Speech perception differences in children with dyslexia and persistent speech delay

Speech perception differences in children with dyslexia and persistent speech delay

Accepted Manuscript Speech perception differences in children with dyslexia and persistent speech delay Kathryn L. Cabbage , Tiffany P. Hogan , Thoma...

1MB Sizes 1 Downloads 63 Views

Accepted Manuscript

Speech perception differences in children with dyslexia and persistent speech delay Kathryn L. Cabbage , Tiffany P. Hogan , Thomas D. Carrell PII: DOI: Reference:

S0167-6393(16)30137-6 10.1016/j.specom.2016.05.002 SPECOM 2378

To appear in:

Speech Communication

Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:

10 July 2015 23 May 2016 25 May 2016

Please cite this article as: Kathryn L. Cabbage , Tiffany P. Hogan , Thomas D. Carrell , Speech perception differences in children with dyslexia and persistent speech delay, Speech Communication (2016), doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2016.05.002

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Running Head: SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

1

Highlights 

The degree to which speech perception deficits overlap is unknown in children with dyslexia and children with persistent speech delay. This study used sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech to

CR IP T



determine whether these children respond similarly to stimuli preserving spectral structure. 

Amplitude comodulation improves speech perception in children with phonological



AN US

impairments, particularly for children with dyslexia.

Children with persistent speech delay have difficulty perceiving words containing their

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

erred sound.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

2

Speech perception differences in children with dyslexia and persistent speech delay Kathryn L. Cabbage1 Tiffany P. Hogan1

1

CR IP T

Thomas D. Carrell2

MGH Institute of Health Professions, 36 1st Avenue, Boston, MA 02129; Cabbage email: [email protected]

2

AN US

Hogan email: [email protected]

University of Nebraska – Lincoln, 357 Barkley Memorial Center, Lincoln, NE 68583;

PT

ED

M

Carrell email: [email protected]

AC

CE

Corresponding author: Kathryn Cabbage MGH Institute of Health Professions 36 1st Avenue Boston, MA 02129 phone: 402-309-3111 email: [email protected]

Abstract Purpose: Deficits in phonology, which are related to the organization and retrieval of speech sounds in the mental lexicon, are associated with two distinct clinical disorders, dyslexia and

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

3

persistent speech delay. Research has shown associated speech perception deficits in individuals with these disorders, but the degree to which the underlying perceptual deficits overlap is unclear. In this study, we investigated how children with dyslexia, children with persistent speech delay and their typically-developing peers differed in speech perception when listening to

CR IP T

speech stimuli that varied in non-linguistic temporal fine structure acoustic characteristics.

Method: Thirty-six children were classified into three groups (dyslexia, speech sound disorder, typically-developing; n=12 in each group) based on their reading and speech articulation skills. Novel single-word sine-wave speech stimuli was aurally presented to the children.

AN US

Results: There were no group differences between children with dyslexia and their typicallydeveloping peers, but the children with persistent speech delay had more difficulty than the other two groups. In particular, the children with persistent speech delay had difficulty recognizing

misarticulated in their own speech.

M

words with limited acoustic structure when the stimuli involved a phoneme that they

ED

Conclusions: These results suggest a specific difficulty in the perception of temporal fine

CE

PT

structure in children with persistent speech delay.

Keywords: speech perception, persistent speech delay, speech sound disorder, dyslexia,

AC

phonological processing

I.

Introduction The development of spoken and written language is reliant on an intact phonological

system. In children, phonological deficits may manifest as difficulty mapping phonemes onto

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

4

letters during the reading process, as is the case with dyslexia (Goswami, 2000). Alternatively, children may manifest phonological deficits in the form of a difficulty producing speech sounds accurately as seen in children with speech delay (Anthony et al., 2011; Rvachew, 2007). Poorly specified phonological representations have been implicated for both children with dyslexia

CR IP T

(Boada & Pennington, 2006; Elbro et al., 1998; Goswami, 2000; Swan & Goswami, 1997) and children with speech delay (Anthony et al., 2011; Edwards, Fourakis, Beckman, & Fox, 1999; Rvachew et al., 2003; Sutherland & Gillon, 2005; Sutherland & Gillon, 2007). Phonological representations refer to the storage of the phonological characteristics of words in long-term

AN US

memory, and it is plausible that deficits in a listener‟s speech perception may impact the quality of phonological representations necessary for speaking and reading processes. For example, if children have deficits in encoding and processing the acoustic-phonetic features of speech

M

stimuli, this may interfere with the establishment of well-specified phonological representations in the mental lexicon (Fowler, 1991). In independent lines of research, speech perception skills

ED

of children with dyslexia and children with speech delay have been studied to better understand the nature of phonological deficits in these children. Although both populations have shown

PT

speech perception deficits, the degree to which the underlying perceptual deficit overlaps is

CE

unclear. The present study investigates the speech perception skills of both children with dyslexia and children with speech delay to determine whether these children differ in their

AC

speech perception for particular acoustic manipulations. In its natural form, running speech comprises multiple redundant cues that signal the

identity of phonemes and, thus, in typical listening situations, it is unlikely that a single cue is responsible for the accurate perception of any particular phoneme. This acoustic redundancy plays an important role in successful speech perception in everyday environments that are rarely

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

5

free of noise, reverberation, or distortion (Ziegler et al., 2009). Individuals with dyslexia have difficulty perceiving speech when presented with a compromised speech signal such as speechin-noise, sine-wave speech, band-limited speech, or the multiple simultaneous distortions found in classrooms (Gruber, 2003; Johnson et al., 2011; Rosner et al., 2003; Ziegler et al., 2009).

CR IP T

Signals that mask or omit the acoustic redundancy present in natural speech may interfere with a listener‟s ability to integrate relevant acoustic properties with their own internal representations. For listeners with poorly specified phonological representations, it may be especially difficult to accurately perceive a signal with limited acoustic detail.

AN US

Compared to a naturally-spoken utterance, sine-wave speech is an acoustically-sparse, speech-based signal generated by creating sinusoidal tones that follow the center frequencies of the first four formants of a naturally-spoken utterance. This manipulation results in a signal that

M

does not contain the fundamental frequencies, harmonic structure, or short-term spectral cues from the original spoken sentences. Thus, the resulting signal comprises several time-varying

ED

inharmonic frequency components that faithfully follow the original formant center frequencies yet the amplitude information of the original signal is so poorly specified that it is nearly binary.

PT

The listener‟s impression of this material is influenced by the instructions in the experiment

CE

(Remez et al., 1980). It ranges from not even recognizing the stimuli as speech-based, to hearing many of the words correctly. Overall, however, listener performance is low regardless of

AC

experimenter instructions. A simple acoustical manipulation, amplitude comodulation of the sine-wave sentences,

greatly increases their intelligibility. Amplitude comodulation refers to a digital manipulation of the signal which simultaneously modulates the sine-wave tones at the same frequency and phase. In other words, it is analogous to rapidly turning on and off each of the four component tones

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

6

simultaneously, that is, to comodulate the four tones. The intelligibility of sine-wave speech increases significantly when the signal is amplitude comodulated for both adults (Carrell & Opie, 1992) and typically-developing children (Cabbage, Carrell & Hogan, 2011; Lewis & Carrell, 2007). Importantly, intelligibility increases despite the fact that no phonetic, syntactic, or

CR IP T

semantic information has been added. Likewise, additional amplitude structure is unrelated to the amplitude envelope of the original utterance and only includes an arbitrary modulation of 80 Hz added to the signal. Amplitude comodulation does not refer to traditionally-defined

suprasegmental information such as intonation, tempo, and other forms of traditional

AN US

suprasegmental information. Amplitude comodulation has been suggested to acoustically group components of sound together based on the phenomenon of comodulation masking release (Carrell & Opie, 1992). The grouping allows the simultaneous tones to be processed as one

M

“object” rather than several for later, perhaps more linguistic, processes. Previous research has shown that typically-developing children have better perception for

ED

amplitude-modulated sine-wave speech signals than sine-wave speech (Lewis & Carrell, 2007). It is plausible that children with phonological impairments will not show these same gains

PT

because of an overall weak sensitivity to acoustic structure as compared to typical children.

