COPHAR-1630; NO. OF PAGES 6
Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
ScienceDirect Epileptogenesis meets Occam’s Razor Robert S Sloviter Pharmacological treatment to prevent brain injury-induced temporal lobe epileptogenesis has been generally unsuccessful, raising the issues of exactly when the conversion process to an epileptic brain state occurs and reaches completion, and which cellular or network processes might be the most promising therapeutic targets. The time course of epileptogenesis is a central issue, with recent results suggesting that injury-induced epileptogenesis can be a much more rapid process than previously thought, and may be inconsistent with a delayed epileptogenic mechanism. Simplification of the seemingly complex issues involved in the use of epilepsy animal models might lead to a better understanding of the nature of injury-induced epileptogenesis, the significance of the ‘latent’ period, and whether current strategies should focus on preventing or modifying epilepsy. Address Departments of Neurobiology and Pharmacology/Toxicology, Morehouse School of Medicine, 720 Westview Drive SW, Atlanta, GA 30310, USA Corresponding author: Sloviter, Robert S (
[email protected])
Current Opinion in Pharmacology 2017, 35:xx–yy This review comes from a themed issue on Tribute to Norman Bowery Edited by David G Trist and Tom Blackburn
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2017.07.012 1471-4892/ã 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Introduction Seizures occur when populations of neurons generate abnormally prolonged and synchronous discharges as a consequence of genetic influences, developmental anomalies, tumors, infections, toxins, sleep deprivation, hypoxia, metabolic disturbances, or brain injuries. Depending on the brain region involved, epileptiform discharges can produce sensory and motor manifestations, as well as changes in awareness, cognition, and other behaviors. Clinically subtle focal seizures occur when epileptiform discharges are spatially constrained, and more clinically obvious seizures occur if the discharges originate in or spread to motor pathways, or involve the neocortex bilaterally. If spontaneous seizures occur repeatedly, and are not associated with fever, sleep deprivation, hypoxia, or www.sciencedirect.com
any reversible pathophysiological or metabolic disturbance, the condition is ‘epilepsy’ [1,2], and the process that causes epilepsy to develop is called ‘epileptogenesis.’
Defining epileptogenesis as a prelude to understanding it Epileptogenesis is alternately defined to be either the initial and finite process that causes the brain to become ‘epileptic’ in the first place [3], or the sum of all processes that both initiate the epileptic state, and then influence its continuing evolution and progression over time [4–6]. Although much about epilepsy is debatable, it should be possible for all to agree that the word ‘genesis’ unambiguously describes the ‘beginning’ or ‘birth’ of something, and not an entity’s entire lifecycle or duration. The Book of Genesis is a story of the beginning; it is not the entire story. Thus, we define epileptogenesis, as discussed here, to be the initial process that causes epilepsy to develop, with any subsequent influences being different entities in need of a different name. And although the term ‘epileptogenesis’ is often discussed as though it is a specific unidentified mechanism, different epileptic states almost certainly involve different epileptogenic mechanisms. Thus, the term epileptogenesis is simply a general term for any process that causes any epileptic state to develop. If epileptogenesis describes the creation of a brain state with a propensity for generating seizures [2], it presumably does not require a first spontaneous clinical seizure, although clinical diagnosis of epilepsy presumably would. A patient with an unrecognized propensity for photosensitive seizures who has not yet encountered a strobe light of the right frequency, and therefore not yet had a first seizure, has nevertheless already undergone epileptogenesis, although for practical reasons it might be stipulated that epileptogenesis ends with the first seizure event that causes any abnormal behavior (i.e. perhaps an aura, at minimum). This is a complex semantic point because completion of epileptogenesis presumably creates an ‘epileptic network’ before a clinically detectable ‘epileptic state’ exists. Regardless, if epileptogenesis describes the process that causes an epileptic brain state to develop, we suggest the term ‘epileptometagenesis’ (‘meta’ meaning ‘what comes after’) to describe all processes that follow epileptogenesis, and influence the progression or modification of the already established epileptic state (Figure 1). ‘Epileptometagenesis’ replaces the previously suggested ‘epileptic maturation’ [3] because, unlike the growth of an organism, changes that occur in epilepsy are not necessarily a maturation process. ‘Metagenesis’ encompasses all that follows ‘genesis.’ Separate terms for the initial development of epilepsy and for all subsequent changes imply Current Opinion in Pharmacology 2017, 35:1–6
Please cite this article in press as: Sloviter RS: Epileptogenesis meets Occam’s Razor, Curr Opin Pharmacol (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2017.07.012
COPHAR-1630; NO. OF PAGES 6
2 Tribute to Norman Bowery
Figure 1
Epileptogenic brain injury (two alternate hypotheses)
“Epileptogenesis” triggered
“Immediate epileptogenesis”
The “latent” period is a silent, pre-epileptic, gestational state of “epileptogenesis” initiated by brain injury. No subclinical or clinical seizures occur.
