Food QualityandPrcfmtlce6 (1995) 121-126 0 1995 Elsevier Science Limited Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved 095c-3293/95/$9.50+.00
0950-3293(94)00021-2
ELSEVIER
THE EFFECTOF LINE ORIENTATIONON THE RECORDING OF TIME-INTENSITY PERCEPTIONOF SWEETENER SOLUTIONS L. M. Duizer,aK. Bloomb & C. J. Findlaya “Compusense Inc., 150 Research Lane, Guelph, Ontario, Canada, NlG 4T2 ‘department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3Gl
(Accqbted11 October1994)
and different
ABSTRACT
scales. Initially,
A trained time-intensity panel
was used to valuate
indicate
the
pen
effect of scale orientation on time-intensity responses. Equisweet samples of aspartame, aces&fame k, sucralose and
eliminate
the transformation
tical
analysis.
commitment
distraction
to the panelists
Birch
1980;
paper.
& Munton
an instrument
To
of the
(1981)
containing
de-
a taste
moved the dial
of the data into a useable
This
procedure
and increased
With the advent
required
form was
a large
the potential
of personal
198Os, computerized
computers
time-intensity
time
for errors. in the early
software
programs
were developed. These programs were used to collect data in a form which could be easily analysed. Programs have differed responses.
in the type of instrument
For example,
used to record
et al., (1985)
Guinard
intro-
duced the use of a joystick which could be moved within a vertical slot, while Lee (1985) attached a game
INTRODUCTION
paddle
sensory
evaluation
allows for the measurement the
time-intensity
methodological Time-intensity
(Schwartz,
strip chart
cumbersome. The curves were measured manually, and the results were then entered into a computer for statis-
Keywords: time-intensity; sweetness; sweeteners; scale orientation.
recording
vertically
1978;
tensity curves of all responses. Although the automated approaches collected time-intensity data efficiently,
multi-attribute evaluations of taste.
and
a
on moving
of chart paper,
the SMURF,
to
& Pangborn,
‘dial box’ and ‘taste dial’. The panelists
the presentation of the scales within and amongst panelists. These results demonstrate the use of time-intensity scales on two dimensions and suggest the possibility of
texture
or
panelists by moving
clockwise in response to changes in sensory perception, and a concealed strip chart recorder provided time-in-
Intensity responses on the horizontal scale. The parame ters of Decrease Angle, Decrease Area and Area Under the Curve were also significantly larger when vertical scales were used than when horizontal scales were used. We suggest that diff erences can be minimized by anchoring refmence samples to the scales and by counterbalancing
in the
1983)
the possible
movement veloped
required
perceptions
(Larson-Powers
et al., 1984) & Lee,
of time-intensity
procedures
time-intensity
horizontally
Schmitt
horizontal and vertical scales yielded similar results. Howevq Maximum Intensity responses on the vertical scale were approximately 13% greater than Maximum
Time-intensity
automated
their
DuBois
9 % sucrose were presented to 10 panelists fm evaluation on both horizontal and vertical scales. For the most part,
visual representations
is a method
of the temporal
flavour
of food.
responses
have
which
At present,
changes
Devices
With the latter instrument,
there are two commercially
ware packages
for
for the measurement
taste perceptions.
undergone
pan-
Both computer
available
soft-
of time-intensity
packages
are used to
collect data over a programmed period of time. However, they differ in the visual orientation of the scale on which the panelists indicate the intensity of their per-
improvement since their inception. data were originally collected by timing
panelists with a stop watch as they recorded their responses to intensity on graph paper (Neilson, 1957). Since then, researchers have developed both automated and computerized time-intensity These procedures have employed different
to a computer.
elists turned a dial on a ‘game paddle’ to move an ‘x’ along a horizontal line shown on the computer monitor.
ception. The PSA-system provides a vertical scale on which the panelists indicate their perceptions of the attribute (Cliff & Heymann, 1993). In contrast, the
procedures. instruments
CSATpATMtemporal 121
profile analysis module
developed
by
122
L. M. Duizer, K. Bloom, C. J. Findlay
Compusense,
provides
a horizontal
scale (equivalent
to
60 pixels) on which the panelists indicate their perception. Compusense
is currently
simultaneously attributes.
measure
This
requires
scales displayed
horizontal
effect
time-intensity
data
scale with the judgement
a vertical
scale to determine
affects response
a
using
scale orientation
eners
on both
testing
AND
eners
in
isosweet paired
distilled
water.
to 9% sucrose,
sweetener
aspartame,
All
testing.
k; 0.018%
each sweetener
samples.
