Accepted Manuscript Enhancing performance expectancies through positive comparative feedback facilitates the learning of basketball free throw in children Gisele Severo Gonçalves, Priscila Lopes Cardozo, Nadia Cristina Valentini, Suzete Chiviacowsky PII:
S1469-0292(17)30718-5
DOI:
10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.03.001
Reference:
PSYSPO 1337
To appear in:
Psychology of Sport & Exercise
Received Date: 30 October 2017 Revised Date:
17 February 2018
Accepted Date: 4 March 2018
Please cite this article as: Gonçalves, G.S., Cardozo, P.L., Valentini, N.C., Chiviacowsky, S., Enhancing performance expectancies through positive comparative feedback facilitates the learning of basketball free throw in children, Psychology of Sport & Exercise (2018), doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.03.001. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Running head: Enhanced expectancies and children’s motor learning
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 2 3
Enhancing performance expectancies through positive comparative feedback facilitates the
4
learning of basketball free throw in children
RI PT
5 6
Gisele Severo Gonçalves and Priscila Lopes Cardozo
8
Federal University of Pelotas, Pelotas, Brazil
9
Nadia Cristina Valentini
10
Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
11
Suzete Chiviacowsky
12
Federal University of Pelotas, Pelotas, Brazil
M AN U
SC
7
13
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to:
TE D
14 15
Suzete Chiviacowsky, Ph.D.
17
Escola Superior de Educação Física
18
Universidade Federal de Pelotas
19
Rua Luís de Camões, 625 - CEP 96055-630
20
Pelotas - RS - BRAZIL
21
e-mail:
[email protected]
AC C
EP
16
22 23 24
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Enhancing performance expectancies through positive comparative feedback facilitates the
2
learning of basketball free throw in children
3 4 5
Running title: Enhanced expectancies and children’s motor learning
RI PT
1
6 7
SC
8 9
M AN U
10 11 12
AC C
EP
TE D
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2 14
Abstract The present study examined the influence of enhancing performance expectancies through
16
comparative feedback on the learning of a sport motor skill, the basketball free throw, in
17
children. Two groups of participants, a positive comparative feedback group (PF) and a
18
control group, practiced 40 basketball free throws. All children received feedback regarding
19
their punctuation scores after each block of practice. Participants in the PF group also
20
received positive social-comparative feedback suggesting that their own punctuation score
21
was better than that of a peer group’s on the block. Learning effects were observed through
22
a transfer test performed one day after practice. Participants in the PF group demonstrated
23
higher learning of the task, showing greater punctuation scores on the transfer test than
24
participants in the control group. Questionnaire results also showed higher levels of
25
perceived competence, importance of doing well, and persistence related to the task among
26
the PF group relative to the control participants. These findings provide the first evidence
27
that enhancing performance expectancies through positive comparative feedback enhances
28
the learning of sport motor skills in children. They also demonstrate the important
29
motivational role of feedback on children’s learning of motor skills.
32 33
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
31
Key words: Self-evaluation, motivation, competence, expectancy, sport.
AC C
30
RI PT
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 3 34
In recent years, a growing number of studies have been conducted to verify the effects of motivational factors on the learning of motor skills. For instance, studies have
36
provided evidence that the provision of autonomy (Carter, Carlsen, & Ste-Marie, 2014;
37
Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2002; Fairbrother, Laughlin, & Nguyen, 2012; Lewthwaite,
38
Chiviacowsky, Drews, & Wulf, 2015; Wu & Magill, 2011; Wulf & Toole, 1999),
39
relatedness (Gonzalez & Chiviacowsky, 2016), and competence (Chiviacowsky & Wulf,
40
2007; Clark & Ste-Marie, 2007; Trempe, Sabourin, & Proteau, 2012; Lewthwaite & Wulf,
41
2010) support for learners during practice has a direct and positive impact on motor
42
learning. Autonomy, relatedness, and competence are considered basic psychological needs
43
and important sources of human motivation (Deci & Ryan, 2000, 2008). In the present
44
study, we examined whether enhancing children’s perceived competence or expectancies
45
for successful performance, through positive comparative feedback, would facilitate the
46
learning of a sport motor skill.
