The shape of the functional response curve of Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) is affected by recent experience

The shape of the functional response curve of Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) is affected by recent experience

Accepted Manuscript The shape of the functional response curve of Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) is affected by recent experience ...

667KB Sizes 0 Downloads 19 Views

Accepted Manuscript The shape of the functional response curve of Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) is affected by recent experience Maryam Yazdani, Michael Keller PII: DOI: Reference:

S1049-9644(15)00097-3 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocontrol.2015.05.004 YBCON 3271

To appear in:

Biological Control

Received Date: Accepted Date:

26 December 2014 11 May 2015

Please cite this article as: Yazdani, M., Keller, M., The shape of the functional response curve of Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) is affected by recent experience, Biological Control (2015), doi: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocontrol.2015.05.004

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

For publication in: Biological control The shape of the functional response curve of Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) is affected by recent experience Maryam Yazdani1, Michael Keller1* 1

School of Agriculture, Food and Wine, Waite Campus, University of Adelaide, SA 5005

*

Corresponding author: [email protected]

1

1

ABSTRACT

2

Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) is the most commonly collected

3

parasitoid of light brown apple moth, Epiphyas postvittana (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae;

4

LBAM) in Australia. We studied the functional response of D. tasmanica, and the effect of

5

recent experience on this behaviour. The functional response was evaluated in wind tunnels

6

and enclosed cages. In both arenas, D. tasmanica exhibited a sigmoid functional response,

7

but there was no clear tendency for a deceleration in the functional response curve at high

8

host densities as would be expected with a typical type III functional response. Another

9

experiment revealed that recent experience with high host densities increases the proportion

10

of hosts parasitized by D. tasmanica, which explains much of the difference between the

11

observed functional response curve and a typical type III curve. In general the searching

12

behaviour of D. tasmanica varies in response to host density in a manner that directly affects

13

its searching rate. Our results have contributed to understanding the behaviour of this

14

parasitoid and indicate its capacity to control its host under laboratory conditions. At lower

15

host densities that are characteristic of field populations, D. tasmanica responded in a

16

density-dependent manner that should contribute to suppression of pest populations before

17

they reach economically damaging levels.

18

Key words: density dependence, searching behaviour, vineyard, braconid wasp, parasitoid-

19

host

20 21 22 23 24 25

2

26

Introduction

27

The functional response of a parasitoid to changing host density provides important

28

information on mechanisms underlying parasitoid-host dynamics (Lipcius and Hines, 1986)

29

and is an essential component of parasitoid-host models (Jeschke et al., 2002). The nature of

30

the functional response determines whether a parasitoid is able to regulate the density of its

31

host (Murdoch and Oaten, 1975). Usually, it is classified into one of three general types

32

(Holling, 1959) named I, II and III, which respectively describe curves that are linear,

33

concave increasing to an asymptote and sigmoid when numbers of parasitised hosts per

34

female are plotted against host density (Figure 1a). However, theoretically there are other

35

possible forms, such as type IV, type V and a functional response with parasitoid interference

36

(Hassell, 1978; Abrams, 1982; Taylor, 1984; Turchin, 2001). Parasitoids displaying a type II

37

response cause maximum parasitism rates at low host density. But with type III functional

38

responses, Parasitism rates increase when host density is low, and then decline at higher host

39

densities as handling time reduces the time available for searching. Holling (1959) suggested

40

that the type II response may be typical of invertebrates, including parasitoids, whereas type

41

III responses are characteristic of species that switch between prey species and display

42

learning. Later work indicated that parasitoids can display type III curves (Fernández-Arhex

43

and Corley, 2003). The population consequences of each type of response are different.

44

Whereas a type I response implies a density-independent parasitoid attack rate, a type II

45

response leads to inverse density-dependent predation or parasitism. The type III functional

46

response is the only response which may lead to direct density dependence when host

47

densities are low, and thus can potentially stabilize parasitoid-host interactions (Hassell et al.,

48

1977; Hassell, 1978; Collins et al., 1981; Chesson and Rosenzweig, 1991; Berryman, 1999;

49

Bernstein, 2000; Fernández-arhex and Corley, 2003).

50

3

51

In unstructured models, sigmoid (type III) functional responses have the potential to stabilize

52

parasitoid-host dynamics due to density-dependent mortality at low host densities. In

53

contrast, a type II functional response destabilizes the dynamics because the parasitoids cause

54

an inverse density-dependent mortality of the host (e.g. Murdoch and Oaten, 1975; Hassell,

55

1978). So, distinguishing between type II and type III functional responses is critical in

56

understanding the parasitoid-host dynamics. Due to the importance of functional responses in

57

ecological processes, numerous empirical studies have characterized functional responses in a

58

variety of predator-prey systems (Fernández-arhex Arhex and Corley, 2003; Okuyama,

59

2013). Therefore in order to understand and predict the parasitoid’ success as control agent,

60

the other aspects of the parasitoid behaviour that might affect the functional response deserve

61

more attention (Fernández-arhex and Corley, 2003).