During
were evaluated
each
with a 5-
The rating scale appeared
as a horizontal
line for four samples and
order.
four samples in a ran-
During
the panelists
crackers
scales. Four
conducted.
samples
the samples,
the 5-min break
cleansed
their palate
and distilled water.
concentrations
0.05%
sucralose.
were poured
were
program of the
of each
Each
aspartame;
Twenty
analysis parameter curve
0.08%
millilitres
of
(CSA
is defined
were
were
curves V4.3).
in Table
horizontally
from
CSATrATM
1 provides and
a
each
1. of the time-intensity
to determine
rated the four sweeteners
the
parameters
parameters
analysed
extracted
using Figure
time-intensity
of the eight
presented
into a 50 ml cup labelled
parameters
time-intensity
diagram
earlier
through
The concentrations
time-intensity
individual
acesulfame
using food grade sweet-
as determined
were: 9% sucrose;
acesulfame
eight
present.
the four sweet-
and vertical
were
and balanced
with unsalted
Eight (sucrose,
were prepared
comparison
line, Testing
Analysis of the time-intensity data
solutions
k and sucralose)
evaluated
the horizontal
min break between
between
METHODS
perception.
was no longer
as a vertical line for the remaining
Sample preparation Four model
the time-intensity
the panelists
of testing
session,
domized
MATERIALS
perception
testing,
on the monitor
to taste perception.
along
in sweetness
of each sample ended when the panelists moved the cursor back to zero on the time-intensity line, indicat-
replications
using
of sweetness
whether
the changes
During
In the present
of sweetness
to move a cursor
ing that sweetness
The
in the literature.
judgements
to
sensory
and vertical
monitor.
on the resultant
reported
study we compared horizontal
both
a system
dependent
on the computer
of scale orientation has not been
developing
two time
mouse
reflecting
differently or vertically.
whether
panelists
when the scale was Orientation
(2) X
with a random Sdigit blinding code. Each covered with a lid and a straw was inserted
cup was through
Sweeteners (4) repeated-measures analyses of variance were conducted for each parameter. When significant
a hole
at room
differences
in the lid. All sweeteners
were served
eners
temperature.
between
occurred,
measures
or amongst
multiple
orientations
comparisons
and sweet-
with repeated
t-tests were conducted.
Time-intensity evaluations All training
and testing
sessions were conducted
Compusense Sensory Research Canada). Ten trained panelists, time-intensity
testing of sweeteners
at the
Centre (Guelph, experienced in participated
in this
RESULTS
AND
When
across
the four sweeteners,
from
vertical
averaged
ratings
differed
study. Prior to testing, the panelists attended eight l-h training sessions to familiarize themselves with the use
eight time-intensity p = 0.003); AUC
of horizontal
ANGLE (F(1,9)
and vertical
line scales.
During
training,
6.17, p= 0.035);
Computerized
four sweeteners
ysis software Guelph, zontal
( CSAr,,TM ) (CSA V4.3;
Canada)
tion of either line
horizontal
Analysis Temporal
was modified
a vertical scale
Profile
Anal-
Compusense
Inc,
to make
the presenta-
line scale or the existing
possible.
