Accepted Manuscript Eating when depressed, anxious, bored, or happy: Are emotional eating types associated with unique psychological and physical health correlates? Abby Braden, Dara Musher-Eizenman, Tanya Watford, Elizabeth Emley PII:
S0195-6663(17)31593-3
DOI:
10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.022
Reference:
APPET 3798
To appear in:
Appetite
Received Date: 1 November 2017 Revised Date:
1 February 2018
Accepted Date: 20 February 2018
Please cite this article as: Braden A., Musher-Eizenman D., Watford T. & Emley E., Eating when depressed, anxious, bored, or happy: Are emotional eating types associated with unique psychological and physical health correlates?, Appetite (2018), doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.022. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1
1
RUNNING HEAD: Emotional Eating
2
RI PT
3
4
SC
5
6
M AN U
7 8 9
Eating when Depressed, Anxious, Bored, or Happy: Are Emotional Eating Types associated with Unique Psychological and Physical Health Correlates?
10
Abby Braden
Dara Musher-Eizenman
12
Tanya Watford
TE D
11
Elizabeth Emley
13 14
EP
15
Corresponding Author:
17
Abby Braden, Ph.D.
18
Assistant Professor
19
Bowling Green State University
20
Bowling Green, OH 43403
21
[email protected]
22
419-372-9405
AC C
16
23
24
Abstract
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2
1
The majority of research on emotional eating has examined general emotional eating, to the exclusion of more distinct emotions such as boredom and positive emotions. The current
3
study aimed to examine whether specific types of emotional eating (i.e., eating in response to
4
depression (EE-D), anxiety/anger (EE-A), boredom (EE-B), and positive emotions (EE-P)) were
5
related to a range of psychological (i.e., global psychological well-being, eating disorder
6
symptoms, emotion regulation) and physical health variables. A sample of adults (n = 189) with
7
overweight/obesity were recruited via Amazon Mechanical Turk. Participants self-reported
8
height and weight and completed a battery of questionnaires. Correlational analyses showed that
9
more frequent EE-D, EE-A, and EE-B were related to poorer psychological well-being, greater
M AN U
SC
RI PT
2
eating disorder symptoms, and more difficulties with emotion regulation. EE-P was not
11
significantly related to outcome variables. In regression analyses, eating in response to
12
depression (EE-D) was the type of emotional eating most closely related to psychological well-
13
being, eating disorder symptoms, and emotion regulation difficulties. Exploratory analyses
14
revealed associations between EE-D, EE-A, and EE-B and facets of emotion regulation and
15
specific disordered eating symptoms. Findings suggest that unique patterns exist between
16
specific types of emotional eating and psychological outcomes.
17
Keywords: Emotional Eating; Obesity; Emotion Regulation.
19
20
21
EP
AC C
18
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 3
1
Emotional eating refers to the tendency to eat in response to emotional triggers as opposed to a true physiological need for food.1 Although normal weight individuals report
3
emotional eating,2 it is an even greater problem for overweight adults. Approximately half
4
(57.3%) of overweight/obese adults endorse high levels of emotional eating.3 Emotional eating
5
is associated with increased body mass index (BMI), greater waist circumference, and more body
6
fat.4 In a convenience sample of adults, subjects who endorsed the highest levels of emotional
7
eating were 13.38 times more likely to be overweight or obese than subjects who endorsed low
8
levels of emotional eating.5 Emotional eating is also predictive of weight gain over time6 and
9
difficulty losing weight.7 As compared to non-emotional eaters, emotional eaters report greater
M AN U
SC
RI PT
2
consumption of sweet and high fat foods,8 eating in response to stressors,9 and more frequent
11
snacking.9 Despite the negative consequences of emotional eating, emotional eating research has
12
been limited by a focus on eating in response to general, as opposed to specific emotions.1
13
Furthermore, most emotional eating studies exclude positive emotions as possible eating triggers.
14
Conceptualization of emotional eating behavior can be improved by investigating links between
15
specific, varied types of emotional eating and health outcomes. A small amount of emotional eating research has examined distinct relationships between
EP
16
TE D
10
specific emotions and eating behavior. According to Macht’s five-way model,10 emotions that
18
differ in valence and intensity have unique influences on eating. Support for this conclusion has
19
been found in cross-sectional and experimental studies. In a study that included obese binge
20
eaters, overweight non-binge eaters, and normal weight adults, each group reported that the urge
21
to eat varied significantly in response to 23 different emotions.11 Furthermore, experimental data
22
showed that eating in response to anxiety12 and trait anxiety13 predicted food intake in a
23
laboratory setting, whereas eating in response to anger and trait anger did not. A recent review
AC C
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4
paper highlighted stress, anxiety, and depression as emotions commonly associating with eating,
2
further supporting the importance of examining links between specific emotions and eating.14 To
3
address previous limitations, the Emotional Eating Scale (EES) was designed to measure eating
4
in response to specific, as opposed to general negative emotions.1 The EES assesses eating in
5
response to 25 specific, negative emotions and yields three subscales: depression, anxiety, and
6
anger/frustration. However, only 1 item from the EES directly assesses eating in response to
7
boredom, and, none of the previously described emotional eating assessments measure positive
8
emotions as eating triggers.
SC
Despite the exclusion of boredom and positive emotions from investigations of emotional
M AN U
9
RI PT
1
eating, some studies have suggested that these are emotional eating triggers that should be
11
examined more closely. College students15 and male adults seeking bariatric surgery16 endorsed
12
boredom as the emotion that most commonly triggers eating. In two separate laboratory studies,
13
completion of a boring task was associated with snacking desire17 and food consumption.18
14
Furthermore, results from a diary study showed that boredom was predictive of calorie and fat
15
consumption.17 Due to an increased interest in boredom, the EES was recently revised to include
16
a more comprehensive focus on boredom.19 However, the EES-revised does not assess the urge
17
to eat due to positive emotions even though positive emotions have been associated with greater
18
meal consumption.20 A diary study showed that positive emotions were more likely than
19
negative to induce eating in a sample of university students.21 The Emotional Appetite
20
Questionnaire (EMAQ) was designed to examine eating in response to negative and positive
21
emotions.22 Despite evidence for boredom and positive emotions as triggers for eating, it
22
remains unknown whether eating in response to these emotions has similar negative physical and
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 5
1
psychological correlates as eating in response to typically examined negative emotions (e.g.,
2
sadness, anxiety).