62

Since Holling’s (1959) seminal work, a number of experiments in a variety of species, as

63

well as theoretical studies have been carried out that draw attention to problems involved in

64

measuring the functional response. On the one hand, it has been debated whether the design

65

of some controlled experiments are representative of the behaviour that leads to the true

66

shape of the functional response curve and how these should be carried out. On the other

67

hand, the statistical analyses of the data and the mathematical models used in analyses have

68

been widely discussed (Livdahl and Stiven, 1983; Houck and Strauss, 1985; Williams and

69

Juliano, 1985; Juliano and Williams, 1987; Trexler et al., 1988; Casas and Hulliger, 1994;

70

Manly and Jamienson, 1999; Juliano, 2001; Fernández-arhex and Corley, 2003). Van

71

Lenteren and Bakker (1976) suggested that the apparent absence of a stabilizing density

72

dependence functional response in invertebrate predators or parasitoids may be caused by

73

experimental procedures in which the numbers of prey or hosts at low densities are higher

74

than what can be expected in the field. Hassell et al. (1977) in turn, argued that the practice of

4

75

doing experiments in a relatively simple laboratory universe may ignore the full range of

76

behaviours which invertebrate predators are capable of showing.

77

Juliano and Williams (1987) pointed out that the type of functional response can be most

78

readily determined by analysing the relationship between the proportion of hosts parasitised

79

and host density (Figure 1b).

80

differ fundamentally. The proportion of parasitised hosts is not affected by varying low host

81

densities with a type I response, and with a type II response the proportion declines

82

monotonically from the maximum at the lowest host density. In contrast, the proportion of

83

parasitised hosts the proportion of parasitised hosts increases at low host densities, peaks at

84

an intermediate density and then declines in a manner similar to type I and II responses at

85

high densities.

86

detected using logistic regression (Juliano 2001).

At low host densities, the tree types of functional responses

This difference between type I, II and III responses can be analytically

87

Insect parasitoids are important subjects of behavioural and population studies because

88

they are remarkably common in nature, are typically easy to rear and handle and, more

89

importantly, are key species for the biological control of many insect pests (Waage and

90

Hassell, 1982; Godfray, 1994; Fernández-arhex and Corley, 2003). It is for this reason that

91

functional responses have been investigated in many insect parasitoids (Fernández-arhex and

92

Corley, 2003). Understanding of the nature of the functional response should indicate likely

93

patterns of parasitism, which is important in evaluating population dynamics and the capacity

94

of a parasitoid to contribute to biological control.

95

In this paper we report the results of a series of experiments designed to investigate the

96

functional

response

of

a

parasitic

wasp,

97

(Hymenoptera: Braconidae). It parasitises the light brown apple moth (LBAM), Epiphyas

98

postvittana (Walker) (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) and other tortricids. LBAM is a polyphagous

99

native species in South-eastern Australia, where it is a key pest in vineyards. It has been 5

Dolichogenidea

tasmanica

(Cameron)

100

introduced to Western Australia, New Zealand, Hawaii, England, and California (Suckling

101

and Brockerhoff, 2010). D. tasmanica is a commonly collected parasitoid of E. postvittana

102

(Paull and Austin, 2006). It is an arrhenotokous, solitary, koinobiont endoparasitoid of the

103

first three instars of LBAM (Yazdani et al., 2014), however no previous study of its

104

functional response has been reported. Our goals were: (1) to characterise the functional

105

response of female D. tasmanica to changing densities of second instar LBAM, (2) to

106

elucidate some of the key factors that affect the shape of functional response curve. A series

107

of experiments was carried out in small wind tunnels to present conditions in which the

108

parasitoid could detect and respond to host cues more naturally because of air flow.

109

Materials and methods

110

Rearing parasitoid and host

111

A laboratory colony of E. postvittana was reared at 22 ± 2º C and a photoperiod of 12 L: 12

112

D on an artificial diet. A culture of D. tasmanica was established from individuals collected

113

from South Australian vineyards. The wasps were reared on larval LBAM infesting plantain,

114

Plantago lanceolata (L.), at 23 ± 2 º C, 14 L: 10 D (for details see Yazdani et al., 2014).

115

Functional response in wind tunnels

116

The functional response of D. tasmanica was investigated in four identical wind tunnels (for

117

details see Yazdani et al., 2014). The wind tunnels had inside dimensions of 35 cm (H)× 50

118

cm (L)× 30 cm (W). The mean wind speed was 29 ± 0.67 cm/s (mean ± SD). Each wind

119

tunnel contained 20 small grape leaves (variety Chardonnay; 3.5 - 4.5 cm L and 4 - 4.5 cm

120

W). Each leaf was placed in a 10 mm diam. × 50 mm glass vial filled with water, and vials

121

were placed 5 cm apart in four rows in the wind tunnel. Six densities of second instar LBAM

122

were tested independently 1, 2, 4, 8, 16 and 32, with 27, 17, 8, 9, 8 and 4 replications

123

respectively. The number of replicates was varied with density in order to achieve equivalent

124

levels of precision across the range of densities that was evaluated. Four experiments were 6

125

run concurrently, with densities chosen at random. For each density, leaves were randomly

126

infested with larvae 24 h before the experiment. Every morning newly emerged females D.

127

tasmanica were collected and caged overnight with 5 males to ensure mating. Naïve 1-2 old

128

mated females were used in the experiments. In order to stimulate the naïve wasps before

129

starting the experiment, each wasp was exposed to a second instar host and allowed to sting it

130

once. The wasp was then released in the wind tunnel 10 cm downwind from the first row of

131

leaves. After 2 h, the wasps were removed, and the LBAM larvae were collected and placed

132

in 100 ml plastic cups that contained a grape leaf for food. They were kept at room

133

temperature for 4 days and then dissected to determine the frequency of parasitism of larvae

134

by D. tasmanica.