CE

Children with unimpaired phonological representations, whether listening to either sine-wave speech or amplitude comodulated sine-wave speech, may use their own internal representations

AC

to “fill in” the gaps and recover missing acoustic structure to make sense of a signal. Children with phonological deficits, and thus likely underspecified phonological representations, may lack the necessary acoustic detail in their own representations to “fill in” the gaps for successful speech perception. Indeed, previous research has demonstrated the importance of long-term phonological representations for success in phonological serial recall tasks that are subject to

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

7

short-term memory decay (Hulme et al., 1997). Thomson, Richardson, and Goswami (2005), investigating children with dyslexia, attributed the erred intrusion of real words during nonword recall as evidence these children have less well-specified phonological representations to draw on to reconstruct nonwords. It is possible that children with phonological deficits (i.e., dyslexia,

CR IP T

speech sound disorder) may evidence similar less robust representations when presented with novel speech stimuli characterized by minimal acoustic structure. Separate lines of research suggest children with dyslexia and children with speech delay may respond differently to

particular acoustic cues during speech perception. Children with dyslexia appear to have more

AN US

difficulty when speech perception is reliant on acoustic features that cue amplitude structure during speech perception (Goswami, 2011; Goswami et al., 2002; Goswami et al., 2011; Muneaux et al., 2004), whereas children with speech delay appear to have more difficulty when

M

speech perception is reliant on acoustic features related to spectral cues (Johnson et al., 2011). Evidence suggests individuals with dyslexia show particular impairments for perception

ED

tasks relying on cues associated with amplitude, or global, structure such as amplitude envelope detection (Goswami, 2011), rise time discrimination (Goswami et al., 2002; Goswami et al.,

PT

2011; Muneaux et al., 2004), and amplitude modulation detection (Lorenzi et al., 2000;

CE

Rocheron et al., 2002). By design, sine-wave speech is devoid of most amplitude structure and comprises primarily spectral information. Thus, because spectral structure is comparatively

AC

preserved in sine-wave speech signals, it is possible that children with dyslexia do not show deficits perceiving sine-wave speech and amplitude-modulated sine-wave speech stimuli. By contrast, children with speech delay may exhibit difficulty with sine-wave speech and amplitudecomodulated sine-wave speech precisely because these signals are limited to primarily spectral information and lack additional acoustic redundancy, such as amplitude structure. Although there

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

8

is considerably less research investigating speech perception skills in children with speech delay, Johnson and colleagues (2011) provided compelling evidence demonstrating that speech perception tasks for speech stimuli containing primarily global amplitude structure, i.e., vocoded speech, did not pose particular difficulty for children with a history of speech delay as compared

CR IP T

to their peers with dyslexia. Children in their study, however, were given another task, syllable discrimination, that required the perception of spectral structure, i.e., formant transitions from fricatives /s, ʃ/ into a following vowel. Notably, the children with a history of speech delay, in a post-hoc comparison, were the only group shown to respond to this spectral cue more poorly

AN US

than typical children. Neither children with dyslexia nor children with dyslexia and speech delay performed differently from typical children. Is it possible that children with speech sound disorder primarily attend to different acoustic cues both in perception and production, than peers

M

with a different type of phonological impairment, such as dyslexia? If this is the case, we would predict that when these children are completing speech perception tasks involving sine-wave

ED

speech, which is characterized by the preservation of spectral structure of speech and very

speech delay.

PT

rudimentary amplitude information, children with dyslexia would outperform children with

CE

On the other hand, children with phonological deficits may simply have difficulty perceiving speech that is impoverished in some way, regardless of the type of preserved structure

AC

in the speech signal. Several studies have demonstrated that children with dyslexia show particular weakness when given poor or noisy speech signals, (Gruber, 2003; Johnson et al., 2011; Rosner et al., 2003; Ziegler et al., 2009), thus it is possible children with dyslexia simply exhibit deficits for perception of impoverished speech. Children with dyslexia performed poorer than their typically-developing peers on perception of speech-in-noise and band-limited speech

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

9

(Ziegler et al., 2009). These deficits were found in comparison to both chronological agematched peers and reading-level matched peers, suggesting the perceptual problem was specifically associated with a disordered, dyslexic profile, instead of delayed reading skills. Rosner and colleagues (2003) presented adults with dyslexia with short sine-wave speech

CR IP T

sentences and found that some, but not all, adults with dyslexia exhibited poor perception as compared to controls.

Although little research has investigated speech perception skills in children with speech sound disorder for speech stimuli that are impoverished in some way, evidence from a variety of

AN US

speech perception tasks for syllables and words has shown these children exhibit particular

difficulty perceiving speech sounds they produce incorrectly (Broen et al., 1983; Hoffman et al., 1985; Locke, 1980; Monnin & Huntington, 1974; Rvachew & Jamieson, 1989; Rvachew et al.,

M

2004). This may indicate a deficiency in the acoustic specificity for these erred phonemes in a child‟s own internal representations. Children who do not produce a particular phoneme

ED

correctly may exhibit particular difficulty perceiving that same phoneme in sine-wave speech or amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech. As a result of this observation, in the present study we

PT

elected to include children with speech delay characterized by an erred production of /r/ to test

CE

whether these children had difficulty with a speech perception task specifically involving /r/. The /r/ phoneme is a late-developing speech sound in speech production and is often associated with

AC

a subset of children with speech delay who are classified as exhibiting persistent speech errors, or persistent speech delay (Shriberg et al., 2010). In the present study, we designed stimuli that removed the majority of acoustic features

normally present in natural speech and presented it to children with dyslexia, persistent speech delay, and their typically-developing peers. Our first research question asked whether children

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

10

with dyslexia and children with persistent speech delay differed in their word recognition for sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech. Because children with dyslexia have demonstrated difficulty with speech perception tasks involving global amplitude structure and children with speech delay have demonstrated particular difficulty with the perception of

CR IP T

temporal fine structure (Johnson et al., 2011), we hypothesized that children with dyslexia would exhibit superior word recognition for sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech as compared to their peers with persistent speech delay.

Our second research question asked whether children with persistent speech delay have

AN US

more difficulty perceiving a phoneme erred in their own production than a phoneme that is

produced correctly in their own speech (Locke, 1980; Shuster, 1998). We hypothesized that children with persistent speech delay would have more difficulty perceiving a phoneme they did

M

not produce correctly (in this study the erred /r/ phoneme) in both sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech, as compared to their peers without speech delay.

ED

Because children with speech delay in the current study were selected to participate if they exhibited persistent speech errors in production of the /r/ phoneme, we predicted these children

PT

would demonstrate a particular difficulty perceiving sine-wave speech and amplitude-

CE

comodulated sine-wave speech tokens of the /r/ phoneme in words. 2. Method

AC

2.1 Participants

Thirty-six children ranging in age from 7;6 – 9;6 (years; months) participated in this

study. This age range was selected to include children who were old enough to have received reading instruction and exhibit word reading deficits, while also including children who were young enough to exhibit speech articulation deficits. Children were grouped into three groups

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

11

(n=12 in each group): dyslexia, persistent speech delay and age-matched typically-developing peers. Children with dyslexia exhibited word reading abilities below the 30th percentile as compared to their peers and exhibited no speech articulation deficits, nor had a history of such deficits per written parent report. Children with persistent speech delay exhibited articulation

CR IP T

deficits in speech production primarily characterized as difficulty producing the /r/ phoneme, described in detail below. Children with persistent speech delay exhibited word reading abilities in the average to above-average range. All children exhibited normal receptive and expressive language skills, were monolingual speakers of Standard American English and passed a pure

AN US

tone hearing screening with thresholds at 20 dB or lower at the octave frequencies between 500 – 8000 Hz. 2.2 Assessment

M

Articulation. Articulation skills were measured using the sounds-in-words subtest of the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 (GFTA-2, Goldman & Fristoe, 2000) and a probe

ED

designed to assess each child‟s production of the /r/ phoneme, which was adapted from a probe developed previously (Preston & Edwards, 2007). A speech-language pathologist transcribed

PT

participant speech production for each word using broad transcription with the International

CE

Phonetic Alphabet. A second transcriptionist independently transcribed a random 20% of samples. Inter-rater reliability was 98.4%. Children with persistent speech delay scored below

AC

the 16th percentile on the GFTA-2 and had difficulty producing /r/ in any or all positions of words. Of the 12 children with persistent speech delay, four children only exhibited difficulty with /r/, and eight children exhibited additional articulation errors including /s, z, sh, th/. All of these phonemes are also considered late-developing (Smit et al., 1990), and are common errors for those with a persistent speech delay. To further quantify each child‟s error pattern for

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

12

production of /r/, a 46-word /r/ probe with words containing /r/ in both prevocalic (i.e. rat, rake) and post-vocalic (i.e. bird, bear) positions of words was administered to all children with persistent speech delay. The nature of /r/ distortions for children with persistent speech delay in this study was primarily characterized as gliding of /r/, /w/ for /r/ substitution, or vowelization of

CR IP T

/r/. All but one child had difficulty producing /r/ in all positions of words. One child produced pre-vocalic /r/ accurately but did not produce /r/ in any post-vocalic contexts.