Both
the
vertical
horiand
lines were 60 pixels long and were labelled
with the anchors ‘not sweet’ and ‘very sweet’. The time-intensity software (CSATpATM)was programmed to collect responses every 0.5 s for a total of 60 s (120 records). The panelists were instructed to drink the
tation,
there
ratings
horizontal
for four
of the
parameters: ZMAX (F( 1,9) = 16,57, (F(1,9) = 12.24, p = O-007); DEC
= 5.59, p = 0.042);
samples of sweetener similar to those used in testing were presented to the panelists for evaluation. The Sensory
DISCUSSION
DECAREA
see Fig. 2. When averaged
were significant
differences
(F(1,9)
=
across orienamongst
for three of the eight time-intensity
the pa-
rameters:
AUC(F(3,27) = 3.88, p = 0.020); INCANGLE (3,27) = 4.45, p = 0.011); DECAREA (F(3,27) = 4.09, p = O-016); see Fig. 3. Representative
time-intensity
curves
for horizontal
and vertical orientation for each sweetener are shown in Fig. 4. Average curves produced by simple arithmetic averaging are skewed toward the panelist with the longest duration and do not provide an accurate illustration of the individual parameters. For this reason,
sample through the straw, hold it in their mouths for 3 s and then swallow. Time-intensity evaluations were started immediately upon ingestion to capture the time
the curves in Fig. 4 are the time-intensity curves from one panelist who is in the middle of the range. These curves graphically illustrate the individual time-inten-
course
sity parameters.
of maximum
intensity.
The
panelists
used
a
Time-Intensity Perqbtion of Sweetener Solutions
INC ANGLE
+
123
DECANGLE
DEC AREA
FIG.
1. Time-intensity
TABLE
curve and parameters.
1. Time-intensity
Parameters
and their Definitions
Parameter Maximum
intensity
Time to maximum Duration
Abbreviation
Definition
ZMAX
The maximum sweetness intensity (up to 60 pixels) of each sample. The time (in seconds) at maximum intensity. The time (in seconds) for sweetness perception (from first perception to the end of the perception). The angle of increase to maximum intensity. This can be interpreted to be the rate of onset of sweetness of the sample. The area under the increasing portion of the curve. The angle of decrease from maximum intensity. This can be interpreted to be the rate of decrease of sweetness perception. The area under the decreasing portion of the curve. The total area under the time-intensity curve.
TMAX DUR
intensity
Increase
angle
ZNC ANGIE
Increase Decrease
area angle
ZNCAREA DEC ANGLE DEC AREA AUC
Decrease area Area Under the Curve
450 T
30
65
I
300 -
T
400 c
29
250 350 _c
28 300 4 27
250 :
: s
200
26
$
3 G
150
200 I 150
25 100 24
61
I
60 I
23 DANGLE
AUC
FIG. 2. Mean values for significant
time-intensity
parameters
affected
by orientation.
DAREA
i
OHorizontd
8Vertical
1
124
L. M. Duizeq K. Bloom, C. J. Findlay 82 _
500 450
81
400 350
80
300 3
200 =
250
i
;
9 19
2
I50
200 78
150 100 50 0
76 INC
AUC
DEC
ANGLE
AREA
FIG. 3. Mean values for significant time-intensity parameters affected by sweetener.
50 -
W
(a)
40 ..
.: 0
5
10
:
I5
!
:
:
20
:
:
25
Time Iatcrvd
:
:
30
:
:
35
:
40
.,,,
I 45
0
5
10
,,,,,,,,,( 15
20
25
30
35
40
45
Time Intern1 (I)
(8)
50 -
50 @I
@I 40 -.
40 -P
: : : 0
5
10
I5
:
20
:
: 25
Time lntervd
FIG. 4. Time-intensity
:
: 30
:
: 35
: 40
:
I 45
(s)
:::::: 0
5
10
IS
:::;::* 20
25
30
35
40
45
Time Intcrvd (I)
curves from one individual for each sweetener: (a) sucralose; (b) aspartame; (c) acesulfame k; (d) sucrose.