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
35
Competence implies the need to feel oneself as confident, rather than feeling
48
incapable, of skillfully mastering challenges in one’s environment (Ryan, 1995). It has long
49
been argued that individual’s performance, choice, effort, and persistence can be explained
50
by their efficacy expectancies or beliefs about how well they will do on a determined task
51
or activity (Bandura, 1982; Deci & Ryan, 2000, 2008; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992). In the
52
OPTIMAL theory of motor learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016), enhancing learners’
53
expectancies for future performance is also considered an important motivational factor.
AC C
54
EP
47
Augmented feedback is considered as information provided by an agent (e.g.,
55
teacher, coach) related to aspects of one’s understanding or performance (Hattie &
56
Timperley, 2007) and one of the most powerful factors affecting motor learning (Schmidt,
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4 & Lee, 2011). Several studies have demonstrated a strong relationship between augmented
58
feedback, competence, and motor learning. For example, learners usually prefer to receive
59
feedback after good rather than poor trials (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2002, 2005; Fairbrother
60
et al., 2012; Grand et al., 2015; Patterson & Carter, 2010; Patterson, Carter & Sanli, 2011).
61
Feedback deliberately provided after good trials (Badami, Vaezmousavi, Wulf, &
62
Namazizadeh, 2012; Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007; Clark & Ste-Marie, 2007; Ste-Marie,
63
Vertes, Rymal, & Martini, 2011) or indicating successful performance through the
64
establishment of relatively easy criteria of good performance (Chiviacowsky & Harter,
65
2015; Chiviacowsky, Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2012; Palmer, Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2016;
66
Trempe, Sabourin, & Proteau, 2012) has been demonstrated to enhance motor learning.
67
Likewise, feedback statements designating performance a result of malleable rather than
68
fixed capacities positively affects performance and learning of motor skills (Chiviacowsky
69
& Drews, 2014).
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
57
A related line of research supporting this “feedback/competence/learning”
71
relationship involves individuals’ competence evaluation amid social or temporal
72
comparison. For instance, participants who received general feedback informing them that
73
their performance had gradually improved across blocks of practice showed higher learning
74
and motivation relative to participants who were informed that their performance had
75
slightly degraded over time (Chiviacowsky & Drews, 2016). Similar effects on motor
76
learning were found in individuals receiving social-comparative feedback during practice,
77
where their outcomes were compared with those of others (Lewthwaite & Wulf, 2010;
78
Navaee et al., 2016; Pascua, Wulf, & Lewthwaite, 2015; Wulf, Chiviacowsky, & Cardozo,
79
2014; Wulf, Chiviacowsky, & Lewthwaite, 2010, 2012).
AC C
EP
70
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 5 80
To date, however, only one experiment has observed the effects of enhancing learners’ expectancies through positive comparative feedback on motor learning in typical
82
children (Ávila, Chiviacowsky, Wulf, & Lewthwaite, 2012). Motivation after practice was
83
higher and the retention of a simple beanbag-throwing task was facilitated in 10-year-old
84
participants receiving positive social-comparative feedback relative to a control group.
85
Nevertheless, it is still largely unknown if this factor would facilitate the learning of more
86
complex motor skills, such as most of the sport skills. The objective of the present
87
experiment was, therefore, to observe the influence of positive social-comparative feedback
88
on the learning of a sport motor skill. Considering that there is a dearth of research on sport
89
skills learning in children, and the acquisition of new skills is considered to be significantly
90
more effective up until early adolescence than later in life (Janacsek, Fiser, & Nemeth,
91
2012), the potential benefits of positive comparative feedback on the acquisition of sport
92
skills in children were therefore deemed an important driver of this research.
SC
M AN U
TE D
93
RI PT
81
Two groups of 10-year-old children were asked to practice the free-throw basketball task. Similar to the study of Ávila et al. (2012), the positive feedback (PF) group received
95
socio-comparative information after each given block of trials in addition to veridical
96
feedback suggesting that their average performance was better than that of a peer group’s
97
on that block. Participants in the control group received veridical feedback but were not
98
provided with social-comparative feedback. The learning of sport skills usually involves
99
task transfer (Schmidt & Lee, 2011) from simple or easy conditions of practice to more
AC C
EP
94
100
difficult or complex environments, normally involving well-established rules of
101
competition. Thus, in order to examine skill-learning effects as a function of positive
102
feedback, a transfer test was performed one day later. A customized questionnaire to assess
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 6 the potential influence on participants’ level of perceived competence, enjoyment,
104
importance of doing well, and persistence related to the task as a function of practice
105
condition was applied at the end of practice. We hypothesized that the positive comparative
106
feedback group would demonstrate higher learning of the task than the control group. We
107
also expected that these participants would report higher levels of enjoyment, satisfaction
108
with performance, importance of doing well, and perhaps greater persistence related to the
109
task after practice, relative to participants in the control group.