“Epileptogenesis” (literally the “birth” of epilepsy) ends with the first spontaneous and unnoticed epileptiform discharge causing abnormal behavior (e.g. an aura).
Onset of epilepsy as “epileptogenesis” reaches a developmental stage that causes the first spontaneous seizure.
“Epileptometagenesis” causes initially subtle focal seizures to become clinically obvious (perhaps via a “kindling” process)
“Epileptogenesis” continues to influence the progression of the established epileptic state throughout life.
“Epileptometagenesis” continues to influence the progression of the established epileptic state throughout life. Current Opinion in Pharmacology
Alternate concepts of ‘delayed’ vs. ‘immediate’ epileptogenesis, and the nature of the ‘latent’ period. Is the ‘latent’ period, when one exists, a seizure-free, pre-epileptic, ‘gestational’ state of epileptogenesis (left), or an immediately-produced subclinical, and frequently unrecognized, focal epileptic state (right) that simply takes time, sometimes decades [15], to cause the first clinically obvious seizures?
that ‘antiepileptogenic’ strategies designed to prevent epilepsy are distinct from ‘disease modification’ strategies designed to modify an already established epileptic state [3,7]. If epilepsy cannot be prevented, it would be beneficial to impede the therapeutically ‘epileptometagenesis’ process by which an initially subtle focal epileptic state becomes increasingly disruptive to a patient’s self-image and their ability to work and function in society.
Genesis of the notion that epileptogenesis involves a time-consuming secondary mechanism Exactly how brain injuries produce epilepsy is still conjectural, and a discussion of possible mechanisms of epileptogenesis is beyond the scope of this review. Generally, however, epileptic network behavior could result from periodic disinhibition/hyperexcitability due to neuron loss or dysfunction [8–11], a change in network excitability caused by the maturation of a delayed secondary epileptogenic process triggered by brain injury [12,13], or the periodic release of unidentified substances that cause transient hyperexcitability without necessarily involving injury, neuron loss, or structural reorganization. Given the plethora of possibilities, why does the prevailing view of epileptogenesis so overwhelmingly favor
involvement of a delayed secondary mechanism that other possibilities are virtually excluded from consideration or discussion? Timing is crucial in this regard. If neuronal or glial cell death or dysfunction, or any other immediate event, were the direct cause of epilepsy, seizures would be expected to start soon after brain injury [14]. The widespread notion of epileptogenesis as a prolonged ‘gestational’ process for an unidentified epileptogenic mechanism is based almost entirely on the clinical assumption that a prolonged and silent ‘latent’ period reliably follows brain injury [15], and that its duration reflects the time needed for the unidentified epileptogenic mechanism to develop [14,16,17]. This is important from a translational perspective because it implies the existence of an extended ‘therapeutic window of opportunity’ when epilepsy might be prevented by a drug that targets this slowly developing epileptogenic mechanism [18]. The concept of the ‘silent’ latent period apparently derives from the observations of Penfield and colleagues, who noted that, “Habitual seizures rarely, if ever, begin at the time of brain injury. There is a ripening period of months or years that follows the initial insult” [19], and that, “insults may result in epilepsy after a silent period of strange ripening. That period lasts for months or years . . . ” [20].
