explained 68.6% of the variance. Based
174
on the scree plot and the interpretability of the factors, a seven-factor solution was considered
175
the best model. Items of the same food type or disgust cue loaded on one factor. They were
176
labeled (1) animal flesh, (2) poor hygiene, (3) human contamination, (4) mold, (5) decaying
177
fruit, (6) fish, and (7) living contaminants. The subscales consisted of two to six items. The
178
alpha reliabilities of the single subscales were very good, ranging from α = .75 (living
179
contaminants) to α = .89 (fish and animal flesh).
180 181
2.3. Discussion
182
The first study developed a new scale that measures food disgust sensitivity. The
183
analysis revealed a seven-factor solution, and the resulting subscales showed good internal
184
consistencies in a sample of adults. Results of the study suggest that the structure of food
185
disgust in adults may be best characterized by the nature of the stimuli, distinguished by the
186
food group and the origin of the disgust-eliciting food cue. However, the factor structure of
187
the FDS observed in Study 1 requires confirmation in another sample. In addition, the
188
vegetable food group was not covered by the items in Study 1, and the living contaminants
189
subscale consisted of only two items. Therefore, some new items were developed with regard
190
to vegetables and living contaminants and included in Study 2.
191 192 193
3. Scale refinement and development of a short version (Study 2)
194
One of the aims of Study 2 was to test if the basic factor structure found in Study 1
195
could be replicated in another sample of Swiss adults. The study also explored if the newly
196
developed items considering the potentially disgust-eliciting cues of vegetables form a new
197
subscale. The newly constructed scale enables differentiation between various types of food
8
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 198
disgust but consists of a large number of items. Therefore, another aim of Study 2 was to
199
develop a short version of the FDS. Gender and age effects were also explored.
200 201
3.1. Material and methods
202 203
3.1.1. Survey participants
204
Data collection for the second study occurred in June 2015 in the German-speaking
205
part of Switzerland. The study participants were recruited from an Internet panel from a
206
commercial provider of sampling services (Respondi AG). The exclusion criteria of Study 1
207
were also applied here (n = 54). Again, quota samples were used with the quota variables of
208
gender and age. The sample consisted of 527 respondents (Table 2).
209 210
3.1.2. Scale refinement and verification
211
Based on the factor structure of Study 1, the vegetable-related items were expected to
212
form a new eighth factor, and the new item related to living contaminants was expected to
213
load on the corresponding factor. This was tested by performing an exploratory factor analysis
214
(varimax rotation) on the 36 items. The same criteria for the number of selected factors like in
215
Study 1 were used. In the second step, the proposed factor model from the exploratory factor
216
analysis was tested with a confirmatory factor analysis using maximum-likelihood estimation.
217
Model fit was examined via the chi-square statistic, the root mean square error of
218
approximation (RMSEA < 0.05), the comparative fit index (CFI > 0.90), and the normed fit
219
index (NFI > 0.90). Modification indices were checked to identify redundant items.
220 221
3.1.3. Development of the FDS short version
222
The FDS subscales identified here are considered specific manifestations of a more
223
general construct (i.e., food disgust). Therefore, a composite score in the form of a short
224
version of the scale was calculated (Clark & Watson, 1995). A common method for
225
constructing a short-form measure is to select items with the highest item-total correlations
226
and the highest face validity (Widaman, Little, Preacher, & Sawalani, 2011, pp. 52-53). This
227
method was used in the present study, and appropriate items were chosen from every subscale
228
of the final FDS. The short form was tested using confirmatory factor analysis with
229
maximum-likelihood estimation, and the model fit criteria described above were applied.
230
9
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 231
3.1.4. Statistical analysis
232
Mean scores and Cronbach’s alphas across the subscales were calculated. Gender
233
differences in food disgust sensitivity were tested using student’s t-test. Associations with age
234
were assessed using correlational analysis. Data were analyzed using the SPSS Statistics
235
software package version 23 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and SPSS AMOS.
236 237
3.2. Results
238
The PCA with the 31 items from Study 1 and the five new items suggested an eight-
239
factor solution. Likewise, the 36-item confirmatory factor analysis indicated an adequate
240
eight-factor fitting model. An examination of the modification indices suggested significant
241
redundancy between four items. The removal of the four items resulted in a 32-item good-
242
2 fitting model, χ (435) = 945.37, p < .001, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.05, NFI = 0.91. Each of
243
the eight factors represent unique aspects of food disgust. The factors representing different
244
types of food disgust were labeled as in Study 1: animal flesh (4 items), poor hygiene (5
245
items), human contamination (4 items), mold (4 items), decaying fruit (4 items), fish (4
246
items), and living contaminants (3 items). The new eighth factor identified in this study was
247
named “decaying vegetables” (4 items). Each of the eight factors demonstrated good internal
248
consistency. The final model, standardized factor loadings, and Cronbach’s alphas for each
249
subscale are presented in Table 3 and Figure 1. The eight subscales were intercorrelated. The
250
intercorrelations were of small or medium size and therefore acceptable. The highest
251
correlation occurred between animal flesh and fish (r = .71, p < .001), which is probably
252
because both originate in or are related to animal-based food. To obtain a good model, the
253
error terms of two items (HYG4, HYG5) were allowed to be correlated. Both of these items
254
are related to body products; therefore, this correlation can be justified.
255
2 The eight-item model fit was good, χ (18) = 44.02, p = .001, CFI = 0.97, RMSEA =
256
0.05, NFI = 0.95. The standardized factor loadings varied between 0.41 and 0.68 (Figure 2).
257
Cronbach’s alpha was .77, which is acceptable for short versions (Widaman et al., 2011, p.
258
46). In addition, the error terms of the items MEAT1 and FISH4, as well as those of the items
259
FRUIT4 and VEGI1, were allowed to be correlated to improve the model fit statistics. This
260
makes sense from a theoretical point of view because the corresponding items belong to
261
similar food groups. The items of the short scale and corrected item-total correlations are
262
depicted in Table 4.
263
Women scored significantly higher on all subscales of the FDS except for decaying
264
fruit and human contamination (Table 5). A larger difference between genders was observed
10
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 265
for the subscales of poor hygiene and animal flesh, while smaller differences were noted for
266
the subscales of mold and decaying vegetables. Significant gender differences were also
267
observed for the short version of the scale. With regard to age, small positive correlations
268
were found with the FDS subscales of poor hygiene (r = .17, p < .001), decaying vegetables (r
269
= .12, p < .01), and human contamination (r = .16, p < .001). Animal flesh was negatively
270
correlated with age (r = -.17, p < .001). The other four subscales and the short FDS were not
271
significantly correlated with age (p > .01).
272 273
3.3. Discussion
274
Results of Study 2 show that an eight-factor model fit the data very well. The new
275
scale allows for the measurement of a disgust experience induced by cues related to the
276
process of food decay and thus induced by specific characteristics of aging foods, such as
277
black spots on a banana or wrinkled radishes. Moreover, the poor hygiene and human
278
contamination subscales cover two distinct sources of disgust caused by other familiar or
279
unfamiliar persons. While the human contamination subscale assesses whether people are
280
disgusted by certain socially accepted behaviors such as sharing a glass with a friend, the poor
281
hygiene subscale includes food preparation and consumption behaviors that are not socially
282
accepted and generally considered unhygienic (e.g., another person’s hair in one’s food).