3
Prior studies have shown a link between general emotional eating and psychopathology symptoms. For example, emotional eating has been linked to greater symptoms of depression in
5
clinical23-25 and population-based samples.2 Similarly, higher levels of emotional eating was
6
related to more severe neuroticism (e.g., anxiety and depression) in a treatment-seeking sample
7
of adults with obesity.26 A recent investigation showed that among Portuguese undergraduate
8
students, emotional eating was associated with a range of psychopathological symptoms
9
including obsessive compulsive behaviors, psychoticism, and hostility, in addition to depression
M AN U
SC
RI PT
4
and anxiety.27 However, previous studies have exclusively examined associations between
11
general emotional eating and psychopathology. No studies have examined relationships between
12
eating in response to boredom or positive emotions and mood or anxiety symptoms. Given that
13
boredom and positive emotions are common eating triggers,17,19 eating in response to these
14
emotions may resemble more normative eating behaviors, associated with less pathology,
15
particularly in non-clinical populations.
Consistent findings also show that emotional eating is related to binge eating and other
EP
16
TE D
10
eating disordered behaviors. Since it is well established that emotions serve as a common trigger
18
for binge episodes,28 it is unsurprising that the link between emotional eating and binge eating
19
has been replicated in multiple samples.29,30 In addition to the greater presence of emotional
20
eating among binge eaters, a study that examined emotional eating in various groups of eating
21
disordered patients showed that emotional eating was also elevated in patients with anorexia (i.e.,
22
restricting and binge/purge types) and bulimia.31 Furthermore, compared to non-emotional
23
eaters, emotional eaters reported significantly greater drive for thinness and body
AC C
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 6
dissatisfaction.32 However, it is possible that the link between general emotional eating and
2
disordered eating is stronger than the link between eating triggered by boredom or positive
3
emotions and disordered eating. For example, in a recent study, eating due to positive and
4
negative emotions were significantly related to binge eating, restrained eating, and preoccupation
5
with body image and weight.33 However, eating due to negative emotions was more strongly
6
correlated with binge eating than eating due to positive emotions. The relationship between
7
boredom eating and disordered eating has not yet been investigated. Thus, eating in response to
8
boredom or positive emotions may be less closely related to eating disordered symptoms than
9
eating in response to other emotions.
SC
M AN U
10
RI PT
1
In addition to general psychopathology and specific eating disorder symptoms, emotional eating has been related to emotion regulation difficulties. Affect regulation models argue that
12
emotional eating is the result of poor emotion regulation, as opposed to negative emotion per
13
se.34 Several studies have concluded that various facets of emotion regulation (i.e., general
14
difficulties in emotion regulation, poor mood regulation expectancies, and emotional awareness)
15
were associated with more frequent emotional eating.35 In addition, two laboratory studies have
16
also demonstrated links between experimentally manipulated emotion regulation strategies (i.e.,
17
suppression and lack of emotional awareness) and food intake.36 Despite evidence showing a
18
relationship between emotional eating and poor emotion regulation, it is unknown whether
19
similar patterns exist between eating triggered by boredom or positive emotions and emotion
20
regulation. One recent study concluded that difficulties in emotion regulation was associated
21
with eating in response to boredom in a college student sample.37 And, to our knowledge, no
22
prior studies have examined whether eating due to positive emotions is related to poor emotion
AC C
EP
TE D
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 7
1
regulation. Thus, it is possible that eating triggered by boredom is associated with poor emotion
2
regulation, but eating triggered by positive emotions may not be. The current study builds upon emotional eating research by simultaneously examining
4
correlates of eating in response to specific negative (i.e., depression, anxiety/anger, boredom)
5
and positive emotions in a sample of adults with overweight/obesity. The primary aim of the
6
study was to examine whether boredom and positive emotional eating were associated with
7
similar, negative psychological (i.e., global psychological well-being, eating disorder symptoms,
8
and emotion regulation) and physical health variables as emotional eating due to depression and
9
anxiety/anger. The present study also examined which types of emotional eating (i.e., eating in
10
response to depression (EE-D), anxiety/anger (EE-A), boredom (EE-B), and positive emotions
11
(EE-P)) are most closely associated with psychological and physical health variables. Even
12
though eating in response to positive emotions and boredom have been frequently reported, it is
13
unknown whether these behaviors are likely to co-occur with related negative psychosocial
14
factors. Furthermore, certain types of emotional eating (e.g., EE-D, EE-A) may be more closely
15
related to negative outcomes than others (e.g., EE-B, EE-P). Finally, the present study examines
16
physical health as a correlate of emotional eating; because, although emotional eating has been
17
associated with certain health outcomes such as BMI4 and weight gain,38 prior studies have not
18
examined whether eating in response to various negative and positive emotions is associated
19
with physical health more broadly. Given links between emotional eating and weight-related
20
variables, it is expected that emotional eating in response to depression and anxiety/anger will be
21
associated with poorer physical health, but it is hypothesized that this same relationship will not
22
be observed between physical health and eating in response to boredom or positive emotions.
23
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
3
Methods
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 8
1
Participants Adults with overweight or obesity (n=189) were recruited to participate in a cross-
3
sectional study on eating behaviors and health. Inclusion criteria included adulthood (≥18
4
years), resident of the U.S., English fluency, and overweight or obesity (i.e., BMI ≥25 kg/m2). A
5
total of 288 adults were initially recruited, and 99 of them were excluded from the final sample.
6
Participants were excluded for evidence of random responding on the survey (i.e., failure to
7
correctly answer at least 4 out of 5 quality control items and/or survey completion in less than 10
8
minutes; 10.4% of the total sample), if the computer IP address fell outside of the U.S. (1%), if
9
the reported BMI was <25 (10.8%), for the reported presence of a severe medical condition affecting weight or appetite (e.g., insulin dependent diabetes, cancer requiring active
11
chemotherapy; 11.8%), or failure to report height and weight (.3%).