Multiple comparisons with repeated-measures t-tests were conducted to isolate the differences between sweeteners for the three significant parameters. The results indicated that the area under the curve was significantly greater for sucralose as compared with aspartame (t(9) = 2.61, p = O-028)) and with sucrose (t(9) = 2.32, p= 0.046). Lik ewise, the area under the curve was significantly greater for acesulfame k compared with aspartame (t(9) = 2.80, p = 0.021)) and with sucrose (t(9) = 2.31, p = 0.046). The angle of increasing intensity was significantly greater for acesulfame k as compared with sucralose (t(9) = 3.31, p = 0.009), and with aspartame (t(9) = 4.01, p = 0.003). Finally, the area under the
decreasing portion of the curve was significantly greater for sucralose as compared with aspartame (t(9) = 3.04, p = O-014), and with sucrose (t(9) = 2.51, p = 0.033), and greater for acesulfame k as compared with aspartame (t(9) = 2.64, p = 0.027). There were no significant Orientation X Sweetener interaction effects for any of the eight parameters (Table 2). This means that differences in perceptions of the four sweeteners (sucralose, acesulfame k, aspartame, and sucrose) were not affected by the orientation of the time-intensity scale. These results provide evidence that both horizontal and vertical scales can be used to assess time-intensity sweetness responses. In addition, the lack of significant
ofSweetener Solutions
Time-Intensity Per@&m differences
between
horizontal
and vertical
scales for
among panelists to homogenize
orientation
the parameters of TMAX, INC ANGLE, INC AREA and DUR provide support for the use of either horizontal or
Results from this study, regardless were similar to those obtained by other
vertical
dependent
time-intensity
It has been affects
the
area
significantly
scales.
demonstrated under
that maximum
the
curve,
high correlations
the significant observed
by
the IMAX and
1992).
In this study
AUC, DEC A??EA and DEC ANGLE results
between
the two orientations
with the significant
1A4AXvalues
Lawless & Clark (1992) evaluation
as indicated
between
the AUC and DEC AREA (Duizer,
intensity
have defined
as something
because Noble
temporal be
to be
greater
line.
Maximum
the vertical that rated
bias. Panelists’
sweetness
intensity
(IMAX)
2). Perhaps
tally. A vertical
on than
These
scale
(Fig.
(1991)
groups
for
movement
strength
of the arm requires
and
hence,
reducing
mouse
along the vertical
not
for
the precision
the use
line. In contrast,
less strength The shoulder
1991).
left
This movement
trolled excursion The
motor
to
is required
of the re-
to move the
and wrist are rotated
right
movement
produces
a smaller,
observed
tions may be minimized
between
to reduce
the
to
(Chaffin, better
of the mouse on the horizontal
differences
control,
of the mouse along the vertical
arm horizontally.
the
fine
in the movement
line. This lack of precision
sults in a larger excursion
accommodate
con-
line.
two orienta-
possible
bias due to
tion on the vertical and horizontal
lines may increase
the
precision of the panel (O’Mahony & Wong, 1989). Further training with reference samples might allow the panelists to become
more familiar with both orientations.
orientation
can
be counterbalanced
Finally,
within
and
Orientation
IMAX
TMAX DUR AUC INCANGLE INCAREA DECANGLE DECAREA (N.S.=p>0.05)
for acesulfame
k indicates
perception
that acesulfame aspartame
by
significantly of acesul-
and sucralose. by Ott et d.,
k had a faster
and alitame.
Time-intensity evaluations may be performed horizontal and vertical orientations. Although the time-intensity ferent,
0.003 N.S. N.S. 0.007 N.S. N.S. 0.042 0.035
Sweetener
Orientation X Sweetener
N.S. N.S.
N.S. N.S. N.S.
N.S.
0.016 0.011 N.S. N.S. 0.018
N.S. N.S.
N.S. N.S. N.S.
parameters
the muscle
groups
zontally compared These
findings
support
scales
difverti-
may be due to the mouse
the use of both
to collect
attributes
will provide
dependent
to move
is greater
hori-
to vertically.
for two different opment
intensity
This difference
required
in both most of
are not significantly
the value for maximum
cally than horizontally.
changes
horizontal
time-intensity simultaneously.
a new
tool
responses This devel-
to investigate
in foods and consumer
time
products.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Financial
support
the Industrial tional
TABLE 2. Summary of pvalues for Sweetener and Orientation Parameter
intensity
The
CONCLUSIONS
and vertical
orientation of the line. For example, the presentation of reference samples to anchor responses in the same posi-
scale
maximum
results were similar to those observed
rate of onset than sucrose,
max’
which was obtained
than that of aspartame
who concluded
of the
the mouse vertically versus horizon-
of the biceps, brachialis and triceps muscles which are the larger muscles in the arm. These larger muscles are used
of onset to the ‘rate
intensity.
