Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders

Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders

JSR-01402; No of Pages 11 Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Safety Research journal...

3MB Sizes 2 Downloads 42 Views

JSR-01402; No of Pages 11 Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Safety Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jsr

4 5 6 7

a

8

a r t i c l e

9 10 11 12 13 18 17 16 15 14 38 39 40 41 42 43

Article history: Received 15 December 2016 Received in revised form 16 February 2017 Accepted 6 June 2017 Available online xxxx

c

Department of Applied Mechanics, Chalmers University of Technology, Gothenburg, Sweden Volvo Group Trucks Technology Advanced Technology & Research, Gothenburg, Sweden School of Transportation Engineering, Tongji University, Shanghai, China

i n f o

a b s t r a c t

In the last 30 years, China has undergone a dramatic increase in vehicle ownership and a resulting escalation in the number of road crashes. Although crash figures are decreasing today, they remain high; it is therefore important to investigate crash causation mechanisms to further improve road safety in China. To shed more light on the topic, naturalistic driving data was collected in Shanghai as part of the evaluation of a behavior-based safety service. The data collection included instrumenting 47 vehicles belonging to a commercial fleet with data acquisition systems. From the overall sample, 91 rear-end crash or near-crash (CNC) events, triggered by 24 drivers, were used in the analysis. The CNC were annotated by three researchers, through an expert assessment methodology based on videos and kinematic variables. The results show that the main factor behind the rear-end CNC was the adoption of very small safety margins. In contrast to results from previous studies in the US, the following vehicles' drivers typically had their eyes on the road and reacted quickly in response to the evolving conflict in most events. When delayed reactions occurred, they were mainly due to driving-related visual scanning mismatches (e.g., mirror checks) rather than visual distraction. Finally, the study identified four main conflict scenarios that represent the typical development of rearend conflicts in this data. The findings of this study have several practical applications, such as informing the specifications of in-vehicle safety measures and automated driving and providing input into the design of coaching/training procedures to improve the driving habits of drivers. © 2017 The Authors. National Safety Council and Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

D

b

F

Giulio Bianchi Piccinini, a,⁎ Johan Engström, a,b Jonas Bärgman, a Xuesong Wang c

O

Q43Q3

R O

2

Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders

P

1Q2

R

E

C

T

E

Keywords: Naturalistic driving data Truck drivers Human factors Traffic safety Active safety systems

37

R

47 45 44 46

1. Introduction

49 50

Over the past 30 years, China has dramatically increased vehicle ownership. In the period 1985–2013, the number of passenger vehicles increased from 284,900 to 105,016,800 and the number of trucks increased from 264,800 to 12,754,900 (National Bureau of Statistics of China, 2016). The boom of motorization in the last three decades has caused the number of crashes to increase, although in the last few years there has been some decline. In total about 4.22 million road traffic crashes occurred in China in 2011, resulting in a total of 62,387 deaths and 237,421 injuries (Zhang, Yau, Zhang, & Li, 2016). Even with the decrease, it is important to continue the efforts to understand causation mechanisms of Chinese crashes to address the most critical aspects of traffic safety.

53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60

U

51 52

N C O

48

⁎ Corresponding author at: Chalmers University of Technology, Department of Applied Mechanics Department, Division of Vehicle Safety, Accident Prevention Group, Lindholmspiren 3, Lindholmen Science Park, 41756 Gothenburg, Sweden. E-mail address: [email protected] (G.B. Piccinini).

Analyzing accident records from 192 Shanghai roads in the year 2009, Deng, Wang, Chen, Wang, and Chen (2013) showed that rearend crashes were the most frequent crash scenarios on urban expressways. Although they accounted for only 13.7% of the crash fatalities in 2009 in China (Zhang, Yau, & Chen, 2013), rear-end crashes are known to provoke several types of injuries — especially neck injuries and other whiplash-associated disorders (Siegmund, Winkelstein, Ivancic, Svensson, & Vasavada, 2009) — and cause significant costs to fleets (Avery & Weekes, 2009). Previous studies in Western countries (primarily in the United States) have indicated that driver inattention (e.g., visual distraction and driver fatigue) and driving-related visual scanning (e.g. looking at the mirror) are the key factors behind rear-end crashes involving commercial vehicles (Engström, Werneke, Bärgman, Nguyen, & Cook, 2013; Woodrooffe et al., 2012). In particular, further quantitative analyses showed that rear-end crashes in commercial vehicle fleets (trucks and buses as well as passenger cars) typically occurred due to a “perfect” timing mismatch between an off-road glance and the lead vehicle braking unexpectedly (Eiríksdóttir, 2016; Victor et al., 2015). Notably, these datasets also included rear-end crashes where the key causation

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004 0022-4375/© 2017 The Authors. National Safety Council and Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36

61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80

96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115

2. Method

117

2.1. Apparatus

118

Overall, 47 vehicles belonging to a commercial fleet were equipped with a data acquisition unit provided by Lytx (2016) and hereafter referred to as a Video Event Recorder (VER): 14 light trucks, 30 vans, and three heavy trucks with trailers. The VER was installed on the vehicles during a 12-month naturalistic driving study as part of a behaviorbased safety service evaluation, similar to previous ones conducted in the US (Hickman & Hanowski, 2011; McGehee, Raby, Carney, Lee, & Reyes, 2007; Simons-Morton et al., 2013). The VER integrated a variety of sensors (e.g. cameras, 3-axis accelerometer and GPS) and stored 12 s of data about safety-critical events every time a strong longitudinal or lateral acceleration was recorded by the unit. Among the overall information collected by the VER, the data used in this study is reported in Table 1.