283
The animal flesh subscale is based on items that are reminders of the animal nature of
284
meat. People seem to vary in their sensitivity to disgust over cues that reflect the origin of
285
meat (e.g., seeing a whole pig en brochette) and the associated bloody slaughter of animals
286
(e.g., seeing raw meat). Previous researchers identified these associations as potential disgust
287
elicitors because they are reminders of death and eating living creatures (Rozin, Haidt, &
288
McCauley, 2000). However, the animal flesh subscale of the FDS should not be confused
289
with the animal reminder subscale of the DS-R. The latter was described as reflecting “the
290
aversion of stimuli that serve as reminders of the animal origins of humans” (Olatunji,
291
Williams, et al., 2007, p. 285). Thus, the animal reminder subscale of the DS-R focuses on
292
humans and reminders of human mortality (e.g., seeing a man with his intestines exposed
293
after an accident), while the animal flesh subscale focuses on meat in the context of food and
294
eating. The fish subscale is based on stimuli (e.g., the smell or texture of fish) that are related
295
to characteristics of the food as well as reminders of the animal nature of the food (e.g.,
296
having a whole fish with its head on one’s plate). In addition, both meat and fish, especially
297
when they are raw, are often contaminated with pathogens that drastically increase in number
298
during the ageing process. This high pathogen load often leads to changes in the appearance
11
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 299
of the food and can cause a number of food-borne illnesses. This might be another aspect that
300
influences peoples’ disgust experience.
301
Some items of the FDS contain information about the texture of a food (e.g., “eating
302
limper salad,” “wrinkled radishes,” or “the texture of certain kinds of fish in the mouth”).
303
Other items give hints about the changed color of the food, indicating decaying food (e.g.,
304
“brown spots on a banana” or “brown flesh of an avocado”). Both texture and color are
305
characteristics that typically change when food gets older, even though such changes are not
306
necessarily an indication of expiration or inedibility. Some people are more sensitive to these
307
kinds of cues than others, as manifested in the varying scores on the subscales. It is
308
noteworthy to mention that, based on the statistical analysis, two separate factors (or
309
subscales) for fruit and vegetables were identified. They were rather similar and highly inter-
310
correlated (r = .69), because they were both related to the process of decay and accompanying
311
color and texture changes. However, the somehow comparable cues might be perceived
312
differently when changes in color (e.g. black spots on a banana) are associated with changes
313
in taste (e.g., increased sweetness).
314
Another aim of Study 2 was to develop a short version of the FDS. The eight-item
315
short version enables researchers to measure food disgust in a time-efficient way. Applied
316
research in particular should use the shorter version when a detailed assessment of food
317
disgust types is not relevant. Applications in the domain of food could be associations with
318
food waste behavior, kitchen hygienic behavior, or the prevalence of food-borne diseases.
319
Although short scales have poorer psychometric properties (e.g., lower Cronbach’s alpha than
320
longer forms), overall results of the present study suggest that the short form of the FDS is
321
adequate for many research purposes.
322
Previous studies have shown that women show more disgust sensitivity than men in
323
various domains such as hygiene, contamination-based disgust, and animal reminder disgust
324
(Haidt et al., 1994; Olatunji, Williams, et al., 2007; Petrowski et al., 2010). The same was
325
observed in the domain of food. Women scored significantly higher on almost all subscales
326
and the short FDS. Only the two subscales of human contamination and decaying fruit
327
showed no gender effects. In line with previous results (Petrowski et al., 2010), the
328
associations with age were weak in the present study. Although people might experience
329
changes in disgust sensitivity after certain events (e.g., transition to motherhood) or
330
physiological circumstances (e.g., illness), food disgust sensitivity might be rather stable over
331
the adult life course. Nevertheless, further research has to confirm this cross-sectional
332
observation.
12
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 333 334 335
4. Validity testing (Study 3)
336
Convergent and discriminant validation was performed for both the short and the long
337
version of the FDS. Convergent validity was tested in relation to overall disgust sensitivity
338
and germ aversion, which describes an individual’s tendency to be frightened of germs
339
(Duncan, Schaller, & Park, 2009). Germ aversion was expected to be positively associated
340
with the FDS. The discriminant validity of the FDS was tested in relation to food neophobia
341
and picky eating. Food neophobia is an established psychological construct that describes a
342
person’s tendency to reject or avoid eating unfamiliar food or food from other cultures. Food
343
neophobia has an impact on people’s diet quality (Siegrist, Hartmann, & Keller, 2013) and
344
can hinder people from accepting new food sources such as insects (Hartmann, Shi, Giusto, &
345
Siegrist, 2015). Picky eating encompasses the rejection of familiar food and can be present in
346
adults (Dovey, Staples, Gibson, & Halford, 2008; Kauer, Pelchat, Rozin, & Zickgraf, 2015).
347
Food disgust was expected to share some common variance with food neophobia and picky
348
eating because there is empirical evidence that disgust is an underlying aspect of both
349
constructs (Al-Shawaf, Lewis, Alley, & Buss, 2015; Kauer et al., 2015).
350
Another validity-supporting relationship that was tested in this study was the
351
association between food disgust sensitivity and the occurrence of food-borne diseases.
352
Again, a function of disgust is to prevent people from the ingestion of potentially pathogenic
353
substances or spoiled food. In other words, disgust may reduce the likelihood of food
354
poisoning that is a direct consequence of eating spoiled or contaminated food. Following this
355
line of thinking, it is reasonable to assume that people with low food disgust sensitivity are
356
more likely to expose themselves to food risks or that people who have experienced several
357
food-borne diseases are more disgust sensitive. Thus, we expected a positive association
358
between food disgust sensitivity and an individual’s experience of food-borne diseases in the
359
last five years.
360 361
4.1. Material and methods
362 363 364
4.1.1. Participants The study used the same sample of Swiss adults as in Study 2 (see Section 3.1.1).
365 366
4.1.2. Measures
13
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 367
The study used the DS-R (Haidt et al., 1994; revised by Olatunji, Williams, et al.,
368
2007). The DS-R consists of 25 items that are supposed to measure disgust sensitivity in three
369
domains: core disgust, animal reminder disgust, and contamination-based disgust. Sample
370
items are “It bothers me to hear someone clear a throat full of mucus” and “Even if I were
371
hungry, I would not drink a bowl of my favorite soup if it had been stirred by a used but
372
thoroughly washed flyswatter.” Since no German version is available for the DS-R, all items
373
had to be translated into German by the first author. The second author and another member
374
of the research group reviewed the translation. Individual sum scores were calculated
375
according to the scoring guide from Olatunji, Williams, et al. (2007), with higher scores
376
indicating higher disgust sensitivity. The following mean values were observed: M = 7.43 (SD
377
= 2.33, possible range = 0–12) for core disgust, M = 4.18 (SD = 2.04, possible range = 0–8,)
378
for animal reminder disgust, M = 1.86 (SD = 1.24, possible range = 0–5) for contamination-
379
based disgust, and M = 13.48 (SD = 4.72, possible range = 0–25) for the whole DS-R. The
380
alpha reliabilities of the DS-R subscales were .69 (core), .75 (animal reminder), and .58
381
(contamination-based). The alpha reliability for the whole scale was 0.82. Mean values were
382
comparable but alpha reliabilities were lower compared with those observed in the original
383
article by Olatunji, Williams, et al. (2007).
384
Food neophobia was measured with the validated German version (Siegrist et al.,
385
2013) of the scale by Pliner and Hobden (1992). A sample item is “I am very particular about
386
the foods I will eat.” As in the original scale, participants answered on a 7-point response
387
scale ranging from -3 (do not agree at all) to 3 (totally agree). The extreme categories were
388
verbally anchored, and the other categories were only numerically anchored. Cronbach’s
389
alpha was good (α = .87). Mean food neophobia scores were calculated by averaging all 10
390
items (M = 2.96, SD = 1.14).
391
Pickiness was measured with four items (i.e., “I consider myself to be a picky eater,”
392
“I have been called a picky eater,” “I think that many foods are disgusting,” and “I find many
393
foods distasteful”) from the food and eating questionnaire by Raudenbush, Van der Klaaus,
394
and Frank (1995). All items were translated into German by the first author and reviewed by
395
the second author and another member of the research group. Answers were given on a 5-
396
point scale ranging from 1 (totally agree) to 5 (do not agree at all). Scale scores represent the
397
average of the four items (M = 2.34, SD = 0.99, Cronbach’s α = .85).