Current Opinion in Pharmacology 2017, 35:1–6 Please cite this article in press as: Sloviter RS: Epileptogenesis meets Occam’s Razor, Curr Opin Pharmacol (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2017.07.012
www.sciencedirect.com
COPHAR-1630; NO. OF PAGES 6
Epileptogenesis after brain injury Sloviter 3
The notion that the latent period in patients is a silent gestational state of epileptogenesis, rather than a clinically subtle focal epileptic state, was reinforced by early anecdotal reports that spontaneous motor seizures were serendipitously observed months after kainate-induced status epilepticus (SE) in rats [21,22], and by studies using non-continuous monitoring methods that missed early seizures [23–28]. As a result, delayed processes such as mossy fiber sprouting [29] and neurogenesis [30,31] were suggested to be epileptogenic mechanisms, in part, because these processes were as delayed in animals as the onset of epilepsy was presumed to be [18]. Although the earliest animal studies cited above appeared to support the idea of a ‘silent’ latent period, more recent studies of rats monitored continuously after pilocarpineinduced SE have now shown that spontaneous seizures begin coincident with the initial injury [32,33,34], and are not delayed. Immediate epilepsy also follows electrical stimulation-induced convulsive SE [35], which eliminates the issue of residual chemoconvulsant as the cause of early seizures. Importantly, a study that involved continuous monitoring only on days 3 and 6 post-SE found that even after 1 h of convulsive SE, spontaneous clinical seizures began during the first days post-injury [36]. The issue of whether the earliest post-injury seizures are a manifestation of acute brain injury, rather than being the first spontaneous epileptic seizures, is an important one. It is unclear how to differentiate an early injury-associated seizure from the first ‘spontaneous’ epileptic seizure, and it is possible for early post-injury seizures to occur in isolation, and not be followed by spontaneous epileptic seizures [37]. We are specifically referring here to spontaneous seizures that begin and then continue to occur for as long as animals are monitored. Regardless, epilepsy develops coincident with the initial brain injury caused by convulsive SE in rats [35] and mice [38], as it can after prolonged convulsive SE in patients [39]. If prolonged convulsive SE involves no latency to clinical epilepsy [32,33,34,35,36–38,39,40,41], perhaps latent periods are caused by less severe brain injuries, such as non-convulsive SE [40]. Thus, the central question would seem to be: is the latent period, when one exists, a seizurefree, pre-epileptic ‘silent’ period, or a clinically subtle focal epileptic state? If the latent period is a clinically subtle focal epileptic state, support for the concepts of ‘delayed epileptogenesis’ and the anti-epileptogenic ‘therapeutic window’ would presumably collapse. Regardless, the many delayed processes that follow experimental brain injuries cannot be epileptogenic mechanisms in animals that develop clinical epilepsy before the secondary mechanisms have time to develop [41].
