Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials and clinical response to prophylactic lithium medication: A replication study

Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials and clinical response to prophylactic lithium medication: A replication study

181 Psychiatry Research, 44:181-191 Elsevier Intensity Dependence of Auditory Evoked Potentials and Clinical Response to Prophylactic Lithium Medic...

711KB Sizes 0 Downloads 40 Views

181

Psychiatry Research, 44:181-191

Elsevier

Intensity Dependence of Auditory Evoked Potentials and Clinical Response to Prophylactic Lithium Medication: A Replication Study Ulrich Hegerl, Hilde Wulff, Received

and Bruno Miiller-Oerlinghausen

March 31.1992; revised version received July 24, 1992; accepted September

6, 1992.

Abstract. A predictor of clinical response to prophylactic lithium treatment in affective psychoses would be of considerable importance. In a pilot study, responders to prophylactic lithium medication, as compared with nonresponders, were characterized by a steeper slope of the amplitude/stimulus-intensity function (ASF slope) of the Nl/ P2 component of the auditory evoked potential. We tried to replicate this finding in 34 stabilized outpatients with affective illness who had been treated with lithium for at least 3 years. As in the pilot study, responders were again characterized by steeper ASF slopes than nonresponders. Since a steep ASF slope seems to indicate low central serotonergic function, it is speculated that a steep ASF slope characterizes those patients with a serotonin deficit who respond to serotonin agonists like lithium. Key Words. Affective disorder, electrophysiology, reducing, major depression, average evoked response,

serotonin, augmenting/ psychopharmacology.

It is estimated that 20-45s of patients with recurrent affective psychoses fail to respond to prophylactic lithium therapy (Greil and Schiiderle, 1986). Since the prophylactic response can be evaluated only after a long medication period, predictors of the individual response would be of great value. The intensity dependence of sensory evoked potentials (EPs) appeared to show promise in this context, because studies using the augmenting/reducing paradigm proposed by Buchsbaum and his associates (Buchsbaum and Pfefferbaum, 1971; Buchsbaum et al., 1983) found that patients whose EPs showed a pronounced intensity dependence (i.e., augmenters) were responders to acute antidepressant treatment with lithium (Buchsbaum et al., 1971, 1979; Baron et al., 1975; Nurnberger et al., 1979; Zerbi et al., 1984). Although these early studies were heterogeneous in their methodology, the results were intriguing enough to raise the question as to whether the intensity dependence of sensory EPs might also be related to the response to prophylactic lithium therapy.

Ulrich Hegerl, M.D., Privatdozent, is Lecturer in Psychiatry and Clinical Psychophysiology, and Supervising Psychiatrist at the Berlin Lithium Clinic, Department of Psychiatry, Freie Universitit Berlin. Bruno Miiller-Oerlinghausen, M.D., is Professor, Laboratory of Clinical Psychophysiology (Chief), Department of Psychiatry, Freie Universitst Berlin. Hilde Wulff is Research Fellow, Berlin Lithium Clinic. (Reprint requests to PD Dr. med. U. Hegerl, Psychiatrische Klinik der Freien Universitgt Berlin, Labor fiir Klinische Psychophysiologie, Eschenallee 3, D-1000 Berlin 19, Germany.) 0165-1781/92/%05.00

@ 1992 Elsevier Scientific

Publishers

Ireland

Ltd.

182 In a pilot study, we found that the slope of the amplitude/stimulus-intensity function (ASF slope), indicating the intensity dependence of auditory EPs (AEPs, NI /P2-component), was steeper in responders to lithium prophylaxis than in nonresponders (Hegerl et al., 1987). Subsequent studies on healthy subjects revealed that the ASF slope shows a satisfactory long-term stability and is not influenced in a relevant manner by variables like age and sex (Hegerl et al., 1988). These results indicate that the ASF slope may be clinically useful as a predictor of individual response to prophylactic lithium medication. We therefore performed a replication study to confirm the hypothesis that a steep ASF slope of the AEP Nl/P2 component is related to a favorable outcome of lithium prophylaxis.