135

Functional response in cages

136

In order to determine if the experimental arena affects the shape of the functional response

137

curve, a second experiment was conducted in cages. In this experiment, six densities of

138

second instar LBAM were presented to wasps, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16 and 32, with 26, 14, 6, 6, 10 and

139

4 replications, respectively. The experiments were conducted in plastic containers with inside

140

dimensions of 17 cm (H) × 20 cm (L) × 13 cm (W). The container was modified by removing

141

one side and replacing it with nylon mesh of the same dimension to allow for aeration. For

142

each density, larval LBAM were placed randomly on 6 grape leaves. Each leaf was placed in

143

a 10 mm diam. × 50 mm glass vial filled with water, and vials were placed 5 cm apart in

144

three rows in the cage. The larval LBAM were exposed to a naïve 1-2 old mated female for 2

145

hours. Parasitism data were recorded as described in the previous experiment.

146

Analysis of functional response curves

147

The data from both functional response experiments were analysed using the approach

148

described by Juliano (2001) which comprised two steps: model selection and hypothesis

149

testing. In Julianos’ approach to determine the shape of functional response a logistic 7

150

regression of proportion of parasitised hosts versus number of host recommended. Also,

151

Trexler et al. (1988) demonstrated that the most effective way to distinguish type II and III

152

functional responses involves logistic regression. The second step hypothesis testing involves

153

using nonlinear least-squares regression of number of parasitised host versus number offered

154

to estimate parameters of functional response and to compare parameters of different

155

functional responses. So in our analysing first, the fraction of hosts parasitised vs. number

156

present was subjected to logistic regression with linear, quadratic and cubic terms using the

157

glm function (generalised linear model; family = binomial) in the statistical package R

158

(version 3.1.0 (2014-04-10), "Spring Dance"). In both cases the coefficient of the linear term

159

was found to be positive, which indicates a type III functional response. Therefore the data

160

were then fitted to a type III functional response curve using the nls function (nonlinear least

161

squares) of R, using the equation (1) suggested by Hassell et al. (1977) and elaborated by

162

Juliano (2001):

163

(1)

(

N par = N 1 − e

(

− ( d + bN )Tt 1+ cN + dhN + bhN 2

)

)

164

The mean fraction of parasitised hosts was used in these analyses, since the raw data had a

165

binomial error distribution and as a result had high levels of inherent variation, particularly at

166

low host densities. Nevertheless, the nonlinear regressions did not converge to stable

167

solutions for either data set using either raw data or mean.

168

Effect of experience on type of functional response

169

In the third experiment, we sought to determine if a rewarding experience of foraging for

170

hosts would lead to greater subsequent success in locating hosts compared to wasps that

171

searched in such an arena where no host were present, and hence they had a non-rewarding

172

experience. In each replicate of this experiment, a pair of 1-2 day-old mated females was

173

selected. One of them was designated as having a rewarding experience. It was released for 1 8

174

h in a wind tunnel containing 10 grape leaves, each infested with one second instar LBAM.

175

The other wasp was designated as having a non-rewarding experience. It was released into a

176

wind tunnel containing 10 uninfested grape leaves. The leaves were spaced in two rows

177

across the width of the wind tunnel, and separated by a distance of 5 cm. Immediately after

178

capturing them, the wasps with two types of experience were released separately in two wind

179

tunnels which contained two second instar LBAM that were randomly placed on 20 grape

180

leaves. Host larval positions were the same in both wind tunnels within a replicate. The

181

leaves were arranged in four rows of five leaves and separated by a distance of 5 cm. After 2

182

h exposure to wasps, the infested leaves were collected, placed in plastic cups with a grape

183

leaf and kept at room temperature for 4 days. The larvae were dissected to determine the

184

frequency of parasitism. This experiment was replicated 10 times. The differences in

185

proportions of larvae parasitism between the two types of experience were analysed using

186

Fishers’s two-tailed exact test (Zar, 1984) on pooled numbers.

187

In the fourth experiment, we sought to determine if rewarding and non-rewarding

188

searching experiences have a longer term effect on searching behaviour. This experiment

189

compared the behaviour of rewarding and non-rewarding experience wasps when foraging at

190

three host densities after an interval of 8 h had elapsed following the first bout of searching.

191

In all trials, wasps were released into wind tunnels containing 20 grape leaves using the

192

same methods and arrangement as were used in the functional response experiment to allow

193

the results to be compared. Naïve 1-2 day-old mated female parasitoids were used. Females

194

considered to have a rewarding experience were released for 1 h in a wind tunnel in which

195

each leaf was infested with one second instar LBAM. Females with a non-rewarding

196

experience were released for 1 h in a wind tunnel with uninfested grape leaves. After 1 h, the

197

wasps were transferred to an 18 mm diam. × 50 mm glass vials with a drop of honey and

198

sealed with damp cotton. After 8 h, wasps were released again into wind tunnels that 9

199

contained 4, 8 or 16 second instar LBAM that were randomly placed on 20 grape leaves.