INC ANGLE observed
fame k is quicker
higher
muscle
the rate
to reach
maximum
affects
in fNC ANGLE
is similar (1990)
of
must
concentration
reflects
measure
the height to
sucrose.
sweeteners
Differences
that the rate of onset of sweetness
13%
this is due to the different
involved in moving
rated
time
time-intensity
scale was approximately on the horizontal
responses
of the time-intensity
sweetener
by Ott & Palmer
by dividing
entation
as
This
to 9%
measurement,
sweeteners
reported
equisweet
parameters.
sweeteners.
dif-
for the four sweeteners,
have stated that for the purpose
perception
equisweet,
the
by the orientation
were
amongst
2, Fig. 2). a bias in sensory
which causes a response
could be a potential
the
samples
effects.
of orientation, researchers. In-
there were no significant
intensity
et al. (1991)
an inaccurate reflection of intensities. The results from this study suggests that for certain parameters line oriwere affected
of orientation, in maximum
time-intensity
are concomitant
(Table
ferences
125
Research
for this study was provided
Research
Assistance
Council
Canada.
not have been completed
without
Program
in part by of the Na-
This research the generous
could cooper-
ation of our panelists.
REFERENCES Birch, G. G. & Munton, S. L. (1981). Use of the ‘Smurf’ in taste analysis. Chem. Senses, 6,45-52. ChafYin, D. B. (1991). &~~puti~na~Biomchanics.John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Toronto, p. 397. Cliff, M. & Heymann, H. (1993). Development and use of time-intensity methodology for sensory evaluation: A review. FoodResearchInternational, 26, 375-385.
126
L. M. Duizer, K Bloom, C.J. Findlay
Dubois, G. E. & Lee, J. F. (1983). A simple technique for the evaluation of temporal taste properties. Chem.Senses,7,237-247. Duizer, L. M. (1992). An assessment of sensory time-intensity and instrumental methods for bovine muscle tenderness measurements. M.Sc. Thesis. University of Guelph, Guelph, ON. Guinard, J-X., Pangborn, R. M. and Shoemaker, C. F. (1985). Computerized procedure for time-intensity sensory measurements. Journal ofFood Science, 50,543-546. Larson-Powers, N. & Pangborn, R. M. (1978). Paired comparison and time-intensity measurements of the sensory prop erties of beverages and gelatins containing sucrose or synthetic sweeteners. Journal ofFood Science, 43,41-46. Lawless, H. T. & Clark, C. C. (1991). Psychological Biases in time-intensity scaling. Food Technology, 46, 81,84-86,90. Lee, W. E. (1985). Evaluation of time-intensity sensory responses using a personal computer. Journal of Food Science, 50,1750-1753. Neilson, A. J. (1957). Time-intensity studies. Drug and Cosm. Indusby, 80,88-93.
Noble, A. C., Matysiak, N. L. & Bonnans, S. (1991). Factors affecting the time-intensity parameters of sweetness. Food Technology, 45, 121-126. O’Mahony, M. & Wong, S-Y. (1989). Time-intensity scaling with judges trained to use a calibrated scale: Adaptation, salty and umami tastes. Journal of Sensq Studies, 3, 217-236. Ott, D. B., Edwards, C. L. & Palmer, S. J. (1991). Perceived taste intensity and duration of nutritive and non-nutritive sweeteners in water using time-intensity evaluations. Journal ofFood Science, 56,535-542. Ott, D. B. & Palmer, S. J. (1990). Ingestion and expectoration sampling methods of four tastes in a model system using time-intensity evaluations. Journal of Sensmy Studies, 13, 53-70. Schmitt, D. J., Thompson, L. J., Malek, D. M. & Munroe, J. H. (1984). An improved method for evaluating time-intensity data. Journal of Sensory Studies, 49, 539-542,580. Schwartz, M. (1980). Sensory screening of synthetic sweeteners using time-intensity evaluation. Journal of Food Science, 45,577-581.