12
Measures
13
Demographics. Participants self-reported demographic characteristics including age, sex, marital
14
status, ethnicity, education level, income, and employment status.
15
Anthropometry. Subjects were asked to report their height in feet and inches and their weight in
16
pounds. Self-reported height and weight were used to calculate BMI (kg/m2).
17
Types of Emotional Eating.
18
EP
TE D
10
AC C
M AN U
SC
RI PT
2
EE-D, EE-A, & EE-B. Eating in response to depression (EE-D), anxiety/anger (EE-A),
19
and boredom (EE-B) were measured with a revised version of the Emotional Eating Scale
20
(EES).1 The EES includes 25-items that assess the urge in eat in response to various negative
21
emotions (e.g., nervous, sad, furious). Item response options are rated on a 5-point scale ranging
22
from 1 (no desire to eat) to 5 (overwhelming desire to eat). Reliability and validity for the EES
23
have been established in clinical1 and non-clinical samples.39 The original EES yielded 3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 9
subscales, depression, anxiety, and anger/frustration. Koball and colleagues15 modified the
2
original EES to include 6 items that measure eating in response to boredom (e.g., restless,
3
unstimulated). After the modification, factor analytic results yielded 3 subscales: depression,
4
anxiety/anger, and boredom. Furthermore, Koball and colleagues15 reported high levels of
5
internal consistency for the total scale and subscales. In the current sample of adults, internal
6
consistency of EES scales was high (Cronbach’s alpha for EE-D = .94; EE-A = .91; EE-B = .91). EE-P. The Emotional Appetite Questionnaire (EMAQ)22 includes 22-items designed to
SC
7
RI PT
1
assess eating in response to positive and negative emotions and situations. In the current study,
9
the 5-item positive emotions subscale was used to specifically evaluate eating in response to
M AN U
8
positive emotions (i.e., confident, happy, relaxed, playful, enthusiastic). Response options range
11
from 1-9, and anchors include “much less,” “the same,” and “much more.” Subjects can also
12
select “NA” if the item does not apply or “DK” if the answer is unknown. Test-retest reliability,
13
internal consistency,22 and construct validity40 have been established. Internal consistency of the
14
eating in response to positive emotions subscale in the current sample was adequate (Cronbach’s
15
alpha = .83).
16
Global Psychological Well-Being. Psychological well-being was assessed with the Symptom
17
Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-global).41 The SCL-90 is a self-report measure of a wide range of
18
psychological symptoms. Subjects are asked to endorse how much they were bothered by 90
19
different mental health symptoms during the past week on a 5-point likert scale ranging from
20
“not at all” to “extremely.” The present study utilized the global severity index which is the
21
average rating assigned to all items. The SCL-90 has demonstrated good psychometric
22
properties, and it has been widely used with clinical and community samples.41 Excellent
23
internal consistency was confirmed in the current sample (Cronbach’s alpha = .99).
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 10
Eating Disorder Symptoms. The Eating Disorders Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q)42 is a
2
self-report measure that was developed as an alternative to the Eating Disorder Examination, 43
3
the gold-standard clinical interview used for eating disorder diagnosis. The EDE-Q includes 36-
4
items that assess eating disorder symptoms present over the previous 28 days. Item responses
5
are provided on a 7-point scale with higher scores indicating more severe eating disorder
6
psychopathology. The EDE-Q is comprised of 4 subscales: restraint, shape concerns, weight
7
concerns, and eating concerns. The current study used the subscale scores and the global score,
8
which is the average of the subscale scores. The EDE-Q has been previously used with
9
overweight/obese adults, and strong reliability and validity have been demonstrated.44 Internal
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
reliability was also confirmed in the current sample with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .81 - .93
11
for global and subscale scores.
12
Emotion Regulation. The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS)45 is a 36-item self-
13
report measure designed to measure difficulties in various dimensions of emotion regulation.
14
Subjects are asked to endorse how frequently each item applies to themselves with response
15
options ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Ten items are reverse scored so that
16
higher scores indicate more problems with emotion regulation. Sample items include: “When
17
I’m upset, I feel guilty for feeling that way” and “I pay attention to how I feel.” The DERS
18
yields 6 subscales that include nonacceptance of emotional responses, difficulties engaging in
19
goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, lack of emotional awareness, limited access
20
to emotion regulation strategies, and lack of emotional clarity. The DERS demonstrated good
21
psychometric properties in a sample of overweight/obese adults.35 In the present sample, internal
22
consistency was high for the total score and subscales with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .85 -
23
.96.
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 11
Physical Health. The short-form health survey (SF-12)46 was administered to examine general
2
physical health. The SF-12 includes 12 items that evaluate eight broad domains of health
3
including physical functioning (i.e., limitations in physical activity due to health problems), role-
4
physical (i.e., limitations in role activities due to physical health problems), bodily pain (i.e.,
5
presence of pain and limitations due to pain), general health (i.e., 1-item rating of general health),
6
vitality (i.e., energy level and fatigue), social functioning (i.e., limitations in social activities due
7
to physical or emotional problems), role-emotional (i.e., limitations in role activities due to
8
emotional health problems), and mental health (i.e., psychological stress and well-being). The
9
SF-12 can be used to derive a Physical Component Summary (PCS) score and a Mental
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
Component Summary (MCS) score. Only the PCS was used in the current study. Scores range
11
from 0-100 with higher scores indicating better functioning. The SF-12 has been extensively
12
used and validated with community and clinical samples.47 Internal consistency was adequate in
13
the present sample of adults (Cronbach’s alpha = .79).
14
Procedures
15
TE D
10
Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) was utilized to recruit subjects and distribute the online survey. MTurk has been shown to provide quality data and recruit a diverse sample.48
17
Informed consent was obtained via an electronic form at the beginning of the survey. Then,
18
participants completed a series of self-report questionnaires, lasting approximately 20-30
19
minutes in duration. A total of 5 quality control items were included to identify possible subjects
20
who were randomly responding to survey questions (i.e., “When you are finished reading this
21
statement, select 66-90%;” “I want you to answer this item by selecting ‘Almost Always;’”
22
“Choose the answer ‘6-12 days’ for this item;” “I try to answer all these questions honestly;”
AC C
EP
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 12
1
“Please select ‘moderately’ for this question”). Eligible subjects who completed the survey were
2
compensated by crediting online accounts with $0.50.