200

Pairs of wasps with rewarding and non-rewarding experience were released into separate

201

wind tunnels with the same density of larval LBAM. After 1 h, wasps were removed and the

202

larvae were placed in plastic cups, held for 4 days with grape leaves and dissected as

203

described previously. This experiment was replicated 8 times for each density and the order

204

of treatments was randomised. The data were analysed with Logistic regression, with linear

205

and quadratic terms for host number using the statistical package R.

206

Results

207

Functional responses in wind tunnels and cages

208

A significant positive linear term in the logistic regression analyses indicated that the data

209

conform best to the type III functional response in both the wind tunnel and cage arenas

210

(Tables 1 and 2; Figures 2 a and b). Parasitism levels were higher at the lower host densities

211

in the cages compared to the wind tunnels, but this trend was reversed at the two highest

212

densities. However in neither case could the data be fitted to the nonlinear type III functional

213

response equation (1) elaborated by Juliano (2001), as the nonlinear least squares analyses

214

did not converge on parameter values that were statistically significant.

215

Effect of experience on searching behaviour

216

Wasp searching behaviour was affected by experience. Females that had a rewarding

217

experience parasitised more larvae at low density than those that had a non-rewarding

218

experience (Table 2). When the interval between initial searching was extended to 8 h,

219

females that had a rewarding experience consistently parasitised more hosts than those that

220

had a non-rewarding experience (Z = -7.715, P < 10-13; Figure 3). Host density also affected

221

the fraction of hosts that were parasitised in a non-linear manner (quadratic term for host

222

number from Logistic regression: Z = -2.185, P = 0.0289), which is consistent with a type III

223

functional response. 10

224

Discussion

225

Invertebrate functional responses are normally measured in small and simple arenas. But

226

Hassell et al. (1977) argued that sigmoid responses are more likely to be found when species

227

can express the full range of searching behavior. In order to diminish the influence of

228

artificial laboratory conditions on behavior, we carried out a series of experiments in wind

229

tunnels to present conditions in which the parasitoid could detect host cues more naturally

230

because of air flow. Also, in our experiments host larvae were distributed randomly among

231

host plant leaves to mimic heterogeneity a wasp would encounter in the field. Thus, the

232

parasitoids could move freely from leaf to leaf and express their full set of host finding

233

behaviours. We also conducted experiments in a simple cage to allow us to determine if the

234

type of arena would lead to a substantial change in the functional response. The cages were

235

smaller than the wind tunnels and the range of densities tested was the same in both arenas,

236

so host density should be perceived to be relatively higher in the cages. Overall the shape of

237

the functional response curve was similar in both arenas (Figures 2 a and b). However,

238

parasitism was higher at the lowest host numbers tested in the cages, while it was higher in

239

the wind tunnels at relatively higher host numbers. These results suggest that wasps could

240

find hosts more easily when the density was low in the smaller area. But in the wind tunnel,

241

the ability to track odour plumes using anemotaxis is likely to have led to greater searching

242

efficiency at higher host densities.

243

Our results in both wind tunnel and cage experiments clearly showed the characteristics

244

of a type III functional response for D. tasmanica at low host densities (Figures 4 a and b).

245

Hassell et al. (1977) argued against the notion that type II functional responses are typical of

246

parasitoids, and suggested that sigmoid type III responses may be common. In subsequent

247

research, hymenoptera species such as Venturia canescens (Grav.) and Campoletis chlorideae

248

(Uchida) (Ichneumonidae), Aphidius uzbekistanicus (Luzhetzki), Diaeretiella rapae 11

249

(M'Intosh) and Aphidius salicis (Haliday) (Aphidiidae), Aphidius colemani (Viereck)

250

(Braconidae), Ibalia leucospoides (Hochenwarth) (Ibaliidae) have been shown to exhibit

251

sigmoid functional responses (Fernández-arhex and Corley, 2003). Fernández-arhex and

252

Corley (2003) suggested that the analyses of some functional response experiments may be

253

overestimating type II curves.

254

experiments may force a type II curve on the insect’s behaviour (van Lenteren and Bakker,

255

1976; Walde and Murdoch, 1988; Ives et al., 1999). Furthermore, type II models may have

256

been used to fit data that could be better served by type III models, especially in older work

257

(Fernández-arhex and Corley, 2003).

For instance, it has been suggested that time-limited

258

It is clear that even the mathematical type III functional response curve does not fit the

259

data (Figures 2 a and b). We could not statistically fit a type III model to the data, even

260

though a Logistic regression analysis clearly showed it is type III in nature. This indicates

261

that the proportion parasitized varied in a complex manner. On the one hand, the results

262

indicate the proportion parasitized varied in a manner consistent with a hyperbolic

263

relationship at low host densities (Juliano, 2001). But at densities of 10 or more hosts per 20

264

leaves, the rate of searching must have increased with increasing host density, which lead to

265

the curve to display characteristics of a type I functional response (Figure 4 a). If the

266

proportion parasitized varied in a purely hyperbolic manner with increasing host density, then

267

the proportion parasitized would asymptotically approach a fixed maximum and the

268

relationship between number parasitised and host density would produce a typical sigmoid

269

curve (Figure 4 b). Thus it seems that a more detailed understanding of the factors that

270

influence proportion parasitized is needed in order to develop models of functional response.

