Training for Long-Duration Space Missions: A Literature Review into Skill Retention and Generalizability

Training for Long-Duration Space Missions: A Literature Review into Skill Retention and Generalizability

14th IFAC on 14th IFAC Symposium Symposium on 14th Symposium on Analysis and Evaluation of Human Machine Systems 14th IFAC IFACDesign Symposium on Ava...

405KB Sizes 0 Downloads 25 Views

14th IFAC on 14th IFAC Symposium Symposium on 14th Symposium on Analysis and Evaluation of Human Machine Systems 14th IFAC IFACDesign Symposium on Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Analysis Design and Evaluation of Human Machine Systems 14th IFAC Symposium on Analysis Design and Evaluation of Human Machine Systems Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019 Analysis Design and Evaluation of Human Machine Systems Tallinn, Estonia, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019 2019 Analysis Design and Evaluation of Human Machine Systems Tallinn, Sept. 16-19, Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019 Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019

ScienceDirect

IFAC PapersOnLine 52-19 (2019) 247–252

Training for Long-Duration Space Missions: Training for Long-Duration Space Missions: Training for Long-Duration Space Missions: Training for Long-Duration Space Missions: A Literature Review into Skill Retention A Literature Review into Skill Retention A Literature Review into Skill Retention A Literature Review into Skill Retention and Generalizability and Generalizability and Generalizability and Generalizability ∗ ∗∗