Methods Thirty-four outpatients without affective symptoms (Beth-Rafaelsen score for mania or melancholia < 4) at the time of recording were investigated. All patients had been

Subjects.

treated continuously with lithium for at least 3 years. These patients were divided into responders and nonresponders (Table 1). Responders (n = 19) were those without any recurrence in the preceding 3 years, and nonresponders (n = 15) were those with one or more recurrences leading to hospitalization (n = 8) or psychopharmacological intervention in the preceding 3 years (n = 7). Recurrences and psychopathology are monitored by a standardized assessment protocol in the Berlin Lithium Clinic. Fifteen of the 34 patients had also participated in the pilot study. AEP recordings were performed after the patient made a routine visit to the clinic. Venous blood was drawn for the assessment of lithium plasma concentration.

Procedure.

lithium

Table 1. Clinical characteristics of the responder and the nonresponder group Nonresponders Reponders i-l

19

Men Women

15

8

3

11

12

50.9 * 14.5

51.6 + 13.8

0.75 k 0.12

0.65 -t 0.14

Neuroleptics

1

2

Antidepressants

1

1

Neuroleptics

0

1

8

5

15

10

Age (mean + SD; yr) Lithium plasma concentration Patients with additional

(mean f SD; mmolil)

medication

plus antidepressants

Others Diagnoses Bipolar depression Unipolar depression

4

2

Schizoaffective

0

3

disorder

Patients with residual affective symptomatology (score < 4) Beth-Rafaelsen Derxession

Scale Mania

2

1

1

2

183 The patient’s physician at the lithium clinic used the Beth-Rafaelsen Scale to assess affective symptomatology. Sensation thresholds to click stimuli were monitored before AEP recording to rule out deficits in auditory acuity. AEP Recording and Data Processing. Recordings were performed in a sound-attenuated room adjacent to the recording apparatus. Patients were sitting with their eyes open in a slightly reclining chair with a head rest. A black disk on the wall, 3 meters in front of the patients, was presented as a point of orientation, although no strict fixation was demanded. Binaural clicks (stimulus duration = 0.9 msec, interstimulus interval = 2.1 set) at four levels of intensity (52, 62, 72, and 82 dB HL) were presented through headphones in random order (Pathfinder II, Nicolet). We recorded with gold-plated cup electrodes from Cz, C3, and C4, and from 1 cm above the external canthus of the left eye, referenced to linked mastoid electrodes. An abrasive paste (Omni Prep) and an electrode paste (Teca) were used to keep the electrode impedances below 5 kOhm. Bandpass filters were set to 1 and 30 Hz (two-pole Butterworth filters, 12 dB/octave roll-off). We recorded from 100 msec prestimulus to 300 msec poststimulus with a sampling rate of 1280 Hz. Eighty responses per intensity were averaged. Responses to the first 30 clicks were excluded to reduce habituation effects. To suppress artifacts, all trials were automatically excluded from averaging whenever, in any of the four leads, the voltage exceeded + 50 p V. A cursor program was used to determine the N 1 component as the most negative amplitude value during the 65 to 125-msec period and the subsequent P2 component as the most positive amplitude during the 120-to 220-msec period. In addition, the individual Nl latency and Nl/P2 amplitude were measured based on the average of the four AEPs that corresponded to the four intensity levels. Fig. 1 presents examples of the AEPs of two patients. A measure of the intensity dependence of the Nl/ P2 component was obtained by fitting a straight line to the amplitude values at each intensity level using the least square technique. The slope of the line (ASF slope) thereby indicates the Nl/ P2 amplitude change due to increasing stimulus intensity.

Fig. 1. Two examples for auditory evoked potentials to the 4 stimulus intensities pat.

Fr.

dl3

I

I

1

-100-60 latency

0 -3

100

160

0

260 (ms) latency

i 100

11

11 160

L 260 (ms)

-3

The responder to lithium prophylaxis (patient He) shows a pronounced amplitude increase with increasing stimulus intensity (steep ASF slope). This is not the case for the nonresponder (patient Fr).