271

We suspected that the lack of fit of the data to a type III model was not simply a

272

statistical problem, so we investigated whether the wasp would change its searching

273

behaviour, and hence the estimated searching rate, at higher densities within the timeframe of 12

274

a two hour experiment. There was a highly significant difference between the parasitism rates

275

associated with wasps with rewarding vs. non-rewarding experience (Figure 3). It seems that

276

the searching behaviour of D. tasmanica varies in response to varying host density in a

277

manner that directly affects its searching rate, even over short periods of time. The results of

278

the experiments reported here elucidate two aspects of the behaviour of D. tasmanica. When

279

wasps search in a non-rewarding area, they subsequently reduce the intensity of their

280

searching activity. But if they search where host densities are high and have a rewarding

281

experience, they subsequently search more intensively which leads to increasing the rates of

282

parasitism.

283

It is noteworthy that the effects of rewarding and non-rewarding experience are observed

284

after the relatively short time of one hour (Table 3), and that these effects persist for at least 8

285

hours (Figure 3). This suggests that the wasp assesses host density and learns the

286

characteristics of a rewarding environment. It implies that D. tasmanica uses information

287

from previously visited patches to adjust its subsequent searching decisions. Learning is

288

regarded as an important factor that leads to the expression of a type III functional response

289

(Real 1979). Parasitoids can change their behaviour in a repeatable way and learn through

290

experience (Vet and Groenewold, 1990; Turlings et al., 1993). Here we conclude that the

291

shape of the observed functional response curve is determined in large part by both host

292

density and the effects of recent experience.

293

Hassell et al. (1977) discussed the implications of type III functional responses for

294

species that search when prey densities are low. A reduction in searching effort in a non-

295

rewarding environment which yields a very low fitness return may be an advantage,

296

particularly if it is not possible to leave that environment. By reducing searching effort, a

297

predator or parasitoid may reduce energetic costs until conditions improve. A similar

298

argument may apply to D. tasmanica searching in a large relatively homogeneous vineyard. 13

299

Although costs and gains are less easily defined for parasitoids, they may involve, for

300

example, the costs that arise from metabolism of carbohydrates (Rivero and Casas, 1999) or

301

exposure of the parasitoid to its own natural enemies, both of which must be balanced against

302

the gains that accrue from the number of hosts successfully parasitised. The sigmoid

303

functional response curve of D. tasmanica at low host densities may reflect a strategy that

304

balances the cost of foraging against expected oviposition success.

305

It is important to put the densities used in our experiments into the context of densities

306

that occur in the field. Supperparasitism was not observed in any dissected larvae, which is

307

consistent with our previous observations (Yazdani et al., 2015). No systematic study of the

308

population dynamics of larval LBAM on grapevines has been published. However, treatment

309

thresholds that guide grape growers in decision-making on the application of insecticides to

310

control damaging infestations of LBAM have been published. On table grapes, the number of

311

larvae per 100 shoots at which insecticidal control is recommended is 10 before flowering

312

and five or less thereafter (Department of Primary Industries Victoria, 2010). The treatment

313

threshold for LBAM on wine grapes is reported to be 20 larvae on foliage per 100 shoots

314

(CCW Cooperative, 2008). Grapes have an indeterminate pattern of annual growth, but over

315

the growing season, between 8 and 30 leaves per shoot are commonly present when LBAM is

316

most likely to reach damaging levels (Lebon et al., 2004). Thus the treatment thresholds for

317

LBAM, which indicate relatively high and damaging populations, are in the order of 0.007 to

318

0.025 larvae/ leaf when between 8 and 30 leaves are present on shoots. These densities are far

319

below those used in the present study. There are two important implications that follow from

320

this. First, the highest densities used in our experiments are arguably extremely high relative

321

to those found in natural populations. Therefore it is not ecologically important that the upper

322

asymptote of the functional response curve was not estimated in this study. The asymptotic

323

maximum percentage parasitism is likely to be ecologically significant only in those species 14

324

where it is approached at commonly observed high densities. Second, the sigmoid shape of

325

the functional response curve at low host densities has the greatest relevance for natural

326

populations (Hassell, 1978; Fernández-arhex and Corley, 2003). This sigmoid shape can lead

327

to direct density-dependent parasitism. Arguably, even lower densities should be used in

328

experiments like ours. But this poses significant practical challenges because larger arenas

329

are needed to determine rates of parasitism at such lower densities, and very high levels of

330

replication are needed to precisely estimate mean parasitism at very low densities. We

331

conclude that experiments on functional response should focus on densities that start at the

332

lowest practical number that can be investigated. Such experiments should be conducted in

333

either outdoor arenas or laboratory arenas like wind tunnels that are conducive to the natural

334

expression of searching behaviour. It is also important to recognise that there are practical

335

limitations to make it impossible to investigate parasitoid behaviour at extremely low host

336

densities with precision.

337

The importance of distinguishing between type II and type III functional responses rests

338

on their very different contributions to population stability (Holling, 1959; Murdoch and

339

Oaten, 1975). Only sigmoid functional responses are density-dependent up to some threshold

340

host density. This contributes to stability if average host densities fall below the threshold.

341

Natural enemies that respond to host in density-dependent manner may be able to quickly

342

suppress pest population before they reach economically damage levels (Cappuccino, 1995;

343

Price, 1997). However, Fernandez-Arhex and Corley (2003) examined the functional

344

responses of parasitoids that have been used in classical biological control programs and

345

found no correlation between the type of response and parasitoid success.