112
Participants
M AN U
111
Methods
SC
110
RI PT
103
Twenty-six children (PF group, M = 9.65; SD = .91; control group, M = 9.60, SD = .92; range age 9 – 12 years-old) participated in the experiment. Participants provided their
114
assent, and informed consent was obtained from the school and the parents/guardians. The
115
university’s institutional review board approved the study. Calculation of the sample size
116
was carried out using G*Power 3.1, with an α level of 5%, effect size (f) of .58, and a
117
power of 80% for the two groups, based on effect sizes previously reported using similar
118
study designs (e.g., ηp² = .78 in Chiviacowsky, 2014; ηp² = .16 in Ávila et al., 2012).
119
Participants were not aware of the specific purpose of the study and had no prior experience
120
with the experimental task.
121
Apparatus and task
EP
AC C
122
TE D
113
The task involved the basketball free throw. A private gymnasium with a basketball
123
court was used for data collection. The free throw line for practice was located at a distance
124
of 3 m from the backboard, which was at a height of 2.60 m from the floor, and an official
125
youth ball (number 5) was used. Scores were established in relation to shot accuracy:
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 7 specifically, 4 points for a converted ball, 3 points for balls that touched both the backboard
127
and the hoop; 2 points for balls that touched only the hoop; 1 point for balls that touched
128
only the backboard; and 0 points for missed shots.
129
Procedure
130
RI PT
126
After completing the consent form, all participants were randomly assigned to the PF group and control group, with an equal number of males and females (9 boys and 4
132
girls) in each group, and introduced to the task. Data were collected individually and one of
133
the researchers for the present study was responsible for instructing all children.
134
Participants were informed about the goal of the task and observed two demonstrations of
135
how to perform it. They were instructed to throw the basketball overhand with both hands,
136
keeping their feet behind a line on the floor. The participants were also informed about the
137
punctuation score system and that they would receive feedback after each block of 10 trials
138
in relation to the sum of points in the specific block. Participants in the PF group were
139
informed that they would also receive comparative feedback on their performance in
140
relation to the performance of another group of children of the same age. More specifically,
141
these participants were informed about (false) peer’s average scores in the block, which
142
were always calculated as 20% less than the participants’ own scores. Thus, participants in
143
the PF group received veridical feedback and (false) positive social-comparative feedback
144
after each block of practice, while the control group received only veridical feedback after
145
each block.
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
146
SC
131
The practice phase consisted of 40 trials, and one day later a transfer test was
147
performed, consisting of 10 trials without any augmented feedback. In this transfer test, the
148
free throw line was located at a distance of 4 m from the backboard, according to Mini
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 8 Basketball official rules. In order to assess the subjective experience related to the task,
150
after the practice phase, all children filled out a customized questionnaire. For each of the
151
four statements (enjoyment, competence, importance and persistence-related), there were
152
four possible responses, ranging for example from “not competent/not important” to “very
153
competent/very important”, and with appropriate “smiley” or “frowny” faces
154
accompanying each response (adapted from Ávila et al., 2012). The responses to each
155
statement were assigned 1, 2, 3, or 4 points, respectively, for analysis purposes.
156
Data analysis
SC
The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to confirm the normality of the data. Punctuation
M AN U
157
RI PT
149
scores were analyzed in 2 (group: PF versus control) × 4 (blocks of 10 trials) analysis of
159
variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures of the last factor for the practice phase, while
160
separated one-way ANOVAs were used for the transfer test and questionnaire responses.
161
Bonferroni test was used for follow-up analysis. Partial eta-squared values were used to
162
indicate effect sizes (ηp²), considering 0.01, 0.06, and 0.14 for a small, moderate, or large
163
effect, respectively (Larson-Hall, 2009), and for all analysis the alpha was set at 0.05.