398
Germ aversion was measured with the germ aversion subscale of the Perceived
399
Vulnerability to Disease Scale by Duncan et al. (2009). Sample items are “It really bothers me
400
when people sneeze without covering their mouths” and “I do not like to write with a pencil
14
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 401
someone else has obviously chewed on.” All items were translated into German by the first
402
author and reviewed by the second author and another member of the research group.
403
Respondents answered on a 7-point response scale ranging from 1 (totally agree) to 7 (do not
404
agree at all). The scale scores represent the average of six items 4 (M = 3.97, SD = 1.01,
405
Cronbach’s α = .51).
406
Participants were asked to indicate how many food-borne diseases (with and without
407
medical diagnosis) they suffered from within the last five years. Typical symptoms of such
408
diseases are diarrhea, stomach cramps, nausea, sickness, vomiting, and fever. The response
409
scale varied from never to more than seven. Most participants (62.2%) indicated that they did
410
not have a food-borne disease within the last five years, 15.7% had one, and 16.1% had two
411
or more. “Do not know” responses were coded as missing (n = 31 or 5.9%).
412 413
4.1.3. Statistical analysis
414
Correlational analyses were used to examine the validity of the FDS scores in relation
415
to the overall disgust measure (DS-R), food neophobia, pickiness, germ aversion, and
416
occurrence of food-borne diseases. The eight subscales of the FDS and the short version of
417
the FDS were tested. Data were analyzed using the SPSS Statistics software package version
418
23 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).
419 420
4.2. Results
421
All FDS subscales were significantly correlated with the DS-R and its subscales, but
422
the strength of the relationships differed between the various subscales (Table 6). For
423
example, the human contamination subscale of the FDS correlated highly with the
424
contamination subscale of the DS-R (r = .46, p < .001), while lower correlations were
425
observed for the other two DS-R subscales (core, animal-reminder). High correlations were
426
also observed between the DS-R core subscale and the FDS subscales of poor hygiene, mold,
427
and living contaminants (r = .43–.53, all p < .001), while lower correlations were found for
428
decaying fruit, fish, animal flesh, and human contamination (r = .28–.31, all p < .001).
429
As depicted in Table 6, the short FDS moderately correlated with food neophobia and
430
pickiness (r = .37 and r = .35, respectively, p < .001). The fish and animal flesh subscales had
431
the highest correlation with food neophobia (r = .42 and r = .32, p < .001), while the poor 4
One item was considered out of date and was not included in the scale (“I avoid using public telephones
because of the risk that I may catch something from the previous user.”). Unfortunately, another item got lost in the programming of the survey (“I prefer to wash my hands pretty soon after shaking someone’s hand.”).
15
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 432
hygiene subscale was not correlated with food neophobia, indicating discriminant validity. A
433
similar pattern of correlations was observed for pickiness.
434
The short FDS and germ aversion were moderately correlated (r = .45, p < .001; Table
435
6). The highest correlations between germ aversion and the FDS were observed for the
436
subscales of poor hygiene (r = .41, p < .001), human contamination (r = .50, p < .001), and
437
living contaminants (r = .38, p < .001). Except for fish (r = .20, p < .001) and animal flesh (r
438
= .22, p < .001), which had relatively lower correlations with germ aversion, the remaining
439
subscales were related to the process of decay and thus reasonably associated with germ
440
aversion. Nevertheless, the effect sizes differed, which again shows convergent and
441
discriminant validity.
442
Correlational analysis revealed that the short FDS and the number of food-borne
443
diseases in the last five years were positively albeit weakly correlated (r = .13, p < .01). No
444
significant correlation was observed for the DS-R and the number of food-borne diseases
445
experienced in the last five years (r = .07, p = .11).
446 447
4.3. Discussion
448
The results of Study 3 provide support for the convergent and discriminant validity of
449
the FDS. The FDS subscales are more or less strongly and significantly correlated with the
450
DS-R subscales. The magnitude and direction of these criterion-related correlations are
451
consistent with theoretical considerations and the item contents of the two scales. The core
452
subscale of the DS-R is a mix of items including eating culturally inappropriate food (e.g.,
453
monkey meat and vanilla ice cream with ketchup), contact with insects and animals (e.g.,
454
seeing a rat on one’s path, seeing a cockroach in someone else’s house, and stepping on an
455
earthworm), consuming spoiled food, and hygienic aspects (e.g., a friend who changes
456
underwear only once a week). Thus, the observed pattern of correlation supports the
457
convergent and discriminant validity of the FDS, with the highest correlations observed for
458
the subscales of poor hygiene, mold, decaying vegetables, and living contaminants, and the
459
lowest correlations observed for fish and animal flesh. Of course, the DS-R is not tailored for
460
application in the food domain, and the pattern of correlations highlights that the established
461
disgust measure, DS-R, does not assess the full range of existing food-related disgust types.
462
The correlational pattern between the FDS subscales on the one hand and pickiness
463
and food neophobia on the other hand suggest that both selective eating behaviors are related
464
to food disgust sensitivity. In particular, people who tend to be easily disgusted by certain
465
food-related offensive stimuli tend to be more reluctant to eat unfamiliar food and food from
16
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 466
other cultures. Even though food disgust sensitivity is one influential factor in food neophobia
467
(Al-Shawaf et al., 2015), the effect size of the relationship confirms that they are different
468
psychological constructs. All food groups covered in the FDS are familiar to most people in
469
Western cultures, which is an important factor that discriminates between both constructs.
470
One of the first studies that empirically showed an association between food neophobia and
471
disgust utilized the Three-Domains Disgust Scale by Tybur et al. (2009). A correlation of .23
472
was found for pathogen disgust and food neophobia, while in the present study, a stronger
473
relationship (r = .37) was observed for food neophobia and food-related disgust sensitivity. In
474
addition, observed correlational associations differed by food disgust type, with the highest
475
correlations observed for fish and animal flesh and no or low correlations observed for poor
476
hygiene and living contaminants. Altogether, the results show that (1) unlike the food
477
neophobia scale or pickiness, the FDS is not a measure of reluctance to eat certain foods; (2)
478
the link between food neophobia and disgust sensitivity is slightly stronger than previously
479
assumed; and (3) not all food disgust types are equally strongly related to food neophobia and
480
pickiness.
481
The results of the present study suggest that food disgust sensitivity is positively
482
linked to the individual’s experiences of food-borne diseases in the last five years. However,
483
the effect size is small, and multiple factors surrounding food preparation and consumption
484
influence whether a person is exposed to contaminated food in the first place and whether the
485
person will eat the food and become ill. Some population subgroups such as children or older
486
people are more susceptible to becoming ill, often because of reduced immune function or
487
unfavorable physiological conditions (Gerba, Rose, & Hass, 1996). People who are low in
488
food disgust sensitivity and rather insensitive to pathogen-related food cues seem to be
489
another subgroup at increased risk. In the present study, the severity of the illness symptoms
490
was not assessed, and people who experienced severe symptoms might be more likely to
491
remember these events. Thus, the relationship between food disgust and experience of food-
492
borne diseases might have been underestimated in this investigation. Moreover, we do not
493
know the direction of the association; longitudinal studies need to further investigate whether
494
food-borne diseases are a cause or a consequence of varying levels of food disgust sensitivity.
495
It must be noted that no statistically significant association was found between the DS-R and
496
the number of food-borne diseases experienced by the individual. Hence, the application of
497
the FDS in the food domain has the potential to outperform previous non-food-specific
498
disgust measures such as the DS-R in the prediction of food behavior and its consequences.
499
17
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 500 501 502 503
5. Test-retest reliability (Study 4) The aim of Study 4 was to evaluate the short-term stability of the FDS scores by examining the two-week test-retest reliability.