Reading. Word reading skills were assessed via the Basic Skills subtests of the

Woodcock Reading Mastery Test – Revised/Normative Update (WRMT-R/NU, Woodcock, 1998).

AN US

All participants were administered the Word Identification and Word Attack subtests. The Word Identification subtest measures participants‟ reading abilities for real words that increase in difficulty from high to low frequency words. The Word Attack subtest measured each child‟s

M

ability to apply phonics skills to nonwords and low-frequency words during reading. A composite Word Reading score was computed based on the combination of results from each

ED

subtest. Participants who received a standard score of 92 or below (< 30th percentile) (Manis et al., 1996) were considered potential candidates for the dyslexia group. Cut-off scores for the

PT

classification of dyslexia varies widely across studies ranging from the 7th percentile (Badian et

CE

al., 1990) to 30th percentile (Manis et al., 1996). Additionally, children in the persistent speech delay and typically-developing groups were required to receive a standard score of at least 100 to

AC

prevent the inclusion of any children in these groups with subclinical reading deficits. Non-verbal intelligence. Children were administered subtests of the Reynolds

Intellectual Assessment Scales (RIAS; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2003) to verify normal non-verbal cognitive ability (standard score > 80).

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

13

Receptive/Expressive vocabulary. Because of the positive relationship between vocabulary knowledge and the specificity of phonological representations (Metsala & Walley, 1998), all children were administered the Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWPVT; Brownell, 2000b) to assess receptive vocabulary skills and the Expressive One-Word

CR IP T

Picture Vocabulary Test (EOWPVT; Brownell, 2000A) to assess expressive vocabulary skills. Phonological Awareness. Phonological awareness skills, which have been shown to be positively correlated with reading ability (Larivee & Catts, 1999), were assessed using selected subtests (i.e. Elision, Blending Words) of the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing

AN US

(CTOPP; Wagner et al., 1999). The Elision subtest was a phoneme deletion task wherein

children were auditorily given a word and asked to delete a specific phoneme and say the resulting target word (ex. Say “meet” without /t/. Answer: “me”.). The Blending Words subtest

M

involved auditorily presenting children with a series of phonemes and asking them to blend the

Answer: “mad”.). 2.3 Stimuli

ED

phonemes into an appropriate word (ex. “What word do these sounds make? /m/ /æ/ /d/.

PT

In this study, we used novel speech stimuli comprising rudimentary frequency and

CE

amplitude information (i.e. sine-wave speech, amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech). The experimental word stimuli consisted of 12 pairs of consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) or

AC

consonant-vowel (CV) rhyming words that began with the initial phonemes /r/ or /m/, to address hypothesized deficits for children with persistent speech delay (/r/) in contrast to a control phoneme (/m/) which was produced correctly by all children. An additional 12 words, all beginning with the phoneme /w/, were matched to and rhymed with the experimental stimuli to serve as foils. To determine eligible words for inclusion in this study, we generated a list of

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

14

common CVC and CV real words with /r/ in the initial positions of words (ex. rake, row, run). We substituted /m/ and /w/ for /r/ in these words and rejected any rhymes that did not yield real words (ex. mun, wope). Of the resulting eligible triads, 12 were selected for inclusion in this study (see Table 1). We calculated word frequency values for each item using an online

CR IP T

calculator based on a child corpus of Spoken American English (Storkel & Hoover, 2010).

Analysis of variance revealed that the words were similar across various word characteristics including word frequency (F(2,35) = 0.665, p = 0.521), part of speech (F(2,35) = 0.295, p =

(F(2,35) = 2.320, p = 0.114).

AN US

0.746), duration (in milliseconds) (F(2,35) = 0.013, p = 0.987), and neighborhood density

Table 1. Stimulus words used for the word recognition task. Word Duration, in Number of Frequencya msb neighborsc

CE

AC

raid rail rake rare rate ray raise real right rock rose row

M

/m/ words 461 443 386 435 404 432 567 401 415 404 601 398 M=449.38 SD= (70.53)

ED

4.06 2.92 3.90 2.20 2.32 4.15 1.78 2.48 3.83 1.90 1.00 1.60 M=2.75 SD=(1.09)

PT

made mail make mare mate may maze meal might mock mows mow

/r/ words 2.00 507 2.20 436 2.04 440 2.61 428 3.32 428 2.28 594 2.72 424 1.30 415 3.86 361 2.88 397 2.93 574 2.56 395 M=2.508 SD= M=445.38

16 15 20 18 21 21 8 14 21 14 9 21 M=16.72 SD= (4.81) 22 16 22 16 25 26 8 14 19 16 15 28 M=18.54

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

wade whale wake wear wait weigh ways wheel white walk woes whoa

SD= (70.04)

1.30 1.00 2.36 2.59 3.27 3.96 1.00 2.75 3.56 3.00 1.00 1.70 M=2.29 SD =1.07

SD= (5.73)

/w/ words 483 616 385 429 433 424 424 413 339 392 643 411 M=450.50 SD =90.57

a

17 2 20 20 18 23 9 6 6 12 22 9 M=23 SD =7.18

CR IP T

(0.67)

15

Word frequency obtained from an online child corpus calculator (Storkel & Hoover, 2010). Word duration, in milliseconds, was calculated from the onset of phonation to offset of the final phoneme in each word. c Number of neighbors refers to the number of real words that can be obtained by a single phoneme omission, substitution calculated by an online child corpus calculator (Storkel & Hoover, 2010).

AN US

b

The /r/ was selected as a target phoneme to address our research hypotheses regarding the

M

perceptual deficit we expected for school-age children with persistent speech delay. The /m/ was

ED

selected as a control phoneme because it is an early developing sound in speech production and rare for school-age children (Smit et al., 1990) with speech delay to exhibit difficulty producing

PT

the /m/ at any point in development. Both phonemes were well-suited for representation in sinewave speech and amplitude-comodulated speech because of their respective acoustic structure,

CE

which depend on frequency relevant features such as formant transitions. Such acoustic structure is readily preserved in both sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech.

AC

Moreover, rudimentary adjustments to amplitude levels enabled the simulation of naturally subtle amplitude differences (i.e. anti-resonance for nasality in /m/). For example, a key feature of the /r/, the height of the third formant frequency, is fully represented in both sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech. The /m/ relies on the blending of the formant transition into the following vowel with minor adjustments to amplitude levels described below

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

16

to mimic features of anti-resonance present in the production of nasal phonemes during natural speech production. Naturally-produced tokens of each word were recorded by an adult female speaker with a standard dialect of Midwest American English while seated in a single-walled isolated acoustic

CR IP T

chamber. The words were randomized into three separate lists for recitation to eliminate order effects of reading during recording. All tokens were recorded at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz and an amplitude resolution of 16 bits, using a desktop microphone (AKG C414B) and a Zoom H4N digital recorder. After recording was complete, audio files were digitally transferred to a personal

AN US

computer and segmented into individual words using CoolEdit 2K. All words were screened for mispronunciations, peak clipping, and background noise and normalized at -.5 dB (re: 16 bits = 96 dB peak). Following this process, one of the remaining tokens of each word was randomly

M

selected for inclusion in this study.