184 The median slope was used as a further measure of the intensity dependence of the N I/ P2 component. The median slope was calculated from the slopes of all possible connections between the four amplitude values corresponding to the four intensities (n = 6, 52-62, 52-72, 52-82, 62-72, 62-82, and 72-82 dB). In another study of 33 healthy subjects, retested after 3 weeks, a test-retest reliability of 0.77 (Cz) was found for the median slope and of 0.74 for the ASF slope (Hegerl, 1992). Recording and processing of AEP data were performed outside the lithium clinic by a person (H.W.) who was not involved in the treatment of the patients and who was largely unaware of the responder/ nonresponder status of the patients.

Results Table 1 presents clinical characteristics of the responders and nonresponders. Lithium plasma concentrations were significantly higher in responders (t test: p < 0.05). No significant differences in the artifact rate were found between responders and nonresponders (responders: Ill+ SD 86; nonresponders: 169 + SD 143; 2 test: p = 0.17). As hypothesized, responders showed significantly steeper ASF slopes (i.e., augmenting) than nonresponders (Fig. 2, Table 2); this effect was confirmed by analysis of variance with the repeated measures factor “lead” (Cz, C3, C4) and the group factor “response” (F = 4.72; df = 1, 32; p < 0.05). Fig. 3 presents the individual ASF slopes (Cz) for responders and nonresponders. When the median slope was used, steeper slopes were again found for the responders (F = 5.67; df = 1, 32; p < 0.05, Table 2). When only the 19 patients who had not participated in the pilot study 2 years ago were considered, responders (n = 13) were again characterized by steeper slopes than nonresponders (n = 6, Table 3). This difference

Fig. 2. Amplitude/stimulus intensity function of responders and nonresponders to prophylactic lithium treatment 14.0

C3

13.0

2

c4

12.0

1:;;;

J

/

6.0,.

5.0-

I 52

62

72

I 62

1 52 rtimulur

More amplitude nonresponders.

increase with increasing

62

Intsnrlty

12

(da

stimulus intensity is found in

I

I

62

52

8 62

12

62

HL)’

responders

(steeper ASF slopes) than in

185

could not be confirmed by analysis of variance in this smaller subgroup (ASF slope: F= 2.36; df = I, 17;~ = 0.14; median-slope: Fr2.94; df= 1, 17;p=O.10). In the pilot study, shorter Nl latencies had been found for responders than for Table 2. ASF slopes (,v V/10 dB), median slopes (,~V/10 dB), and Nl latency (msec) of responders (n = 19) and nonresponders (n = 15) Nonreswnders

Resmnders ASF

slope

2.1 It 1.1

1.4 f

1.1

c3

1.4 +

0.7

0.8 +

0.8

c4

1.6 +

0.7

l.Of

0.7 1.0

Cz

Median slope Cz

2.1 f

1.1

1.3f

c3

1.4 +

0.7

0.8 +

0.9

c4

1.5 +

0.7

l.Of

0.8 8.1

Nl latency

Cz

91.2 +

8.6

87.0 +

c3

91.9 *

9.2

89.8 + 10.0

c4

90.1 + 10.9

92.5 * 10.7

Analysis of variance F=4.72; df= 1,32;p
F=5.67;df=l,32;~<0.05

NS

function. Data are presented as mean f SD.

Note. ASF = amplitude/stimulus-intensity

Fig. 3. Individual ASF slopes of responders and nonresponders

0

0

ooo

0

0.0

0

1: IO 0

-1.0

Res"TFB I

ASF = amplitude/stimulus-intensity nonresponders.

function.

MR =

.Ncmyy;den zm

mean

value

for responders.

MNR

=

mean

value

for

186

Table 3. ASF slopes (,ttV/lO dB) (excluding

patients

of who had participated

Responders (n= 13)

Nonresponders (n=6)

CZ

2.3 + 1.1

1.6 i 0.9

c3

1.4* 0.7

1.0 f 0.5

c4

1.6+ 0.8

1.1zt0.6

N&e. ASF = amplitude/stimulus-intensity

responders and nonresponders in the pilot study) Analysis of variance F=2.36;df=1,17;~=0.14

function. Data are presented as mean * SD.

nonresponders in an exploratory analysis. That difference was not replicated in this study (Table 2). Furthermore, Nl/ P2 amplitudes were not significantly different between responders and nonresponders (F = 1.69; df = 1, 32; p = 0.20).