Applying Occam’s Razor to the concept of epileptogenesis Whether injury-induced temporal lobe epileptogenesis is a rapid or delayed process has important implications for www.sciencedirect.com
understanding the fundamental nature of this specific epileptogenic process, and for developing pharmacological strategies to prevent or modify it. Resolution of this issue requires an animal model with a latency to the first clinically obvious seizures if the ‘latent’ period is to be revealed as either a ‘silent’ seizure-free interval, or a prolonged interval of subclinical or subtle focal seizures that precedes the onset of clinically obvious seizures [40]. However, even if an extended latent period can be reliably produced in an animal model, and its nature revealed as either an ‘epileptic’ or ‘pre-epileptic’ state, we would still be faced with the challenge of determining which of the many changes caused by brain injury might mediate epileptogenesis, and whether any pharmacological intervention can prevent epilepsy after a potentially epileptogenic brain injury has been sustained. Prolonged convulsive SE and traumatic brain injuries cause excitotoxic and ischemic neuron loss, vascular and glial changes, increased neurogenesis, inflammation, blood/brain barrier breakdown, structural reorganization, epigenetic effects, altered receptor/channel expression, and a multitude of other molecular changes [12,13,42,43]. Which of the hundreds or thousands of changes that follow brain injury actually cause epilepsy? Is a unique combination of changes necessary for epilepsy to develop, or is a single epileptogenic mechanism initiated by a variety of brain injuries? If every newly identified effect of brain injury is going to be suggested to be the epileptogenic mechanism, and none of the many candidate mechanisms can be separated from the other parallel effects of brain injury, how can progress be made? When anything being studied has so many intertwined and inseparable facets, it is useful to consider using Occam’s Razor to cut through the complexity. We suggest that this approach might be effective in trying to understand epileptogenesis. In essence, William of Ockham’s perspective, called ‘nominalism’ (meaning ‘in name only’), addressed the 14th Century question of whether the ability to create names for ideas that exist within the mind (e.g. unicorns or ‘the future’) means that the entities named must have a real and independent existence outside the mind [44]. This became a pressing question because it had been asserted by influential authorities that the presence within the mind of the idea of God proved that God must exist. Ockham’s radical view that a name is just a name, and does not prove the existence of the entity that the name represents, was revolutionary and used to eliminate an accretion of imagined things. Ockham’s original thinking (denying the existence of non-existent things) caused friction with the Papal authorities. Although he was never officially condemned for heresy, Ockham was excommunicated in 1328 and fled Avignon for the safety of Bavaria, where he apparently died peacefully in April of 1347, six months before the arrival Current Opinion in Pharmacology 2017, 35:1–6
Please cite this article in press as: Sloviter RS: Epileptogenesis meets Occam’s Razor, Curr Opin Pharmacol (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2017.07.012
COPHAR-1630; NO. OF PAGES 6
4 Tribute to Norman Bowery
in Sicily of the plague-infected rats that brought the Black Death to Europe. Ockham was officially rehabilitated by Pope Innocent VI in 1359, which permitted his ideas to survive and influence scientific thought during the Renaissance. Ockham’s perspective, as applied to science, states that you should not postulate the existence of entities, mechanisms, or processes without justification, and should not suggest an unnecessarily complex explanation when a simple one suffices [44]. If what originally seemed to be a silent period after brain injury is, in fact, only clinical silence, and focal epilepsy starts soon after injury [35], then there is no need to hypothesize the involvement of a delayed secondary mechanism in the initial epileptogenic process. A multitude of processes triggered by brain injury may affect the progression of the epileptic state, but the initial transformation from a normal to an epileptic brain state could simply be related to initial neuron loss or dysfunction, or other immediate changes. Since spontaneous granule cell-onset seizures may be coincident with initial neuron loss after convulsive or non-convulsive SE [3,35,38,40], there may be little justification for hypothesizing that delayed secondary mechanisms are necessary or likely epileptogenic mechanisms, at least in the animal models studied. If delayed secondary processes are going to continue to be claimed to be epileptogenic mechanisms, experiments should first show that seizures never occur before the candidate mechanism develops. Had this been done prior to suggesting mossy fiber sprouting as the primary cause of epilepsy [29], for example, a great deal of time and resources might have been spared. In addition, any pharmacological treatment that decreases the frequency or severity of spontaneous seizures without stopping them entirely is modifying the disorder, not preventing it. Since correlations fail to establish a causal relationship between any variable studied and ‘epileptogenesis,’ the solution to the complexity of the many parallel effects that brain injuries cause may involve a simpler approach. If, for example, seizure-induced dentate gyrus neuron loss or dysfunction is directly epileptogenic, as we have suggested [45], then selective elimination or silencing of the right neurons should produce granule cell-onset epilepsy without involving brain injury, tissue trauma, inflammation, blood/brain barrier breakdown, synaptic reorganization, neurogenesis, or any other injury-associated phenomenon. This has not yet been demonstrated. Similarly, if inflammation [46] or blood/brain barrier breakdown [47], for example, are epileptogenic mechanisms, rather than tangential or modulating influences, then inflammation alone, or blood/brain barrier breakdown alone, should produce a spontaneous epileptic state in vivo without involving an acute brain injury. Thus, progress might result from experiments specifically designed to demonstrate causality, rather than
correlation, because adding more circumstantial evidence to an existing body of circumstantial evidence does not strengthen the case for causality.