To create sine-wave speech from these tokens, a wide-band spectrogram of each

ED

recording was produced using standard audio software (WinSnoori 1.34). Algorithms within WinSnoori estimated and traced the center frequencies of each of the first four formants. Visual

PT

inspection, by the primary investigator, was used to examine the algorithm-produced tracings to

CE

confirm the estimations and to adjust parameters to better approximate the formant frequencies visible on the original spectrogram. Typically, only minor adjustments were required. Using the

AC

Klatt interface in the program, numerical values for each formant frequency were tabulated at 4 ms intervals. All values were entered into a custom-designed computer program that constructed waveforms made up of independent sinusoidal waves (TONE16, Tice & Carrell, 2011). Peak amplitudes for the tones representing the four formants were set to 90-, 84-, 76-, and 70-dB RMS (re: 16 bits = 96-dB peak) to approximate the spectral slope of speech. These amplitudes were

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

17

lowered by 3-dB, 5-dB, 10-dB, or set to 0 to approximate the original formant amplitudes for specific phonemes. For example, for nasal phonemes, amplitudes of the first and second formants were lowered by 3-dB to simulate decreased amplitude as a result of anti-resonance in natural speech. All SWS tokens were generated with a sampling rate of 10 kHz. Amplitude-

CR IP T

modulated sine-wave speech (AMSWS) words originated from the SWS tokens described above. Each sine-wave speech token was amplitude-comodulated at 80 Hz using a custom-designed modulating software, AMOD32 (Tice & Carrell, 2011). A modulation cycle of 80 Hz was

chosen as previous research has shown optimal intelligibility for amplitude co-modulation of

AN US

SWS signals at rates between 50-100 Hz (Lewis & Carrell, 2007; Carrell & Opie, 1992). The modulating signal was a triangular wave with a duty cycle of 83%. On each 12.5-msec modulating cycle, the amplitude rose to 100% in 8 milliseconds (ms), dropped to 0% in 2 ms,

M

and remained at 0% for the final 2.5 ms. All stimuli in the present experiment were equilibrated to 72-dB (re: 96 dB maximum level with 16-bits of amplitude resolution). See Figure 1 for

AC

CE

PT

sine-wave speech.

ED

spectrograms representative of natural speech, sine-wave speech, and amplitude-comodulated

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

18

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

AN US

CR IP T

Figure 1. Spectrograms representative of natural speech, sine-wave speech, and amplitudecomodulated sine-wave speech. Panel A is a spectrogram of the naturally-spoken word “rare” showing tracings of the center frequencies of the formant frequencies using audio software Wavesurfer1.8.8. Panel B shows the spectrogram for the unmodulated sine-wave version of the same word based on the first four formants. Panel C shows the spectrogram of the amplitudecomodulated (80-Hz) sine-wave version of the same word.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

19

The sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech stimuli were piloted on both typically-developing adult and child listeners because none of the words had been used in previous research. Consistent with previous findings, amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech tokens were reliably more intelligible than the sine-wave speech tokens from which they

CR IP T

were created (Carrell & Opie, 1992; Lewis & Carrell, 2007; Shapley, 2003). Listeners

consistently identified more phonemes in amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech as compared to sine-wave speech tokens. As a perceptual reference for the acoustic quality of each of these types of novel stimuli, pilot children described sine-wave speech as sounding like “squeals”,

AN US

“beeps”, or “R2D2 from Star Wars”, whereas amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech was usually described as sounding “like a robot”. 2.4 Procedure

M

Children were tested in two research sessions, the first of which involved the administration of the standardized assessment measures and the second of which involved completing the word

ED

recognition task. The session took place in a child-friendly room that was acoustically treated with absorbent foam designed to significantly reduce the ambient noise level of the testing

PT

environment. All participants were tested individually while seated at a computer running E-

CE

prime 2.0 experimental software. Children were fitted with insert earphones (Etymotic ER-1A) set to a comfortable listening level. Four children did not tolerate placement of the earphones in

AC

the ear canal and were thus fitted with noise-cancelling headphones (Sennheiser HD280Pro) set to a comfortable listening level. Throughout the experiment, a research assistant sat beside the child to monitor the child‟s attention to the task but was unable to hear the auditory stimuli due to presentation via headphones for the child.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

20

The word recognition task involved children listening to both types of novel word stimuli described above. Stimuli were presented in the context of novel talkers: an alien (sine-wave speech) or robot (amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech). Pre-recorded instructions were provided via computer. Children were instructed that the alien and robot talkers “sounded funny”

CR IP T

and the child‟s help was enlisted to determine what they were saying by “interpreting” what was said. Children responded verbally and their response was audio recorded using a desktop

microphone (AKG C414B) connected to a Marantz (PMD670) digital recorder. All responses were transcribed in real time. A second transcriber transcribed a random 20% of samples offline.

AN US

Inter-rater reliability was 94.3%. Because some children, by definition, had difficulty producing speech sounds, children also pointed to the first letter of their chosen word on a provided letter strip secured to the table to eliminate ambiguity about the child‟s intended response. The letter

M

strip contained 12 letters including the target letters, i.e. r/m, and 9 additional letters: b, w, s, n, p, g, d, l, f, c, and a “not here” option for any alternate letters the child might have wanted to select.

ED

Because of the simplicity of the words used, spelling deficiency was not expected to interfere with a child correctly identifying the first letter of their response. In all cases, if there was a

PT

mismatch between the initial phoneme of the child‟s initial response and the child‟s first-letter

CE

selection, the item was scored according to the letter the child pointed to on the letter strip. The task totaled 48 trials. Of children in the typically-developing group, one child had one mismatch

AC

between the spoken word and the selected first letter. All other typically-developing children exhibited 100% consistency between their transcribed verbal response and the selection of the first letter for all 48 trials. Of children in the dyslexia group, one child had two mismatches between the verbal response and the selected first letter. All other children in the dyslexia group exhibited 100% consistency between verbal responses and letter strip selections. Of children in

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

21

the residual speech delay group, any verbal response potentially containing an /r/ was transcribed as the child produced it. As a result, for these children it is difficult to calculate the true mismatch rate between what the child said and what the child intended, hence this particular procedure. For verbal responses that did not contain a distorted phoneme, no child with a

CR IP T

residual speech delay had any mismatch between their verbal response and the letter strip

response. Thus, it is assumed for distorted articulation in their verbal responses, these children confidently pointed to their intended first letter response.

The word recognition task began with a familiarization phase during which the child was

AN US

introduced to each novel speaker, i.e. the alien or the robot, which individually appeared as a still image on the computer screen. Beginning with the alien, which spoke in sine-wave speech, the computer presented a sample item three times. The child was encouraged to guess the word that

M

they heard. The child was told the word by the research assistant and then listened to the item a fourth time. This procedure was repeated using a different sample item for the robot who spoke

ED

with the amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech. All children successfully completed the familiarization tasks. The experimental phase comprised four blocks of 12 words, for a total of

PT

48 words. Two blocks contained sine-wave speech tokens and two blocks contained amplitude-

CE

comodulated sine-wave speech tokens. All words were presented in both sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech conditions. To control for order effects of word

AC

exposure, presentation order was counterbalanced across participants and groups. Within each block, all stimuli were randomized. Each stimulus was heard three times in succession, during or after which children provided their verbal and pointing responses. After the child‟s response, the research assistant advanced the program to the next trial. Children were not provided feedback regarding the accuracy of their responses. During the task, the alien or robot (depending on the

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

22

block) advanced a step along a pathway towards a finish line, providing children a visual cue for their progress in the task. The dependent measure for these tasks was the number of initial phonemes (i.e. /r/, /m/) accurately recognized for each acoustic condition. In cases where the transcription of a child‟s

CR IP T

response was not consistent with the letter to which they pointed indicating the first letter of the response (ex. The child said “wake” for “rake”, but pointed to the letter “r” as the first letter), the item was scored based on the child‟s pointing response.To ensure any obtained group differences were not attributable to knowledge of specific vocabulary used in this study, after the completion

AN US

of the word recognition task, all participants were administered a vocabulary probe comprising words used in the tasks. Children were presented with picture plates with four pictures, one of which represented the target word. The other three pictures represented phonological, semantic,

M

and unrelated foils. A one-way ANOVA revealed no difference between groups in performance on this probe, F(2,35) = 1.872 (p=.170). Correlation analysis further verified there were no

ED

statistically significant relationships between vocabulary probe accuracy and any of the word recognition dependent variables for any of the groups (all ps > 0.10). The data were analyzed

PT

using a series of repeated-measures ANOVAs to address our research questions. Throughout the

CE

analyses, post-hoc comparisons were used to decompose interactions. In all cases, an alpha level of .05 was used to determine statistical significance.

