Does apraxia support spatial and kinematic or mirror neuron approaches to social interaction? A commentary on Binder et al. (2017)

Does apraxia support spatial and kinematic or mirror neuron approaches to social interaction? A commentary on Binder et al. (2017)

Accepted Manuscript Does apraxia support spatial and kinematic or mirror neuron approaches to social interaction? A commentary on Binder et al. (2017)...

581KB Sizes 0 Downloads 4 Views

Accepted Manuscript Does apraxia support spatial and kinematic or mirror neuron approaches to social interaction? A commentary on Binder et al. (2017) Arran T. Reader, Matteo Candidi PII:

S0010-9452(17)30366-0

DOI:

10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.018

Reference:

CORTEX 2167

To appear in:

Cortex

Received Date: 2 June 2017 Revised Date:

22 August 2017

Accepted Date: 21 October 2017

Please cite this article as: Reader AT, Candidi M, Does apraxia support spatial and kinematic or mirror neuron approaches to social interaction? A commentary on Binder et al. (2017), CORTEX (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.018. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Does apraxia support spatial and kinematic or mirror neuron approaches to social interaction? A commentary on Binder et al. (2017) Arran T. Reader1* & Matteo Candidi2,3

Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK

RI PT

1. Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical

2. Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy 3. IRCCS, Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy

SC

*Corresponding author: Arran T. Reader, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Earley Gate, Whiteknights Road, Reading, RG6 6AL, UK.

M AN U

Telephone: +44 (0)118 378 8522. E-mail: [email protected]

Acknowledgements: This submission was supported by the Economic and Social Research

AC C

EP

TE D

Council (grant number ES/J500148/1 to ATR).

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

In a recent article in Cortex Binder et al. (2017) present data from 44 left-hemisphere stroke

2

patients with (n = 18) and without (n = 26) apraxia. They tested these patients, alongside healthy

3

controls (n = 19), on three experimental tasks (meaningful gesture recognition, comprehension, and

4

imitation), and two control tasks (control recognition, control comprehension). They also performed

5

a voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) in order to associate lesion locations with

6

experimental task performance in patients. They were specifically interested in examining whether

7

regions associated with the putative human mirror neuron system (MNS) are involved critically, and

8

to a similar degree, in recognising, understanding, and imitating actions.

RI PT

1

9

Their results showed that apraxic patients were significantly worse than non-apraxic patients

11

and healthy controls in gesture comprehension, gesture imitation, gesture recognition, as well as in

12

the control recognition task (in which participants had to decide in which of two gestures the hand

13

was closer to the head). Performance in the three experimental tasks was significantly correlated.

14

Defective gesture comprehension was associated with cortical lesions in the operculum, insula, and

15

inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), whilst defective gesture imitation was associated with cortical lesions

16

in the postcentral gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, intraparietal sulcus,

17

supramarginal gyrus, operculum, and insula. The authors state that the deficits in gesture

18

comprehension are mainly associated with anterior regions of the MNS (IFG), whilst gesture

19

imitation deficits are associated with the posterior MNS (inferior parietal lobule, superior temporal

20

sulcus). They conclude that their data “not only support the notion of [a MNS], but characterize its

21

functional role by showing its relevance for action comprehension and imitation” (p. 135).

M AN U

TE D

22

SC

10

The putative human frontoparietal MNS has strongly influenced discussions of social

24

interaction (Cook & Bird, 2013; Hamilton, 2014; Kilner & Lemon, 2013; Press & Cook, 2015), and

25

Binder et al. (2017) are right to bridge the gap between studies of social interaction in healthy

26

people and neuropsychological research. Whilst we recognise that simulative comparisons between

27

observed and stored actions may have a role to play in action comprehension and imitation, and that

28

these processes are possibly subserved by frontoparietal regions, we believe that the claims of

29

Binder et al. (2017) require further discussion. In particular, we believe that the pattern of results

30

from the study highlights the possible contribution of body-centered spatial representations or

31

kinematics in apraxia, beside that of action simulation mechanisms, and that these functions may

32

also be associated with proposed mirror regions. We aim to make this distinction clear in order to

33

stimulate a deeper understanding of the possible role for the putative human MNS for higher-order

34

action understanding and perception.