Marc A. ∗∗ Marc A. Pieters Pieters ∗∗∗ Peter Peter M. M. T. T. Zaal Zaal ∗∗ Marc Marc A. A. Pieters Pieters ∗ Peter Peter M. M. T. T. Zaal Zaal ∗∗ ∗∗ Marc A. Pieters Peter M. T. Zaal ∗ ∗ San Jose State University, NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett ∗ San Jose State University, NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett ∗ San Jose State University, NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett San Jose NASAUniversity Ames Research Center, Moffett Field, CAState 94035University, USA and and Delft Delft of Technology, Technology, Delft, ∗ Field, CA 94035 USA of Delft, San Jose State University, NASAUniversity Ames Research Center, Moffett Field, 94035 USA and Delft University of Technology, Delft, Field, CA CA 94035 USA and Delft University of Technology, Delft, Netherlands (e-mail: [email protected]). Netherlands (e-mail: [email protected]). Field, CA 94035 USA and Delft University of Technology, Delft, Netherlands (e-mail: [email protected]). ∗∗ Netherlands (e-mail: [email protected]). ∗∗ San Jose State University, NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett ∗∗ San Jose State University, Ames Research Center, Moffett Netherlands (e-mail:NASA [email protected]). ∗∗ San Jose State University, NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett Jose State University, NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett CA 94035 USA (e-mail: [email protected]). ∗∗ SanField, CA 94035 USA (e-mail: [email protected]). SanField, Jose State University, NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett Field, CA CA 94035 94035 USA USA (e-mail: (e-mail: [email protected]). [email protected]). Field, Field, CA 94035 USA (e-mail: [email protected]). Abstract: Abstract: On On long-duration long-duration space space missions, missions, skill skill retention retention and and generalizability generalizability become become ever ever Abstract: On space missions, skill retention and generalizability become ever Abstract: On long-duration long-duration spaceincreases, missions,for skill retention and generalizability become ever astronaut more important as mission length it is through these capabilities that more important as mission length increases, for it is through these capabilities that astronaut Abstract: On long-duration spaceincreases, missions,for skill retention and generalizability become ever more important as mission length it is through these capabilities that astronaut more as mission length simulators increases, for is through thesein astronaut crews achieve autonomy. Because are used extensively all types of training, the crews important achieve autonomy. autonomy. Because simulators areit used extensively incapabilities all types types of ofthat training, the more important as mission length simulators increases, for itused is through thesein capabilities that astronaut crews achieve Because are extensively all training, the crews achieve autonomy. Because simulators are used extensively in all types of training, the on skill retention and generalizability effects of simulator fidelity are paramount to understand. effects of simulator fidelity on skill retention and generalizability are paramount to understand. crews achieve autonomy. Because simulators are used extensively in all types of training, the effects of fidelity on and are paramount to understand. effects of simulator simulator on skill skill retention retention and generalizability generalizability paramount understand. A survey was to current in retention and A literature literature surveyfidelity was performed performed to identify identify current research researchare gaps in skill skill to retention and effects of simulator fidelity on skill retention and generalizability aregaps paramount to understand. A literature survey was performed to identify current research gaps in skill retention and A literature survey was performed to identify current research gaps in skill retention and for a structured and quantifiable approach to generalizability. The survey identified a need generalizability. The survey identified a need for a structured and quantifiable approach to A literature survey was performed to aidentify current researchand gaps in skill retention and generalizability. The survey identified need for a structured quantifiable approach to generalizability. The survey identified using a need for a structured and Such quantifiable approach to characterize skill decay, for example, aa cybernetic approach. an approach would characterize skill decay, for example, using cybernetic approach. Such an approach would generalizability. The survey identified a need for a structured and quantifiable approach to characterize skill decay, for example, using a cybernetic approach. Such an approach would characterize skill decay, for example, using a cybernetic approach. Such an approach would allow for gaining a deeper understanding of the mechanisms through which skill decay operates. allow for gaining a deeper understanding of the mechanisms through which skill decay operates. characterize skill decay, for example, using a cybernetic approach. Such an approach would allow for a deeper of mechanisms through which skill operates. allow for gaining gaining deeper understanding understanding of the thethree mechanisms which skill decay decayfor operates. opportunities future Furthermore, the survey research gaps Furthermore, thea literature survey identified identified researchthrough gaps and and opportunities future allow for gaining aliterature deeper understanding of thethree mechanisms through which skill decayfor operates. Furthermore, the literature survey identified three research gaps and opportunities for future Furthermore, the literature survey identified three research gaps and opportunities for future research: (1) developing skill decay functions provides theoretical insights into skill decay research: (1) developing skill decay functions provides theoretical insights into skill decay Furthermore, the literature survey identified three research gaps andinsights opportunities for future research: (1) developing skill decay functions provides theoretical into skill decay research: (1) developing skill decay functions provides theoretical insights into skill decay allow training, (2) and could for several practical applications, such as planning refresher and could allow for several practical applications, such as planning refresher training, (2) research: (1) developing skill decay functions provides theoretical insights into skill decay and could allow for several practical applications, such as planning refresher training, (2) and could allow for several practical applications, such as planning refresher training, (2) investigating the effects of simulator fidelity on skill decay functions could allow for better investigating the effects of simulator fidelity on skill decay functions could allow for better and could allow for several practical applications, such as planning refresher training, (2) investigating the effects of simulator fidelity on skill decay