3
Statistical Analyses Of the final sample of 189 adults with overweight/obesity, 1 participant was excluded due
RI PT
4
to a reported BMI that was identified as an outlier. Outliers were defined as values that were
6
greater than 1.5 interquartile ranges below the first or above the third quartile. Descriptive
7
statistics were calculated to examine sample demographics. Next, correlations were calculated
8
between types of emotional eating (i.e., EE-D, EE-A, EE-B, EE-P) and demographic variables
9
(i.e., age, sex, BMI, income). Correlations were then calculated between types of emotional
M AN U
SC
5
10
eating and psychological (i.e., global psychological well-being, eating disorder symptoms, and
11
emotion regulation) and physical health variables.
Regression analyses were used to further examine associations between types of
13
emotional eating and outcome variables that were related in bivariate analyses, and a Bonferroni
14
adjustment was used. Each regression model was tested at a significance level of .004 (.05/12).
15
In each regression analysis, BMI was entered at Step 1, types of emotional eating were entered
16
simultaneously as independent variables at Step 2, and outcome variables were entered as the
17
dependent variable. Given the relationship between BMI and emotional eating,4 BMI was
18
entered as a covariate in regression models to examine whether types of emotional eating and
19
outcome variables were related, independent of the influence of BMI.
EP
AC C
20
TE D
12
Exploratory analyses were used to examine relationships between types of emotional
21
eating and facets of emotion regulation. Additional exploratory analyses were calculated to
22
examine relationships between types of emotional eating and specific eating disorder symptoms.
23
In each regression analysis, BMI was entered at Step 1, types of emotional eating were entered
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 13
1
simultaneously as independent variables at Step 2, and outcome variables were entered as the
2
dependent variable. All calculations were performed using SPSS 20.0 (www.SPSS.com). Results
3
5
Descriptive Statistics
RI PT
4
Preliminary analyses examined characteristics of the study sample of 188 adults with overweight/obesity (mean BMI = 33.17, SD = 6.98). Approximately two-thirds (64.9%) of the
7
sample were women. Mean age was 41.78 (13.61), and ages ranged from 19-83. The majority
8
of participants identified as non-Latino white (87.2%), and almost half (44.1%) were married.
9
Education level varied. Approximately half (49.5%) of the sample reported earning a bachelor’s
10
or graduate degree, one-third (36.2%) reported some college or a technical/vocation school, and
11
14.4% reported less education (i.e., some high school or a high school degree). Approximately
12
two-thirds of the sample was employed part-time (12.2%) or full-time (55.9%). Reported
13
income was less than $20,000 (19.7%), $20-50,000 (39.4%), $50-75,000 (21.3%), and greater
14
than $75,000 (19.7%).
15
Correlations
TE D
M AN U
SC
6
Correlations between types of emotional eating, demographics (i.e., age, sex, BMI, and
17
income), and outcome variables (i.e., psychological well-being, eating disorder symptoms, and
18
difficulties in emotion regulation) were calculated (see Table 1). Greater BMI was related to EE-
19
D (r = .21, p <.01) and EE-A (r = .29, p <.001). Female sex was related to greater EE-D (r = .15,
20
p <.05) and EE-B (r = .15, p <.05). EE-P was not related to any demographic characteristics.
21
Eating in response to depression, anxiety/anger, and boredom (i.e., EE-D, EE-A, and EE-B) were
22
related to poorer psychological well-being, greater eating disorder symptoms, and more
23
difficulties with emotion regulation. EE-P was unrelated to main outcome variables (i.e.,
AC C
EP
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 14
psychological well-being, eating disorder symptoms, and difficulties in emotion regulation) and
2
was therefore not included in primary regression analyses. All types of emotional eating were
3
unrelated to physical health (SF-12).
4
Regression Analyses
5
RI PT
1
To examine which types of emotional eating were most closely associated with outcome variables, a series of regression analyses were conducted. BMI was entered at Step 1, and EE-D,
7
EE-A, and EE-B were entered at Step 2 in each regression model. The first model was
8
significant (F = 21.51, p <.002) and explained 31.3% of the variance in psychological well-being
9
(Table 2). EE-D was the only type of emotional eating significantly associated with
M AN U
SC
6
psychological well-being (β = .40, p <.002). The second model was significant (F = 20.18, p
11
<.002) and explained 29.1% of the variance in eating disorder symptoms (Table 3). EE-D was
12
the only type of emotional eating significantly associated with eating disorder symptoms (β =
13
.36, p <.002). The third model was also significant (F = 20.07, p <.002) and explained 30.2% of
14
the variance in emotion regulation (Table 4). EE-D (β = .32, p <.002) was the only type of
15
emotional eating significantly associated with emotion regulation. BMI was not significantly
16
related to outcome variables in the final models.
17
Exploratory Analyses
EP
Regression analyses were conducted to examine associations between types of emotional
AC C
18
TE D
10
19
eating (EE-D, EE-A, EE-B, EE-P) and facets of emotion regulation, after controlling for BMI
20
(Table 5). The model predicting lack of emotional awareness was not significant (p = .14). All
21
other models were significant (p < .05) and explained 13.1% - 28.1% of the variance in specific
22
facets of emotion regulation. EE-D was significantly associated with nonacceptance of
23
emotions, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, and
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 15
limited access to emotion regulation strategies. EE-B was significantly associated with
2
difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior and lack of emotional clarity. EE-A was
3
significantly associated with impulse control difficulties. EE-P and BMI were not significantly
4
associated with facets of emotion regulation in multivariate models.
RI PT
1
Additional exploratory analyses were conducted to examine associations between types
6
of emotional eating and (EE-D, EE-A, EE-B, EE-P) and EDE-Q subscales, after controlling for
7
BMI (Table 6). The model predicting restraint was not significant (p = .07). All other models
8
were significant (p < .05) and explained 25.2% - 43.3% of the variance in EDE-Q subscales (i.e.,
9
eating, weight, and shape concerns). EE-D was significantly associated with eating, weight, and
M AN U
SC
5
10
shape concerns. EE-A was significantly associated with eating concerns. EE-B was
11
significantly associated with shape concerns. EE-P and BMI were not significantly associated
12
with facets of emotion regulation in multivariate models.