Conclusion The perspective presented here has significant implications for strategies designed to prevent the development of epilepsy, or to modify the disorder once established. If the time course of epileptogenesis is unknown, how can the optimal time of therapeutic intervention be rationally determined [48]? If the injury itself is epileptogenic, whether mediated by neuron loss or neuronal dysfunction, or other immediate changes, then the ‘therapeutic window’ may only be open for a brief period, as in stroke therapy, and pharmacological treatment in the weeks or months after brain injury would be predicted to fail, as some studies have found [49,50]. Conversely, if animal studies poorly reflect the human epileptogenic process, the latent period in humans could, in fact, be a silent ‘gestational’ state, as conceived by Penfield [19,20], although then it would have to be explained why prolonged convulsive SE in humans can produce epilepsy with no apparent latency [38]. We suggest that more extensive injuries cause clinical epilepsy without delay [35,39], whereas less extensive injuries cause an initially subtle focal epileptic state (during the latent period) that can eventually become a clinically obvious epileptic state [10,40]. If there are two distinct processes operating in both animals and humans, one a rapid process that causes the initial epileptic transformation (‘epileptogenesis’), and a second process or series of processes that influence the clinical features and refractoriness of the epileptic state over time (‘epileptometagenesis’), then preventing epileptogenesis may be more challenging than currently assumed, and strategies might more fruitfully focus on keeping focal seizures focal by interrupting or retarding epileptometagenesis, that is, the progressive worsening of the epileptic state when that occurs. It is now important to determine which of the many effects of brain injury mediate the initial process (epileptogenesis), which processes underlie subsequent modifications of the epileptic brain state (epileptometagenesis), and which effects of brain injury are epiphenomena mechanistically related to neither. Finally, it should be emphasized, in a discussion of complexity and simplicity as it applies to epileptogenesis, that although simplicity should be a default setting, it is also possible that epileptogenesis may involve significant complexity, with multiple cascades of mechanisms that can be targeted to prevent or reduce the severity of clinical epilepsy [12,13,51]. Determining exactly when epileptogenesis occurs in any given animal model is a crucial consideration for discriminating between possible epileptogenic and epileptometagenic mechanisms, and
Current Opinion in Pharmacology 2017, 35:1–6 Please cite this article in press as: Sloviter RS: Epileptogenesis meets Occam’s Razor, Curr Opin Pharmacol (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2017.07.012
www.sciencedirect.com
COPHAR-1630; NO. OF PAGES 6
Epileptogenesis after brain injury Sloviter 5
determining which of the many effects of brain injury may not play significant roles in either process.
15. French JA, Williamson PD, Thadani VM, Darcey TM, Mattson RH, Spencer SS, Spencer DD: Characteristics of medial temporal lobe epilepsy: I. Results of history and physical examination. Ann Neurol 1993, 34:774-780.
Conflict of interest statement
16. Bragin A, Wilson CL, Engel J Jr: Chronic epileptogenesis requires development of a network of pathologically interconnected neuron clusters: a hypothesis. Epilepsia 2000, 41(Suppl. 6):S144-S152.
Nothing declared.
Acknowledgements I thank Drs. Argyle Bumanglag, Marc Dichter, David Henshall, Wolfgang Lo¨scher, and Robert Schwarcz for their insightful comments and constructive criticisms of earlier versions of the manuscript. Special thanks to Dr. Daniel Lowenstein for useful discussions, constructive criticism, and repeated editing of this work. Supported by NIH grant NS083932.
References and recommended reading Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as: of special interest of outstanding interest 1.
Engel J Jr: Introduction to temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 1996, 26:141-150.