127 128 129 130

t1:1 t1:2

R

O

C

125 126

N

123 124

U

121 122

R

116

119 120

139

2.3.1. Event selection and annotation The safety-critical events acquired through the VER were primarily screened to discard events in which the camera was obstructed, events which occurred in a context not authorized for further analysis (e.g. in company yards), and events for which the participants had not provided written consent. After that, a second filtering process was conducted to retain only rear-end events which could be classified as nearcrashes or crashes, since the aim of the study was to understand causation mechanisms. The final sample comprised 91 rear-end events, which were annotated by two persons who had been previously trained and had already performed the same task in other projects. The aim of the annotation process was to extract significant information about the event (e.g. weather conditions, driver's involvement in secondary task), from the videos. The variables annotated were retrieved from the SHRP2 researcher dictionary for video reduction (SHRP2, 2016). For the sake of brevity, not all variables are listed in the paper but Table 2 reports an overview of the most relevant ones and, in the Appendix, the attribute levels are indicated for each variable.

140

2.3.2. Causation analysis The annotated variables - described in Section 2.3.1 - and the forward and face videos were used for the Causation Analysis for Naturalistic Driving Events (CANDE). The CANDE method, based on the expert assessment of videos, kinematic variables, and narratives, builds on previous methodologies developed for crash causation analysis (Engström et al., 2013; Habibovic, Tivesten, Uchida, Bärgman, & Aust, 2013; Ljung Aust et al., 2012). The aim of the analysis is to determine the observable causal mechanisms behind crashes and near-crashes by identifying component causes, which constitute necessary but insufficient conditions for the crash or near-crash to occur. The general crash model underlying the CANDE methodology is illustrated in Fig. 1. The first part of the analysis (the lower part in Fig. 1) characterizes how the conflict between the subject vehicle (SV) and the conflict partner (CP; here, the lead vehicle) developed. A conflict here is based on Svensson and Hydén (2006): “a situation where two or more collision partners approach each other in time and space to such an extent that a collision is imminent if their movements remain unchanged”. A further conflict criterion adopted in CANDE is that the conflict should be unintended by at least one of the conflict partners. The coding of conflict initiation involves characterizing the preconflict behaviors of the SV and CP as well as the critical action(s) that induced the conflict. The pre-conflict behavior represents the kinematic state of the SV or the CP relative to other road users and the infrastructure just before the conflict was initiated, that is, just prior to the conflict start. In rear-end conflicts, typical examples of pre-conflict behaviors are given by the SV following CP or the approaching stationary or slowly-moving CP. By contrast, the critical action(s) refers to an action by the SV or CP that instantly induces a conflict, such as establishing a collision course by turning into another vehicle's path, accelerating into an intersection or braking in front of a following vehicle. In rearend conflicts, the critical action is most often performed by the CP (e.g., braking or cutting in) but may also involve SV actions such as accelerating towards CP. SV-initiated critical actions are more common for other crash types (e.g., the SV is pulling out at an intersection). It

158

F

94 95

2.3. Data selection process and analysis methodology

O

92 93

132 133

R O

90 91

The study involved 37 truck and commercial vehicle drivers employed at a customer fleet in China. Before starting the study, the participants were requested to fill in a questionnaire including demographic information (gender, age, years holding driving license) and sign a consent form. The participants were all male, their mean age was 32.2 years (SD = 7.40) and, in average, they held a driving license for 5.54 years (SD = 3.62) and drove 91,400 km yearly (SD = 35,200).

P

88 89

131

D

87

2.2. Participants

T

85 86

C

83 84

mechanisms were unrelated to inattention, being the subject vehicle following too closely behind a lead vehicle, which braked unexpectedly when drivers were looking at the road. Naturalistic driving studies previously conducted in China identified short time headways, rather than eyes off road, as the main contributing factor in rear-end crashes (Lin et al., 2008). However, none of those studies focused on commercial vehicles; as a result, there is little insight into the role played by the drivers of commercial vehicles in the development of traffic conflicts. While crash data is available for these vehicles, it is clear that they do not always provide information about contributing factors such as distraction (Blower & Woodrooffe, 2013); hence the need for a naturalistic study including commercial drivers as participants. To prevent rear-end crashes, in-vehicle safety systems such as forward collision warning (FCW) and automatic emergency brake systems (AEBS), as well as different levels of vehicle automation, are being introduced into the Chinese market. Other safety measures, such as behavior-based safety (BBS) services, are also likely to get increased deployment in the Chinese market. Since differences exist between China and other countries with regard to the safety culture (Atchley, Shi, & Yamamoto, 2014), it is necessary to target local problems to ensure the effectiveness of those measures. Scrambling behaviors — defined as “the behaviors of drivers, pedestrians, or cyclists that challenge for right of way in violation of traffic codes” (Shi, Bai, Tao, & Atchley, 2011, p. 1540) — are a typical feature of Chinese traffic which could impact the development and success of safety measures. The main objective of the present study was to conduct a detailed naturalistic investigation of causal mechanisms behind commercial vehicle rear-end crashes and near-crashes in China. The target population addressed by the study was represented by professional drivers in China. A further aim was to compare the obtained results to the ones available on rear-end crash/near-crash causation from Western countries (in particular the US), for use in the development of different safety measures. To this end, an event-triggered naturalistic data collection was conducted in Shanghai.

E

81 82

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

E

2

Table 1 Data recorded by the VER and used in the study.

t1:3

Data acquired

Sensor source

Sensor recording frequency

t1:4 t1:5 t1:6 t1:7 t1:8 t1:9

Video of forward view Video of driver view Longitudinal acceleration Lateral acceleration Vertical acceleration Speed

Camera Camera 3-axis accelerometer 3-axis accelerometer 3-axis accelerometer GPS

4 Hz 4 Hz 20 Hz 20 Hz 20 Hz 1 Hz

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

134 135 136 137 138

141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157

159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx t2:1 t2:2

3

Table 2 Description of variables annotated.

t2:3

Variable name

Variable description

t2:4 t2:5 t2:6

Weather Light condition Road surface condition

t2:7 t2:8 t2:9 t2:10 t2:11 t2:12 t2:13

Number of crashesa Number of near-crashesb Crash/near-crash type Visual obstructions Evasive maneuver Secondary task Driver drowsiness