14
Discussion
TE D
13
To our knowledge, the present study is the first to simultaneously examine psychosocial correlates of eating in response to depression, anxiety/anger, boredom, and positive emotions. In
16
the current sample of adults with overweight/obesity, eating triggered by depression,
17
anxiety/anger, and boredom were associated with poorer psychological well-being, greater eating
18
disorder symptoms, and more emotion regulation difficulties. Eating triggered by positive
19
emotions was not associated with negative outcomes. Furthermore, all types of emotional eating
20
were unrelated to poorer self-reported physical health. Despite recent interest in eating in
21
response to positive emotions, eating in response to negative emotions seems to be more closely
22
related to problematic psychological outcomes. Furthermore, the unique pattern of relationships
AC C
EP
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 16
1
that emerged provides further support for the importance of examining emotional eating as a
2
multi-dimensional construct.
3
In multivariate models, eating triggered by depression was the type of emotional eating that was most closely related to lower psychological well-being, eating disorder symptoms, and
5
poor emotion regulation. Findings are consistent with previous research that has demonstrated
6
links between more emotional eating and increased symptoms of depression,23 anxiety,26 and
7
disordered eating behaviors.31 The current study builds upon previous research by showing that
8
eating triggered by depression was more closely associated with lower psychological well-being,
9
eating disorder symptoms, and emotion regulation difficulties than eating triggered by other
M AN U
SC
RI PT
4
10
negative and positive emotions. Overweight/obese adults report a tendency to eat in response to
11
various emotions,11 but adults who experience the urge to eat in response to depression may be
12
most at risk for experiencing related psychological difficulties.
Despite significant correlational relationships, in multivariate models, eating triggered by
TE D
13
boredom and anxiety/anger were not uniquely associated with negative outcomes, suggesting
15
that eating due to boredom and anxiety/anger may be indicative of less psychopathology than
16
eating due to depression. It is possible that eating when bored or anxious/angry could be most
17
problematic when occurring in the presence of other types of negative emotional eating (i.e.,
18
eating when depressed). It is important to note that in the present sample, correlations showed a
19
fairly high level of overlap between eating when depressed, anxious/angry, and bored,
20
suggesting that these behaviors often occur simultaneously. Nonetheless, adults with
21
overweight/obesity who rarely eat when feeling depressed (e.g., exclusively boredom eaters)
22
may represent a less pathological sub-group of emotional eaters who are less likely to report co-
23
morbid psychological symptoms.
AC C
EP
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 17
Perhaps surprisingly, eating in response to positive emotions was not significantly related
2
to poorer psychological well-being, greater eating disorder symptoms, or emotion dysregulation.
3
Despite a recent interest in the presence of eating triggered by positive emotions, very little
4
research has been done to examine its relationship to negative outcomes. Our findings are
5
somewhat in contrast to a previous study which showed that eating in response to positive
6
emotions was associated with disordered eating symptoms, but to a lesser degree than was eating
7
in response to negative emotions.33 In the current study, eating in response to positive emotions
8
was unrelated to negative psychological factors, suggesting that adults with overweight/obesity
9
who eat in response to positive emotions exclusively may not experience additional, problematic
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
psychological symptoms. Of note, the Sultson study33 included a broad sample of women, not
11
restricted by weight, which may explain the differing results. Relatedly, some authors have
12
suggested that eating triggered by positive emotions is a separate construct than eating triggered
13
by negative emotions.49 In the present sample, positive emotional eating was related to boredom
14
eating but unrelated to eating in response to depression or anxiety/anger.
15
TE D
10
Contrary to our hypothesis, none of the types of emotional eating were associated with poorer physical health in our sample of adults with overweight/obesity. Despite this, as
17
expected, greater BMI was significantly associated with physical health impairment.
18
Consequently, this null finding may be explained by the inclusion of a sample of adults with a
19
restricted weight range. Previous studies that used the same physical health measure
20
administered in the current study (i.e., SF-12)46 found that similar physical health functioning
21
was reported among obese non-binge eaters and binge eaters (with and without obesity),50,51 and
22
that these scores were lower than scores obtained from non-obese, non-binge eaters.50 Thus,
AC C
EP
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 18
1
given the health consequences of obesity, it is likely that physical functioning is broadly
2
impaired in this group, regardless of the level of emotional eating.
3
Exploratory analyses suggest possible unique relationships between types of emotional eating and specific facets of emotion regulation. Difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior
5
emerged as a specific facet of emotion regulation that was associated with eating triggered by
6
depression and boredom. It is possible that when experiencing low intensity negative emotions
7
(e.g., depression, boredom), an inability to attend to alternative, adaptive tasks may increase
8
vulnerability for eating as a strategy for regulating negative affect. Lack of emotional clarity was
9
also uniquely related to eating due to boredom (but not other negative emotions). Thus, when
10
adults with overweight/obesity are feeling bored, confusion about what they are feeling could
11
promote eating. Impulse control difficulties were closely related to the tendency to eat in
12
response to depression and anxiety/anger which suggests that these two types of emotional eating
13
may share an underlying impairment in inhibition. Other facets of emotion regulation (i.e.,
14
nonacceptance of emotional reactions and limited access to emotion regulation strategies) were
15
also related to eating triggered by depression. Few studies have examined how specific aspects
16
of emotion regulation are related to emotional eating. Findings from the present study build
17
upon a previous study that identified limited access to emotion regulation strategies, difficulties
18
engaging in goal-directed behavior, and lack of emotional clarity as the emotion regulation
19
strategies most closely related to emotional overeating (i.e., eating in response to general
20
negative emotions) in a sample of treatment-seeking adults with obesity and binge eating
21
disorder.35 Taken together, findings suggest that certain emotion regulation strategies may be
22
more closely linked to various types of emotional eating. Future research investigations may aim
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 19
1
to examine whether treatment programs should target and teach specific emotion regulation
2
strategies to different types of emotional eaters.