504 505
5.1. Material and methods
506 507
5.1.1. Survey participants
508
Participants from an Internet panel completed the online version of the FDS twice, two
509
weeks apart. The Internet panel is composed of individuals from the general population who
510
voluntarily participate in online studies from time to time. The first survey had 224
511
participants, and the second survey had 236 participants. The data cleaning procedure
512
included the deletion of participants whose demographic characteristics (i.e., gender, age , and
513
educational level) differed between the first and second survey. Data from both surveys were
514
matched using an anonymized personalized code. The final sample consisted of 170 persons
515
(Table 2).
516 517
5.2. Results and discussion
518
Cronbach’s alphas were calculated and Pearson correlations were computed for the
519
eight subscales and the 8-item short version. Cronbach’s alphas varied between .79 and .93
520
for the subscales, and Cronbach’s alpha was .77 for the short FDS (Table 7). These values
521
were similar to those observed in Study 2, which suggests that the Cronbach’s alphas of the
522
FDS scale are stable across different study samples. Test-retest correlations for the subscales
523
varied between .75 and .90; for the FDS short scale, the correlation was .86, which indicates
524
good test-retest reliability. Thus, the scale consistently reflects food disgust sensitivity over a
525
period of two weeks.
526 527 528
6. Incremental validity (Study 5)
529
Having provided support for the convergent and discriminant validity of the FDS
530
scale, it is still unclear whether the FDS is incrementally valid and has predictive potential for
531
behavioral responses to certain foods. The aim of Study 5 was to test whether consumers’
532
willingness to eat unfamiliar, novel food (i.e., products made from insects) varies as a
533
function of food disgust sensitivity.
18
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 534
In most Western societies including Switzerland, insects are viewed as non-food,
535
dirty, health risks, and food contaminants (Kellert, 1993; Looy, Dunkel, & Wood, 2014) and
536
might even be indicators of low hygienic standards. In addition, they are of animal origin and
537
therefore processed insect-based products might have animal reminders that provoke disgust.
538
Hence, three out of the eight FDS subscales were predicted to be highly relevant to the
539
willingness to eat insect-based products (i.e., living contaminants, animal flesh, and poor
540
hygiene). Individuals who score high on these subscales are probably more reluctant to try
541
insects or insect-based meat substitutes than those who score lower on these subscales. In
542
addition, previous studies showed that overall disgust sensitivity (Ruby, Rozin, & Chan,
543
2015) and food neophobia (e.g., Hartmann et al., 2015; Verbeke, 2015) were important
544
constructs in predicting the willingness to eat insects. It was hypothesized that FDS scores
545
predict additional variance in the willingness to eat insects over and above food neophobia.
546
This was tested with the following experiment.
547 548
6.1. Material and methods
549 550
6.1.1. Participants
551
This study was part of another project about the willingness to eat insects (Hartmann
552
& Siegrist, 2016). The study took place in the German-speaking part of Switzerland at the
553
research facilities of ETH Zurich. A convenience sample was recruited through web-based
554
advertisements, flyers in supermarkets, and an Internet panel of people who agreed to
555
regularly participate in experiments and surveys. From 107 recruited individuals from the
556
general population, 18 vegans and vegetarians were excluded in addition to three other people
557
for reasons previously reported (for details see Hartmann & Siegrist, 2016). The original
558
study sample consisted of a control group and an experimental group. For the present study,
559
only data from the control group were analyzed (N = 43; Table 2).
560 561
6.1.2. Measures
562
Participants answered a computer-administered version of the FDS. Cronbach’s alphas
563
were calculated for the three relevant subscales and the FDS short version: .77 for animal
564
flesh, .88 for poor hygiene, .89 for living contaminants, and .78 for the FDS short scale. These
565
values are similar to those observed in Study 2. Mean scores were as follows: M = 3.35 (SD =
566
0.93) for the FDS short scale, M = 2.51 (SD = 1.1) for animal flesh, M = 4.99 (SD = 0.93) for
19
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 567
poor hygiene, and M = 4.53 (SD = 1.31) for living contaminants. These mean scores were
568
comparable to those observed in Study 2.
569
Food neophobia was assessed with the German version (Siegrist et al., 2013) of the
570
food neophobia scale by Pliner and Hobden (1992). For a more detailed description of the
571
scale, see section 4.1.2. Cronbach’s alpha for food neophobia (α = 0.84) was good. The mean
572
score for food neophobia was M = 2.40 (SD = 0.84).
573
Participants were asked whether they were willing to eat insect-based products. These
574
products are made with ground insects so that the insect origin is no longer visible. Following
575
the procedure previously described (Hartmann et al., 2015; Hartmann & Siegrist, 2016),
576
participants were informed about the following ideas regarding insects: Insects are a good
577
source of high-value protein, their production requires little space, and their feed conversion
578
rate is efficient. Therefore, eating insects can provide benefits in terms of sustainability. After
579
receiving this information, the participants indicated their willingness to eat three different
580
kinds of products made with processed insects: cookies made with cricket flour, a drink made
581
with silkworm protein, and a burger made from buffalo worms. The cookies and the burger
582
were displayed in a picture. All three items were answered on a 10-point scale ranging from 1
583
(do not agree at all) to 10 (totally agree). The extreme categories were verbally anchored; the
584
other categories were only numerically anchored. A mean value for willingness to eat across
585
all three products was calculated. Cronbach’s alpha for willingness to eat (α = 0.90) was
586
excellent, and the mean score (M = 6.52, SD = 2.69) was slightly above the midpoint of the
587
scale.
588 589
6.1.3. Statistical analysis
590
In the first hierarchical regression, the three FDS subscales of animal flesh, poor
591
hygiene, and living contaminants were included as independent variables to estimate their
592
predictive validity on willingness to eat. Two additional hierarchical linear regressions were
593
conducted to determine whether the FDS short scale and the relevant FDS subscales
594
accounted for unique variance in willingness to eat above and beyond the variance explained
595
by food neophobia.
596 597
6.2. Results
598
The predictive validity of the FDS subscales of animal flesh, poor hygiene, and living
599
contaminants on the willingness to eat insect-based products was examined using linear
600
regression analysis (Table 8). The final model explained 46% of the variance in willingness to
20
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 601
eat, F(3, 39) = 10.92, p < .001. The subscales of animal flesh (ß = -0.29, p < .05) and poor
602
hygiene (ß = -0.45, p < .001) were statistically significant predictors for willingness to eat
603
insect-based products. A second regression analysis was conducted with the significant
604
predictors from the first regression (i.e., animal flesh and poor hygiene) as well as food
605
neophobia as predictors of the willingness to eat insects. The final model explained 50% of
606
the variance in willingness to eat insect-based products, F(3, 39) = 13.07, p < .001. The poor
607
hygiene subscale turned out to be the strongest predictor (ß = -0.38, p < .01), which helped
608
explain slightly more variance than food neophobia (ß = -0.29, p < .05). The animal flesh
609
subscale was also a significant predictor (ß = -0.26, p < .05).
610
The linear regression model with the FDS short scale and food neophobia as
611
predictors and willingness to eat insect-based products as the dependent variable was
612
significant and explained 37% of the variance, F(2, 39) = 11.68, p < .001. Both the short scale
613
(ß = -0.34, p < .05) and food neophobia (ß = -0.35, p < .05) were significant predictors.
614 615
6.3. Discussion
616
Results of Study 5 support the predictive validity of two FDS subscales, animal flesh
617
and poor hygiene, and highlight that the FDS explains additional variance over and above
618
food neophobia. Consistent with previous results (e.g., Hartmann et al., 2015; Verbeke, 2015),
619
food neophobia was an important construct in the prediction of consumers’ acceptance of
620
novel food such as insects. Further analyses shows that another driver of the rejection of
621
insects as food is disgust sensitivity related to poor hygienic food behavior and reminders of
622
the animal nature of the food. People who scored high on the subscales of animal flesh and
623
poor hygiene reported lower willingness to eat insects. Surprisingly, the living contaminants
624
subscale was not a significant predictor in the regression analysis. People who are sensitive to
625
disgust toward living food contaminants (e.g., a worm in an apple) are not necessarily
626
reluctant to try insects as food. Of course, living contaminants are not supposed to be in food,
627
while insect-based products are conceptualized as food.