Discussion Our findings confirm the hypothesis that a steep ASF slope is related to favorable response to prophylactic lithium medication. This relationship between the intensity dependence of AEP and lithium response could also be demonstrated when the median slope was used instead of the ASF slope. The median slope may therefore be an alternative measure to the ASF slope; the median slope has the advantage of not needing to assume a linear relationship between amplitudes and stimulus intensities (in decibels) (Connolly and Gruzelier, 1982). When only those patients who had not participated in the pilot study were considered, steeper ASF slopes were again found for responders. The difference, however, was not significant in this smaller subgroup. It is unlikely that the differences in ASF slope between lithium responders and nonresponders are due to effects of covariables. The higher lithium plasma concentrations found in the responder group cannot explain the steeper ASF slopes in this group, because lithium treatment has been reported in the literature to have no effect or a flattening effect on ASF slopes (Buchsbaum et al., 1971, 1977; Hubbard et al., 1980; Hegerl et al., 1990). The artifact rate may influence the ASF slope, because there is evidence that a longer recording duration can result in a flattening of the ASF slope in some subjects (Hegerl et al., 1989). Responders have a lower artifact rate than nonresponders. However, this difference is not significant and, therefore, does not seem to be an explanation for our results. The findings of this study lend support to the assumption that a steep ASF slope is related to a favorable outcome of lithium prophylaxis and may prove useful to clinicians through its provision of some information about the response probability for the individual patient. The predictive utility of the ASF slope is presently being assessed in a prospective study. Since a specific predictor of nonresponse would be especially helpful for the clinician, an ASF slope of 0.8 PI’/ 10 dB was chosen as the cutoff point for this prospective study. Six of the seven patients with ASF slopes below this cutoff point have been nonresponders to lithium prophylaxis (Fig. 3). Apart from its predictive value, the replicated finding of a relationship between ASF slope and lithium response is of interest for theoretical reasons. Lithium has serotonin-agonistic effects, probably mainly at the presynaptic level (Miiller-

187 Oerlinghausen, 1985; Price et al., 1990) that may be related to its clinical effects. Hence, it is of interest that independent lines of evidence suggest that the intensity of central serotonergic dependence of sensory EPs may be an indicator neurotransmission: 0 Intraindividual changes of blood serotonin concentration after a fluvoxamine test dose as well as during phototherapy have been found to be negatively correlated to corresponding changes in the ASF slope in depressed patients (AEP, Nl/P2component; Hegerl et al., 1991). 0 A steep ASF slope of the AEP has been related to “action oriented” personality traits like sensation seeking and impulsiveness (Mullins and Lukas, 1984; Orlebecke et al., 1984; Lukas and Mullins, 1985; Zuckerman et al., 1988; Hegerl et al., 1989; negative findings by Stenberg et al., 1988; Lolas et al., 1989) which are themselves associated with low serotonergic function (Linnoila et al., 1983; Schalling et al., 1984; Brown and Linnoila, 1990). l During sleep, when there is a decrease in the firing rate of serotonergic neurons in the dorsal raphe nuclei, steeper ASF slopes (AEP, P2 component) have been observed (Buchsbaum et al., 1975). These results suggest that the serotonergic input to the auditory cortex may modulate the intensity dependence of the Nl /PZcomponent. In line with this view is the finding that the highest concentrations of cortical serotonin and the highest synthesis rates have consistently been found in the primary sensory cortex, especially in the primary auditory cortex (Brown et al., 1979; Takeuchi and Sano, 1983; Azmitia and Gannon, 1986; Lewis et al., 1986; Campbell et al., 1987; Diop et al., 1987; Papadopoulos and Parnavelas, 1991), the region where the NI / P2 component is mainly generated (Pantev et al., 1988; Scherg et al., 1989; Pineda et al., 1991; Scherg, 1991). Serotonergic function also seems to be related to the intensity dependence of EPs in the visual modality (visual EP) as suggested by negative correlations between levels of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in cerebrospinal fluid and ASF slopes (Gottfries et al., 1976; Knorringet al., 1980; Knorring and Perris, 1981), as well as by the effects of serotonin agonists on the ASF slopes (Knorring et al., 1980; Knorring, 1982). After lithium medication, a flattening of the ASF slope (visual EP) has been observed in patients with affective symptoms (Buchsbaum et al., 1971; Hubbard et al., 1980). Buchsbaum et al. (1971) noted that this effect was most evident after the second week of lithium therapy. When lithium effects were assessed after fewer than 2 weeks of medication, no flattening of the ASF slope was observed in patients (visual EP: Buchsbaum et al., 1977) and healthy subjects (AEP: Hegerl et al., 1990; Hegerl, 1992). These findings, although they derive in part from methodologically heterogeneous studies, lend support to the hypothesis that a steep ASF slope of the sensory EP characterizes patients with low central serotonergic activity. The arguments for this hypothesis are presented in more detail elsewhere (Hegerl and Juckel, in press). With this hypothesis in mind, it is not surprising that patients with such a neurophysiological characteristic are responders not only to prophylactic lithium medication as found in our studies, but also to antidepressant lithium medication

