Re: Examining Penile Sensitivity in Neonatally Circumcised and Intact Men Using Quantitative Sensory Testing

Re: Examining Penile Sensitivity in Neonatally Circumcised and Intact Men Using Quantitative Sensory Testing

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 ...

228KB Sizes 2 Downloads 68 Views

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57

Re: Examining Penile Sensitivity in Neonatally Circumcised and Intact Men Using Quantitative Sensory Testing J. A. Bossio, C. F. Pukall and S. S. Steele J Urol 2016; 195: 1848e1853.

To the Editor: Bossio et al assessed penile sensitivity in circumcised and intact males using quantitative sensory measures, and concluded that infant circumcision likely entails only “minimal long-term implications for penile sensitivity.” However, this conclusion does not follow logically from their findings. Despite a small sample size (62 sexually nondysfunctional males 18 to 37 years old, of whom 30 were neonatally circumcised and 32 were intact), the study confirmed findings from a prior investigation by Sorrells et al, which revealed significantly lower tactile thresholds (ie greater tactile sensitivity) in the foreskin compared to other examined penile sites (the glans and 2 shaft positions).1 Additionally the current study found significantly greater warmth sensitivity at the foreskin than at the glans (p ¼ 0.02). Finally, the authors assessed thresholds for tactile pain and heat pain, and failed to find any significant evidence that the foreskin is more or less pain sensitive than other parts of the penis. These results suggest that 1) the foreskin is more sensitive to gentle touch than any other tested penile site, replicating prior research,1 2) the foreskin may be more sensitive to subtle variations in temperature, at least compared to the glans, and 3) the foreskin appears to be no more or less sensitive to potentially uncomfortable tactile or temperature induced stimuli leading to sensations of penile pain. From these observations the logical conclusion would be that neonatal circumcision appears to be associated with reduced sensitivity in the adult penis. It is noteworthy that the findings of Bossio et al of greater foreskin sensitivity to tactile and temperature related stimuli emerged despite the fact that the examined outer cutaneous part of the foreskin may not even be the most sensitive part of this complex, double layered structure.1 Figure 2 in the article combines individual sensitivity threshold scores from the 30 circumcised and 32 intact males. To better understand the relevance and meaning of the study findings, I kindly urge the authors to repeat their analysis and provide a supplementary figure, in which they divide the results depicted in figure 2 into 2 separate panels (1 for intact and 1 for circumcised males), illustrating each of the 4 sensory threshold graphs as demonstrated for the combined group of 62 males in this figure. An additional simplified 4-graph panel for the 32 intact males may help readers to judge whether and to what extent the foreskin is more sensitive than other parts of the intact penis. Due to the apparent lack of statistically significant differences in sensitivity among the various nonforeskin penile sites, I suggest that Bossio et al recombine the original 5 tested anatomical sites, ie the foreskin, other penile sites (including the glans and the 2 penile shaft sites) and the forearm, to gain statistical power. I also would be interested to see the associated p values provided in a manner similar to that used in figure 2. Respectfully, Morten Frisch Center for Sexology Research Department of Clinical Medicine Aalborg University Aalborg, Denmark

0022-5347/16/1966-0001/0 THE JOURNAL OF UROLOGY® Ó 2016 by AMERICAN UROLOGICAL ASSOCIATION EDUCATION

AND

RESEARCH, INC.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.juro.2016.05.127 Vol. 196, 1-5, December 2016 Printed in U.S.A.