628
Results further indicate that the FDS short scale explains additional variance in
629
willingness to eat insects over and above food neophobia. The comparable lower variance that
630
can be explained by the FDS short scale can be ascribed to the multidimensional nature of the
631
FDS scale. Overall, the results suggest not only the incremental validity but also the
632
convergent validity of the newly developed construct.
633 634
21
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 635
7. General discussion
636
The presented studies examine the psychometric properties of the newly developed
637
FDS, which offers researchers interested in the functional and dysfunctional effects of food
638
disgust on food behavior a reliable and valid self-report measure for a construct that has not
639
received much attention in previous food research. The FDS is a trait measure that assesses an
640
individual’s emotional disposition to react with disgust to certain food-related stimuli. It is a
641
self-report measure for adults, which can be applied as an 8-item short version or a
642
comprehensive 32-item long version. The long version contains eight subscales that capture
643
different types of food disgust sensitivity. Each subscale consists of three to five items. Eight
644
distinct factors of food disgust sensitivity can be measured (i.e., animal flesh, human
645
contamination, poor hygiene, decaying fruit, decaying vegetables, mold, fish, and living
646
contaminants).
647
The eight-factor solution for the FDS was confirmed with both exploratory and
648
confirmatory factor analysis. An eight-factor model has important implications for the study
649
of food disgust sensitivity because the examination of profile scores on the FDS subscales and
650
may offer greater predictive validity than an overall measure of food disgust, as highlighted in
651
Study 5. Across the four samples used in the present investigation, Cronbach’s alphas for the
652
eight subscales were good or very good, as well as rather stable. The slightly lower reliability
653
of the FDS short version may be partly accounted for by the multidimensionality of food
654
disgust. Nevertheless, the findings revealed that the short version is a reliable measure and
655
suitable for some applications. Results of the test-retest reliability analysis confirmed that the
656
FDS scale can be considered temporarily stable over two weeks. Construct validity was also
657
supported: The FDS was related to measures of overall disgust sensitivity and the number of
658
food-borne diseases experienced by the individual in the last five years. Evidence was also
659
presented for the incremental validity of FDS scores predicting willingness to eat unfamiliar
660
food (i.e., insects) over and above food neophobia.
661
The FDS, like other disgust scales such as the TDDS (Tybur et al., 2009), use a
662
disgust-rating format. According to the Oxford Dictionary of current English, the term
663
‘disgust’ can be used to indicate a strong feeling of dislike, disapproval of somebody, or
664
something that one feel is unacceptable (Turnbull et al., 2010) and it is also used to indicate
665
anger. Therefore, while disgust can be used to express that something has an extremely
666
unpleasant smell or taste, it can also be used to indicate unpleasant personal habits and people
667
who have them. In German, the word disgusting (eklig) is used to indicate that something is
668
unpleasant and revolting or to indicate that something is morally reprehensible. It is, however,
22
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 669
not used to indicate anger. The FDS was developed in German and the meaning of disgust
670
might be broader in the English language than in German. Therefore, researchers who are
671
interested in using the FDS in an English-speaking context should consider to substitute the
672
word disgust by grossed out or alternatively to further specify to study participants the
673
intended meaning of disgust.
674
Researchers have stated that the English term ‘disgust’ literally means ‘bad’ or ‘bad
675
taste’ (Rozin et al., 2017). To rule out the alternative explanation that participants in our
676
studies had ‘liking’ in mind instead of ‘disgusting’ (in terms of being grossed out), the
677
following aspects were considered in the development of the scale. We focused on foods that
678
are commonly consumed in Western societies and are part of a traditional Western diet (e.g.,
679
meat, fish, bread, cheese, marmalade, tomato, and apples). Therefore, the likelihood that a
680
significant number of participants did not like a majority of the foods was rather low.
681
Moreover, for many food items in the scale it would not make sense for a participant to
682
indicate liking. Notably, ‘liking’ simply does not apply to any of the items in the poor
683
hygiene, human contamination, and living contaminants subscales. ‘Liking’ would not apply,
684
for example, to item 1 (‘To put animal cartilage into my mouth’), item 4 (‘To see a whole pig
685
en brochette’), item 22 (‘To have a whole fish with its head on the plate’), and item 24 (‘The
686
smell in a fish shop or in fish sections with fresh fish”). Indicating disgust here is definitely
687
not a question of liking or not liking the taste of e.g. cartilage (which is tasteless) or of a fish
688
with its head on the plate (which tastes like a fish without its head on the plate). In addition, if
689
we would have measured liking, we would have expected weaker correlations between the
690
FDS and the overall disgust measure DS-R (FDS short r = .59). Nevertheless, we would also
691
expect a correlation between disgust and liking of some foods, since disgust influences
692
people’s food choices and thus their food preferences.
693
The various food-cues combinations that were tested in the first study and those
694
incorporated into the FDS might not have been exclusive, and other cues might have also
695
been suitable. However, we expected a high correlation between reactivity towards the cues
696
tested here and other cues in the food-domain.
697 698
7.1. A new line of research
699
The scale was developed and tested among Swiss adults. The question remains as to
700
how the scale will perform in samples outside Switzerland. In general, the associations
701
between food disgust sensitivity and other attitudinal as well as behavioral variables of food
702
behavior need to be explored in further research. Depending on the region of the world,
23
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 703
people’s attitudes toward certain foods can differ substantially. Therefore, the scale should be
704
tested and used in a multinational context. In addition, experiences, illness-mediated food
705
aversions, parental and group influences, culture, and genetic predisposition are all factors
706
that might determine whether a person is more or less sensitive to disgust. However, how
707
people’s disgust sensitivity evolved over their lifetime was not part of this investigation;
708
further research might be necessary to identify promoters of increasing disgust sensitivity.
709
Food disgust sensitivity might be a barrier to the acceptance of new food sources and
710
technologies. This is a problem because new technologies are important for developing
711
innovations and consequently for economic growth. Informing people about the benefits of
712
certain foods, new food sources, or food technologies might be insufficient when some food
713
characteristics are considered disgusting and hazardous, even though such perceptions might
714
be “irrational” from a scientific point of view. The new psychometric tool enables the
715
identification of consumer groups who are likely to reject novel food sources and
716
technologies. It will also help classify the type of food disgust that is elicited by novel food
717
products. Laypeople’s risk perceptions in the food domain are not only relevant when it
718
comes to the acceptance of new food technologies and sources, but also determine food
719
safety–related behaviors or public acceptance of governmental resource allocations in food
720
safety controls, among others. Understanding the psychological factors and individual
721
differences such as food disgust sensitivity that drive food risk perceptions is therefore
722
important if effective food safety policy and risk communication are to be developed. Thus,
723
the new FDS scale enables a new line of research that will supply further insights into aspects
724
of dietary selectivity as well as the rejection and acceptance of food products.
725 726 727
8. Conclusion
728
The rejection of substances based on disgust prevents consumers from ingesting
729
pathogenic substances and contracting food-borne diseases or infections. Thus, it is a useful
730
preventive behavior; but under some circumstances, it can be counterproductive. In this study,
731
the FDS scale was developed and psychometrically tested within various consumer samples.
732
The study provided empirical evidence that food disgust sensitivity is related to popular
733
constructs of food behavior such as food neophobia; thus, food disgust sensitivity could be a
734
barrier to dietary quality. Food disgust might also hinder consumers’ acceptance of novel food
735
sources and technologies and might further determine laypeople’s food risk perceptions. The
24
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 736
quantification of people’s food disgust sensitivity offers further insights into the drivers of
737
food acceptance and rejection as well as consumers’ responses related to food safety.