AC C

EP

23

1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

35

Though Binder et al. (2017) claim that “lesions to the core regions of the putative hMNS

37

critically affected key functions ascribed to it” (p. 135), simulative mechanisms are by no means the

38

only essential component of these functions, as has been noted by others (Buxbaum & Kalénine,

39

2010). Similarly, mirror mechanisms are not the only process associated with proposed mirror

40

regions such as the left inferior parietal lobule (IPL). To expand on this point, Binder et al. (2017)

41

found that apraxia patients were also significantly worse than non-apraxic patients and healthy

42

controls in the gesture recognition control task (testing the ability to evaluate the spatial distance

43

between the model’s hand and head). They suggest that the significant effect in this task could be

44

due to proposed left hemisphere apraxia deficits in ‘body part coding’ (Goldenberg & Karnath,

45

2006). This approach to apraxia suggests that the observed deficits can be considered a problem of

46

correctly assessing the spatial relationship between body parts (such as the hand) and other body

47

parts or objects. Thus, they interpret their results in the recognition control task by suggesting that

48

the coding of spatial relationships between body parts may have been impaired in their apraxic

49

patients. However, this may also provide an explanation for the other results observed in this

50

experiment, since an inability to accurately assess the spatial relationships between body parts in

51

different gestures could feasibly make recognition, comprehension, and imitation harder to

52

complete. Unfortunately, the authors do not report whether any lesioned areas were associated with

53

this control task and whether these overlapped with the reported regions predicting the impairments

54

in the experimental tasks.

SC

M AN U

TE D

55

RI PT

36

Notable in Binder and colleague’s (2017) study is the anterior-posterior split between lesions

57

associated with gesture comprehension (anterior) and lesions associated with gesture imitation

58

(posterior). Previous evidence does seem to support a vital role for the left IFG in action

59

understanding (Urgesi et al., 2014), which could be associated with the proposed simulative role of

60

mirror neurons. However, there may be a different explanation for the role of posterior regions in

61

imitation. A VLSM study by Buxbaum et al. (2014) suggests that left IPL damage is associated with

62

deficits in the kinematic aspects of imitation, whereas damage to the posterior temporal lobe is

63

associated with deficits in the postural aspects of imitation. Similar results were reported more

64

recently by Dressing et al. (2016). Since Binder et al. (2017) did not distinguish between the

65

kinematic and postural elements of their imitation task during analysis, it is unclear to what degree

66

these different elements of movement might better explain their results. Discussion on these terms

67

may better account for the fact that left parietal lesions frequently cause deficits in the imitation of

68

novel actions (Goldenberg, 2009). Since mirror approaches to social cognition rely on the idea that

AC C

EP

56

2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

mirror neurons are involved in visuomotor matching of observed and stored actions (Rizzolatti et

70

al., 2014), it is yet unclear how visuomotor matching is a feasible explanation for defective

71

imitation of novel actions, since there should be no stored action representation to match. On the

72

other hand, both novel and known actions could be coded in terms of the observed postural or

73

kinematic parameters, with novel actions showing a greater reliance on this type of information

74

(Rumiati et al., 2009; Rumiati & Tessari, 2002; Tessari & Rumiati, 2004). It is possible then that in

75

this instance the anterior-posterior split does not necessarily reflect two mirror mechanisms, but

76

rather a distinction between simulative and spatial or kinematic mechanisms for social motor

77

behaviour.

RI PT

69

78

Finally, we are in agreement with Binder et al. (2017) when they state that the “lack of

80

significant result for the VLSM of our Gesture Recognition task is in line with [the claim] that in

81

contrast to gesture comprehension a mere recognition of correctly performed familiar gestures is not

82

a core function of the human MNS” (p. 134). In fact, action recognition could be principally

83

subserved by occipitotemporal regions (Lingnau & Downing, 2015; Peelen & Downing, 2017), as

84

recent evidence suggests that visual representations of body parts and action in this area are

85

organised in terms of semantics, transitivity, and sociality (Bracci et al., 2015; Wurm et al., 2017),

86

and that the structure of these representations could possibly assist in higher-level social cognition

87

in a bottom-up manner (Reader, 2016). Previous VLSM approaches to deficits in gesture

88

recognition, particularly for semantic aspects of action, reflect the importance of these regions

89

(Kalénine et al., 2010).

M AN U

TE D

90

SC

79

In conclusion, whilst Binder et al. (2017) confirm the role of frontoparietal regions in

92

apraxia, there is still much work to be done to fully understand the contribution of these areas to

93

both apraxia and social motor behaviour as a whole. Importantly, deficits following damage to

94

proposed regions of the human mirror network can also provide support for proposed functions of

95

these regions apart from a mirror mechanism. In social interaction we believe it is fundamental to

96

understand the contribution of regions coding for the kinematic, spatial, or postural features of

97

action, over a purely mirror neuron driven approach.