188

(visual EP: Buchsbaurn et al., 1971; Zerbi et al., 1984; Baron et al., 1975; Nurnberger et al., 1979; somatosensory EP: Buchsbaum et al., 1979) and to other serotonin agonists such as fenfluramine (AEP: Bruneau et al., 1989). Acknowledgment.

This research was supported Forschungsgemeinschaft” (DFG, MU 477/6-l).

by a grant

from

the

“Deutsche

References Azmitia, E.C., and Gannon, P.J. The primate serotonergic system: A review of human and animal studies and a report on Macacafascicularis. Advances in Neurology, 43:407-468, 1986. Baron, M.; Gershon, E.R.; Rudy, V.; Jonas, W.; and Buchsbaum, M.S. Lithium carbonate response in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32: 1107-I 111, 1975. Brown, G.L., and Linnoila, M.I. CSF serotonin metabolite (5HIAA) studies in depression, impulsivity, and violence. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 5 1(Suppl. 4) 3 l-41, 1990. Brown, R.M.; Crane, A.M.; and Goldman, P.S. Regional distribution of monoamines in the cerebral cortex and subcortical structures of the rhesus monkey: Concentrations and in vivo synthesis rates. Brain Research, 168:133-150, 1979. Bruneau, N.; Barthelemy, C.; Roux, S.; Jouve, J.; and Lelord, G. Auditory evoked potential modifications according to clinical and biochemical responsiveness to fenfluramine treatment in children with autistic behavior. Neuropsychobiology, 21:48-52, 1989. Buchsbaum, MS.; Gillin, J.C.; and Pfefferbaum, A. Effect of sleep stage and stimulus intensity on auditory average evoked resposes. Psychophysiology, 12:707-712, 1975. Buchsbaum, M.S.; Goodwin, F.; Murphy, D.; and Borge, G. AER in affective disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry,

128:19-25, 1971.

Buchsbaum, M.S.; Haier, R.J.; and Johnson, J. Augmenting and reducing: Individual differences in EPs. In: Gale, A., and Edwards, J.A., eds. Physiological Correlates of Human Behaviour. Vol. III. London: Academic Press, 1983. pp. 117-138. Buchsbaum, M.S.; Lavine, R.; Davis, G.; Goodwin, F.; Murphy, D.; and Post, R. Effects of lithium on somatosensory EPs and prediction of clinical response in patients with affective illness. In: Cooper, T.; Gershon, S.; Kline, N.; and Schou, M., eds. Lithium-Controversies and Unresolved Issues. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica, 1979. pp. 685-702. Buchsbaum, M.S., and Pfefferbaum, A. Individual differences in stimulus intensity response. Psychophysiology, 8:600-611, 197 1. Buchsbaum, M.S.; van Kammen, D.; and Murphy, D. Individual differences in average evoked responses to d- and I-amphetamine with and without lithium carbonate in depressed patients. Psychopharmacology, 51:129-135, 1977. Campbell, M.J.; Lewis, D.A.; Foote, S.L.; and Morrison, J.H. Distribution of cholin tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoacetyltransferase-, serotonin-, dopamine-/J-hydroxylase-, reactive fibers in monkey primary auditory cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 261:209-220,

1987.