www.jurology.com Dochead: Letters to the Editor/Errata

j

1

COR 5.4.0 DTD  JURO13850_proof  13 September 2016  7:51 pm  EO: JU-16-949

58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114

LETTERS TO THE EDITOR/ERRATA

2

115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171

To the Editor: In a study fraught with methodological problems Bossio et al conclude that “circumcision is not associated with changes in penile sensitivity” and believe they have provided “preliminary evidence to suggest that the foreskin is not the most sensitive part of the penis.” However, neither of these assertions is supported by their data. Although nowhere explicitly stated in the article, we assume that all penile sensitivity tests were performed in penises in the flaccid state. Such a testing condition can only provide an indirect hint as to what would occur during sexual arousal, which is the relevant situation to consider when studying penile sensitivity in this context. For unclear reasons the authors tested only a single site on the foreskinda location on the dorsal unretracted external skin, somewhat proximal to the more sensitive tip of the penisdthat previous research has already indicated may be less sensitive than other parts of the foreskin.1 Specifically if the authors wanted to determine the sensory thresholds of the foreskin of intact men compared to other penile sites, why would they not test its most sensitive point, or at least include a site representing the (widely considered to be) more sensitive transitional or internal surfaces of the foreskin that become exposed when the structure is pulled back?1 Due to differences in erection mechanics in the intact and circumcised penis, the tested sites on the penile shaft may not be comparable between the 2 groups during sexual activity. The penile shaft skin of circumcised males does not move back and forth a great deal during sex, so the penile shaft sites tested by Bossio et al in flaccid circumcised males may serve as a reasonable proxy for what those same subjects will experience while erect but not for their intact counterparts. During intercourse a considerable part of the penile shaft of intact males is covered intermittently by the everted mucosal portion of the foreskin, which rolls back and forth over the glans. During penetration the skin that covered the penile shaft in the flaccid state moves proximally closer to the pubis. Consequently the penile shaft skin measurements by Bossio et al are unlikely to represent analogous penile sites in sexually aroused intact and circumcised males. To provide a more meaningful comparison of penile shaft sensitivities in intact and circumcised males that might serve as a plausible proxy for what occurs in the erect state, penile shaft sensitivity tests in the flaccid state should have been carried out with the foreskin pulled back (as typically will be the case during sexual arousal) in intact males. Even with its underpowered sample and inadequate choice of cutaneous testing sites the conclusion of the authors that the foreskin is not the most sensitive part of the penis is certainly puzzling in light of figure 2, part A in the article, which shows the foreskin to be significantly more sensitive than any other tested site. The inference that surgical removal of this most sensitive penile segment would not decrease penile sensitivity seems logically and anatomically incoherent. Repectfully, Alexandre T. Rotta Department of Pediatrics Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine Cleveland, Ohio

To the Editor: We congratulate the authors for their new findings leading them to dismiss the claim by Sorrells et al1 that the foreskin is the most sensitive part of the penis. Sorrells et al, whose study had serious flaws,2 compared fine touch for 8 sites on the foreskin and frenulum with the ventral scar of circumcised men and concluded that the orifice rim of the foreskin was significantly more sensitive. However, after Bonferroni correction their p value became nonsignificant. In a defense of Sorrells et al Van Howe3 side stepped the issue of Bonferroni correction of those data by referring instead to the mixed model used by Sorrells et al, ignoring the fact that the critique did not apply Bonferroni correction to the mixed model. The mixed model adjusted for underwear preferences and Hispanic race, neither of which would seem intuitively to have any obvious effect on penile sensitivity. A danger of sophisticated models is that adjustments for random characteristics can, by trial and error, generate a “desired” result. Unlike the current study, Sorrells et al1 failed to compare the same anatomical sites on the circumcised and uncircumcised penis. After using the data of Sorrells et al to compare the same anatomical sites on the circumcised and uncircumcised penis Waskett and Morris found no Dochead: Letters to the Editor/Errata