functions could allow for better investigating the effects of simulator fidelity on skill decay functions could allow forlearned better simulator utilization during training, and (3) investigating the generalizability of skills simulator utilization during training, and (3) investigating the generalizability of skills learned investigating the effects of simulator fidelity on skill decay functions could allow forlearned better simulator utilization during training, and (3) investigating the generalizability of skills simulator utilization duringtasks training, (3) investigating the generalizability of skills learned in to could provide space greater in initial initial training training to other other couldand provide space crews crews with with greater autonomy. autonomy. simulator utilization duringtasks training, and (3) investigating the generalizability of skills learned in in initial initial training training to to other other tasks tasks could could provide provide space space crews crews with with greater greater autonomy. autonomy. in initial training to other tasks could provide space withreserved. greater autonomy. Copyright © 2019. The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.crews All rights Keywords: Keywords: Training, Training, Spaceflight, Spaceflight, Simulator Simulator Fidelity, Fidelity, Skill Skill Decay, Decay, Skill Skill Generalizability. Generalizability. Keywords: Keywords: Training, Training, Spaceflight, Spaceflight, Simulator Simulator Fidelity, Fidelity, Skill Skill Decay, Decay, Skill Skill Generalizability. Generalizability. Keywords: Training, Spaceflight, Simulator Fidelity, Skill Decay, Skill Generalizability. minutes, making support from control 1. INTRODUCTION 1. INTRODUCTION INTRODUCTION minutes, making making real-time real-time support support from from aa mission mission control control 1. minutes, 1. INTRODUCTION minutes, making real-time real-time support from aa mission mission control center impossible. Complex mission-critical tasks can thus center impossible. Complex mission-critical tasks can thus 1. INTRODUCTION minutes, making real-time support from a mission control center impossible. Complex mission-critical tasks can center impossible. Complex mission-critical tasksanymore. can thus thus not be handled in the more traditional manners not be handled in the more traditional manners anymore. Ever since the days of the first manned spaceflights, center impossible. Complex mission-critical tasksanymore. can thus Ever since since the the days days of the the first manned manned spaceflights, spaceflights, not be handled in traditional manners Ever not handled in the the more more traditional manners The role of specialized crew or real-time ground support Ever since the days of of attention the first first on manned spaceflights, The be role of aaa specialized specialized crew or real-time real-time groundanymore. support NASA spent meticulous the training of its not be handled in the more traditional manners anymore. NASA spent meticulous attention on the training of its The role of crew or ground support Ever since the days of the first manned spaceflights, NASA spent meticulous attention on the training of its The role of a specialized crew or real-time ground support has to be taken over by the ability to generalize skills or NASA spent meticulous attention on the training of its has to be taken over by the ability to generalize skills or astronauts. The goal of training is to realize two main The role of a specialized crew or real-time ground support astronauts. The goal of of training training isonto tothe realize two of main to taken NASA spentThe meticulous attentionis training its has astronauts. goal realize two main has to be be training. taken over over by by the the ability ability to to generalize generalize skills skills or or on-board training. astronauts. The goal of training is to realize two main on-board objectives: skill acquisition and transfer of training. Skill has to be taken over by the ability to generalize skills or objectives: skill acquisition and transfer of training. Skill on-board training. astronauts. skill The acquisition goal of training is to realize two main objectives: and of Skill on-board training. objectives: and transfer transfer of training. training. acquisition is initial to aa certain task training. In order to design the training for long-duration space acquisition skill is the theacquisition initial learning learning to perform perform certain Skill task on-board objectives: skill acquisition and transfer of training. Skill In order order to to design the the training training for for long-duration long-duration space space acquisition is the initial learning to perform a certain task design acquisition is the initial learning to perform a certain task In and transfer of training subsequently projects this onto In order to design the training for long-duration space missions, it thus is important to investigate skill decay and transfer of training subsequently projects this onto acquisition is the initial learning to perform a certain task missions, it thus is important to investigate skill decay and transfer of training subsequently projects this onto In order to design the training for long-duration space missions, it thus is important to investigate skill decay and transfer of training subsequently projects this onto the for it thus is important skill decay and generalizability. Simulators are used for the training the operational operational domain. Especially for long-duration long-duration space and transfer of domain. trainingEspecially subsequently projects this space onto missions, and generalizability. generalizability. Simulators to areinvestigate used for for the the training the operational domain. for space missions, it thus is important to investigate skill decay Simulators are used training the operational domain. Especially Especially for long-duration long-duration space and missions, two additional goals become apparent: retention and generalizability. Simulators are used for the training of many tasks astronauts will perform. Therefore, undermissions, two additional goals become apparent: retention the operational domain. Especially for long-duration space of of many many tasks astronauts astronauts will perform. perform. Therefore, undermissions, two additional goals become apparent: retention and generalizability. Simulators are usedTherefore, for the training tasks will undermissions, two additional goals become apparent: retention of skills and generalizability across tasks. of many tasks astronauts will perform. Therefore, understanding the of simulation fidelity on skill decay of skills and and tasks. missions, twogeneralizability additional goalsacross become apparent: retention standing standing the effects effects of simulation fidelity on skill decay of of many tasks astronauts will perform. Therefore, underthe effects of simulation fidelity on skill skillof decay of skills skills and generalizability generalizability across across tasks. tasks. standing the effects of simulation