738 739
Acknowledgment
740
We would like to thank Aisha Egolf for her support in conducting Study 3.
741
Funding sources
742
This research was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (project number
743
100014_165630).
744
References
745
Al-Shawaf, L., Lewis, D. M. G., Alley, T. R., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Mating strategy ,
746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753
disgust , and food neophobia. Appetite, 85, 30-35. Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Understanding disgust. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251(1), 62-76. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7, 309-319. Curtis, V., & Biran, a. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease. Is hygiene in our genes? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44(1), 17-31. Dovey, T. M., Staples, P. a., Gibson, E. L., & Halford, J. C. G. (2008). Food neophobia and
754
'picky/fussy' eating in children: a review. Appetite, 50(2-3), 181-193.
755
Duncan, L. a., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease:
756
Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and
757
Individual Differences, 47, 541-546.
758
Fergus, T. A., & Valentiner, D. P. (2009). The Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale–
759
Revised: An examination of a reduced-item version. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,
760
23(5), 703-710.
761 762 763
Gerba, C. P., Rose, J. B., & Hass, C. N. (1996). Sensitive populations - who is at the greatest risk? International Journal of Food Microbiology, 30, 113-123. Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A
764
scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual
765
Differences, 16, 701-713.
25
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 766
Hartmann, C., Keller, C., & Siegrist, M. (2016). Compensatory beliefs, nutrition knowledge
767
and eating styles of users and non-users of meal replacement products. Appetite, 105,
768
775-781.
769
Hartmann, C., Shi, J., Giusto, A., & Siegrist, M. (2015). The psychology of eating insects: A
770
cross-cultural comparison between Germany and China. Food Quality and Preference,
771
44, 148-156.
772 773 774 775 776 777
Hartmann, C., & Siegrist, M. (2016). Becoming an insectivore: Results of an experiment. Food Quality and Preference, 51, 118-122. Kauer, J., Pelchat, M., Rozin, P., & Zickgraf, H. F. (2015). Adults picky eating. Phenomenology, taste sensitivitiy and psychological correlates. Appetite, 90, 219-228. Kellert, S. R. (1993). Values and Perceptions of Invertebrates. Conservation Biology, 7(4), 845-855.
778
Kleinknecht, R. A., Kleinknecht, E. E., & Thorndike, R. M. (1997). The role of disgust and
779
fear in blood and injection-related fainting symptoms: A structural equation model.
780
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(12), 1075-1087.
781 782 783
Looy, H., Dunkel, F. V., & Wood, J. R. (2014). How then shall we eat? Insect-eating attitudes and sustainable foodways. Agriculture and Human Values, 31(1), 131-141. Martins, Y., & Pliner, P. (2006). "Ugh! That's disgusting!": Identification of the
784
characteristics of foods underlying rejections based on disgust. Appetite, 46(1), 75-85.
785
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism.
786
Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303-321.
787
Olatunji, B. O., Cisler, J. M., Deacon, B. J., Connolly, K., & Lohr, J. M. (2007). The Disgust
788
Propensity and Sensitivity Scale-Revised : Psychometric properties and specificity in
789
relation to anxiety disorder symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 918-930.
790
Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M.,
791
& Elwood, L. S. (2007). The Disgust Scale: item analysis, factor structure, and
792
suggestions for refinement. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 281-297.
793
Petrowski, K., Paul, S., Schmutzer, G., Roth, M., Brähler, E., & Albani, C. (2010). Domains
794
of disgust sensitivity: revisited factor structure of the questionnaire for the assessment
795
of disgust sensitivity (QADS) in a cross-sectional, representative German survey.
796
BMC Medical Research Methodology, 10, 95-95.
797 798
Pliner, P., & Hobden, K. (1992). Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite, 19(2), 105-120.
26
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 799
Raudenbush, B., Van der Klaaus, N. J., & Frank, R. A. (1995). The contribution of
800
psychological and sensory factors to food preference patterns as measured by the food
801
attitudes survey (FAS). Appetite, 25, 1-15.
802 803
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 2323.
804
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones,
805
& L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (2 ed., pp. 637-653). New York:
806
Guilford Press.
807 808 809 810 811 812 813
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2017). Disgust Handbook of Emotions (pp. 815-834). New York: Guilford Press. Ruby, M. B., Rozin, P., & Chan, C. (2015). Determinants of willingness to eat insects in the USA and India. Journal of Insects as Food and Feed, 1(3), 215-225. Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The Behavioral Immune System (and Why It Matters). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 99-103. Schienle, A., Walter, B., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2002). Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung der
814
Ekelempfindlichkeit (FEE). Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie,
815
31(2), 110-120.
816
Scott, S. E., Inbar, Y., & Rozin, P. (2016). Evidence for absolute moral opposition to
817
genetically modified food in the United States. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
818
11(3), 315-324.
819
Siegrist, M., Hartmann, C., & Keller, C. (2013). Antecedents of food neophobia and its
820
association with eating behavior and food choices. Food Quality and Preference,
821
30(2), 293-298.
822
Stark, R., Walter, B., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D. (2005). Psychophysiological correlates of
823
disgust and disgust sensitivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 50-60.
824
Stevens, J. P. (2012). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences: Routledge.
825
Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). The behavioral immune system and
826
social conservatism: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(2), 99-108.
827
Turnbull, J., Lea, D., Parkinson, D., Phillips, P., Francis, B., Webb, S., . . . Ashby, M. (2010).
828
Oxford Advanced Learner's Dictionary of current English (Eighth ed.). New York:
829
Oxford University Press.
830
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality:
831
individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality
832
and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103-122.
27
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 833 834 835
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 65-84. van Overveld, M., de Jong, P. J., Peters, M. L., Cavanagh, K., & Davey, G. (2006). Disgust
836
propensity and disgust sensitivity: Separate constructs that are differentially related to
837
specific fears. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(7), 1241-1252.
838
van Overveld, M., de Jong, P. J., Peters, M. L., & Schouten, E. (2011). The Disgust Scale-R:
839
A valid and reliable index to investigate separate disgust domains? Personality and
840
Individual Differences, 51(3), 325-330.
841
van Overveld, M., Jong, P. J. D., & Peters, M. L. (2010). The Disgust Propensity and
842
Sensitivity Scale - Revised: Its predictive value for avoidance behavior. Personality
843
and Individual Differences, 49, 706-711.
844 845
Verbeke, W. (2015). Profiling consumers who are ready to adopt insects as a meat substitute in a Western society. Food Quality and Preference, 39, 147-155.