TE D
158
166 167
Throwing accuracy Practice
AC C
165
Results
EP
164
During the practice phase, participants in both groups improved their punctuation
168
scores across blocks (Figure 1). While the main effect of group, F (1, 24) = .72, p = .401,
169
ηp² = .029; and block, F (3, 72) = 1.72, p = .169, ηp² = .067 were not significant, the group
170
× block interaction, F (3, 72) = 4.06, p = .010, ηp² = .145 was significant. Follow-up
171
analysis showed that participants in the PF group significantly improved across blocks, F
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 9 172
(3, 36) = 3.34, p = .030, ηp² = .281, while improvement in control group participants failed
173
to reach significance, F (3, 36) = 2.31, p = .092, ηp² = .162. Transfer
175
Punctuation scores were significantly higher for the PF group compared with the
176
control group on the transfer test, F (1, 24) = 4.86, p = .037, ηp² = .168 (Figure 1).
177
179
[Insert Figure 1 about here] Questionnaire results
SC
178
RI PT
174
The groups did not differ in terms of enjoyment of performing the free throws following the practice phase, F (1, 24) = .353, p = .558, ηp² = .014 (Figure 2). However,
181
they differed regarding how satisfied they were with their performance, with participants in
182
the PF group rating their perceived competence F (1, 24) = 6.25, p = .020, ηp² = .207, and
183
importance of doing well in the task, F (1, 24) = 5.34, p = .030, ηp² = .182, significantly
184
higher than control participants. Differences were also found regarding persistence in
185
practicing the task, F (1, 24) = 7.00, p = .014, ηp² = .226, with participants of the PF group
186
reporting higher willingness to perform more free throws compared with control
187
participants.
190
TE D
EP
189
[Insert Figure 2 about here]
AC C
188
M AN U
180
Discussion
The present experiment was designed to examine whether enhancing performance
191
expectancies through positive social-comparative feedback, suggesting that participants’
192
performance was generally higher than the performance of peers, would enhance the
193
learning of the basketball free throw in children. The findings confirm our hypothesis,
194
showing that the group receiving positive comparative feedback demonstrated higher
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 10 learning of the basketball free throw, measured in the transfer test, than the control
196
participants. Thus, in line with previous studies that manipulated participants’ competence
197
evaluation through social (e.g., Ávila et al., 2012; Lewthwaite & Wulf, 2010; Navaee et al.,
198
2016; Wulf et al., 2010, 2012;) or temporal-comparative feedback (Chiviacowsky &
199
Drews, 2016), providing children with general positive feedback, implying that their
200
performance was slightly higher than the performance of peers, led to more effective
201
learning of a sport skill than not giving them such information. The PF group also reported
202
higher perceived competence, importance of doing well, and persistence in practicing the
203
task at the end of practice compared with participants in the control group.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
195
How can the observed effects be explained? The feeling of improving and
205
demonstrating one’s abilities or competence is considered fundamentally satisfying and
206
motivating (Deci & Ryan, 2000; White, 1959). Confidence can affect performance
207
expectancies, preparing individuals for further positive experiences, and impacting
208
cognitive, emotional and motor preparatory activity (Schmidt, Braun, Wager, & Shohamy,
209
2014; Schunk, 1991; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992; Wulf et al., 2012). According to the
210
OPTIMAL theory of motor learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016), enhanced expectancies
211
for performance are considered to strengthen the coupling of goals to actions, readying the
212
motor system for task execution and helping to consolidate memories. Performance
213
expectancies can also influence effort tolerance (Hutchinson, Sherman, Martinovic, &
214
Tenenbaum, 2008), positive affect (Stoate, Wulf, & Lewthwaite, 2012), importance of
215
doing well, and persistence in practicing the task (present study).