COR 5.4.0 DTD  JURO13850_proof  13 September 2016  7:51 pm  EO: JU-16-949

172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228

LETTERS TO THE EDITOR/ERRATA

229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285

3

significant difference.2 Bossio et al observed that the foreskin was more sensitive to touch (a finding that would have remained significant after Bonferroni correction). We suggest that the sensitivity of the foreskin to fine touch (which activates Ab, large diameter, myelinated nerve fibers) might have served as an “early warning system” in our naked upright forebears from biting insects and parasites, while protecting the glans. One important methodological difference was that Bossio et al used modified von Frey filaments, which provide a range of forces to the skin to find the force at which the subject reacts because the sensation is painful. In contrast, Sorrells et al1 measured only fine touch sensitivity using Semmes-Weinstein monofilament Touch-TestÒ sensory evaluators, which exert 10 gm force when bowed into a C shape against the skin for 1 second. Sensitivity to vibration (not tested in the current study or by Sorrells et al1) is the only stimulus known to correlate with sexual response sensitivity. However, vibrational sensitivity appears similar in circumcised and uncircumcised men. A detailed systematic review of all studies of histological correlates of sexual sensation concluded that the glans, not the foreskin, is involved in sexual sensation.4 Bossio et al cite a study implicating C-fibers (activated by thermal stimuli and punctate pain) in erotic sensation and sexual arousal.5 Similar unmyelinated free nerve endings predominate in the glans, not the foreskin.4 A large randomized controlled trial (not cited by Bossio et al) that surveyed young men for up to 18 months following circumcision found, on average, enhanced sensitivity and sexual pleasure.6 These findings should help allay concerns that appear in the accompanying editorial comment by Mathews. This report provides physiological data consistent with previous strong studies. An implication is that the foreskin does not contribute significantly to sexual pleasure. Respectfully, Brian J. Morris Department of Physiology University of Sydney School of Medical Sciences Sydney, Australia e-mail: [email protected]

and John N. Krieger Section of Urology University of Washington School of Medicine Seattle, Washington

To the Editor: This study examining penile sensitivity left with us several concerns. The report fails adequately to describe the main outcomes of the study, namely measurements broken down by circumcision status. Instead of average scores and standard deviations, the reader is provided with p values, statements about failing to find a statistically significant difference and a figure that collapses the data from the 2 groups into 1 group. Of interest is whether fine touch sensitivity was reduced in the glans in the flaccid penis of circumcised men, as has been reported in 3 previous studies.1,7,8 However, these results were not provided. Instead, the authors provide what could be characterized as pilot data, along with the number of participants needed for a more definitive study, while failing to report the results that are needed to design such a study. Several of the conclusions of Bossio et al do not follow from their findings. Although several of their tests were underpowered, and the maximum participant age was only 37 years (mean 24.2), the authors make sweeping generalizations regarding long-term implications of circumcision on penile sensitivity. Similarly they state that “this study challenges past research suggesting that the foreskin is the most sensitive part of the penis,” while at the same time reporting that the “foreskin of intact men was more sensitive to tactile stimulation.” Finally, the authors conclude that they “failed to consistently replicate the findings by Sorrells et al across stimuli” when they did, in fact, replicate our findings1 along the only dimension that was consistentdand hence even potentially replicabledbetween the 2 studies, namely assessment of fine touch thresholds. The authors struggle to explain some of their results but the histology may help.9 The glans is innervated mainly by free nerve endings, which primarily sense deep pressure and pain, so it is Dochead: Letters to the Editor/Errata

COR 5.4.0 DTD  JURO13850_proof  13 September 2016  7:51 pm  EO: JU-16-949

286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342

LETTERS TO THE EDITOR/ERRATA

4

343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399

not surprising that the glans was more sensitive to pain. By contrast, the foreskin has a paucity of free nerve endings and is primarily innervated by fine touch neuroreceptors, so it was comparatively less sensitive to pain. On a positive note, it is heartening that the term “intact” has replaced “uncircumcised,” which is considered by many to be a pejorative term.10 Respectfully, Robert S. Van Howe, Morris L. Sorrells, James L. Snyder, Mark D. Reiss and Marilyn F. Milos Department of Pediatrics Central Michigan University College of Medicine Saginaw, Michigan