846
Widaman, K. F., Little, T. D., Preacher, J., & Sawalani, G. M. (2011). On creating and using
847
short forms of scales in secondary research Seconary Data Analysis - An introduction
848
for Psychologists (pp. 39-61). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
849 850
28
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 851
Table 1. Overview of popular scales to measure disgust sensitivity and their limitations
852
regarding food disgust Author
Scale
Domains
Limitations
Haidt et al.,
Disgust Scale (DS)
Food, body products,
Food subscale not
sex, death, body
internally consistent
envelope violations,
across studies, alpha
animal, hygiene, magic
reliabilities of the food
1994
items below .40 Olatunji et al.,
Disgust Scale–
Core, animal-reminder,
No food subscale, 7 food
2007
revised (DS-R)
contamination
items (rotting, contamination) diffusely spread among subscales
Schienle et al.,
Fragebogen zur
Death, body secretions,
No food subscale, 12 food
2002 [revised
Erfassung der
spoilage, hygiene, oral
items diffusely spread
German
Ekelempfindlichkeit defense
among subscales with
version of the
(FEE)
mostly low factor loadings
DS]
[Questionnaire for
(on average .40)
the assessment of Disgust sensitivity] Petrowski et
Questionnaire for
Core, animal-reminder,
No food subscale, 12 food
al, 2010
the assessment of
contamination
items diffusely spread
[revised
Disgust sensitivity
among subscales with
English
(QADS)
mostly low factor loadings
version of the
(on average .45)
FEE] Kleinknecht et
Disgust Emotion
Animals, injections,
al., 1996
Scale (DES)
blood draws, mutilation,
Focus on rotting food
death, rotting food, smells Tybur et al.,
Three Domain
Pathogen, sexual, moral
No food subscale, 4 food
2009
Disgust Scale
items included (rotting,
(TDDS)
contamination) with low loadings on pathogen disgust subscale
29
FOOD DISGUST SCALE Van Overveld
Disgust propensity
et al., 2006
and disgust
(revised van
sensitivity scale
Overveld,
(DPSS)
Jong, & Peters, 2010)
Disgust propensity (the tendency to react with the emotion of disgust), disgust sensitivity (tendency to experience disgust as something ‘horrid’)
No food items
853
30
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 854
Table 2. Overview of the study samples Study 1:
Study 2 + 3: Scale
Study 5:
Scale
refinement &
Study 4:
Incremental
development
validation
Test-Retest
validity
N
318
527
170
43
Men [%]
49.4
48.8
61.0
55.8
Age [Mean (sd)]
49.50 (16.22)
44.89 (14.12)
62.10 (12.36)
34.98 (11.09)
20-39 years [%]
32.1
38.5
69.8
40-64 years [%]
45.0
52.2
27.9
65-79 years [%]
22.3
9.3
2.3
80+ years [%]
0.6
0
0
Highest level of education [%] Primary, lower secondary school
5.7
6.5
1.2
0
49.3
56.4
32.9
48.8
32.3
17.6
25.9
7.0
and above
11.6
9.9
38.3
41.8
Missing
1.1
9.7
1.7
2.3
Secondary school, vocational education, senior high school Higher vocational education College, university
31
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 855
Table 3. Final 32-item Food Disgust Scale with eight subscales measuring different types of
856
food disgust (data from Study 2, N = 527). Standardized Label
Item
loadings (CFA)
Animal flesh (α = .79) 1
MEAT1a
To put animal cartilage into my mouth
.55
2
MEAT2
To see raw meat
.77
3
MEAT3
To eat a steak that is still bloody insideb
.76
4
MEAT4
To see a whole pig en brochette
.76
Poor hygiene (α = .87) 5
HYG1 a
To eat with dirty silverware in a restaurant
.74
6
HYG2
A meal prepared by a cook who has greasy hair and dirty
.90
fingernailsb 7
HYG3
If the cook in a restaurant has an open cut
.82
8
HYG4
If people blow their nose before they serve my meal
.63
9
HYG5
Another person’s hair in my soup
b
.65
Human contamination (α = .85) 10 HUCO1 a
Food donated from a neighbor whom I barely know
.52
11 HUCO2
If a friend bites into my bread
.88
12 HUCO3
To drink from the same drinking glass a friend has already
.91
drunk fromc 13 HUCO4
If friends or acquaintance have touched my food
.76
To eat the mold-free part of a moldy tomato
.83
To eat bread from which mold was cut away
.84
To eat hard cheese from which mold was cut off
.84
To eat marmalade from which mold was removed from the
.82
Mold (α = .90) 14 MOLD1 15 MOLD2 16 MOLD3
a
17 MOLD4
surface Decaying fruit (α = .87) 18 FRUIT1
To eat overripe fruits
.69
19 FRUIT2
To eat a banana that has black spots
.79
To eat fruits (e.g., apple and peach) with pressure marks
.89
To eat apple slices that turned brown when exposed to air
.82
20 FRUIT3 21 FRUIT4
a
32
FOOD DISGUST SCALE Fish (α = .87) 22 FISH1
To have a whole fish with its head on the plate
.78
23 FISH2
To eat raw fish like sushi
.78
24 FISH3
The smell in a fish shop or in fish sections with fresh fish
.76
25 FISH4 a
The texture of some kinds of fish in the mouth
.87
Decaying vegetables (α = .89) 26 VEGI1a
To eat brown-colored avocado pulp
.70
27 VEGI2
To eat an overripe cucumber that can already be bent
.85
28 VEGI3
To eat shrunken radishes
.90
29 VEGI4
To eat salad that is not crispy anymore
.82
Living contaminants (α = .90)
857 858 859 860 861 862
30 LCON1
There is a maggot in the cherry that I wanted to eat
.85
31 LCON2a
There is a little snail in the salad that I wanted to eat
.85
32 LCON3
There is a worm in my apple
.92
Note. Instruction for participants: “Please indicate how disgusting you perceive the following products or situations to be.” Response scale varied from 1 (not disgusting at all) to 6 (extremely disgusting). a
Items are included in the 8-item short version of the FDS (α = .77). These are item number 1, 5, 10, 16, 21, 25, 26 and 31.
b
Item adapted from Schienle, Walter, Stark, and Vaitl (2002).
c
Item adapted from Olatunji, Williams, et al. (2007).
CFA: Confirmatory factor analysis.
33
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 863
Table 4. Eight-item short version of the Food Disgust scale (FDS short) and corrected item-
864
total correlations (data from study 2, N = 527) Corrected Label
Item
item-total correlations
865
MEAT1
To put animal cartilage into my mouth
.48
HYG1
To eat with dirty silverware in a restaurant
.42
HUCON1
Food donated from a neighbor whom I barely know
.37
MOLD3
To eat hard cheese from which mold was cut off
.53
FRUIT4
To eat apple slices that turned brown when exposed to air
.43
FISH4
The texture of some kinds of fish in the mouth
.43
VEGI1
To eat brown-colored avocado pulp
.51
LCON2
There is a little snail in the salad that I wanted to eat
.58
Note: α = .78
866
34
FOOD DISGUST SCALE 867
Table 5. Mean values and standard deviations for the eight subscales and the short version of
868
the Food Disgust Scale (data from Study 2). All
Women
Men
(N=527)
(n = 269)
(n = 258)
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
MEAT
2.75
1.30
3.15
1.30
2.34
1.16
7.50**
HYG
5.22
0.87
5.44
0.69
4.98
0.97
6.25**
HUCON
2.94
1.19
2.97
1.20
2.90
1.18
0.77
MOULD
4.24
1.49
4.43
1.42
4.03
1.53
3.08*
FRUIT
2.65
1.18
2.67
1.20
2.62
1.15
0.47
FISH
3.22
1.51
3.50
1.54
2.92
1.42
4.43**
VEGI
3.32
1.27
3.50
1.26
3.14
1.24
3.33**
LCON
4.83
1.30
5.08
1.17
4.57
1.37
4.58**
3.76
0.93
3.97
0.88
3.53
0.94
5.69**
t-test for gender
FDS subscales
FDS short (8 items) 869 870 871
Note. FDS subscales: MEAT = Animal flesh, HYG = Poor Hygiene, HUCON = Human contamination, MOLD = Mold, FRUIT = Fruit in decay, FISH = Fish, VEGI = Vegetables in decay, LCON = Living contaminants; Possible scoring range 1 (not disgusting at all) to 6 (very disgusting); **p ≤ .001; *p ≤ .01.
35
FOOD DISGUST SCALE Table 6. Associations between the Food Disgust Scale and measures of validity (Study 3, N = 527) MEAT
HYG
HUCON
MOLD
FRUIT
FISH
VEGI
LCON
FDS
DS-R
Core
Animal
Con.
short
Food
Picki.