3
Exploratory analyses also showed that types of emotional eating may be more closely related to specific eating disorder symptoms than others. Similar to the pattern described above,
5
eating triggered by depression was the type of emotional eating most closely associated with
6
several eating disorder symptoms (i.e., concerns about eating, shape, and weight). This provides
7
further evidence that emotional eating triggered by a depressed mood is most indicative of
8
related psychopathology. Additional findings showed that eating in response to boredom was
9
uniquely related to shape concerns, and eating in response to anxiety/anger was uniquely related
M AN U
SC
RI PT
4
10
to eating concerns. Thus, although eating triggered by anxiety/anger and boredom are less
11
robustly associated with negative outcomes than eating triggered by feelings of depression, they
12
can be independently associated with disordered eating symptoms.
The current study is limited by a cross-sectional design and reliance on self-report
14
questionnaires that are subject to bias. Recent studies have critiqued the use of self-report
15
measures of emotional eating, suggesting that these scales may not actually be assessing true
16
emotional eating behavior.52 In addition, multiple analyses were conducted in the present study;
17
and, although a Bonferroni was used to determine significance in multivariate analyses, the
18
possibility of Type I error remains, particularly for correlational and exploratory analyses.
19
Furthermore, the current study recruited a general sample of adults with overweight/obesity
20
which may not be representative of emotional eaters who are not overweight/obese or a
21
treatment-seeking sample. Despite its limitations, the current study makes an important
22
contribution to the emotional eating literature by examining correlates of eating in response to
23
discrete emotions. It appears that various types of emotional eating are associated with unique
AC C
EP
TE D
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 20
psychological factors. Adults who eat in response to feelings of depression, anxiety/anger,
2
boredom, or positive emotions likely have distinct psychosocial characteristics and consequently,
3
may require tailored treatment approaches.
RI PT
1
4
5
SC
6
M AN U
7
8
9
13
14
15
16
EP
12
AC C
11
TE D
10
17
18
References
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 21
1. Arnow B, Kenardy J, Agras WS. The emotional eating scale: The development of a measure
2
to assess coping with negative affect by eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders.
3
1995;18(1):79-90.
4
2. Konttinen H, Mannisto S, Sarlio-Lhteenkorva S, Silventoinen K, Haukkala A. Emotional
5
eating, depressive symptoms and self-reported food consumption. A population-based study.
6
Appetite. 2010;54(3):473-479.
7
3. Peneau S, Menard E, Mejean C, Bellisle F, Hercberg S. Sex and dieting modify the association
8
between emotional eating and weight status. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition.
9
2013;97(6):1307-1313. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.054916 [doi].
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
4. Konttinen H, Silventoinen K, Sarlio-Lhteenkorva S, Mannisto S, Haukkala A. Emotional
11
eating and physical activity self-efficacy as pathways in the association between depressive
12
symptoms and adiposity indicators. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2010;92(5):1031-
13
1039.
14
5. Ozier AD, Kendrick OW, Leeper JD, Knol LL, Perko M, & Burnham J. (2008). Overweight
15
and obesity are associated with emotion-and stress-related eating as measured by the eating and
16
appraisal due to emotions and stress questionnaire. Journal of the American Dietetic Association,
17
108(1), 49-56.
18
6. Koenders PG, van Strien T. Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior, drives weight
19
gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. Journal of Occupational and Environmental
20
Medicine. 2011;53(11):1287-1293.
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 22
7. Braden A, Flatt SW, Boutelle KN, Strong D, Sherwood NE, Rock CL. Emotional eating is
2
associated with weight loss success among adults enrolled in a weight loss program. Journal of
3
Behavioral Medicine. 2016;39(4):727-732.
4
8. Camilleri GM, Mejean C, Kesse-Guyot E, Andreeva VA, Bellisle F, Hercberg S, Péneau S.
5
The associations between emotional eating and consumption of energy-dense snack foods are
6
modified by sex and depressive symptomatology. Journal of Nutrition. 2014 May 21:jn- 114.
7
9. O'connor DB, Jones F, Conner M, McMillan B, Ferguson E. Effects of daily hassles and
8
eating style on eating behavior. Health Psychology. 2008;27(1):S20.
9
10. Macht M. How emotions affect eating: A five-way model. Appetite. 2008;50(1):1-11.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
11. Zeeck A, Stelzer N, Linster HW, Joos A, Hartmann A. Emotion and eating in binge eating
11
disorder and obesity. European Eating Disorders Review. 2011;19(5):426-437.
12
12. Schneider KL, Panza E, Appelhans BM, Whited MC, Oleski JL, Pagoto SL. The emotional
13
eating scale. Can a self-report measure predict observed emotional eating? Appetite.
14
2012;58(2):563-566.
15
13. Schneider KL, Appelhans BM, Whited MC, Oleski J, Pagoto SL. Trait anxiety, but not trait
16
anger, predisposes obese individuals to emotional eating. Appetite. 2010;55(3):701-706.
17
14. Devonport TJ, Nicholls W, Fullerton C. A systematic review of the association between
18
emotions and eating behaviour in normal and overweight adult populations. Journal of Health
19
Psychology. 2017:1359105317697813.
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 23
15. Koball AM, Meers MR, Storfer-Isser A, Domoff SE, Musher-Eizenman DR. Eating when
2
bored: Revision of the emotional eating scale with a focus on boredom. Health Psychology.
3
2012;31(4):521.
4
16. Walfish S, Brown TA. Self-assessed emotional factors contributing to increased weight in
5
presurgical male bariatric patients. Bariatric Nursing and Surgical Patient Care. 2009;4(1):49-
6
52.
7
17. Moynihan AB, van Tilburg WA, Igou ER, Wisman A, Donnelly AE, Mulcaire JB. Eaten up
8
by boredom: Consuming food to escape awareness of the bored self. Frontiers in psychology.
9
2015;6.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
18. Abramson EE, Stinson SG. Boredom and eating in obese and non-obese individuals.
11
Addictive Behaviors. 1977;2(4):181-185.