AC C

EP

91

3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

98

References

99

Binder, E., Dovern, A., Hesse, M.D., Ebke, M., Karbe, H., Saliger, J., Fink, G.R., & Weiss, P.H.

100

(2017). Lesion evidence for a human mirror neuron system. Cortex, 90, 125-137.

101

doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.008

102 Bracci, S., Caramazza, A., & Peelen, M.V. (2015). Representational similarity of body parts in

104

human occipitotemporal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(38), 12977-12985.

105

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4698-14.2015

RI PT

103

106

Buxbaum, L.J., & Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment

108

in the two action systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 201-218.

109

doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05447.x.

SC

107

M AN U

110 111

Buxbaum, L.J., Shapiro, A.D., & Coslett, H.B. (2014). Critical brain regions for tool-related and

112

imitative actions: a componential analysis. Brain, 137(7), 1971-1985. doi:10.1093/brain/awu111

113 114

Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2013). Do mirror neurons really mirror and do they really code for action

115

goals? Cortex, 49(10), 2944-2945. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.05.006

TE D

116

Dressing, A., Nitschke, K., Kümmerer, D., Bormann, T., Beume, L., Schmidt, C.S.M., Ludwig,

118

V.M., Mader, I., Willmes, K., Rijntjes, M., Kaller, C.P., Weiller, C., & Martin, M. (2016). Distinct

119

contributions of dorsal and ventral streams to imitation of tool-use and communicative gestures.

120

Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw383

121

EP

117

Goldenberg, G. (2009). Apraxia and the parietal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1449-1459.

123

doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.014

124

AC C

122

125

Goldenberg, G., & Karnath, H. (2006). The neural basis of imitation is body part specific. Journal

126

of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6282-6287. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0638-06.2006

127 128

Hamilton, A.F. de C. (2014). Cognitive underpinnings of social interaction. Quarterly Journal of

129

Experimental Psychology, 68(3), 417-432. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.973424

130

4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

131

Kalénine, S., Buxbaum, L.J., & Coslett, H.B. (2010). Critical brain regions for action recognition:

132

lesion symptom mapping in left hemisphere stroke. Brain, 133(11), 3269-3280.

133

doi:10.1093/brain/awq210

134 Kilner, J.M., & Lemon, R.N. (2013). What we know currently about mirror neurons. Current

136

Biology, 23(23), R1057-1062. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.051

137

RI PT

135

138

Lingnau, A., & Downing, P.E. (2015). The lateral occipitotemporal cortex in action. Trends in

139

Cognitive Sciences, 19(5), 268-277. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.03.006

140

Peelen, M.V., & Downing, P.E. (2017). Category selectivity in human visual cortex: beyond visual

142

object recognition. Neuropsychologia, 17, 30121-30125.

143

doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.03.033

M AN U

144

SC

141

145

Press, C., & Cook, R. (2015). Beyond action-specific simulation: domain-general motor

146

contributions to perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(4), 176-178.

147

doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.006

148

Reader, A.T. (2016). Semantic organization of body part representations in the occipitotemporal

150

cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(2), 265-267. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3766-15.2016

151

TE D

149

Rizzolatti, G., Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., & Rozzi, S. (2014). Cortical mechanisms underlying

153

the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding.

154

Physiological Reviews, 94(2), 655-706. doi:10.1152/physrev.00009.2013

AC C

155

EP

152

156

Rumiati, R.I., Carmo, J.C., & Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C. (2009). Neuropsychological perspectives on

157

the mechanisms of imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B,

158

Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2337-2347. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0063

159 160

Rumiati, R.I., & Tessari, A. (2002). Imitation of novel and well-known actions: the role of short-

161

term memory. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 425-433. doi:10.1007/s00221-001-0956-x

162

5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

163

Tessari, A., & Rumiati, R.I. (2004). The strategic control of multiple routes in imitation of actions.

164

Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 30(6), 1107-1116.

165

doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.6.1107

166 Urgesi, C., Candidi, M., & Avenanti, A. (2014). Neuroanatomical substrates of action perception

168

and understanding: an anatomic likelihood estimation meta-analysis of lesion-symptom mapping

169

studies in brain injured patients. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(344).

170

doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00344

RI PT

167

171

Wurm, M.F., Caramazza, A., & Lingnau, A. (2017). Action categories in lateral occipitotemporal

173

cortex are organized along sociality and transitivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(3), 562-575.

174

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1717-16.2017

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

172

6