Germ aversion
Neo
FDS subscales MEAT HYG HUCON MOLD FRUIT FISH VEGI LCON FDS short DS-R Core Animal Con. Food Neo
Pickiness
1.00
.22**
.29**
.27**
.27**
.63**
.28**
.32**
.58**
.35**
.30**
.32**
.25**
.32**
.33**
.22**
1.00
.33**
.39**
.23**
.26**
.41**
.46**
.55**
.45**
.47**
.28**
.37**
0.07
.11**
.41**
1.00
.27**
.31**
.24**
.36**
.28**
.49**
.38**
.31**
.24**
.46**
.21**
.25**
.50**
1.00
.38**
.31**
.54**
.54**
.70**
.44**
.43**
.32**
.33**
.17**
.19**
.31**
1.00
.24**
.63**
.35**
.58**
.38**
.35**
.28**
.33**
.25**
.20**
.29**
1.00
.32**
.37**
.64**
.33**
.28**
.30**
.24**
.42**
.30**
.20**
1.00
.47**
.73**
.47**
.43**
.33**
.44**
.22**
.25**
.35**
1.00
.70**
.50**
.51**
.36**
.34**
.21**
.16**
.38**
1.00
.59**
.56**
.44**
.48**
.37**
.35**
.45**
1.00
.90**
.84**
.74**
.21**
.18**
.52**
1.00
.59**
.57**
.14**
.13**
.47**
1.00
.46**
.19**
.14**
.37**
1.00
.24**
.23**
.50**
1.00
.51**
.18**
1.00
.18**
Germ aversion
1.00
Note. FDS subscales: MEAT = Animal flesh, HYG = Poor Hygiene, HUCON = Human contamination, MOLD = Mold, FRUIT = Fruit in decay, FISH = Fish, VEGI = Vegetables in decay, LCON = Living contaminants; DS-R = Disgust Scale-revised (Olatunji et al., 2007); Con. = contamination; Food Neo = food neophobia (Pliner & Hobden, 1992); *p < .001.
36
FOOD DISGUST SCALE Table 7. Results of the two-weeks test-retest reliability analysis for the 32-item long and the 8-item short version of the Food Disgust Scale (Study 4, N=170). Cronbach’s
Pearson
Alpha at T1
correlation
MEAT
.79
.86
HYG
.86
.75
HUCON
.87
.82
MOLD
.88
.83
FRUIT
.86
.81
FISH
.85
.90
VEGI
.92
.79
LCON
.93
.78
.77
.86
FDS subscales
FDS short (8 items)
Note. FDS subscales: MEAT = Animal flesh, HYG = Poor Hygiene, HUCON = Human contamination, MOLD = Mold, FRUIT = Fruit in decay, FISH = Fish, VEGI = Vegetables in decay, LCON = Living contaminants.
37
FOOD DISGUST SCALE Table 8. Results of three separate stepwise regressions predicting willingness to eat products made with processed insects (Study 5, N = 43).
Predictors
Step 1
Step 2
Step 3
Beta ß
Beta ß
Beta ß
Regression 1 FDS_MEAT
-.50*** -.34*
FDS_HYG
-.29*
-.47*** -.45***
FDS_LCON ∆R2 .25***
-.15 .19***
.02
Regression 2 Food neophobia -.53*** -.40*** -.29* FDS_MEAT
-.34*** -.26*
FDS_HYG
-.38** ∆R2 .29***
.10*
.12**
Regression 3 Food neophobia -.53*** -.35* FDS short
-.34* ∆R2 .29***
.08*
Note. FDS: Food Disgust scale; FDS_MEAT: Animal flesh subscale of the FDS, FDS_HYG: Poor hygiene subscale of the FDS; FDS_LCON: Living contaminants subscale of the FDS; FDS short: 8-item short version of the FDS; R2 = 0.46 (regression 1), R2 = 0.50 (regression 2), R2 = 0.37 (regression 3); ***p ≤ .001, **p ≤ .01, * p ≤ .05.
38
FOOD DISGUST SCALE
Figure 1. The final measurement model for the Food Disgust scale (long format). The 322 item good fitting model, χ (435) = 945.37, p < .001, CFI= .95, RMSEA = .05, NFI = .91, is
based on eight subscales.
39
FOOD DISGUST SCALE
Figure 2. The final model for the Food Disgust short scale (FDS short). The good fitting 2 model (χ (18) = 44.02, p=.001, CFI= .97, RMSEA = .05, NFI = .95) is based on 8 items from
the 32-item version of the FDS.
40
FOOD DISGUST SCALE Supplementary File: German version of the Food Disgust Scale (FDS) Label Tierfleisch 1 MEAT1a 2 MEAT2 3 MEAT3 4 MEAT4
Item
Überhaupt nicht eklig 1 2
3
4
Extrem eklig 5 6
Einen Tierknorpel in den Mund nehmen Der Anblick von rohem Fleisch Ein innen noch blutiges Steak essenb Den Anblick von einem ganzen Schwein am Spiess
Schlechte Hygiene 5 HYG1 a Die Vorstellung mit unsauberen Besteck in einem Restaurant zu essen 6 HYG2 Das zubereitete Essen von einem Koch, der fettige Haare und dreckige Fingernägel hatb 7 HYG3 Wenn der Koch in einem Restaurant eine unverbundene Schnittwunde am Finger hat 8 HYG4 Wenn sich Leute die Nase putzen, bevor sie mir Essen servieren 9 HYG5 Ein fremdes Haar in der Suppeb Menschliche Kontamination 10 HUCON1 a Das Essen, welches mir Nachbarn geschenkt haben, die ich kaum kenne 11 HUCON2 Wenn eine Freundin oder ein Freund von meinem Brot abbeisst 12 HUCON3 Die Vorstellung, aus demselben Glas zu trinken, aus dem schon ein Freund oder eine Freundin getrunken hatc 13 HUCON4 Wenn Freunde oder Bekannte mein Essen angefasst haben Schimmel 14 MOLD1 15 MOLD2 16 MOLD3 a 17 MOLD4
Von einer angeschimmelten Tomate den Teil ohne Schimmel essen Brot essen, von welchem Schimmel weggeschnitten wurde Hartkäse essen, von welchem zuvor Schimmel weggeschnitten wurde Konfitüre essen, die an der Oberfläche Schimmel hatte, der entfernt wurde
41
FOOD DISGUST SCALE
Früchte im Verfallprozess Überreife Früchte essen 18 FRUIT1 19 FRUIT2 Eine Banane mit schwarzen Stellen essen 20 FRUIT3 Früchte (z.B. Apfel, Pfirsich) mit Druckstellen essen 21 FRUIT4 a Apfelstücke, die sich an der Luft verfärbt haben essen Fisch 22 FISH1 23 FISH2 24 FISH3 25 FISH4 a
Einen ganzen Fisch mit Kopf auf meinem Teller zu haben Die Vorstellung rohen Fisch wie Sushi zu essen Der Geruch in einem Fischladen oder einer Abteilung mit frischem Fisch Die Konsistenz einiger Fischarten im Mund
Gemüse im Verfallprozess 26 VEGI1 a Braunverfärbtes Fruchtfleisch von einer Avocado essen 27 VEGI2 Eine ältere Gurke essen, die sich bereits biegen lässt 28 VEGI3 Schrumpelige Radieschen essen 29 VEGI4 Salat essen, der nicht mehr knackig ist Lebende Kontaminanten 30 LCON1 In einer Kirsche, die ich gerade essen möchte, ist eine Made 31 LCON2 a Eine kleine Schnecke in meinem Salat, den ich gerade esse 32 LCON3 Ein Wurm in einem Apfel Note. Instruction for study participants: “Bitte geben Sie an, wie eklig Sie die folgenden Situationen oder Produkte finden.“ a
Items are included in the 8-item short version of the FDS.
b c
Item adapted from Schienle, Walter, Stark, and Vaitl (2002).
Item adapted from Olatunji, Williams, et al. (2007).
42
FOOD DISGUST SCALE Highlights
•
The psychometrically sound Food Disgust Scale (FDS) was developed.
•
The FDS is a multidimensional measure of different types of food disgust.
•
The FDS is associated with food neophobia, picky eating, and germ aversion.
•
FDS scores and the number of food-borne illnesses are positively correlated.
•
The FDS incrementally predicts willingness to eat insects over and above food neophobia.
43