346

Paull, Schellhorn and Austin (2014) conducted large-scale field experiments to quantify

347

and characterize the population response of D. tasmanica to different densities of LBAM in

348

the field. In an apparent contradiction to our results, they concluded that the population 15

349

response of D. tasmanica to varying host density was inversely density-dependent, which

350

implies the species exhibits a type II functional response. However, they did not investigate

351

the components of functional and numerical responses that underlie the pattern of parasitism

352

at the population level. They argued that an inversely density-dependent response may be due

353

to inadequate resources such as access to carbohydrates, specific nutrients, shelter or

354

alternative hosts, which are not available or are in short supply in vineyards. This is because

355

parasitoids are likely to expend more energy and time searching for these resources when

356

they are limiting and, as a result, the time available to maximize their response to increasing

357

host density is reduced (Desouhant et al., 2005; Paull, Schellhorn and Austin, 2014). The

358

realised lifetime fecundity of D. tasmanica is also significantly increased in the presence of

359

flowers, although this is a consequence of the increase in longevity, rather than an increase in

360

daily fecundity (Berndt and Wratten, 2005). And without flowers, offspring sex ratios are

361

strongly male biased, but when females have access to flowers an approximately equal sex

362

ratio is produced. Wasps in our experiment were well-fed, so their behaviour should not have

363

been affected by hunger. We conclude that the functional response must be considered in

364

conjunction with other aspects of biology and behaviour when developing models of

365

parasitoid-host population dynamics.

366

Our results showed that D. tasmanica can parasitise LBAM in a density-dependent

367

manner at low host densities, which is important in regulation of host populations. This

368

suggests that D. tasmanica can contribute to biological control of LBAM. Additional studies

369

are needed, however, to investigate the role that experience and learning play in shaping the

370

functional response over the lifetime of a wasp. It is known that experience over time can

371

influence the searching behaviour of the parasitoid V. canescens (Froissart et al., 2012). It is

372

likely that generalist species like D. tasmanica similarly responds to experience with a range

373

of host-related cues over the span of its adult life. 16

374

Acknowledgments

375

Financial support for this research was provided by the Adelaide International Scholarship

376

(ASI) to Maryam Yazdani. The wind tunnels were constructed by Samantha Scarratt and

377

Michael Keller.

378

References

379

Abrams, P., 1982. Functional responses of optimal foragers. Am. Nat. 120, 382-390.

380

Berndt, L.A., Wratten, S.D., 2005. Effects of alyssum flowers on the longevity, fecundity,

381

and sex ratio of the leafroller parasitoid Dolichogenidea tasmanica. Biol. Control 32,

382

65-69. DOI: 10.1016/j.biocontrol.2004.07.014

383

Bernstein, C., 2000. Host-parasitoid models. The story of successful failure. In: Hochberg

384

ME, Ives AR (eds) Parasitoid Population Biology. Princeton University Press,

385

Princeton, New Jersey, USA, pp. 41-57.

386

Berryman, A.A., 1999. The theoretical foundations of biological control. In: Hawkins BA,

387

Cornell HV (eds) Theoretical Approaches to Biological Control. Cambridge University

388

Press, Cambridge, pp. 3-21.

389

Cappuccino, N., 1995. Novel approaches to the study of population dynamics. In: Cappucino

390

N, Price PW (eds) New Approaches and Synthesis. Academic Press, New York, New

391

York, USA, pp. 3-16.

392 393 394 395

Casas, J., Hulliger, B., 1994. Statistical analysis of functional response experiments. Biocontrol Sci. Techn. 4, 133-145. Chesson, P., Rosenzweig, M., 1991. Behaviour, heterogeneity, and the dynamics of interacting species. Ecology 72, 1187-1195.

396

Collins, M.D., Ward, S.A., Dixon, A.F.G., 1981. Handling time and the functional response

397

of Aphelinus thomsoni, a predator and parasite of the aphid Drepanosiphum

398

platanoidis. J. Anim. Ecol. 50, 479-487. 17

399

Department of Primary Industries, Victoria, 2010. Pest monitoring for table grape exports.

400

Department of Primary Industries, Melbourne, Australia. ISBN 978-1-74264-564-3

401

[www.australiangrapes.com.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0003/11388/Pest-monitoring-for-

402

table-grape-exports-Protocol.pdf, accessed 7 June 2014]

403

Desouhant, E., Driessen, G., Amat, I., Bernstein, C., 2005. Host and food searching in a

404

parasitic wasp Venturia canescens: a trade-off between current and future

405

reproduction? Anim. Behav. 70, 145-152.

406

Fernández-arhex, V., Corley, J.C., 2003. The functional response of parasitoids and its

407

implications for biological control. Biocontrol Sci. Techn. 13, 403-413. DOI:

408

10.1080/0958315031000104523

409

Froissart, L., Bernstein, C., Humblot, L., Amat, I., Desouhant, E., 2012. Facing multiple

410

information sources while foraging on successive patches: how does a parasitoid deal

411

with experience? Anim. Behav. 83, 189-199.