Weather conditions at the start of the event General light conditions during the event Type of roadway condition that would affect the vehicle's coefficient of friction at the initiation of the precipitating event. For a description of precipitating event, please refer to SHRP2 (2016) The number of crashes visible during the event The number of near-crashes visible during the event The type of crash/near-crash that the SV has with other objects of conflict for the most severe type of crash or near-crash The visual factors that are likely to have contributed to the event The subject driver's reaction or avoidance maneuver in response to the event/incident Observable driver engagement in secondary tasks at any point during the event Observer's rating of the drowsiness of the driver

205 206 207 208 209

F

O

R O

P

D

E

204

T

202 203

C

200 201

E

198 199

such (lack of) attention to driving, expectation (mismatch) or (reduced) visibility. The coding of attention to driving identifies mismatches between the current allocation of attention and that demanded by activities critical for safe driving: examples of mismatches are sleep-related attentional impairment, internal or external distraction, and visual scanning mismatches. The coding of expectation aims to establish when the expectation of the SV driver does not match how the situation develops, resulting in a delayed or lacking reaction to the conflict. The coding of visibility aims to determine any possible reduction of SV driver's visibility which affected the delayed (or the lack of) action: this could be due to lighting conditions (e.g. darkness, glare), environmental factors (e.g. rain, fog), low CP conspicuity (CP headlights malfunctioning) or occluding objects (e.g. parked vehicle, tree, vehicle sunshield). The 91 events were analyzed by the first three authors of the paper using the CANDE method and the results are described in the next section.

R

196 197

should be noted that in some scenarios, such as when the SV closes in on the CP, the conflict evolves gradually without a distinct critical action. The second part of the analysis (the upper part in Fig. 1) characterizes the nature of the corrective action (if any), such as steering and/or braking, performed by the SV driver in response to the conflict. This includes the corrective action's effect (e.g., controlled braking, steering or skidding) and how the effect is influenced by the vehicle or roadsurface conditions. The vehicle conditions can have an influence when the vehicle's braking or steering systems are not functioning properly. The road-surface conditions can also impact the corrective action's effect when the road surface is slippery due to weather (e.g. rain, snow, ice), type of road surface (e.g. gravel) or residual material (e.g. oil). Moreover, the timing of the corrective action (if any) is characterized as either normal, or delayed where the latter is defined as a corrective action initiated significantly later than a corrective action performed under normal conditions (attentive driver and no visibility problems). Finally, component causes of delayed or lacking actions are identified,

R

194 195

N C O

193

a A crash is any contact that the subject vehicle has with another conflict partner, either moving or fixed, at any speed that is observable or in which kinetic energy is measurably transferred or dissipated. This excludes roadway features meant to be driven over, such as speed bumps. b A near-crash is any circumstance that requires a rapid evasive maneuver by at least one conflict partner to avoid a crash.

U

t2:14 t2:15 t2:16

Fig. 1. Template for CANDE coding (SV refers to subject vehicle and CP to conflict partner).

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226

4 t3:1 t3:2 Q1

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

Table 3 CNC and crashes triggered by each driver and corresponding vehicle.

t3:3

Driver ID

Vehicle type

Number of CNC

Number of crashes

t3:4 t3:5 t3:6 t3:7 t3:8 t3:9 t3:10 t3:11 t3:12 t3:13 t3:14 t3:15 t3:16 t3:17 t3:18 t3:19 t3:20 t3:21 t3:22 t3:23 t3:24 t3:25 t3:26 t3:27 t3:28 t3:29 t3:30

Driver_1 Driver_2 Driver_3 Driver_4 Driver_5 Driver_6 Driver_7 Driver_8 Driver_9 Driver_10 Driver_11 Driver_12 Driver_13 Driver_14 Driver_15 Driver_16 Driver_17 Driver_18 Driver_19 Driver_20 Driver_21 Driver_22 Driver_23 Driver_24 Total vans Total light trucks Total

Van Van Light truck Van Van Van Light truck Van Light truck Light truck Van Light truck Van Van Van Van Light truck Light truck Van Van Van Van Van Van

19 14 7 7 4 4 4 4 4 3 3 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 69 22 91

2 1

1

3.2. Aggregation of results for crashes and near-crashes

235

In the 91 rear-end CNC analyzed, there were overall 7 crashes and 84 near-crashes. Figs. 2 and 3 show the aggregation charts resulting from the coding with the CANDE methodology, respectively for only crashes and for crashes and near crashes altogether. First, for both the 7 crashes and the 91 CNC, the most common preconflict behavior/state for the SV relative to the CP was that the SV followed the CP at a (relatively) constant short headway. The headway was coded as “short” if the estimated time gap was below 1.75 s, a criterion based on pre-conflict behaviors recorded in previous studies (Eiríksdóttir, 2016; Victor et al., 2015). However, it should be noted that even shorter headways (below 1 s) were very common. In all the crashes and in the CNC with short headway, the conflict originated with the CP braking, typically due to a stopped or braking vehicle ahead of it. In the other main pre-conflict situation, the SV and the CP were initially driving in different lanes and the conflict was usually induced by the CP suddenly braking or cutting in front of the SV. Second, the corrective action typically involved controlled braking — in some CNC combined with swerving — at strong (N 6 m/s2) or medium (4–6 m/s2) deceleration levels. Thus, skidding due to a slippery road surface, or weak deceleration due to malfunctioning brakes, was uncommon in CNC but occurred more often in crashes (notably, in 2 crashes). Moreover, the timing of the corrective action was coded as “normal” (i.e., not delayed or lacking) in 74.7% of the CNC but this percentage dropped to 57.1% for crashes. Finally, for the CNC involving a delayed reaction, the dominant reason was a visual scanning mismatch (e.g., related to mirror checking). Driver distraction did occur, but only to a limited extent and, in

236 237

O

R O

1

D

P

2 5 7

3. Results

228

3.1. Participants of selected events

229

The 91 rear-end CNC used for the analysis involved 24 of the 37 drivers. 69 CNC (76%) involved participants driving vans, 22 CNC

U

N

C

O

R

R

E

C

T

E

227

230

231 232

F

1 1

(24%) involved participants driving light trucks and no CNC involved participants driving heavy trucks. The number of CNC and the number of crashes triggered for each driver (and corresponding vehicle type) are reported in Table 3.

Fig. 2. Aggregation chart for crashes.