2.
Fisher RS, Acevedo C, Arzimanoglou A, Bogacz A, Cross JH, Elger CE, Engel J Jr, Forsgren L, French JA, Glynn M et al.: ILAE official report: a practical clinical definition of epilepsy. Epilepsia 2014, 55:475-482.
3.
Sloviter RS, Bumanglag AV: Defining “epileptogenesis” and identifying “antiepileptogenic targets” in animal models of acquired temporal lobe epilepsy is not as simple as it might seem. Neuropharmacology 2013, 69:3-15.
4.
Pitka¨nen A: Therapeutic approaches to epileptogenesis — hope on the horizon. Epilepsia 2010, 51(Suppl 3):2-17.
5.
Dudek FE, Staley KJ: The time course of acquired epilepsy: implications for therapeutic intervention to suppress epileptogenesis. Neurosci Lett 2011, 497:240-246.
6.
Pitka¨nen A, Engel J Jr: Past and present definitions of epileptogenesis and its biomarkers. Neurotherapeutics 2014, 11:231-241.
Lo¨scher W, Hirsch LJ, Schmidt D: The enigma of the latent period in the development of symptomatic acquired epilepsy — traditional view versus new concepts. Epilepsy Behav 2015, 52:78-92. This paper discusses the importance of the ‘latent’ period and its implications regarding antiepileptogenic therapy.
7.
8.
9.
Sloviter RS: Decreased hippocampal inhibition and a selective loss of interneurons in experimental epilepsy. Science 1987, 235:73-76. Sloviter RS: Permanently altered hippocampal structure, excitability, and inhibition after experimental status epilepticus in the rat: the dormant basket cell hypothesis and its possible relevance to temporal lobe epilepsy. Hippocampus 1991, 1:41-66.
17. Leite JP, Garcia-Cairasco N, Cavalheiro EA: New insights from the use of pilocarpine and kainate models. Epilepsy Res 2002, 50:93-103. 18. Stables JP, Bertram E, Dudek FE, Holmes G, Mathern G, Pitka¨nen A, White HS: Therapy discovery for pharmacoresistant epilepsy and for disease-modifying therapeutics: summary of the NIH/NINDS/AES models II workshop. Epilepsia 2003, 44:1472-1478. This consensus paper declares that a seizure-free ‘latent’ period lasting several weeks reliably follows convulsive status epilepticus in animals. 19. Penfield W, Paine K: Results of surgical therapy for focal epileptic seizures. Can Med Assoc J 1955, 73:515-531. 20. Penfield W: Introduction. Epilepsia 1961, 2:109-110. 21. Pisa M, Sandberg PR, Corcoran ME, Fibiger HC: Spontaneous recurrent seizures after intra-cerebral injections of kainic acid in rats: a possible model of human temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Res 1980, 200:481-487. 22. Nadler JV: Minireview. Kainic acid as a tool for the study of temporal lobe epilepsy. Life Sci 1981, 29:2031-2042. 23. Cavalheiro EA, Riche DA, Le Gal La Salle G: Long-term effects of intrahippocampal kainic acid injection in rats: a method for inducing spontaneous recurrent seizures. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1982, 53:581-589. 24. Cavalheiro EA, Leite JP, Bortolotto ZA, Turski WA, Ikonomidou C, Turski L: Long-term effects of pilocarpine in rats: structural damage of the brain triggers kindling and spontaneous recurrent seizures. Epilepsia 1991, 32:778-782. 25. Nissinen J, Halonen T, Koivisto E, Pitkanen A: A new model of chronic temporal lobe epilepsy induced by electrical stimulation of the amygdala in rat. Epilepsy Res 2000, 38:177205. 26. Glien M, Brandt C, Potschka H, Voigt H, Ebert U, Loscher W: Repeated low-dose treatment of rats with pilocarpine: low mortality but high proportion of rats developing epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2001, 46:111-119. 27. Brandt C, Glien M, Potschka H, Volk H, Loscher W: Epileptogenesis and neuropathology after different types of status epilepticus induced by prolonged electrical stimulation of the basolateral amygdala in rats. Epilepsy Res 2003, 55:83-103. 28. Kobayashi M, Buckmaster PS: Reduced inhibition of dentate granule cells in a model of temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurosci 2003, 23:2440-2452. 29. Tauck DL, Nadler JV: Evidence of functional mossy fiber sprouting in hippocampal formation of kainic acid treated rats. J Neurosci 1985, 5:1016-1022.