12
19. Koball AM, Meers MR, Storfer-Isser A, Domoff SE, Musher-Eizenman DR. Eating when
13
bored: Revision of the emotional eating scale with a focus on boredom. Health psychology.
14
2012;31(4):521.
15
20. Cardi V, Leppanen J, Treasure J. The effects of negative and positive mood induction on
16
eating behaviour: A meta-analysis of laboratory studies in the healthy population and eating and
17
weight disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2015;57:299-309.
18
21. Evers C, Adriaanse M, de Ridder DT, de Witt Huberts, Jessie C. Good mood food. positive
19
emotion as a neglected trigger for food intake. Appetite. 2013;68:1-7.
20
22. Geliebter A, Aversa A. Emotional eating in overweight, normal weight, and underweight
21
individuals. Eating Behaviors. 2003;3(4):341-347.
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 24
23. Masheb RM, Grilo CM. Emotional overeating and its associations with eating disorder
2
psychopathology among overweight patients with binge eating disorder. International Journal of
3
Eating Disorders. 2006;39(2):141-146.
4
24. Schulz S, Laessle RG. Associations of negative affect and eating behaviour in obese women
5
with and without binge eating disorder. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia,
6
Bulimia and Obesity. 2010;15(4):287-293.
7
25. Clum GA, Rice JC, Broussard M, Johnson CC, Webber LS. Associations between depressive
8
symptoms, self-efficacy, eating styles, exercise and body mass index in women. Journal of
9
Behavioral Medicine. 2014;37(4):577-586.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
26. Elfhag K, Morey LC. Personality traits and eating behavior in the obese: Poor self-control in
11
emotional and external eating but personality assets in restrained eating. Eating Behaviors.
12
2008;9(3):285-293.
13
27. Rui Poínhos, Bruno M.P.M. Oliveira, Flora Correia, Psychopathologic correlates of eating
14
behaviour among portuguese undergraduate students, Nutrition. 2017;
15
https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.nut.2017.10.009.
16
28. Leehr EJ, Krohmer K, Schag K, Dresler T, Zipfel S, Giel KE. Emotion regulation model in
17
binge eating disorder and obesity-a systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
18
2015;49:125-134.
19
29. Ricca V, Castellini G, Sauro CL, Ravaldi C, Lapi F, Mannucci E, Rotella CM, Faravelli C.
20
Correlations between binge eating and emotional eating in a sample of overweight subjects.
21
Appetite. 2009;53(3):418-421.
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 25
30. Pinaquy S, Chabrol H, Simon C, Louvet J, Barbe P. Emotional eating, alexithymia, and
2
binge‐eating disorder in obese women. Obesity. 2003;11(2):195-201.
3
31. Ricca V, Castellini G, Fioravanti G, Lo Sauro C, Rotella F, Ravaldi C, Lazzeretti L, Faravelli
4
C. Emotional eating in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Comprehensive Psychiatry.
5
2012;53(3):245-251.
6
32. Lindeman M, Stark K. Emotional eating and eating disorder psychopathology. Eating
7
Disorders. 2001;9(3):251-259.
8
33. Sultson H, Kukk K, Akkermann K. Positive and negative emotional eating have different
9
associations with overeating and binge eating: Construction and validation of the positive-
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
negative emotional eating scale. Appetite. 2017;116:423-430.
11
34. Wiser S, Telch CF. Dialectical behavior therapy for binge‐eating disorder. Journal of
12
Clinical Psychology. 1999;55(6):755-768.
13
35. Gianini LM, White MA, Masheb RM. Eating pathology, emotion regulation, and emotional
14
overeating in obese adults with binge eating disorder. Eating Behaviors. 2013;14(3):309-313.
15
36. Evers C, Stok FM, de Ridder DT. Feeding your feelings: Emotion regulation strategies and
16
emotional eating. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2010;36(6):792-804.
17
37. Crockett AC, Myhre SK, Rokke PD. Boredom proneness and emotion regulation predict
18
emotional eating. Journal of Health Psychology. 2015;20(5):670-680. doi:
19
10.1177/1359105315573439 [doi].
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 26
38. Koenders PG, van Strien T. Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior, drives weight
2
gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. Journal of Occupational and Environmental
3
Medicine. 2011;53(11):1287-1293. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31823078a2 [doi].
4
39. Waller G, Osman S. Emotional eating and eating psychopathology among non‐eating‐
5
disordered women. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1998;23(4):419-424.
6
40. Nolan LJ, Halperin LB, Geliebter A. Emotional appetite questionnaire. Construct validity
7
and relationship with BMI. Appetite. 2010;54(2):314-319.
8
41. Derogatis LR, Savitz KL. The SCL–90–R and brief symptom inventory (BSI) in primary
9
care. In: Maruish ME (ed) Handbook of psychological assessment in primary care settings.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Mahwah, NJ. 2000:297-334.
11
42. Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ. Assessment of eating disorders: Interview or self‐report
12
questionnaire? International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1994;16(4):363-370.
13
43. Cooper Z, Fairburn C. The eating disorder examination: A semi‐structured interview for the
14
assessment of the specific psychopathology of eating disorders. International Journal of Eating
15
Disorders. 1987;6(1):1-8.
16
44. Berg KC, Peterson CB, Frazier P, Crow SJ. Psychometric evaluation of the eating disorder
17
examination and eating disorder examination‐questionnaire: A systematic review of the
18
literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2012;45(3):428-438.
19
45. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation:
20
Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation
21
scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2004;26(1):41-54.
AC C
EP
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 27
46. Ware Jr JE, Kosinski M, Keller SD. A 12-item short-form health survey: Construction of
2
scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Medical Care. 1996;34(3):220-233.
3
47. Ware Jr JE. SF-36 health survey update. Spine. 2000;25(24):3130-3139.
4
48. Casler K, Bickel L, & Hackett E. Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data
5
gathered via Amazon’s MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Computers in
6
Human Behavior, 2013;29(6), 2156-2160.
7
49. van Strien T, Cebolla A, Etchemendy E, Gutierrez-Maldonado J, Ferrer-Garcia M, Botella C,
8
Banos R. Emotional eating and food intake after sadness and joy. Appetite. 2013;66:20-25.