Reply by Authors: Based on a careful review of these letters, we believe that our conclusions have been misconstrued in different ways by the authors (and others, as apparent in media reports and on social media). We will attempt to clarify what our article indicates, what it does not indicate and how the responses to it highlight the need to improve the empirical rigor of research on the impact of circumcision on the sexual lives of men and their partners. Despite the widespread global practice of circumcision,11 as well as the public beliefs about the procedure, there is limited objective peer-reviewed research assessing the sexual correlates of circumcision. The purpose of our study was to use objective measures to assess penile sensitivity across circumcision status (intact, circumcised). We examined sensitivity on certain penile areas, as well as on the forearm, with a focus on the glans penis and the foreskin, using touch (punctate or fine touch pressure, pain) and heat (warmth detection, heat pain). The stimulus modalities we used are expected to activate penile nerve fibers more likely associated with sexual pleasure than measuring fine touch pressure thresholds alone.1,4,5 Statistical analyses revealed that the sensory thresholds of intact and circumcised men were remarkably similardhence, the data were collapsed across the 2 groups. Although presented in graphic format in the full article, we have included a breakdown of descriptive statistics in this response, as requested by Van Howe ½T1 et al (see table). To reiterate the findings of the study, the foreskin was observed to be most sensitive to fine touch pressure thresholds. Indeed, this finding replicated the results reported by Sorrells et al.1 However, fine touch pressure, which was only 1 of 4 stimulus modalities assessed, activates nerve fibers that are likely less relevant for sexual pleasure than fibers activated by the other stimuli used in this study (stimuli that did not exhibit significant between group differences).4,5 Therefore, we maintain that we “failed to consistently replicate the findings by Sorrells et al across stimuli” (emphasis added). We also urge caution in the overinterpretation of this result (that the fine touch pressure threshold at the foreskin was significantly less than in other areas). This finding alone does not prove that circumcision reduces penile sensitivity, and it also does not prove that circumcision has no impact on the sexual lives of men. Insisting that this finding supports either the pro or anti-circumcision “camp” is not warranted, as it does not take into account the other study findings and ignores the limitations (eg small sample size). Instead, we consider the outcome of this study an indication of the need for further examination.

Descriptive statistics for repeated measures ANOVA for quantitative sensory testing of sensation and pain thresholds Threshold Mean  SD tactile (gm): Circumcised Intact Mean  SD pain (gm): Circumcised Intact Mean  SD warmth detection (C): Circumcised Intact Mean  SD heat pain (C): Circumcised Intact