AC

3. Results

The purpose of this study was to determine how children with different phonological

deficits (i.e., dyslexia or persistent speech delay) perceive amplitude comodulation structure of words compared to novel sine-wave words during a word recognition task. Our first research question asked whether children with dyslexia or persistent speech delay exhibited more

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

23

difficulty perceiving sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech as compared to their typically-developing peers. Because individuals with persistent speech delay have demonstrated difficulty perceiving fine temporal structure in speech signals as compared to their peers with dyslexia, we predicted that children with dyslexia would have less difficulty

CR IP T

perceiving sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech. Our second

research question asked whether children with persistent speech delay have more difficulty

perceiving a phoneme erred in their own production (Locke, 1980; Shuster, 1998). We predicted that children with persistent speech delay would have more difficulty perceiving the /r/ phoneme

AN US

in both sine-wave and amplitude-comodulated words, as compared to their peers with dyslexia and their typically-developing peers. 3.1 Descriptive Measures

M

All children were administered a battery of articulation, language and reading assessments for assignment of participants to the appropriate group. Descriptive statistics

ED

regarding word reading, articulation, nonverbal intelligence, vocabulary and phonological awareness skills are displayed in Table 2. As selected, the groups differed in word reading such

PT

that children with dyslexia exhibited significantly lower word reading scores than those in the

CE

persistent speech delay and typically-developing groups, F (2, 35)=29.026, p<.001. Likewise, as selected the groups differed in speech articulation such that children in the persistent speech

AC

delay group exhibited significantly lower articulation skills than those in the dyslexia and typically-developing groups F(2,35)=51.839, p<.001. Furthermore, children in the persisent speech delay group performed significantly worse than children in the typically-developing and dyslexia groups on the R-probe, F(2,35)=990.398, p<.001. Of the 12 children with persistent speech delay, 11 failed to produce /r/ in any position of words in isolation or connected speech.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

24

One child with speech sound disorder exhibited a correct /r/ production in pre-vocalic positions of words (ex. rat, green), but failed to produce /r/ post-vocalically (ex. deer, fire). There were no group differences in receptive vocabulary, F(2,35) = .650, p=.528, expressive vocabulary, F(2,35) =2.805, p=.075, or nonverbal intelligence, F(2,35) = 1.927, p=.162. Although not

CR IP T

statistically different, there were marginal differences in phonological awareness (F(2,35) = 3.003, p=.063) with the children with dyslexia performing numerically poorer than the other children.

SSD (n=12) Mean (SD) 100.25 (5.79)a 112.58 (8.69)a 77.83 (10.88)b 112.08 (9.61)a 105.42 (12.03)a

AN US

DYS (n=12) Mean (SD) 106.26 (9.29)a 89.41 (3.96)b 102.42 (5.93)a 107.58 (12.23)a 96.25 (12.70)a

M

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics. TD (n=12) Mean (SD) Age (in months) 103.3 (8.53)a WRMT-R/NU 111.75 (11.08)a 105.17 (1.95)a GFTA-2 ROWPVT 112.58 (13.33)a EOWPVT 107.92 (13.34)a

106.00 (14.41)a 94.1667 (7.22)a

101.5 (12.95)a

RIAS R-Probe (out of 46)

116.5 (13.37)a 46.00 (0.0)a

113.25 (16.45)a 1.42 (4.91)b

ED

CTOPP

106.00 (9.52)a 46.00 (0.0)a

Note. Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p<.05.

AC

CE

PT

(TD = Typically-developing; DYS = Dyslexia; SSD = Speech sound disorder; WRMT-R/NU=Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised/Normative Update, Basic Skills cluster; GFTA-2=Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2nd Ed., standard score (SS) reported; ROWPVT=Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, SS reported; EOWPVT=Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, SS reported; CTOPP=Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, SS reported; RIAS=Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales, SS reported; R-probe, raw score reported)

3.2 Experimental Measures The purpose of the word recognition task was to determine whether children differed in their recognition of impoverished speech signals in two acoustic conditions. One condition, sine-wave speech, presented words as four independent time-varying sinusoids designed to

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

25

mimic center frequency values of the first four formants of a naturally-spoken word. The other condition, amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech, applied a modulating waveform of 80 Hz to the sine-wave speech words. Previous research has demonstrated that both adults and children typically recognize more amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech words than sine-wave speech

CR IP T

words, when all other acoustic and linguistic structure is held constant (Carrell & Opie, 1992; Lewis & Carrell, 2007).

To analyze these data, we used a 2 (modulation: sine-wave speech, amplitude-

comodulated sine-wave speech) x 2 (phoneme: /r/, /m) x 3 (group: dyslexia, speech sound

AN US

disorder, typically-developing) mixed factorial design repeated-measures ANOVA. Results revealed a main effect of modulation, F(1,33) = 27.920, p < .001, ηp2 = .458 such that on average, as expected, children recognized initial phonemes in the sine-wave speech condition

M

with less accuracy than words in the amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech condition. Figure 2 shows average performance on /m/ and /r/ words across groups.

AC

CE

PT

ED

Figure 2. Initial phoneme accuracy for /m/ and /r/ collapsed across all participants. The plain white bar represents sine-wave speech (SWS) and the striped bar represents amplitudecomodulated sine-wave speech (AMSWS). Error bars reflect standard error of the mean.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

26

The main effect of group was not significant, F(2,33) = 2.268, p=0.119, ηp2 = .121, but the modulation by group interaction was significant, F(1,33) = 3.581, p=.039, ηp2 = .178, suggesting that the groups differed in their recognition accuracy across the two modulation conditions. Planned comparisons across the different phoneme conditions for each group

CR IP T

revealed that the children with dyslexia recognized more amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech words than sine-wave speech words for the /m/ phoneme, t(11) = -5.014, p<.001, and the /r/ phoneme, t(11) = -3.544, p=.005. By contrast, children with persistent speech delay

recognized more amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech words than sine-wave speech words

AN US

for the /m/ phoneme, t(11) = -2.785, p=.018, and there was no difference in accuracy between amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech and sine-wave speech for the /r/ phoneme, t(11) = .951, p = .362. Children in the typically-developing group did not differ in accuracy between

M

amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech and sine-wave speech for the /m/ phoneme, t(11) 2.057, p = .064 nor the /r/ phoneme, t(11) -1.358, p = .202. Nonsignificant differences for the

ED

typically-developing group may be attributable to ceiling effects, as the typically-developing children recognized a high percentage of phonemes in the sine-wave speech condition and thus

PT

did not have the opportunity for substantial gains in phoneme recognition in the amplitude-

CE

comodulated sine-wave speech condition. Additionally, using word stimuli comprising simple CVC structures may have contributed to the high rates of accuracy in these children.

AC

There were no group differences in accuracy for sine-wave speech without comodulation for both the /m/ and /r/ phonemes (all ps > .05). All groups achieved similar levels of accuracy in the amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech condition for the /m/ phoneme (all ps > .05), but children with persistent speech delay recognized significantly fewer /r/ phonemes in the amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech condition, as compared to their peers with dyslexia

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

27

(t(11) = 2.098, p = .048) and those who were typically-developing (t(-2.721, p=.012). The threeway interaction between modulation, phoneme and group was not significant, F(1,33) = .254, p=.777, ηp2 = .015, nor was the two-way interaction of modulation and phoneme significant, F(1,33) = 2.857, p=.100, ηp2 = .080. Figures 3 and 4 show performance level for each group by

CR IP T

each phoneme and condition, and include individual data points for all participants (N=36) for the reader to more fully conceptualize performance across each group.

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

AN US

Figure 3. Initial phoneme accuracy for /m/ for each participant and participant group. Each dot represents a child and connecting lines delineate performance in sine-wave speech (SWS) versus amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech (AMSWS). Bolded lines indicate more than one child achieved this pattern of scores. Please see Appendix A for individual performance scores for each child in each group. The first panel shows children in the dyslexia (DYS) group with SWS performance on the left and AMSWS performance on the right. The second panel shows word recognition performance for children with persistent speech delay (PSD) and the third panel shows performance for typically-developing (TD) children. Hashed lines represent the group mean for each group for each condition. Asterisks indicate a statistically significant difference (p < .05) between SWS and AMSWS.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

28

CE

PT

ED

M

AN US

CR IP T

Figure 4. Initial phoneme accuracy for /r/ for each participant and participant group. Each dot represents a child and connecting lines delineate performance in sine-wave speech (SWS) versus amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech (AMSWS). Bolded lines indicate more than one child achieved this pattern of scores (e.g. in the TD group, 6 children achieved 100% accuracy for both SWS and AMSWS conditions). Please see Appendix A for individual performance scores for each child in each group. The first panel shows children in the dyslexia group with SWS performance on the left and AMSWS performance on the right. The second panel shows word recognition performance for children with persistent speech delay (PSD) and the third panel shows performance for typically-developing (TD) children. Hashed lines represent the group mean for each group for each condition. Asterisks indicate a statistically significant difference (p < .05) between SWS and AMSWS.