10. Sloviter RS: The functional organization of the hippocampal dentate gyrus and its relevance to the pathogenesis of temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann Neurol 1994, 35:640-654.
30. Parent JM, Lowenstein DH: Seizure-induced neurogenesis: are more new neurons good for an adult brain? Prog Brain Res 2002, 135:121-131.
11. Lowenstein DH, Thomas MJ, Smith DH, McIntosh TK: Selective vulnerability of dentate hilar neurons following traumatic brain injury: a potential mechanistic link between head trauma and disorders of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 1992, 12:4846-4853.
31. Parent JM, Yu TW, Leibowitz RT, Geschwind DH, Sloviter RS, Lowenstein DH: Dentate granule cell neurogenesis is increased by seizures and contributes to aberrant network reorganization in the adult rat hippocampus. J Neurosci 1997, 17:3727-3738.
12. Chang BS, Lowenstein DH: Mechanisms of disease; epilepsy. N Engl J Med 2003, 349:1257-1266. 13. Rakhade SN, Jensen FE: Epileptogenesis in the immature brain: emerging mechanisms. Nat Rev Neurol 2009, 5:380-391. 14. Wasterlain CG, Mazarati AM, Shirasaka Y, Thompson KW, Penix L, Liu H, Katsumori H: Seizure-induced hippocampal damage and chronic epilepsy: a Hebbian theory of epileptogenesis. Adv Neurol 1999, 79:829-843. www.sciencedirect.com
32. Harvey BD, Sloviter RS: 1: Hippocampal granule cell activity and c-Fos expression during spontaneous seizures in awake, chronically epileptic, pilocarpine-treated rats; implications for hippocampal epileptogenesis. J Comp Neurol 2005, 488:442463. This paper reports that pilocarpine-treated animals do not exhibit dentate granule cell-onset seizures, an assumed property of the pilocarpine model. Current Opinion in Pharmacology 2017, 35:1–6
Please cite this article in press as: Sloviter RS: Epileptogenesis meets Occam’s Razor, Curr Opin Pharmacol (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2017.07.012
COPHAR-1630; NO. OF PAGES 6
6 Tribute to Norman Bowery
33. Raol YH, Lund IV, Bandyopadhyay S, Zhang G, Roberts DS, Wolfe JH, Russek SJ, Brooks-Kayal AR: Enhancing GABAA receptor alpha 1 subunit levels in hippocampal dentate gyrus inhibits epilepsy development in an animal model of temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurosci 2006, 26:11342-11346. 34. Goffin K, Nissinen J, Van Laere K, Pitkanen A: Cyclicity of spontaneous recurrent seizures in pilocarpine model of temporal lobe epilepsy in rat. Exp Neurol 2007, 205:501-505. 35. Bumanglag AV, Sloviter RS: Minimal latency to hippocampal epileptogenesis and clinical epilepsy after perforant pathway stimulation-induced status epilepticus in awake rats. J Comp Neurol 2008, 510:561-580. This paper demonstrates for the first time that hippocampal injury causes granule cell-onset epilepsy, and does so with minimal latency. 36. Jung S, Jones TD, Lugo JN, Sheerin JH, Miller JW, D’Ambrosio R, Anderson AE, Poolos NP: Progressive dendritic HCN channelopathy during epileptogenesis in the rat pilocarpine model of epilepsy. J Neurosci 2007, 27:13012-13021. 37. Brandt C, To¨llner K, Klee R, Bro¨er S, Lo¨scher W: Effective termination of status epilepticus by rational polypharmacy in the lithium-pilocarpine model in rats: window of opportunity to prevent epilepsy and prediction of epilepsy by biomarkers. Neurobiol Dis 2015, 75:78-90. 