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

233 234

238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262

5

D

P

R O

O

F

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

T

267

Separate analyses were conducted for four common conflict 268 scenarios defined by the kinematic situation and the behavior of the 269

R

E

C

3.3. Analysis of specific conflict scenarios

R

266

particular, in crashes where external distraction with gaze diversion was the main reason for delayed reaction (66.7%). Finally, sleeprelated attentional impairment and visibility-related factors were very uncommon.

N C O

264 265

U

263

E

Fig. 3. Aggregation chart for crashes and near-crashes.

Fig. 4. Aggregation chart for conflict scenario 1 (SV following CP).

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

T

E

D

P

R O

O

F

6

278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297

R

R

276 277

O

274 275

• CS1: SV following CP: 31 CNC (34%; 27 near-crashes and 4 crashes) • CS2: SV closing in on CP: 16 CNC (18%; 15 near-crashes and 1 crashes) • CS3: CP cutting in front of SV: 29 CNC (32%; 29 near-crashes and 0 crashes) • CS4: SV changing lane behind CP: 10 CNC (11%; 9 near-crashes and 1 crash).

C

272 273

SV and CP preceding the crash or near crash. These four scenarios together accounted for 94.5% of the 91 rear-end CNC in the study:

3.3.1. CS1: SV following CP Fig. 4 shows the aggregation chart for conflict scenario 1 (CS1). As can be observed, a clear causation pattern emerges in which the SV follows the CP before the start of the conflict. In a prototypical scenario, the SV initially follows the CP at a short headway while the CP is also following another vehicle; the headway was coded as “short” (b1.75 s) in 87.1% of the CNC and the CP was following another vehicle in 74.2% of the CNC. The CP then typically braked suddenly due to a braking/ stopped road user ahead (61.3% of the CNC). In most cases, the SV driver reacted relatively quickly by braking only (71.0% of the CNC) or braking and swerving (29.0%) in a controlled fashion. However, delayed reactions were also relatively common in this scenario (occurring in about one third of the CNC), typically caused by visual scanning mismatches or external/internal visual distraction. Overall, these conflicts typically occurred in dense traffic, with short headways, where the critical situation was induced by a sudden unexpected event ahead (e.g., a vehicle cutting in or a pedestrian entering the road), and the SV driver's reaction was sometimes (but most often not) delayed by the eyes being taken off the forward roadway.

N

271

U

270

E

C

Fig. 5. Aggregation chart for conflict scenario 2 (SV closing in on CP).

3.3.2. CS2: SV closing in on CP In this scenario, like CS1, the SV is initially behind the CP, in the same lane. However, here the SV initially closes in on the CP rather than following at a constant headway. In the prototypical case for this scenario, the SV is closing in on a decelerating CP, which then suddenly brakes due to a braking or stopped vehicle ahead. However, a few CNC involve a CP which is leading the SV without braking, or which is stopped/moving slowly before the start of the conflict. As shown in Fig. 5, the SV driver's corrective reaction typically involves controlled braking (87.5% of the CNC) initiated without a marked delay (delayed reactions were observed in 18.8% of the CNC). Thus, the typical causation mechanism is similar to CS1, where the CP brakes unexpectedly in dense traffic when the SV is following closely. However, one key difference is the lower prevalence of delayed reactions and visual inattention in CS2 (18.8% in CS2 compared to 35.5% in CS1), which may be due to the fact that the driver is closing in on the CP, and thus less likely to take his eyes off the road. Another difference is the lower prevalence of combined braking and swerving (6.3% in CS2 compared to 29.0% in CS1), which may be explained by the fact that the driver generally has better control over the situation, since attention and gaze were initially allocated towards the CP, although the SV driver did not expect it to brake. This may also explain the lower proportion of crashes in this scenario (6.3% in CS2 compared to 12.9% in CS1).

298

3.3.3. CS3: SV changing lane behind braking SV The aggregation chart shown in Fig. 6 describes the analysis for conflict scenario 3 (CS3). In the prototypical case, the SV is initially engaged in a lane change with the aim to merge behind the CP, traveling in the adjacent lane, and the CP then brakes suddenly, most often due to a

321 322

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320

323 324 325

7

E

D

P

R O

O

F

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

331 332 333 334 335

E

R

330

R

328 329

braking or stopped road user ahead. The conflict initiation in CS3 is thus very similar to that in CS2, with the main difference that the SV is changing lanes behind the CP, rather than approaching the CP in the same lane. As in CS2, the corrective action mainly results in controlled braking (90% of the CNC); however, in CS3, delayed reactions occur relatively frequently (50% of the CNC). The delayed reactions were all attributed to visual scanning mismatches, mainly ascribed to the SV driver checking the mirror to perform the lane change.

N C O

326 327

C

T

Fig. 6. Aggregation chart for conflict scenario 3 (SV changes lane behind braking CP).

349

4. Discussion

350

Event-triggered naturalistic data was collected in Shanghai, using data acquisition units provided by Lytx as part of their behaviorbased safety service. Forty-seven vehicles (trucks and vans) were instrumented, belonging to a local commercial fleet. After initial

336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346

351 352 353

U

347 348

3.3.4. CS4: CP cutting in front of SV Fig. 7 displays the aggregation chart for conflict scenario 4 (CS4), in which the CP cuts in front of the SV. An analysis of the pre-conflict behavior indicates that the SV is usually following a vehicle other than the CP (48.3% of the CNC) or driving without a vehicle in front (24.1% of the CNC). The CP usually drives in the lane adjacent to the SV (75.9% of the CNC) before changing lanes in front of it. During the lane change, the CP is typically driving at a slower speed than the SV (75.9% of the CNC) but, in some CNC, the CP also brakes while or after cutting in (24.1% of the CNC). In the great majority of CNC, the SV driver reacts quickly to the conflict (without a marked delay) by braking. Delayed reactions are uncommon (6.9% of the CNC) and exclusively induced by visual scanning mismatches. Moreover, combined braking and swerving is rare (6.9% of the CNC).