AC

As these figures show, for the /m/ phoneme, the majority of children across all three groups showed either improved word recognition from sine-wave speech to amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech or exhibited a very similar performance between the two conditions. Visual inspection of individual performance for the /r/ phoneme, however, across the three groups of children shows a greater variability in performance for children with persistent speech delay

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

29

versus children in the dyslexia and typically-developing group. Notably, several children in this group performed poorer in the amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech condition. 4. Discussion The purpose of the present study was to determine the degree to which children with

CR IP T

differing phonological deficit profiles, namely children with dyslexia and children with persistent speech delay, perceived impoverished speech signals containing varying amounts of acoustic information. Thirty-six children aged 7;6 – 9;6, based on word reading and speech articulation skills, were classified into three groups: 1) dyslexia, 2) persistent speech delay, and 3) typically-

AN US

developing. All children listened to two types of impoverished speech: sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech. We had two main hypotheses: 1) Children with dyslexia would exhibit superior word recognition for sine-wave speech and amplitude-

M

comodulated sine-wave speech, as compared to their peers with persistent speech delay; and 2) children with persistent speech delay would exhibit particularly poor word recognition for

ED

stimuli containing a phoneme erred in their own production. We discuss two key findings here. 4.1 Amplitude comodulation improved speech perception in children with phonological

PT

impairments, particularly for children with dyslexia

CE

Regardless of the presence or absence of a phonological impairment, all children in this study, on average, performed better for the amplitude-comodulated stimuli as compared to the

AC

traditional sine-wave stimuli. While this response pattern has been reported for typical adults and children (Cabbage, Carrell, & Hogan, 2011; Carrell & Opie, 1992; Lewis & Carrell, 2007), it was unknown whether children with deficient phonological representations would perform similarly. As compared to typically-developing children, children with dyslexia exhibited substantial difficulty perceiving unmodulated sine-wave speech. When the stimuli were

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

30

amplitude -comodulated, however, children with dyslexia achieved a performance at the same level as their typically-developing peers. That is, amplitude comodulation yielded substantial gains in perception for children with dyslexia. It is unsurprising that children with dyslexia showed poor perception of phonemes in the unmodulated sine-wave speech as this is consistent

CR IP T

with previous work showing that adults with dyslexia struggle to perceive sine-wave speech as compared to typical adult listeners (Rosner, et al., 2003). Modifying the stimuli via amplitude comodulation, however, yielded substantial gains in perception for the children with dyslexia suggesting amplitude comodulation provided a particularly necessary cue for children with

AN US

dyslexia.

Given the benefits in perception for sine-wave speech versus amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech across all groups in this study, we hypothesize these stimuli are processed at an

M

acoustic, or sensory, level as opposed to a more linguistic, or phonological, level. The two types of stimuli presented here contained identical phonetic parameters including formant frequency

ED

values, transitions, and duration. The only difference between the stimuli was the acoustic manipulation of amplitude comodulation suggesting that it is the only factor that is contributing

PT

to the improved intelligibility for these children. Some have argued the perceptual deficit in

CE

children with dyslexia and persistent speech delay is attributable to a deficit at a more phonetic, or linguistic, speech level of processing (Manis et al., 1997; Studdert-Kennedy & Mody, 1995),

AC

rather than a non-speech sensory auditory level of processing (Tallal, 1980; Vandermosten et al., 2011). While there has been much debate in the literature on this issue (see Rosen, 2003, for a review), it seems that while a subgroup of children with dyslexia may exhibit some low-level sensory processing deficiencies, it is not likely the basis of the phonological deficit seen in most

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

31

children with dyslexia. From these results, it appears that children with dyslexia are indeed sensitive to acoustic manipulations that do not depend on linguistic, or phonetic, processing. The mechanism by which amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech is usually more intelligible than sine-wave speech, despite the absence of any additional phonetic or linguistic

CR IP T

structure is not well understood. One possibility for the better perception for amplitude-

comodulated sine-wave speech over unmodulated sine-wave speech is that in typical speech perception, neural fibers of the auditory nerve respond, in synchrony, to the harmonic elements of a spoken utterance. According to Koch et al. (1999), it is precisely the timing pattern of the

AN US

neural impulses and the synchronization of neural firing that are critical for speech perception in compromised situations, such as a noisy background. Such timing and synchronization may also be important when listening to impoverished speech signals. Because sine-wave speech

M

comprises independent sinusoids, it lacks any harmonic structure and thus, synchronous neural firing in the brain during speech perception is unlikely. It is possible that amplitude

ED

comodulation, which applies a simultaneous modulating signal to the independent sinusoids comprising the sine-wave speech signal, mimics the harmonic structure found in natural spoken

PT

utterances. The brain may process the sinusoids together for synchronous neural firing during

CE

processing, which results in more “speech-like” perception of amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech as compared to sine-wave speech. While this was not specifically tested in this study, it

AC

would be an important future direction to address whether amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech induces a level of neural synchrony that boosts intelligibility as compared to sine-wave speech.

Studies investigating amplitude modulation detection at very slow rates (i.e., 4 Hz, 10 Hz) have demonstrated that individuals with dyslexia show different patterns of response, both

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

32

behaviorally (Goswami et al., 2002) and at the level of neuronal processing (Goswami, 2011; Hamalainen et al., 2012). These slow rates of amplitude modulation have been thought to be relevant to the extraction of syllable-level and prosodic elements, whereas faster rates of modulation are related more to the extraction of acoustic-phonetic information (Hamalainen et

CR IP T

al., 2012). If neural synchrony plays a significant role in the improved intelligibility of

amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech, which provides a faster level of amplitude modulation (i.e., 80 Hz, in the present study), the present findings would lend support to the notion that

dyslexic individuals have specific deficits in realms of slower, as opposed to rapid, temporal

AN US

processing (Goswami, 2002; Goswami, 2011; Hamalainen et al., 2012). This follows from tasks that emphasize the salience of amplitude structure, such as amplitude envelope detection and band-limited speech tasks, for which dyslexic listeners show difficulty (Goswami, 2002; Lorenzi

M

et al., 2000; Pasquini, Corriveau, & Goswami, 2007; Rocheron et al., 2002). The temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia posits that children with

ED

dyslexia exhibit specific difficulty for the perception of amplitude, or „global‟, structure in complex signals such as speech (Goswami, 2011). Although the current study sought to

PT

determine the degree to which these children responded to stimuli constrained to include

CE

primarily spectral detail, it would be important to understand how these same children respond to stimuli constrained by the removal of most spectral detail, such as vocoded speech. Vocoded

AC

speech is composed of an utterance that has been band-pass filtered into a selected number of channels, which are then processed with a designated type of noise (e.g. speech-shaped noise, white noise, etc), in order to remove temporal fine structure detail. In a study using vocoded speech sentences with 10- to 11-year-old children, Johnson et al. (2011) showed marginal effects of a perceptual deficit for children with dyslexia-only and statistically significant deficits for

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

33

children with a history of both speech sound disorder and dyslexia. Children with a history of speech sound disorder-only (with no dyslexia) performed more similarly to their typical peers when listening to band-limited speech (Johnson et al., 2011). Because listeners with dyslexia have reportedly exhibited particular difficulty with band-

CR IP T

limited speech (Ziegler et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2011), it suggests they may more heavily rely on spectral structure for speech perception accuracy. The results of the current study suggest this indeed may be the case. In contrast to band-limited speech, sine-wave speech comprises

reasonably preserved spectral structure and so it is plausible that children with dyslexia may

AN US

show a particular aptitude for sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech as compared to their peers with persistent speech delay, because of the preserved temporal fine structure present in the signal. Further evidence suggests individuals with dyslexia show

M

particular impairments for perception tasks relying on cues associated with amplitude structure such as amplitude envelope detection (Goswami, 2011), rise time discrimination (Goswami et

ED

al., 2002; Goswami et al., 2011; Muneaux et al., 2004), and amplitude modulation detection (Lorenzi et al., 2000; Rocheron et al., 2002). All of these tasks preserve amplitude structure,

PT

which primarily transmits information regarding energy variation over time cueing syllable and

CE

word-level features, as opposed to fine spectral detail that cues specific phonetic information, such as formant transitions.