Connolly, J.F., and Gruzelier, J.H. Amplitude and latency changes in the visual evoked potential to different stimulus intensities. Psychophysiology, 19:599-608, 1982. Diop, L.; Brikre, R.; Grondin, L.; and Reader, T.A. Adrenergic receptor and catecholamine distribution in rat cerebral cortex: Binding studies with (sH)prazosin, (3H)idaxoxan and (3H)dihydroalprenolol. Brain Research, 402:403-408, 1987. Gottfries, C.; von Knorring, L.; and Perris, C. Neurophysiological measures related to levels of 5-HIAA, HVA, and tryptophan in CSF of psychiatric patients. Neuropsychobiology, 2:1-8, 1976.

Greil, W., and Schiiderle, M. Rezidivprophylaxe affektiver Psychosen mit Lithium. In: Risiken, Miiller-Oerlinghausen, B., and Greil, W., eds. Die Lithiumtherapie-Nutzen, Alternativen. Berlin: Springer, 1986. pp. 138-163. Hegerl, U. Ereigniskorrelierte Potentiale als Pradiktoren klinischer Verlaufsaspekte in der Psychiatric. Habilitatiosschrift, vorgelegt bei der Freien Universitat Berlin, 1992.

189 Hegerl, U.; Herrmann, W.M.; Ulrich, G.; and Miiller-Oerlinghausen, B. Effects of lithium on auditory EPs in healthy subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 27:555-X0, 1990. Hegerl, U., and Juckel, G. Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials as an indicator of central serotonergic neurotransmission-Arguments for a new hypothesis. Biological Psychiatry, in press. Hegerl, U.; Juckel, G.; Rao, M.-L.; and Miiller-Oerlinghausen, B. Blood serotonin and auditory EPs under fluvoxamine challenge and phototherapy. In: Cassano, G.B., and Akiskal, H.S., eds. Serotonin-rebated Psychiatric Syndromes: Clinical and Therapeutic Links. New York: Royal Society of Medicine Services Limited, 1991. pp. 163-170. Hegerl, U.; Prochno, I.; Ulrich, G.; and Miiller-Oerlinghausen, B. Are auditory EPs suitable for predicting the response to lithium prophylaxis? A study on the effects of repeated measurement, age, gender, and personality on the amplitude/stimulus intensity function in healthy volunteers. Pharmacopsychiatry, 21:329-330, 1988. Hegerl, U.; Prochno, I.; Ulrich, G.; and Miiller-Oerlinghausen, B. Sensation seeking and auditory EPs. Biological Psychiatry, 25:179-190, 1989. Hegerl, U.; Ulrich, G.; and Miiller-Oerlinghausen, B. Auditory evoked potentials and response to lithium prophylaxis. Pharmacopsychiatry, 20:213-216, 1987. Hubbard, R.; Judd, L.; Huey, L.; Kripke, D.; Janowsky, D.; and Lewis, A. Visual cortical EPs in alcoholics and normals maintained on lithium carbonate: Augmentation and reduction phenomena. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 126573-577, 1980. Knorring, L. von. Effect of imipramine and zimelidine on the augmenting/reducing responce of visual EPs in healthy volunteers. Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 9:81-86, 1982. Knorring, L. von; Johansson, F.; and Almay, B. Augmenting/reducing response in visual EPs in patients with chronic pain syndromes. Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 4:55-62, 1980. Knorring, L. von, and Perris, C. Biochemistry of the augmenting/reducing response in visual EPs. Neuropsychobiology, 7: 1-8, 198 1. Lewis, D.A.; Campbell, M.J.; Foote, S.L.; and Morrison, J.H. The monoaminergic innervation of primate neocortex. Human Neurobiology, 5: 18 1-188, 1986. Linnoila, M.; Virkunnen, M.; Scheinin, M.; Nuutila, A.; Rimon, R.; and Goodwin, F.K. Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. Life Sciences, 33:2609-2614, 1983. Lolas, F.; Camposano, S.; and Etcheberrigaray, R. Augmenting/reducing and personality: A psychometric and evoked potential study in a Chilean sample. Personality and Individual D$ferences, IO:1173-I 176, 1989. Lukas, J.H., and Mullins, L.F. Auditory augmenters are sensation seekers and perform better under high work-loads. (Abstract) Psychophysiology, 22:580-581, 1985. Miiller-Oerlinghausen, B. Lithium long-term treatment-Does it act via serotonin? Pharmacopsychiatry, 18:214-217, 1985. Mullins, L.F., and Lukas, J.H. Auditory augmenters are sensation seekers-if they attend the stimuli. (Abstract) Psychophysiology, 21:589, 1984. Nurnberger, J.; Gershon, E.; Murphy, D.; Buchsbaum, M.S.; Goodwin, F.; Post, R.; Lake, C.; Guroff, J.; and McGuiness, M. Biological and clinical predictors of lithium response in depression. In: Cooper, T.; Gershon, S.; Kline, N.; and Schou, M., eds. LithiumControversies and Unresolved Issues. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica, 1979. pp. 241-256. Orlebeke, J.F.; Kok, A.; and Zeillemaker, C.W. Augmenting-reducing (disinhibition) and the processing of auditory stimulus intensity. (Abstract) Psychophysiology, 21:591, 1984. Pantev, C.; Hoke, M.; Lehnertz, K.; Lhtkenhgner, B.; Anogianakis, G.; and Wittkowski, W. Tonotopic organization of the human auditory cortex revealed by transient auditory evoked magnetic fields. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 69: 160-170, 1988. Papadopoulos, G.C., and Parnavelas, J.G. Monoamine systems in the cerebral cortex: Evidence for anatomical specifity. Progress in Neurobiology, 36: 195-200, 199 1.