screening, 91 rear-end CNC (84 near-crashes and 7 crashes) were analyzed by 3 researchers, using the CANDE methodology for expertbased causation mechanism analysis based on videos and kinematic variables. The preliminary descriptive analysis shows that the 91 rear-end CNC were triggered by 24 drivers and, more in detail, that roughly 80% of the CNC were triggered by about 30% of the drivers. Interestingly, this result roughly follows the 80/20 rule or Pareto Principle suggesting that “20% of something always are responsible for 80% of the results” (Reh, 2005). The principle was originally created to describe the unequal distribution of wealth but later applied to other phenomena, including road crashes (Koch, 2011). The analyses conducted with the CANDE methodology yielded a fairly clear picture of the main causation patterns behind the collected rear-end CNC. In contrast to analyses of US rear-end crashes and nearcrashes (Eiríksdóttir, 2016; Engström et al., 2013; Victor et al., 2015), the “perfect mismatch” between an off-road glance and the lead vehicle braking was relatively rare in the current Chinese dataset. As a matter of fact, in the majority of the CNC, the SV driver had his eyes on the road and reacted fast in response to the evolving conflict. When delayed reactions occurred, this was mainly due to driving-related visual scanning mismatches (e.g., mirror checks prior to changing lanes) rather than visual distraction associated to secondary tasks (which was the dominating cause of inattention in both Engström et al., 2013; Victor et al., 2015). It is worth to notice though that delayed reactions were mostly due to external distraction with gaze diversion in the very limited number of crashes available. The most common factor behind most of the CNC was, then, the adoption of very small safety margins (in terms of short headways or tight cut-ins) combined with the erratic, unpredictable behaviors of other road users. These results confirm the findings of a previous

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

E

D

P

R O

O

F

8

394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416

E

R

R

392 393

O

390 391

C

388 389

naturalistic driving study conducted with passenger cars in Beijing (Lin et al., 2008) but differ from the outcome of US data described earlier, where the headway initially adopted by the SV driver before the conflict was typically relatively large. The propensity to adopt small safety margins can be related to the scrambling behaviors (Shi et al., 2011). Although such behaviors were not explicitly coded in the present analysis, it was frequently observed that Chinese drivers typically have to “create space” for themselves by cutting in front or plunging ahead at a T-junction with very small time gaps, thus knowingly creating a kinematic conflict. Such behaviors also appear common among vulnerable road users such as riders of powered two-wheelers, bicyclists, and pedestrians, leading to frequent critical encounters between multiple road users which develop rapidly and are hard to predict. In such a traffic environment, there is little leeway (or spare attentional capacity) for engaging in distracting activities, which is the likely reason for the difference from the US data in this respect. However, the need to scan the road and mirrors inevitably creates situations where drivers take their eyes away from the forward roadway to some extent, and thus become more vulnerable to a suddenly braking lead vehicle. Thus, it appears that differences in traffic culture between China and US (Atchley et al., 2014) lead to different crash/near-crash causation patterns. In the US, the slower and more predictable traffic dynamics allow drivers to engage in distracting activities while driving, although it leaves them vulnerable to unexpected events occurring during glances away from the road. In contrast, in China drivers have to adopt a more aggressive driving style to create space for themselves in competition with other road users, leading to smaller safety margins and a greater degree of uncertainty in the traffic system as a whole. Chinese drivers (in particular professional drivers) could be expected to have adapted to this situation and thus generally attend to the driving task more than Western drivers. Hence, it seems that, because the

N

386 387

U

385

C

T

Fig. 7. Aggregation chart for conflict scenario 4 (CP cuts in front of SV).

Chinese traffic culture requires attentive and vigilant drivers, distracting activities are not the main cause of rear-end crashes and near-crashes. This has important implications for the development of countermeasures, as discussed below. This study also identified four main conflict scenarios in the development of rear-end conflicts: SV following CP, SV closing in on CP, SV changing lanes behind CP, and CP cutting in front of SV. As reported in a previous study conducted in Beijing with passenger cars (Lin et al., 2008), the scenario in which the SV follows the CP is the most prevalent (34.0% of the CNC in the current study and 52.0% of those in Lin et al., 2008). However, the remaining conflict scenarios cannot easily be compared with those in Lin et al. (2008), due to differences in the methodology used to classify the scenarios: for example, they considered “Turning” to be a specific category for cases when the SV and/or the LV is/are turning left or right, while in this paper, those situations were coded according to the combination of SV and CP pre-conflict behaviors. Furthermore, the relevance of conflict scenarios in which the SV is following or closing in on the CP is shown by the naturalistic data collection conducted in Shanghai by Li, Zhu, and Ma (2014). In their research, the scenario “Following vehicle approaching a decelerating vehicle” was responsible for 20% of all types of near-crashes, not just rear-end near-crashes. Overall, the results obtained from this research on rear-end crashes and near-crashes could be used to define specifications of in-vehicle safety measures (Kusano & Gabler, 2014; Ljung Aust, 2010) and automated driving (Rau, Yanagisawa, & Najm, 2015) and to design coaching/training procedures to improve driving habits (Sagberg, Selpi, Bianchi Piccinini, & Engström, 2015; Zhang, Yau, & Zhang, 2014). In particular, it is clear from this study that designers of in-vehicle safety measures, autonomous vehicles, and BBS services need to have a different mindset for deployment in China compared to other markets, such as the US. Some more specific suggestions follow.