216 217
AC C
EP
TE D
204
Furthermore, enhanced expectancies may help learners to set higher performance goals (Bandura, 1997; Bandura & Locke, 2003; Locke & Latham, 2006), potentially
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 11 affecting effort and attention paid during performance and resulting in performance gains
219
(Bandura & Jourden, 1991; West, Dark-Freudeman, & Bagwell, 2009; West & Thorn,
220
2001; West, Welch, & Thorn, 2001). In fact, greater self-efficacy has indeed been directly
221
associated with increased task-relevant attentional control during practice (Themanson &
222
Rosen, 2015), and observed to be a predictor of motor performance (for a review, see
223
Moritz, Feltz, Fahrbach, & Mack, 2000) as well as learning (Chiviacowsky, 2014;
224
Chiviacowsky et al., 2012; Stevens et al., 2012; Wulf et al., 2014). Less confident learners,
225
however, tend to occupy themselves with counterproductive self-evaluative concerns,
226
decreasing effort and attention or explicit monitoring activity to important aspects of the
227
task, whereby degrading performance (Bandura, 1982; Bandura & Wood, 1989; Sarason,
228
1984; Wine, 1971) and motor learning (Cardozo & Chiviacowsky, 2015; Heidrich &
229
Chiviacowsky, 2015; McKay, Wulf, Lewthwaite, & Nordin, 2015).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
218
In conclusion, the findings provide the first evidence that enhancing learners’
231
performance expectancies through positive comparative feedback facilitates the learning of
232
sport motor skills in children. They also emphasize the important motivational role of
233
augmented feedback in motor learning. Future studies may reveal further comparative
234
feedback effects on the learning of different kinds of sport skills in various populations.
235
Considering practical applications, it is suggested that professionals involved with
236
teaching-learning processes take advantage of enhancing expectancies strategies, for
237
example highlighting positive aspects of performance, or facilitating success experience, in
238
order to benefit sport skills learning.
239
AC C
EP
TE D
230
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 12 240 241
References Ávila, L. T. G., Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Positive socialcomparative feedback enhances motor learning in children. Psychology of Sport and
243
Exercise, 13, 849–853.
244
RI PT
242
Badami, R., VaezMousavi, M., Wulf, G., & Namazizadeh, M. (2012). Feedback About
More Accurate Versus Less Accurate Trials: Differential effects on self-confidence
246
and activation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83, 196–203.
248
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122–147.
M AN U
247
SC
245
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
250
Bandura, A., & Jourden, F. J. (1991). Self-regulatory mechanisms governing the impact of
251
social comparison on complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social
252
Psychology, 60, 941-951.
254 255
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of applied psychology, 88(1), 87. Bandura, A., & Wood, R. (1989). Effect of perceived controllability and performance
EP
253
TE D
249
standards on self-regulation of complex decision making. Journal of Personality
257
and Social Psychology, 56, 805-814.
AC C
256
258
Cardozo, P. L., & Chiviacowsky, S. (2015). Overweight stereotype threat negatively
259
impacts the learning of a balance task. Journal of Motor Learning and
260
Development, 3, 2, 140-150.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 13 261
Carter, M. J., Carlsen, A. N., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2014). Self-controlled feedback is
262
effective if it is based on the learner’s performance: a replication and extension of
263
Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2005). Frontiers in Psychology, 5. Chiviacowsky, S. (2014). Self-controlled practice: Autonomy protects perceptions of
RI PT
264 265
competence and enhances motor learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15,
266
505–510.
269
SC
268
Chiviacowsky, S., & Drews, R. (2016). Temporal-comparative feedback affects motor learning. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 4, 208-218.
Chiviacowsky, S., Harter, N. M. (2015). Perceptions of competence and motor learning:
M AN U
267
270
performance criterion resulting in low success experience degrades learning. Brazilian
271
Journal of Motor Behavior, 9, 1, 1-10.
272
Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2002). Self-Controlled Feedback: Does it Enhance Learning Because Performers Get Feedback When They Need It? Research Quarterly for
274
Exercise and Sport, 73, 408–415.
277 278 279 280 281
the learner’s performance. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76, 42–48.
EP
276
Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2005). Self-controlled feedback is effective if it is based on
Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2007). Feedback After Good Trials Enhances Learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 40–47.
AC C
275
TE D
273
Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Self-controlled learning: the importance of protecting perceptions of competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3.
http://doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00458
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 14 282
Clark, S. E., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2007). The impact of self-as-a-model interventions on
283
children's self-regulation of learning and swimming performance. Journal of Sports
284
Sciences, 25(5), 577-586. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘‘what’’ and ‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits: Human needs
286
and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
RI PT
285
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-determination theory: A macrotheory of human
288
motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychology, 49, 182–185.