190 Pineda, J.A.; Holmes, T.C.; and Foote, S.L. Intensity-amplitude relationships in monkey event-related potentials: Parallels to human augmenting-reducing responses. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78:4X-465, 1991. Price, L.H.; Charney, D.S.; Delgado, P.L.; and Heninger, G.R. Lithium and serotonin function: Implications for the serotonin hypothesis of depression. Psychopharmacology, 100:3-12, 1990. Schalling, D.; .&berg, M.; Edman, G.; and Levander, S. Impulsivity, nonconformity, and sensation to biological markers for vulnerability. Clinical seeking as related Neuropharmacology, 7(Suppl. 1):746-747, 1984. Scherg, M. Akustisch evozierte Potentiale-Grundiagen, Entstehungsmechanismen. Quellenmodell. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer, 199 1. Scherg, M.; Vajsar, J.; and Picton, T.W. A source analysis of the late human auditory EPs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1:336-355, 1989. Stenberg, G.; Rosen, I.; and Risberg, J. Personality and augmenting/ reducing in visual and auditory EPs. Personality and Individual Differences, 9571-579, 1988. Takeuchi, Y., and Sano, Y. Immunohistochemical demonstration of serotonin nerve fibers in the neocortex of the monkey (Macacafuscata). Anatomy and Embryology, 166:155-168, 1983. Zerbi, F.; Bezzi, G.; Tosca, P.; Fenoglio, L.; Romani, A.; and Spagliardi, R. Potenziali evocati visivi: Un possible marker neurofisiologico predittivita della resposta ai sali di litio. Rivista Sperimentale Freniatria e Medicina Legale delle Alienazioni Mentali, 108: 1718-1725, 1984. Zuckerman, M.; Simons, R.F.; and Como, P.G. Sensation seeking and stimulus intensity as modulators of cortical, cardiovascular and electrodermal response: A cross-modality study. Personality and Individual DifSerences, 9:361-372, 1988.