AC

4.2 Children with persistent speech delay have difficulty perceiving words containing their erred sound

Previous evidence has suggested children with speech sound disorder show particular

perceptual vulnerability for phonemes which are erred in their own speech production (Hoffman et al., 1985; Rvachew & Jamieson, 1989). Children were presented with words beginning with

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

34

the /m/ phoneme, a phoneme not produced in error by children with persistent speech delay, and were also presented with words beginning with the /r/ phoneme, a phoneme which was produced incorrectly by all children with speech sound disorder in our study. Children in the speech sound disorder group exhibited comparable recognition of /m/ words for sine-wave speech and

CR IP T

amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech, as compared to their peers. For words beginning with the /r/ phoneme, however, children with persistent speech delay did not show a gain in accuracy between sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech, while their peers did, thus supporting our second hypothesis. While children with persistent speech delay did not show

AN US

an overall difficulty perceiving phonemes presented in the context of impoverished speech, they showed specific difficulty perceiving a phoneme that was produced in error. This is consistent with previous research that has demonstrated some children with speech sound disorder have

M

difficulty perceiving words containing phonemes that are distorted in their own speech production (Hoffman et al., 1985; Rvachew & Jamieson, 1989). Some have argued that

ED

imprecise articulation skills may be attributable to a weakness in the development of robust phonological representations (Carroll, Snowling, Hulme, & Stevenson, 2003; Raitano et al.,

PT

2004) for adequate speech production. Deficits in speech perception suggest that children with

CE

speech sound disorder may exhibit weak phonological representations, particularly for erred sounds in their own speech.

AC

By definition, sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech comprise primarily spectral structure. Notably, the children in the persistent speech delay group only exhibited difficulty perceiving the /r/ phoneme, but did not show the same difficulty for the correctly produced /m/ phoneme. While this may lead to the conclusion that these children do not have difficulty perceiving fine temporal structure, it is also possible that these children

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

35

capitalized on the fact that rudimentary amplitude structure was mimicked and provided in the creation of the /m/ stimuli in this study. The /m/ stimuli were created by reducing the overall intensity of the independent sinusoids by 5 dB to mimic reduced amplitude caused by antiresonance in the natural production of nasal phonemes. To further test this hypothesis,

CR IP T

children with persistent speech delay should be given sine-wave speech and amplitude-

comodulated sine-wave speech containing phonemes that are not produced in error, but contain acoustic cues similarly constrained by fine temporal structure as the /r/ phoneme such as /l/. This study, like all studies, is not without limitations that should fuel future work. The

AN US

present study investigated speech perception skills in a small subset of children with dyslexia or persistent speech delay. Importantly, the children included here met stringent criteria in both word reading and articulation skills to isolate phonological deficits specific to dyslexia or

M

persistent speech delay. To date, only one known study of speech perception in children has accounted for both reading and speech production skill (Johnson et al., 2011) and the results of

ED

the present study likewise noted that children with dyslexia and children with speech sound disorder exhibit distinct profiles of speech perception skill. In our study, children provided

PT

verbal responses as well as pointing responses in an attempt to circumvent ambiguous speech

CE

production second to speech articulation errors in the group of children with persistent speech delay. Arguably, having children provide a verbal response during a perceptual task may have

AC

inadvertently interfered with the speech perception process. Future research should seek to mitigate this interference by designing tasks that require no verbal response via spelling responses or alternate paradigms that do not require generative responses. Additionally, in this study we investigated speech perception performance on a limited set of phonemes (i.e., /m/, /r/) in sine-wave speech and amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech. Future work should include

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

36

additional phonemes as well as additional acoustic manipulations to further describe the nature of the perceptual deficits in these children. 5. Conclusion The nature of the underlying perceptual deficits in children with phonological

CR IP T

impairment, whether dyslexia or speech sound disorder, is not well understood. The present study sought to disentangle the commonalities and differences in these children by specifically addressing whether children with dyslexia and children with speech sound disorder responded to spectral structure in speech signals with the same degree of sensitivity. Although all children

AN US

benefitted from added structure (i.e., sine-wave speech vs. amplitude-comodulated sine-wave speech) to combine acoustic cues into meaning, the present study lends support to the notion that children with dyslexia benefit particularly from this additional acoustic structure prior to the

M

engagement of higher-level linguistic processes, as this gain was seen without the addition of phonetic, syntactic, or semantic context. Importantly, when subjected to a perceptual task that

ED

involved an erred phoneme in their own speech production, children with speech sound disorder failed to show significant improvements in perception from sine-wave speech to amplitude-

PT

comodulated sine-wave speech. The present results suggest that children with dyslexia and

CE

speech sound disorder exhibit distinct perceptual strengths and weaknesses when subjected to

AC

impoverished speech stimuli that preserves spectral structure.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

37

Acknowledgements This research was supported by funds from the Barkley Memorial Trust at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln (Cabbage), the American Speech-Language-Hearing Foundation (Cabbage),

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

AN US

CR IP T

and the National Institutes of Health, R03 DC9667 (Hogan).

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

38 References

Anthony, J.L., Aghara, R.G., Dunkelberger, M.J., Anthony, T.I., Williams, J.M., & Zhang, Z. (2011). What factors place children with speech sound disorders at risk for reading problems? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 146- 160.

boys. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 221-243.

CR IP T

Badian, N., McAnulty, G., Duffy, G., & Als, H.S. (1990). Predication of dyslexia in kindergarten

Boada, R., & Pennington, B.F. (2006). Deficient implicit phonological representations in children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 153-93.

AN US

Broen, P.A., Strange, W., Doyle, S.S., & Heller, J.H. (1983). Perception and production

of approximant consonants by normal and articulation-delayed preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 601-608.

M

Brownell, R. (2000a). The Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

ED

Brownell, R. (2000b). The Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

PT

Cabbage, K.L., Carrell, T.D., & Hogan, T.P. (2011). Amplitude modulation effects on

CE

speech perception in children and adults. Poster presentation at American SpeechLanguage-Hearing Association Annual Convention, San Diego, CA.

AC

Carrell, T., & Opie, J. (1992). The effect of amplitude comodulation on auditory object formation in sentence perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 52, 437-437.

Carroll, J.M., Snowling, M.J., Hulme, C., & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913-923. Edwards, J., Fourakis, M., Beckman, M.E., & Fox, R.A. (1999). Characterizing knowledge

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

39

deficits in phonological disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 169-186. Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D.K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The

Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 36-60.

CR IP T

importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items.

Fowler, A. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme

awareness. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research, 105/106, 53-64.

MN: American Guidance Service.

AN US

Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman-Fristoe test of articulation (2nd ed). Circle Pines,

Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: Towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia, 6(2).

M

Goswami, U. (2002). Phonology, reading development, and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 141-163.

ED

Goswami, U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Science, 15(1), 3-10.

PT

Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., & Scott, S.K.

CE

(2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(16), 10911-10916.

AC

Goswami, U., Wang, H.L.S., Cruz, A., Fosker, T., Mead, N., & Huss, M. (2011). Languageuniversal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 325-337.

Gruber, M. (2003). Dyslexics’ phonological processing in relation to speech perception. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Umea University, Umea, Sweden.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

40

Hamalainen, J.A., Salminen, H.K., Leppanen, P.H.T. (2012). Basic processing deficits in dyslexia: Systematic review of the behavioral and event-related potential/field evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities. Hoffman, P.R., Stager, S., & Daniloff, R.G. (1983). Perception and production of

CR IP T

misarticulated /r/. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 210-215.

Hoffman, P.R., Daniloff, R.G., Bengoa, D., & Schuckers, G. (1985). Misarticulating and

normally articulating children‟s identification and discrimination of synthetic [r] and [w]. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 46-53.

AN US

Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G.D.A., Martic, S. & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Learning

M

Memory and Cognition, 23, 1217-1232.

Johnson, E.P., Pennington, B.F., Lowenstein, J.H., & Nittrouer, S. (2011). Sensitivity to

ED

structure in the speech signal by children with speech sound disorder and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(3), 294-314.