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512

F

O

R O

468 469

P

466 467

D

464 465

E

462 463

Furthermore, this analysis is primarily based on near-crashes, and the included crashes are of relatively low severity. Although Eiríksdóttir (2016) and Victor et al. (2015) report that the prevalence of eyes-off road is similar for near-crashes and crashes, the overall mechanisms behind near-crashes and crashes are not necessarily the same. In this regard, Knipling (2015) recently challenged the validity of safety-critical events such as near-crashes to draw conclusions about serious crashes. Therefore caution is warranted when comparing the present results with previous ones obtained from US data (Eiríksdóttir, 2016; Engström et al., 2013). For example, it is possible that delayed reactions due to driver inattention would be more prevalent for crashes, as a delayed reaction may be precisely what distinguishes a crash from a near-crash in many situations (Victor et al., 2015). Indeed, delayed reactions due to visual diversion appear relatively more common in the present crashes (compared to the nearcrashes), although the sample of crash events is too small to safely draw any conclusions. In any case, extending the present analysis to a larger number of rear-end crashes is clearly necessary to be able to draw firmer conclusions about crash-causation mechanisms. It would also be preferable to have not only low severity crashes, but also the full spectrum of crash severity. Finally, the present analysis only included rear-end conflicts. Analyzing other event types will give a more comprehensive picture of the causation mechanisms behind Chinese crashes and how they may relate to Chinese traffic culture. In particular, the analysis of conflicts involving pedestrians, bicyclists and motorcyclists will be extremely relevant, considering that they represent the majority of fatalities on Chinese roads.

T

460 461

C

458 459

E

456 457

R

455

R

453 454

N C O

451 452

• Design of in-vehicle safety measures: Although this study is focused on rear-end scenarios, several of the findings are probably generalizable to other conflict scenarios as well. In general, it seems unlikely that forward collision warning (FCW) systems will have more than a very minor effect on reducing the number or severity of crashes in China. The role of the warning is to alert drivers to the hazard when they are inattentive, speeding up the corrective action (Kusano & Gabler, 2014). However, in the majority of the present CNC, the driver was already attentive when the conflict started to develop. Moreover, the small safety margins adopted in Chinese traffic would make it difficult to avoid nuisance warnings. On the other hand, automatic emergency braking systems (AEBS) could have a strong benefit, at least for reducing impact speed if the collision cannot be avoided; the effect strongly depends on the initial safety margin. However, it should be taken into account that the small headways adopted in China may increase the risk of being hit harder from behind due to the AEBS' intervention. This also means that countermeasures addressing whiplash injuries (Linder et al., 2013; Svensson, Lövsund, Håland, & Larsson, 1996) may play a more significant role in Chinese traffic than in the US and even more so with AEBS systems on the market. In the near future, computer simulations of the operation of FCW and AEBS in typical Chinese rear-end scenarios could help estimate their potential effectiveness. • Design of automated driving: The results of this study show that Chinese drivers adopt smaller headways compared to Western drivers and that scrambling behaviors are frequent during daily driving. A consequent challenge will be the design of automated vehicles that can guarantee both safety and comfort, given the dynamic Chinese driving environment. If automated vehicles reproduce the scrambling behavior, they might not meet safety requirements. On the other hand, if autonomous vehicles are designed to keep Western safety margins in China, it is unlikely that they would be accepted by Chinese drivers, in particular if automated vehicles are mixed with human-driven vehicles. • Education/coaching: Education and/or regular coaching of drivers based on performance feedback could be very effective in inducing a safer driving style (see Sagberg et al., 2015 for a review). In our Chinese dataset, conflicts seem to be primarily caused by the interaction of small time headways, in some cases combined with short visual diversions from the forward roadway, which are usually related to driving rather than distraction (secondary tasks). A more cautious attitude with larger safety margins (in particular larger headways while following) could lead to a decrease in crashes. If such a behavioral change could be accomplished in a driver fleet, it must be further ensured that the drivers don't exploit this additional safety margin to engage in distracting activities. Nevertheless, for the implementation of BBS measures in China, it should be taken into account that a certain degree of aggressiveness is needed to be able to move forward at all in Chinese traffic.

U

449 450

As in all studies, the current research has some limitations. First of all, no information was available about drivers' skills and attitude towards driving: hence, further studies with Chinese professional drivers (as private drivers) should assess drivers' characteristics to draw broader conclusions. It is also important to take into account the limited geographical area where the study was conducted: the vehicles included in this study delivered their goods in an area centered around the distribution hub in Shanghai. Caution should be taken in generalizing the results to the whole of China and in comparing the outcome with previous studies conducted in the US, which covered a broader geographical area, including both urban and rural environments. Future studies should be performed in other Chinese cities and in similar road environments in the US to provide a broader dataset for drawing conclusions.

9

513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540

Acknowledgments

541

The authors are grateful to Vinnova and the China Sweden Research Centre for Traffic Safety (CTS) for providing funding for the project, to the colleagues at Chalmers for the interesting discussions, to Mia Cheng for the assistance during the management of the project, and to Rusty Weiss and Patrick Huang for their support in collection. The authors would also like to thank Kristina Mayberry for language revision. This work has been carried out at SAFER - Vehicle and Traffic Safety Centre at Chalmers, Sweden.

542 543

References

550

Atchley, P., Shi, J., & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Cultural foundations of safety culture: A comparison of traffic safety culture in China, Japan and the United States. Transportation Research Part F, 26, 317–325. Avery, M., & Weekes, A. (2009). Autonomous braking systems and their potential effect on whiplash injury reductionProceedings of the21st International Technical Conference on the Enhanced Safety of Vehicles, Stuttgart, Germany (June 15–18, 2009). Blower, D., & Woodrooffe, J. (2013). Survey of the status of truck safety: Brazil, China, Australia, and the United States. (UMTRI-2012-13). Deng, B., Wang, H., Chen, J., Wang, X., & Chen, X. (2013). Traffic accidents in Shanghaigeneral statistics and in-depth analysisProceedings of the23rd International Technical Conference on the Enhanced Safety of Vehicles, Seoul, Republic of Korea (May 27– 30, 2013). Eiríksdóttir, H. (2016). Quantitative analysis of rear-end crash causation mechanisms based on naturalistic crash data. Master's thesis Göteborg, Sweden: Chalmers University of Technology. Engström, J., Werneke, J., Bärgman, J., Nguyen, N., & Cook, B. (2013). Analysis of the role of inattention in road crashes based on naturalistic on-board safety monitoring dataProceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Driver Distraction and Inattention, Gothenburg, Sweden (September 4–6, 2013). Habibovic, A., Tivesten, E., Uchida, N., Bärgman, J., & Aust, M. L. (2013). Driver behavior in car-to-pedestrian incidents: An application of the Driving Reliability and Error Analysis Method (DREAM). Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 554–565. Hickman, J. S., & Hanowski, R. J. (2011). Use of a video monitoring approach to reduce atrisk driving behaviors in commercial vehicle operations. Transportation Research Part F, 14(3), 189–198. Knipling, R. R. (2015). Naturalistic driving events: No harm, no foul, no validityProceedings of 8th International Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training, and Vehicle Design, Salt Lake City, USA (June 22–25, 2015). Koch, R. (2011). The 80/20 principle: The secret to achieving more with less. Crown Business. Kusano, K. D., & Gabler, H. C. (2014). Comprehensive target populations for current active safety systems using national crash databases. Traffic Injury Prevention, 15(7), 753–761.