289
SC
287
Fairbrother, J. T., Laughlin, D. D., & Nguyen, T. V. (2012). Self-controlled feedback facilitates motor learning in both high and low activity individuals. Frontiers in
291
Psychology, 3, 1–8 [Article 383].
292
M AN U
290
Grand, K. F., Bruzi, A. T., Dyke, F. B., Godwin, M. M., Leiker, A. M., Thompson, A. G., ... & Miller, M. W. (2015). Why self-controlled feedback enhances motor learning:
294
Answers from electroencephalography and indices of motivation. Human Movement
295
Science, 43, 23-32.
298 299 300 301 302
Psychological Research, 1-9.
EP
297
Gonzalez, D. H., & Chiviacowsky, S. (2016). Relatedness support enhances motor learning.
Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The Power of Feedback. Review of Educational Research, 77, 81–112.
AC C
296
TE D
293
Heidrich, C., & Chiviacowsky, S. (2015). Stereotype threat affects the learning of sport motor skills. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 18, 42-46.
Hutchinson, J. C., Sherman, T., Martinovic, N., & Tenenbaum, G. (2008). The effect of
303
manipulated self-efficacy on perceived and sustained effort. Journal of Applied
304
Sport Psychology, 20, 4, 457-472.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 15 305
Janacsek, K., Fiser, J., & Nemeth, D. (2012). The best time to acquire new skills: age
306
related differences in implicit sequence learning across the human lifespan.
307
Developmental Science, 15, 496-505.
309 310
Larson-Hall J. (2009). A guide to doing statistics in second language research using SPSS.
RI PT
308
London: Routledge.
Lewthwaite, R., Chiviacowsky, S., Drews, R., & Wulf, G. (2015). Choose to move: The
motivational impact of autonomy support on motor learning. Psychonomic Bulletin
312
& Review, 22, 1383–1388.
314 315
Lewthwaite, R., & Wulf, G. (2010). Social-comparative feedback affects motor skill
M AN U
313
SC
311
learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 738–749. Lewthwaite, R., & Wulf, G. (2012). Motor learning through a motivational lens. In N. J. Hodges & A. M. Williams (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory &
317
practice (2nd ed., pp. 173– 191). London: Routledge.
319 320 321
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2006). New directions in goal-setting theory. Current Directions In Psychological Science, 15, 265–268. Magill, R.A. (2011) Motor Learning and Control: Concepts and Applications. New York:
EP
318
TE D
316
McGraw-Hill.
McKay, B., Wulf, G., Lewthwaite, R., & Nordin, A. (2015). The self: Your own worst
323
enemy? A test of the self-invoking trigger hypothesis. The Quarterly Journal of
324 325
AC C
322
Experimental Psychology, 68, 1910-1919.
Moritz, S. E., Feltz, D. L., Fahrbach, K. R., & Mack, D. E. (2000). The relation of
326
selfefficacy measures to sport performance: a meta-analytic review. Research
327
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 280e294.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 16 328
Navaee, S. A., Farsi, A., & Abdoli, B. (2016). The effect of normative feedback on stability
329
and efficacy of some selected muscles in a balancing task. International Journal of
330
Applied Exercise Physiology, 5(1), 43-52.
332 333
Palmer, K., Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2016). Enhanced expectancies facilitate golf putting. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 22, 229-232.
RI PT
331
Pascua, L. A. M., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2015). Additive benefits of external focus and enhanced performance expectancy for motor learning. Journal of Sports
335
Sciences, 33, 58–66.
337 338
Patterson, J. T., & Carter, M. (2010). Learner regulated knowledge of results during the
M AN U
336
SC
334
acquisition of multiple timing goals. Human Movement Science, 29, 214–227. Patterson, J. T., Carter, M., & Sanli, E. (2011). Decreasing the proportion of self-control trials during the acquisition period does not compromise the learning advantages in
340
a self-controlled context. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, 624–633.
342 343
Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal of Personality, 63, 397–427.
Saemi, E., Porter, J. M., Ghotbi-Varzaneh, A., Zarghami, M., & Maleki, F. (2012).
EP
341
TE D
339
Knowledge of results after relatively good trials enhances self-efficacy and motor
345
learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 378-382.