9
50. Grucza RA, Przybeck TR, & Cloninger CR. Prevalence and correlates of binge eating
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
disorder in a community sample. Comprehensive psychiatry, 2007;48(2), 124-131.
11
51. Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B, & Owen C. Recurrent binge eating with and without the
12
“undue influence of weight or shape on self-evaluation”: Implications for the diagnosis of binge
13
eating disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2007;45(5), 929-938.
14
52. Bongers P, Jansen A. Emotional eating is not what you think it is and emotional eating scales
15
do not measure what you think they measure. Frontiers in psychology. 2016;7.
17
18
19
EP
AC C
16
TE D
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
28
Table 1. Correlations between types of emotional eating, demographics, and outcome variables Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 --
2. EE-A
.74***
--
3. EE-B
.61***
.53***
--
4. EE-P
-.04
.06
.23**
--
5. age
-.12
-.04
-.08
.08
--
6. sex
.15*
.07
.15*
-.13
.04
7. income
.02
.08
.04
-.01
.05
8. BMI
.21**
.29***
.13
-.07
.04
9. SCL-90
.57***
.51***
.39***
.02
10. EDE-Q
.54***
.47***
.40***
-.04
11. DERS
.52***
.44***
.45***
12. SF-12
-.11
-.12
-.08
11
12
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1. EE-D
10
--
--
.03
.01
--
.23
.01
-.12
.14
--
-.13
.18
-.08
.13
.45***
--
EP
TE D
.01
-.29***
-.01
.02
.08
.65***
.45***
--
-.02
-.13
-.04
.02
-.22**
-.12
.01
.03
AC C
.07
--
Note. EE-D = depression subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-A = anxiety/anger subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-B = boredom subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-P = positive emotions subscale of the Emotional Appetite Questionnaire; SCL90 = Symptom Checklist-90-revised; EDE-Q = Eating Disorders Examination Questionnaire; DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; SF-12 = Short-Form Health Survey. * p <.05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 29
Table 2. Regression analysis examining associations between types of emotional eating and global psychological well-being (SCL-90) Variable
B
SE B
.02
.01
BMI
.01
.01
EE-D
.03
.01
EE-A
.02
.01
EE-B
.01
.01
Β
.02
M AN U
Step 2
.16
.40*
SC
BMI
RI PT
Step 1
.18
.09
AC C
EP
TE D
Note. EE-D = depression subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-A = anxiety/anger subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-B = boredom subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale * Significant based on Bonferroni correction alpha level of 0.004.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 30
Table 3. Regression analysis examining associations between types of emotional eating and eating disorder symptoms (EDE-Q) Variable
B
SE B
.02
.01
BMI
-.01
.01
EE-D
.05
.01
EE-A
.02
.01
EE-B
.02
.01
β
-.01
M AN U
Step 2
.12
.36*
SC
BMI
RI PT
Step 1
.10
.12
AC C
EP
TE D
Note. EE-D = depression subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-A = anxiety/anger subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-B = boredom subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale * Significant based on Bonferroni correction alpha level of 0.004.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 31
Table 4. Regression analysis examining associations between types of emotional eating and emotion regulation (DERS) Variable
B
SE B
.31
.27
BMI
-.17
.24
EE-D
.89
.27
EE-A
.41
.34
EE-B
.65
.25
Β
-.04
M AN U
Step 2
.08
.32*
SC
BMI
RI PT
Step 1
.12
.20
AC C
EP
TE D
Note. EE-D = depression subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-A = anxiety/anger subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-B = boredom subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale * Significant based on Bonferroni correction alpha level of 0.004.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
32
Table 5. Regression analyses examining associations between types of emotional eating and facets of emotion regulation (DERS), after controlling for BMI
Goals
Impulse
Awareness
Strategies
RI PT
Nonacceptance
Clarity
B
SE B
β
B
SE B
Β
B
SE B
Β
B
SE B
β
B
SE B
Β
B
SE B
β
EE-D
.19
.07
.29**
.18
.06
.33**
.13
.06
.23*
.01
.06
.01
.33
.09
.40***
.07
.05
.17
EE-A
.09
.09
.10
.02
.07
.02
.20
.07
.26**
-.02
-.04
.11
.10
.10
.01
.06
.01
EE-B
.11
.07
.14
.13
.06
.21*
.06
.06
.09
.12
.06
.21
.07
.08
.08
.12
.05
.24*
EE-P
.21
.37
.04
.02
.29
.01
.47
.30
.11
-.55
.30
-.13
.65
.43
.10
-.19
.23
-.06
SC
Variable
TE D
M AN U
.07
AC C
*p < .05, **p < .01, **p <.001.
EP
Note. EE-D = depression subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-A = anxiety/anger subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-B = boredom subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-P = positive emotions subscale of the Emotional Appetite Questionnaire; Nonacceptance = nonacceptance of emotional reactions; Goals = difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior; Impulse = impulse control difficulties; Awareness = lack of emotional awareness; Strategies = limited access to emotion regulation strategies; Clarity = lack of emotional clarity.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
33
Table 6. Regression analyses examining associations between types of emotional eating and EDE-Q subscales after controlling for BMI Eating Concerns
Shape Concerns
Weight Concerns
RI PT
Restraint SE B
β
B
SE B
Β
B
SE B
Β
B
SE B
β
EE-D
.02
.02
.09
.07
.02
.42***
.05
.02
.31**
.05
.02
.31**
EE-A
.02
.02
.10
.05
.02
.23**
.03
.02
.12
.02
.02
.10
EE-B
-.01
.02
-.01
.02
.02
.10
.04
.02
.18*
.03
.02
.17
EE-P
-.22
.10
-.17
.08
.08
.06
.11
.09
-.08
-.05
.08
-.04
M AN U
SC
Variable B
AC C
EP
TE D
Note. EDE-Q = Eating Disorders Examination Questionnaire; EE-D = depression subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-A = anxiety/anger subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-B = boredom subscale of the Emotional Eating Scale; EE-P = positive emotions subscale of the Emotional Appetite Questionnaire. *p < .05, **p < .01, **p <.001.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
34