551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

544 545 546 547 548 549

Appendix A (continued) Variable name

D

C

E

O

R

R

Visual obstructions

N

Variable name

t4:6 t4:8 t4:10 t4:12 t4:14 t4:16 t4:18 t4:20 t4:22 t4:24 t4:26 t4:28

Weather

Light condition

U

t4:4

Attribute levels

No adverse conditions Fog Mist/light rain Raining Snowing Sleeting Rain and fog Snow/sleet and fog Other Unknown Dawn Daylight

O

P

Crash/near-crash type

R O

Number of nearcrashes

Evasive maneuver

Dusk Darkness, lighted Darkness, not lighted Unknown Dry Wet Snowy Icy Muddy Sand, oil, dirt Gravel Other Unknown 0 1 2 3 4+ 0 1 2 3 4+ Rear-end, striking Rear-end, struck Road departure (left or right) Road departure (end) Sideswipe, same direction (left or right) Opposite direction (head-on or sideswipe) Straight crossing path Turn across path Turn into path (same direction) Turn into path (opposite direction) Backing, fixed object Backing into traffic Pedestrian-related Pedal cyclist-related Animal-related Other Unknown None No obstruction Rain, snow, fog, smoke, sand, dust Reflected glare Sunlight Headlights Curve or hill Building, billboard, or other roadway infrastructure design features Trees, crops, vegetation Moving or stopped vehicle (with or without load) Parked vehicle Splash or spray of passing vehicle Inadequate defrost or defog system Inadequate roadway lighting system Inadequate vehicle headlamps Obstruction interior to vehicle Mirrors Broken or improperly cleaned windshield Other obstruction Vision obscured - no details Unknown whether vision was obstructed No driver present No reaction Braked Released brakes Steered to left Steered to right Braked and steered left Braked and steered right Accelerated Accelerated and steered left Accelerated and steered right Other actions Unknown if action was attempted

F

Number of crashes

Appendix A. Attribute levels for categorical variables reported in Table 2. t4:1

Attribute levels

Road surface condition

T

Li, L., Zhu, X., & Ma, Z. (2014). Driver braking behaviour under near-crash scenarios. International Journal of Vehicle Safety, 7(3–4), 374–389. Lin, Q., Feng, R., Cheng, B., Lai, J., Zhang, H., & Mei, B. (2008). Analysis of causes of rear-end conflicts using naturalistic driving data collected by video drive recorders. SAE Technical Paper No. 2008-01-0522. Linder, A., Schick, S., Hell, W., Svensson, M., Carlsson, A., Lemmen, P., ... Tomasch, E. (2013). ADSEAT—Adaptive seat to reduce neck injuries for female and male occupants. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 60, 334–343. Ljung Aust, M. (2010). Generalization of case studies in road traffic when defining precrash scenarios for active safety function evaluation. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(4), 1172–1183. Ljung Aust, M., Habibovic, A., Tivesten, E., Sander, U., Bärgman, J., & Engström, J. (2012). Manual for DREAM version 3.2. Retrieved from http://publications.lib.chalmers.se/ records/fulltext/204828/204828.pdf. Lytx (2016). DriveCam program. Retrieved from http://www.lytx.com/our-solutions/ drivecam-programs. McGehee, D. V., Raby, M., Carney, C., Lee, J. D., & Reyes, M. L. (2007). Extending parental mentoring using an event-triggered video intervention in rural teen drivers. Journal of Safety Research, 38(2), 215–227. National Bureau of Statistics of China (2016). Possession of private vehicles. Retrieved from http://www.stats.gov.cn/tjsj/ndsj/2014/zk/html/Z1826E.htm. Rau, P., Yanagisawa, M., & Najm, W. G. (2015). Target crash population of automated vehiclesProceedings of the 24th International Technical Conference on the Enhanced Safety of Vehicles (ESV), Gothenburg, Sweden (June 8–11, 2015). Reh, F. J. (2005). Pareto's principle—The 80-20 rule. Business Credit, 107(7), 76. Sagberg, F., Selpi, Bianchi Piccinini, G. F., & Engström, J. (2015). A review of research on driving styles and road safety. Human Factors, 57(7), 1248–1275. Shi, J., Bai, Y., Tao, L., & Atchley, P. (2011). A model of Beijing drivers' scrambling behaviors. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 43(4), 1540–1546. SHRP2 (2016). SHRP2 researcher dictionary for video reduction data. Version 3.4 Retrieved from https://vtechworks.lib.vt.edu/bitstream/handle/10919/56719/V4.1_ ResearcherDictionary_for_VideoReductionData_COMPLETE_Oct2015_10-5-15.pdf? sequence=1&isAllowed=y. Siegmund, G. P., Winkelstein, B. A., Ivancic, P. C., Svensson, M. Y., & Vasavada, A. (2009). The anatomy and biomechanics of acute and chronic whiplash injury. Traffic Injury Prevention, 10(2), 101–112. Simons-Morton, B. G., Bingham, C. R., Ouimet, M. C., Pradhan, A. K., Chen, R., Barretto, A., & Shope, J. T. (2013). The effect on teenage risky driving of feedback from a safety monitoring system: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53(1), 21–26. Svensson, Å., & Hydén, C. (2006). Estimating the severity of safety related behaviour. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38(2), 379–385. Svensson, M. Y., Lövsund, P., Håland, Y., & Larsson, S. (1996). The influence of seat-back and head-restraint properties on the head-neck motion during rear-impact. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 28(2), 221–227. Victor, T., Dozza, M., Bärgman, J., Boda, C. N., Engström, J., Flannagan, C., ... Markkula, G. (2015). Analysis of naturalistic driving study data: SAFER glances, driver inattention, and crash risk SHRP 2 safety project SO8. Washington, DC: Transportation Research Board of the National Academy of Sciences (doi: ISBN: 978-0-309-274234. SHRP 2 SO8A, from http://onlinepubs.trb.org/onlinepubs/shrp2/SHRP2_S2-S08ARW-1.pdf). Woodrooffe, J., Blower, D., Bao, S., Bogard, S., Flannagan, C., Green, P. E., & LeBlanc, D. (2012). Performance characterization and safety effectiveness estimates of forward collision avoidance and mitigation systems for medium/heavy commercial vehicles. (US DOT. UMTRI-2011-36). Zhang, G., Yau, K. K., & Chen, G. (2013). Risk factors associated with traffic violations and accident severity in China. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 59, 18–25. Zhang, G., Yau, K. K., & Zhang, X. (2014). Analyzing fault and severity in pedestrian–motor vehicle accidents in China. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 73, 141–150. Zhang, G., Yau, K. K., Zhang, X., & Li, Y. (2016). Traffic accidents involving fatigue driving and their extent of casualties. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 87, 34–42.