412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423

Godfray, H.C.J., 1994. Parasitoids: Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA. Hassell, M.P., Lawton, J.H., Beddington, J.R., 1977. Sigmoid functional response by invertebrate predators and parasitoids. J. Anim. Ecol. 46, 249-262. Hassell, M.P., 1978. The Dynamics of Arthropod Predator-Prey Systems, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA. Hassell, M.P., 2000. The Spatial and Temporal Dynamics of Host-Parasitoid Interactions. Oxford Series in Ecology and Evolution, Oxford University Press, London. Holling, C.S., 1959. Some characteristics of simple types of predation and parasitism. Can. Entomol. 91, 385-398. Houck, M.A., Strauss, R.E., 1985. The comparative study of functional responses: experimental design and statistical interpretation. Can. Entomol. 117, 617-629. 18

424

Ives, A.R., Schooler, S.S., Jagar, V.J., Knuteson, S.E., Grbic, M., Settle, W.H., 1999.

425

Variability and parasitoid foraging efficiency: a case study of pea aphids and Aphidius

426

ervi. Am. Nat., 154, 652-673.

427

Jeschke, M.J., Kopp, M., Tollrian, R., 2002. Predator functional response: discriminating

428

between handling and digesting prey. Ecol. Monograph 72, 95-112. DOI:

429

10.2307/3100087

430

Juliano, S.A., Williams, F.M., 1987. A comparison of methods for estimating the functional

431

response parameters of the random predator equation. J. Anim. Ecol. 56, 641-653.

432

Juliano, S.A., 2001. Nonlinear Curve Fitting: Predation and Functional Response Curves.

433

Scheiner SM, Gurevitch J (eds.), Design and Analysis of Ecological Experiments

434

Chapman and Hall, New York, USA, pp. 178-196.

435

Lebon, E., Pellegrino, A., Tardieu, F., Lecoeur, J., 2004. Shoot development in grapevine

436

(Vitis vinifera) is affected by the modular branching pattern of the stem and intra- and

437

inter-shoot trophic competition. Ann. of Bot. 93, 263-274. DOI: 10.1093/aob/mch038

438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447

Lipcius, R.N., Hines, A.H., 1986. Variable function responses of a marine predator in dissimilar homogeneous microhabitats. Ecology 67, 1361-1371. Livdahl, T.P., Stiven, A.E., 1983. Statistical difficulties in the analysis of predator functional response data. Can. Entomol. 115, 1365-1370. Manly, B.F.J., Jamienson, C.D., 1999. Functional response and parallel curve analysis. Oikos. 85, 523-528. DOI: 10.2307/3546701 Murdoch, W.W., Oaten, A., 1975. Predation and population stability. Adv. Ecol. Res. 9, 2131. Okuyama, T., 2013. On selection of functional response models: Holling’s models and more. BioControl 58, 293-298.

19

448

Paull, C.A., Austin, A.D., 2006. The hymenopteran parasitoids of light brown apple moth,

449

Epiphyas postvittana (Walker) (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) in Australia. Aust. J.

450

Entomol. 45, 142-156. DOI: 10.1111/j.1440-6055.2006.00524.x

451

Paull, C.A., Schellhorn, N.A., Austin, A.D., 2014. Response to host density by the parasitoid

452

Dolichogenidea tasmanica (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) and the influence of grapevine

453

variety. Bull. Entomol. Res. 104, 79-87.

454

DOI: http://dx.doi.org.proxy.library.adelaide.edu.au/10.1017/S0007485313000527

455

CCW Cooperative, Berri, South Australia. 6 p. [www.ccwcoop.com.au/__files/f/4382/CCW

456

FACT SHEET 4 - LBAM.pdf, accessed 7 June 2014]

457

Price, P.W., 1997. Insect Ecology. Academic Press, New York, USA.

458

Real, L.A., 1979. Ecological determinants of functional response. Ecology 60, 481-485.

459

Rivero, A., Casas, J., 1999. Incorporating physiology into parasitoid behavioural ecology: the

460

allocation of nutritional resources. Res. Popul. Ecol. 41, 39-45.

461

Suckling, D.M., Brockerhoff, E.G., 2010. Invasion Biology, Ecology and Management of the

462

Light Brown Apple Moth (Tortricidae). Annu. Rev. Entomol. 55, 285-306.

463

DOI: 10.1146/annurev-ento-112408-085311

464

Taylor, R.J., 1984. Predation. Chapman and Hall, New York, USA.

465

Trexler, J.C., Mccullogh, C.E., Travis, J., 1988. How can the functional response best be

466 467 468

determined? Oecologia 76, 206-214. DIO: 10.1007/BF00378751 Turchin, P., 2001. Complex Population Dynamics: A Theoretical/ Empirical Synthesis. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA.

469

Turlings, T.C.L., Wäckers, F.L., Vet, L.E.M., Lewis, W.J., Tumlinson, J.H., 1993. Learning

470

of Host-Finding Cues by Hymenopterous Parasitoids. Papaj DR, Lewis AC (eds.)

471

Insect learning: ecology and evolutionary perspectives. Springer Science+Business

472

Media, Dordrecht, Netherlands, pp. 51-78. 20

473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481

van Lenteren, J.C., Bakker, K., 1976. Functional responses in invertebrates. Neth. J. Zool. 26, 567-572. Vet, L.E.M., Groenewold, A.W., 1990. Semiochemicals and learning in parasitoids. J. Chem. Ecol. 16, 119-35. Waage, J.K., Hassell, M.P., 1982. Parasitoids as biological control agents: a fundamental approach. Parasitology 84, 241-268. Walde, S.J., Murdoch, W.W., 1988. Spatial density dependence in parasitoids. Annu. Rev. Entomol., 33, 441-466. Williams, F.M., Juliano, S.A., 1985. Further difficulties in the analysis of functional response

482

experiments

483

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.4039/Ent117631-5

484

and

a

resolution.