346 347 348 349 350
AC C
344
Saemi, E., Wulf, G., Varzaneh, A. G., & Zarghami, M. (2011). Feedback after good versus poor trials enhances motor learning in children. Brazilian Journal of Physical Education and Sport, 25, 671-679.
Sarason, I. G. (1984). Stress, anxiety, and cognitive interference: reactions to tests. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 929.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 17 351
Schmidt, L., Braun, E. K., Wager, T. D., & Shohamy, D. (2014). Mind matters: Placebo
352
enhances reward learning in Parkinson’s disease. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 1793-
353
1797. Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Frequent augmented feedback can degrade learning: Evidence and
RI PT
354 355
interpretations. In J. Requin & G. E. Stelmach (Eds.), Tutorials in motor neuroscience
356
(pp. 59-75). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
359 360 361
ed.). Champaign: Human kinetics.
SC
358
Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. (2011). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis (5th
Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26, 207-231
M AN U
357
Ste-Marie, D. M., Vertes, K., Rymal, A. M., & Martini, R. (2011). Feedforward selfmodeling enhances skill acquisition in children learning trampoline skills. Frontiers
363
in Psychology, 2, 155.
365 366
Stoate, I., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Enhanced expectancies improve movement efficiency in runners. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30, 8, 815-823. Swinnen, S. P. (1996). Information feedback for motor skill learning: A review. In H. N.
EP
364
TE D
362
Zelaznik (Ed.), Advances in Motor learning and control (p. 37–66). Champaign, IL:
368
Human Kinetics.
369
AC C
367
Themanson, J. R., & Rosen, P. J. (2015). Examining the relationships between self
370
efficacy, task relevant attentional control, and task performance: Evidence from
371
event related brain potentials. British Journal of Psychology, 106, 2, 253-271.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 18 372
Trempe M., Sabourin, M., & Proteau, L. (2012). Success modulates consolidation of a
373
visuomotor adaptation task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
374
Memory, and Cognition, 38, 52–60. West, R. L., Dark-Freudeman, A., & Bagwell, D. K. (2009). Goals-feedback conditions and
376
episodic memory: Mechanisms for memory gains in older and younger adults.
377
Memory, 17(2), 233-244.
RI PT
375
West, R. L., Welch, D. C., & Thorn, R. M. (2001). Effects of goal-setting and feedback on
379
memory performance and beliefs among older and younger adults. Psychology and
380
Aging, 16(2), 240.
383 384 385 386
M AN U
382
West, R. L. Thorn, R. M. (2001). Goal-setting, self-efficacy, and memory performance in older and younger adults. Experimental Aging Research, 27, 41-65. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.
TE D
381
SC
378
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 1, 68-81. Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92-104.
388
Wu, W. F., & Magill, R. A. (2011). Allowing learners to choose: self-controlled practice
390 391 392 393
schedules for learning multiple movement patterns. Research Quarterly for Exercise
AC C
389
EP
387
and Sport, 82, 449-457.
Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Cardozo, P. L. (2014). Additive benefits of autonomy support and enhanced expectancies for motor learning. Human Movement Science, 37, 12–20.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 19
395 396 397
Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Lewthwaite, R. (2010). Normative Feedback Effects on Learning a Timing Task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81, 425–431. Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Altering mindset can enhance motor learning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 27, 14–21.
RI PT
394
Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2016). Optimizing Performance through Intrinsic Motivation
399
and Attention for Learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychonomic
400
Bulletin & Review, 23, 1382-1414.
401
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
402
SC
398
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 20 403
M AN U
SC
RI PT
404
405 406
Figure 1. Punctuation scores of the PF group and control group during practice and transfer.
408
Error bars indicate standard errors. Asterisk denotes significant difference
409
between PF versus Control group.
EP AC C
410 411
TE D
407
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 21
M AN U
SC
RI PT
412
413 414
Figure 2. Questionnaire scores, after practice, of the PF and control groups. Error bars
TE D
415
indicate standard errors. Asterisks denote significant differences between PF
417
versus Control group.
419 420
AC C
418
EP
416
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Highlights
We investigate the effects of enhancing performance expectancies in children. Participants practice in two conditions: positive comparative feedback (PCF) and
RI PT
control. Learning of the basketball free throw was facilitated in the PCF condition.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
Enhanced expectancies through PCF enhance sport learning in children.