C

582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

E

10

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004

t4:30 t4:31 t4:32 t4:33 t4:34 t4:35 t4:36 t4:37 t4:38 t4:39 t4:40 t4:41 t4:42 t4:43 t4:44 t4:45 t4:46 t4:47 t4:48 t4:49 t4:50 t4:51 t4:52 t4:53 t4:54 t4:55 t4:56 t4:57 t4:58 t4:59 t4:60 t4:61 t4:62 t4:63 t4:64 t4:65 t4:66 t4:67 t4:68 t4:69 t4:70 t4:71 t4:72 t4:73 t4:74 t4:75 t4:76 t4:77 t4:78 t4:79 t4:80 t4:81 t4:82 t4:83 t4:84 t4:85 t4:86 t4:87 t4:88 t4:89 t4:90 t4:91 t4:92 t4:93 t4:94 t4:95 t4:96 t4:97 t4:98 t4:99 t4:100 t4:101 t4:102 t4:103 t4:104 t4:105 t4:106 t4:107

G.B. Piccinini et al. / Journal of Safety Research xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

Giulio Bianchi Piccinini is appointed as Assistant Professor at Chalmers University of 643 Technology. He obtained his PhD from the Faculty of Engineering of the University of Porto 644 in 2014. 645

Appendix A (continued) Attribute levels

646

Johan Engström is appointed as Group Leader, Human Factors and Advanced System 647 Testing Group at Virginia Tech Transportation Institute. He obtained his PhD from 648 Chalmers University in 2011. 649

650

Jonas Bärgman is appointed as Group Leader, Accident Prevention Group at Chalmers 651 University of Technology. He obtained his PhD from Chalmers University in 2016. 652

653

Xuesong Wang is appointed as Professor at Tongji University. He obtained his PhD from 654 the University of Central Florida in 2003. 655

656

D

P

R O

O

F

657

U

N C O

R

R

E

C

T

Secondary task

Not Applicable No secondary tasks (or no additional secondary tasks) Talking/singing, audience unknown Dancing Reading Writing Passenger in adjacent seat - interaction Passenger in rear seat - interaction Child in adjacent seat - interaction Child in rear seat - interaction Moving object in vehicle Insect in vehicle Pet in vehicle Object dropped by driver Reaching for object, other Object in vehicle, other Cell phone, holding Cell phone, talking/listening, hand-held Cell Phone, texting Cell Phone, browsing Cell Phone, dialing hand-held Cell Phone, dialing hand-held using quick keys Cell Phone, dialing hands-free using voice-activated software Cell Phone, locating/reaching/answering Cell phone, other Tablet device, locating/reaching Tablet device, operating Tablet device, viewing Tablet device, other Adjusting/monitoring climate control Adjusting/monitoring radio Inserting/retrieving CD (or similar) Adjusting/monitoring other devices integral to vehicle Looking at previous crash or incident Looking at pedestrian Looking at animal Looking at an object external to the vehicle Distracted by construction Other external distraction Reaching for food-related or drink-related item Eating with utensils Eating without utensils Drinking with lid and straw Drinking with lid, no straw Drinking with straw, no lid Drinking from open container Reaching for cigar/cigarette Lighting cigar/cigarette Smoking cigar/cigarette Extinguishing cigar/cigarette Reaching for personal body-related item Combing/brushing/fixing hair Applying make-up Shaving Brushing/flossing teeth Biting nails/cuticles Removing/adjusting clothing Removing/adjusting jewelry Removing/inserting/adjusting contact lenses or glasses Other personal hygiene Other non-specific internal eye glance Other known secondary task Unknown type (secondary task present) Unknown Alert Slight sleepiness Severe sleepiness Unknown

E

Variable name t4:108 t4:109 t4:110 t4:111 t4:112 t4:113 t4:114 t4:115 t4:116 t4:117 t4:118 t4:119 t4:120 t4:121 t4:122 t4:123 t4:124 t4:125 t4:126 t4:127 t4:128 t4:129 t4:130 t4:131 t4:132 t4:133 t4:134 t4:135 t4:136 t4:137 t4:138 t4:139 t4:140 t4:141 t4:142 t4:143 t4:144 t4:145 t4:146 t4:147 t4:148 t4:149 t4:150 t4:151 t4:152 t4:153 t4:154 t4:155 t4:156 t4:157 t4:158 t4:159 t4:160 t4:161 t4:162 t4:163 t4:164 t4:165 t4:166 t4:167 t4:168 t4:169 t4:170 t4:171 t4:172 t4:173 t4:174 t4:175

11

Driver drowsiness

Please cite this article as: Piccinini, G.B., et al., Factors contributing to commercial vehicle rear-end conflicts in China: A study using on-board event data recorders, Journal of Safety Research (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2017.06.004