Forearm

Glans Penis

Midline Shaft

Proximal to Midline Shaft

Foreskin

0.18  0.16 0.14  0.12

0.26  0.23 0.28  0.26

0.31  0.45 0.29  0.24

0.35  0.52 0.38  0.28

– 0.13  0.11

8.08  4.97 9.25  3.85

5.78  4.06 6.26  3.72

6.77  4.54 8.34  4.32

7.72  4.32 9.12  3.65

– 7.34  4.39

40.08  4.03 39.92  3.98

40.58  3.68 41.03  3.82

39.50  2.09 40.71  2.83

40.00  2.96 40.54  3.28

– 38.82  3.71

47.28  0.78 46.75  1.51

45.51  2.93 45.61  2.94

45.18  2.35 45.95  2.12

46.30  1.82 45.85  2.40

– 44.90  3.48

Dochead: Letters to the Editor/Errata

COR 5.4.0 DTD  JURO13850_proof  13 September 2016  7:51 pm  EO: JU-16-949

400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456

LETTERS TO THE EDITOR/ERRATA

457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513

5

In light of the misinterpretations of our findings we believe it necessary to further clarify what the results of this particular study do not indicate. The results of this series are not to be taken as the definitive answer concerning the circumcision debate. As we discuss in the article, the results do not address the role of the foreskin during sexual activity, nor was the study, as Rotta suggests, an exploration of the biomechanics of the foreskin during intercourse (an interesting question, indeed, but one that would be incredibly difficult to measuredand one that we did not examine). This study was not an exploration of the direct impact of circumcision status on sexual function or on the experience of sexual pleasure. Furthermore, and importantly, we do not state whether the findings offer support to either side of the circumcision debate, but instead highlight the need for more empirically rigorous research. The strong reactions to this self-described preliminary study highlight the need for more research and serve as a “call to arms” for researchers interested in examining the sexual correlates of circumcision. This body of research is plagued by weak study design, such as the inclusion of nonrandom samples, equating outcomes of adult and neonatal circumcision without evidence to suggest that the 2 are comparable, failure to control for participant expectations of study outcomes and reliance on self-report to the exclusion of objective measures.12 These shortcomings represent a serious problem in this contentious field because they allow room for participant and author bias. One does not have to search far for these biases in the circumcision literature, such as frequent references to nonpeer-reviewed articles and author involvement in anti or pro-circumcision advocacy groups. Are we, as scientists, not responsible for stepping back from questions about which we have a personal stake in the outcome? Research in this area should be conducted by those who are not personally invested in the circumcision debate. That was our initial goal. But following the severe reactions we have received, we wonder about who would want to continue (or even start) working on this topic without biased motivations. However, that is not what science is about. As comedian John Oliver stated in a recent commentary on the current state of science, “In science you don’t just get to cherry pick the parts that justify what you were going to do anyway.” We will continue to report what the data show, regardless of whether that reflects the popular vote, and hope that others in this field do the same. 1. Sorrells ML, Snyder JL, Reiss MD et al: Fine-touch pressure thresholds in the adult penis. BJU Int 2007; 99: 864.

7. Payne K, Thaler L, Kukkonen T et al: Sensation and sexual arousal in circumcised and uncircumcised men. J Sex Med 2007; 4: 667.

2. Waskett JH and Morris BJ: Fine-touch pressure thresholds in the adult penis. BJU Int 2007; 99: 1551.

8. Bleustein CB, Fogarty JD, Eckholdt H et al: Effect of neonatal circumcision on penile neurologic sensation. Urology 2005; 65: 773.

3. Van Howe RS: Math is Your Friend: A Consumer’s Primer to Understanding Epidemiology. Available at http://www.academia. edu/13843697/Math_is_Your_Friend_A_Consumer_s_Primer_to_ Understanding_Epidemiology. Accessed May 9, 2016. 4. Cox G, Krieger JN and Morris BJ: Histological correlates of penile sexual sensation: does circumcision make a difference? Sex Med 2015; 3: 76. 5. J€onsson EH, Backlund Wasling H, Wagnbeck V et al: Unmyelinated tactile cutaneous nerves signal erotic sensations. J Sex Med 2015; 12: 1338. 6. Krieger JN, Mehta SD, Bailey RC et al: Adult male circumcision: effects on sexual function and sexual satisfaction in Kisumu, Kenya. J Sex Med 2008; 5: 2610.

Dochead: Letters to the Editor/Errata

9. Cold CJ and Taylor JR: The prepuce. BJU Int, suppl., 1999; 83: 34. 10. Wallace WG: An undeniable need for change: the case for redefining human penis types: intact, circumcised, and uncircumcised (all three forms exist and all are different). Clin Anat 2015; 28: 563. 11. Male Circumcision: Global Trends and Determinants of Prevalence, Safety and Acceptability. Geneva: World Health Organization and Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS 2007. Available at http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/43749/1/9789241596169_ eng.pdf. Accessed July 6, 2016. 12. Bossio JA, Pukall CF and Steele S: A review of the current state of the male circumcision literature. J Sex Med 2014; 11: 2847.

COR 5.4.0 DTD  JURO13850_proof  13 September 2016  7:51 pm  EO: JU-16-949

514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570