38. Mouri G, Jimenez-Mateos E, Engel T, Dunleavy M, Hatazaki S, Paucard A, Matsushima S, Taki W, Henshall DC: Unilateral hippocampal CA3-predominant damage and short latency epileptogenesis after intraamygdala microinjection of kainic acid in mice. Brain Res 2008, 1213:140-151. 39. Mikaeloff Y, Jambaque I, Hertz-Pannier L, Zamfirescu A, Adamsbaum C, Plouin P, Dulac O, Chiron C: Devastating epileptic encephalopathy in school-aged children (DESC): a pseudo encephalitis. Epilepsy Res 2006, 69:67-79. This paper reports that prolonged convulsive status epilepticus in humans causes epilepsy with minimal latency, as it does in experimental animals. 40. Bumanglag AV, Sloviter RS: Immediate hippocampal granule cell epileptogenesis in experimental temporal lobe epilepsy. 2017 Neuroscience Meeting Planner. Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience; 2017. in press. This abstract reports that the ‘latent’ period after hippocampal injury is a focal epileptic state in transition to a clinically obvious epileptic state, rather than a ‘gestational’ state of epileptogenesis.
41. Sloviter RS: Hippocampal epileptogenesis in animal models of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis; the importance of the ‘latent period’ and other concepts. Epilepsia 2008, 49(Suppl 9):85-92. 42. Sloviter RS: The neurobiology of temporal lobe epilepsy: too much information, not enough knowledge. CR Biol 2005, 328:143-153. 43. Sloviter RS: Experimental status epilepticus in animals: what are we modeling? Epilepsia 2009, 50(Suppl 12):11-13. 44. Sloviter RS: Apoptosis: a guide for the perplexed. Trends Pharmacol Sci 2002, 23:19-24. 45. Sloviter RS, Bumanglag AV, Schwarcz R, Frotscher M: Abnormal dentate gyrus network circuitry in temporal lobe epilepsy. In Jasper’s Basic Mechanisms of the Epilepsies. Edited by Noebels JL, Avoli M, Rogawski MA, Olsen RW, Delgado-Escueta AV. Oxford University Press; 2012:454-469. 46. Aronica E, Bauer S, Bozzi Y, Caleo M, Dingledine R, Gorter JA, Henshall DC, Kaufer D, Koh S, Lo¨scher W et al.: Neuroinflammatory targets and treatments for epilepsy validated in experimental models. Epilepsia 2017, 58 (Suppl 3):27-38. 47. van Vliet EA, da Costa Arau´jo S, Redeker S, van Schaik R, Aronica E, Gorter JA: Blood–brain barrier leakage may lead to progression of temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 2007, 130:521534. 48. Sloviter RS: Progress on the issue of excitotoxic injury modification vs. real neuroprotection; implications for posttraumatic epilepsy. Neuropharmacology 2011, 61:1048-1050. 49. Temkin N: Preventing and treating posttraumatic seizures: the human experience. Epilepsia 2009, 50(Suppl. 2):10-13. 50. Eastman CL, Verley DR, Fender JS, Stewart TH, Nov E, Curia G, D’Ambrosio R: Antiepileptic and antiepileptogenic performance of carisbamate after head injury in the rat: blind and randomized studies. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2011, 336:779790. 51. Henshall DC, Hamer HM, Pasterkamp RJ, Goldstein DB, Kjems J, Prehn JH, Schorge S, Lamottke K, Rosenow F: MicroRNAs in epilepsy: pathophysiology and clinical utility. Lancet Neurol 2016, 15:1368-1376.
Current Opinion in Pharmacology 2017, 35:1–6 Please cite this article in press as: Sloviter RS: Epileptogenesis meets Occam’s Razor, Curr Opin Pharmacol (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2017.07.012
www.sciencedirect.com