Entomol.

117,

631-640.

Yazdani, M., Feng, Y., Glatz, R., Keller, M.A., 2014. Host stage preference of tasmanica

(Cameron)

485

Dolichogenidea

486

Entomology. DOI: 10.1111/aen.12130

487

Can.

(Hymenoptera:

Braconidae).

Austral

Zar, J.H., 1984. Biostatistical analysis. Prentice-Hall Inc., Englewood Clifffs, New Jersey.

488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497

21

498

Figure legends

499

Figure1. Three types of functional response.

500

hosts parasitised and host density.

501

parasitised and host density.

502

Figure2. Mean fraction of larvae parasitised (± standard error) by D. tasmanica for the 6

503

densities (1, 2, 4, 6, 8, 16 and 32) of second instar LBAM in the wind tunnels. The dotted line

504

depicts the expected functional response if atype III data and the black line showsthe fitted

505

Logistic regression curve a)in wind tunnels and b) in small cages.

506

Figure 3. The effect of previous rewarding or non-rewarding foraging experience by D.

507

tasmanica on the mean fraction of larvae parasitised (± standard error) when foraging 8 h

508

later at densities of 4, 8 and 16 larvae per 20 grape leaves.

509

Figure . Type III functional response curves (solid lines; b = 0.005, c = 0.04, d = 0.000, h = 8,

510

T = 120) and the effect of a switch to a type I response (dashed lines). a. Relationship

511

between host number and fraction parasitised in the wind tunnel. b. Relationship between

512

host number and number parasitised in the wind tunnel

a) The relationship between the number of

b) The relationship between the proportion of hosts

513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522

22

523

Figure 1.

524

a)

Number of hosts parasitised/individual

525

Type I Type II Type III

0 0

526

b)

Type I Type II Type III

Proportion of hosts parasitised/individual

527

0 528

0

Host density

529 530 531 532

23

533 534 535

Figure 2

536

a)

Fraction parasitised (mean ± SE)

1

Observed

Type III

0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0

537

Logistic

5

10

15 20 Host density

538

539

540

541

542

543

544

24

25

30

35

545

b)

Fraction parasitised (mean ± SE)

546

1

Observed

Logistic

Type III

0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0

547

5

10

15

20

Host number

548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558

25

25

30

35

559 560

Figure 3

Fraction parasitised (mean + SE)

0.8

Rewarding

0.6

0.4

0.2

0 0

561

Non-rewarding

4

8 12 Number of host larvae

562

563

564

565

566 567 568 569 570 571

26

16

20

572

Figure 4

574

a.

Fraction parasitised

573

0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0

575

0

10

20 Host number

30

40

0

10

20 Host number

30

40

b.

Number parasitised

25 20 15 10 5 0

576 577 578 579 580

27

581 582

Table 1. Results of logistic regression analysis of the fraction of hosts parasitised by D.

583

tasmanica vs. host number in small wind tunnels.

584

Coefficient

Estimate

Std. Error

z value

Prob.

Intercept

-0.8238131

0.4015574

-2.052

0.04021

Host number

0.4047707

0.1447773

2.796

0.00518

(Host number)2

-0.0279509

0.0116589

-2.397

0.01651

(Host number)3

0.0005274

0.0002362

2.233

0.02553

585

Null deviance: 122.98 on 72 degrees of freedom

586

Residual deviance: 108.59 on 69 degrees of freedom

587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598

28

599 600

Table 2. Results of logistic regression analysis of the fraction of hosts parasitised by D.

601

tasmanica vs. host number in cages. Coefficient

Estimate

Std. Error

z value

Prob.

Intercept

-0.0741800

0.4347062

-0.171

0.86450

Host number

0.3851388

0.1675998

2.298

0.02156

(Host number)2

-0.0368871

0.0134945

-2.733

0.00627

(Host number)3

0.0007866

0.0002712

2.900

0.00373

602

Null deviance: 151.49 on 65 degrees of freedom

603

Residual deviance: 136.03 on 62 degrees of freedom

604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616

29

617 618

Table 3. The effect of experience on the frequency of parasitism of second instar LBAM by

619

D. tasmanica when presented with two hosts in a wind tunnel.

620

Previous experience Fate of larva

Rewarding

Non-rewarding

Unparasitised

5

16

Parasitised

15

4

Total No. wasps

10

10

Fisher’s two-tailed exact test, P = 0.0012

30

1, 2, 4, 8, 16 or 32 larvae Wind

Fan

Fraction parasitised

Variable density of larval Light Brown Apple Moth 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2

0 0

10

20

30

Host number

Wind tunnel

40



Dolichogenidea tasmanica exhibited a sigmoid functional response in wind tunnels and enclosed cages.



Parasitism rates were lower in the cages, possibly due to the lack of moving air which provides a directional cue.



Recent experience with high host densities increases the searching rate of D. tasmanica,



At lower host densities that are characteristic of wild populations, D. tasmanica responded in a density-dependent manner



This manner should contribute to suppression of pest populations before they reach economically damage levels.