PT

Koch, D.B., McGee, T.J., Bradlow, A.R., & Kraus, N. (1999). Acoustic-phonetic approach

CE

toward understanding neural processes and speech perception. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 10, 304-318.

AC

Larivee, L.S., & Catts, H.W. (1999). Early reading achievement in children with expressive phonological disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 118-128.

Lewis D.E., & Carrell, T.D. (2007). The effect of amplitude modulation on intelligibility of time-varying sinusoidal speech in children and adults. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(7), 1140-1151.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

41

Locke, J.L. (1980). The inference of speech perception in the phonological disordered child. Part II: Some clinically novel procedures, their use, some findings. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 45, 445-468. Lorenzi, C., Dumont, A., & Fullgrabe, C. (2000). Use of temporal envelope cues by

CR IP T

children with developmental dyslexia, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1367-1379.

Manis, F.R., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M.S., Keating, P., Doi, L.M., Munson, B., & Petersen, A. (1997). Are speech perception deficits associated with developmental

AN US

dyslexia? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 211-235.

Metsala, J.L. & Walley, A.C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early

M

reading ability. In J.L. Metsala & L.C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89-120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

ED

Monnin, L.M. & Huntington, D.A. (1974). Relationship of articulatory defects to speech- sound identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 17, 352-366.

PT

Muneaux, M., Ziegler, J.C., Truc, C., Thomson, J., & Goswami, U. (2004). Deficit in beat

CE

perception and dyslexia: evidence from French. NeuroReport, 15, 1255-1259. Pasquini, E.S., Corriveau, K.H., & Goswami, U. (2007). Auditory processing of amplitude

AC

envelope rise time in adults diagnosed with developmental dyslexia, Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(3), 259-286.

Preston, J.L., & Edwards, M.L. (2007). Phonological processing skills of adolescents with residual speech sound errors. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 297-308.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

42

Raitano, N.A., Pennington, B.F., Tunick, R.A., Boada, R., & Shriberg, L.D. (2004). Pre-literacy skills of subgroups of children with speech sound disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 821-835.

Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.

CR IP T

Reynolds, C.R., & Kamphaus, R.W. (2003). Reynolds intellectual assessment scales. Lutz, FL:

Rocheron, I., Lorenzi, C., Fullgrabe, C., & Dumont, A. (2002). Temporal envelope perception in dyslexic children. Neuroreport, 13, 1683-1687.

Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: is there a

AN US

deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics, 31, 509-527. Rosner, B.S., Talcott, J.B., Witton, C., Hogg, J.D., Richardson, A.J., Hansen, P.C., Stein, J.F. (2003). The perception of “sine-wave speech” by adults with developmental dyslexia.

M

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 68-79. Rvachew, S. (2007). Phonological processing and reading in children with speech sound

ED

disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 260-270. Rvachew, S., & Jamieson, D.G. (1989). Perception of voiceless fricatives by children with a

PT

functional articulation disorder. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 193-208.

CE

Rvachew, S., Nowak, M., & Cloutier, G. (2004). Effect of phonemic perception training on the speech production and phonological awareness skills of children with

AC

expressive phonological delay. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 250-263.

Rvachew, S., Ohberg, A., Grawburg, M., & Heyding, J. (2003). Phonological awareness and phonemic perception in 4-year-old children with delayed expressive phonology skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(4), 463.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

43

Shapley, K. L. (2003). The interaction of acoustic and linguistic aids to sentence intelligibility. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nebraska- Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska. Shriberg, L.D, Fourakis, M., Hall, S.D., Karlsson, H.B., Lohmeier, H.L., McSweeny, J.L.,...Wilson, D. L. (2010). Extensions to the speech disorders classification system

CR IP T

(SDCD). Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 24, 795-824.

Shuster, L. (1998). The perception of correctly and incorrectly produced /r/. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(4), 941.

Smit, A.B., Hand, L., Freilinger, J.J., Bernthal, J.E., & Bird, A. (1990). The Iowa articulation

AN US

norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 779-798.

Storkel, H.L., & Hoover, J.R. (2010). An on-line calculator to compute phonotactic

M

probability and neighborhood density based on child corpora of spoken American English. Behavior Research Methods, 42(2), 497-506.

ED

Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Mody, M. (1995). Auditory temporal perception deficits in the reading-impaired: A critical review of the evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin &

PT

Review, 2(4), 508-514.

CE

Sutherland, D., & Gillon, G.T. (2005). Assessment of phonological representations in children with speech impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the

AC

Schools, 36, 294-307. Sutherland, D., & Gillon, G.T. (2007). Development of phonological representations and phonological awareness in children with speech impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 42, 229-250.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

44

Swan, D. & Goswami, U. (1997). Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,66(1), 18-41.

children. Brain and Language, 9, 182-198.

CR IP T

Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in

Thomson, J.M., Richardson, U., & Goswami, U. (2005). Phonological similarity neighborhoods and children‟s short-term memory: Typical development and dyslexia. Memory & Cognition, 33(7), 1210-1219.

AN US

Tice, R., & Carrell, T. (2011). Tone: Tone-analog waveform synthesizer. Available at UNL Digital Commons.

Tice, R., & Carrell, T. (2011). Amod32: Amplitude modulation signal processor. Available at

M

UNL Digital Commons.

Torgesen, J.K., Wagner, R.K., & Rashotte, C.A. (1999). TOWRE: Test of Word Reading

ED

Efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

Vandermosten, M., Boets, B., Luts, H., Poelmans, H., Golestani, N., Wouters, J., &

PT

Ghesquiere, P. (2010). Adults with dyslexia are impaired in categorizing speech and

CE

nonspeech sounds on the basis of temporal cues. Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, 107(23), 10389-10394.

AC

Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.

Woodcock, R. (1998). The Woodcock reading mastery tests-Revised-Normative Update. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Ziegler, J.C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., & Lorenzi, C. (2009). Speech-perception-in- noise

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

45

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

AN US

CR IP T

deficits in dyslexia. Developmental Science, 12(5), 732-745.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT SPEECH PERCEPTION DIFFERENCES

46 Appendix A

Table 1. Word recognition accuracy for each child in each group (dyslexia, persistent speech delay, typically-developing) for sine-wave speech (SWS) and amplitude co-modulated sine-wave speech (AMSWS) for /m/ and /r/ phonemes. Scores are reported as percent correct (out of 12 words in each condition). /m/

CE

AC

/r/ AMSWS 100.00% 83.33% 91.67% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 83.33% 66.67% 100.00% 75.00% 91.67% 100.00%

SWS 100.00% 83.33% 58.33% 75.00% 16.67% 41.67% 66.67% 66.67% 91.67% 16.67% 41.67% 83.33%

AMSWS 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 50.00% 100.00% 100.00% 75.00% 100.00% 100.00%

100.00% 66.67% 25.00% 91.67% 100.00% 41.67% 91.67% 0.00% 100.00% 16.67% 41.67% 91.67%

100.00% 91.67% 66.67% 91.67% 100.00% 75.00% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 25.00% 83.00% 91.67%

46.17% 33.33% 25.00% 66.67% 66.67% 91.67% 83.33% 0.00% 100.00% 91.67% 100.00% 100.00%

33.33% 75.00% 58.33% 100.00% 75.00% 58.33% 100.00% 58.33% 83.33% 100.00% 83.33% 58.33%

100.00% 83.33% 100.00% 91.67% 91.67% 100.00% 83.33% 91.67% 100.00% 66.67% 83.33% 50.00%

83.33% 100.00% 50.00% 100.00% 91.67% 91.67% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 50.00% 83.33%

83.33% 100.00% 91.67% 100.00% 100.00% 75.00% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 100.00% 83.33% 91.67%

AN US

M

ED 66.67% 100.00% 58.33% 100.00% 75.00% 100.00% 75.00% 75.00% 100.00% 83.33% 41.67% 0.00%

CR IP T

SWS 100.00% 41.67% 66.67% 66.67% 83.33% 41.67% 0.00% 41.67% 33.33% 50.00% 16.67% 75.00%

PT

Child ID Dyslexia D1 D2 D3 D4 D5 D6 D7 D8 D9 D10 D11 D12 Persistent Speech Delay SD1 SD2 SD3 SD4 SD5 SD6 SD7 SD8 SD9 SD10 SD11 SD12 Typically-Developing TD1 TD2 TD3 TD4 TD5 TD6 TD7 TD8 TD9 TD10 TD11 TD12