fidelity on decay and generalizability is important. The objective this of skills and generalizability across tasks. and generalizability is important. The objective of this To illustrate this, the NASA Human Research Program evstanding the effects of simulation fidelity on skillofdecay To illustrate this, the NASA Human Research Program evand generalizability is important. The objective this To illustrate this, the NASA Human Research Program evand generalizability isto important. The of date this objective literature review was summarize the research to To illustrate this, the NASA Human Research Program evliterature review was to summarize the research to date idence report on training deficiencies presents some of the and generalizability istoimportant. The objective of date this idence reportthis, on training training deficiencies presentsProgram some of of the review summarize to To illustrate the NASA Human Research ev- literature idence report on deficiencies presents some literature review was was summarize the the research research date on skill retention and generalizability, and impact idence report on training deficiencies presents some of the the on on skill skill retention retention andto generalizability, and the the to impact training issues involved with long-duration space missions. literature review was togeneralizability, summarize the research to date training issues involved with long-duration space missions. and and the impact idence report on training deficiencies presents some of the training issues involved with long-duration space skill retention and impact of simulator fidelity on these aspects of training. A training involved long-duration space missions. missions. on issues of simulator simulator fidelityand on generalizability, these two two aspects aspects of the training. A One main issues is current on skill retention and generalizability, and the impact One of of the the main issueswith is the the current training-time-totraining-time-tofidelity on these of A training issues involved with long-duration space missions. of One of the main issues is the current training-time-toof simulator fidelity onidentify these two two aspectsgaps of training. training. A further aim was to to identify literature gaps relating to One of the issues is the current training-time-tomain further aim was to to literature relating to mission-time ratio, which 10 to 1 for International Space of simulator fidelity on these two aspects of training. A mission-time ratio, which is 10 to 1 for International Space further aim was to to identify literature gaps relating to One of the main issues thetocurrent training-time-tomission-time ratio, which is Space further aim was to to literature gaps relating to these specific areas, and guide future research. mission-time ratio, whichand is 10 10Dempsey to 11 for for International International Space these these specific specific areas, andidentify guide future future research. Station [Barshi (2016)]. further aim was to to identify literature gaps relating to Station missions missions [Barshi and (2016)]. A A mission mission areas, and guide research. mission-time ratio, whichand is 10Dempsey to 1 for International Space these Station missions [Barshi Dempsey (2016)]. A mission specific areas, and guide future research. Station missions [Barshi and Dempsey (2016)]. A mission to Mars might take up to 32 months [Mars Architecture these specific areas, and guide future research. After defining defining aa list list of of peer-reviewed peer-reviewed sources sources in in journals, journals, to Mars Marsmissions might take take up to 32Dempsey months [Mars [Mars Architecture Station [Barshi and (2016)]. A mission After After to up 32 Architecture defining aa list sources in journals, to Mars might might up to tofollowing 32 months months Architecture Grouptake (2009)]: the[Mars current training- After Steering defining list of of peer-reviewed peer-reviewed sources in journals, conference proceedings, or technical reports of relevant Steering Group (2009)]: the current trainingto Mars might take up tofollowing 32 months [Mars Architecture conference proceedings, or technical reports of relevant Steering Group (2009)]: following the current trainingAfter defining a list of peer-reviewed sources in journals, conference proceedings, or technical reports of relevant Steering Group (2009)]: following the current trainingto-mission ratio would be unfeasible. Nevertheless, initial conference proceedings, or technical reports of relevant organizations, such as NASA, FAA, or similar, these keyto-mission ratio would be unfeasible. Nevertheless, initial Steering Group (2009)]: following the current trainingorganizations, such as NASA, FAA, or similar, these keyto-mission ratio would be unfeasible. Nevertheless, initial conference proceedings, or technical reports of relevant organizations, such as NASA, FAA, or similar, these keyto-mission ratio would be unfeasible. Nevertheless, initial training will be aa lengthy process and thus skills might organizations, such as NASA, FAA, or similar, these keywords were used in the literature search: skill decay, acquitraining will be lengthy process and thus skills might to-mission ratio would be unfeasible. Nevertheless, initial words were used in the literature search: skill decay, acquitraining will be a lengthy process and thus skills might organizations, such as NASA, FAA, or similar, these keywords were used in the literature search: skill decay, acquitraining be a launch. lengthyFurthermore, willbefore process and crews thus skills might decay will to used intraining, the literature search: skill decay, acquisition, were retention, training, transfer of training, training, simulator decay even even launch. Furthermore, crews will have have to words training willbefore be a launch. lengthyFurthermore, process and crews thus skills might sition, retention, transfer of simulator decay even will to words were used intraining, the literature search: skill decay, acquiretention, transfer ofwas training, simulator decay even before before launch. Furthermore, crews will have have to sition, be in transit to their destination for several months, which sition, retention, training, transfer of training, simulator fidelity. Furthermore, another choice made: Nicholas be in transit to their destination for several months, which decay even before launch. Furthermore, crews will have to fidelity. fidelity.retention, Furthermore, another choiceofwas was made:simulator Nicholas be in transit to their destination for several months, which sition, training, transfer training, Furthermore, another choice made: Nicholas be in transit to their destination for several months, which also results in skill decay. This extended transit period Furthermore, choice wascrews made: for Nicholas and Foushee (1990) state that space longalsoinresults results in skill decay. decay. This for extended transit period period be transit in to their destination several months, which fidelity. and Foushee Foushee (1990) another state that that space crews for longalso This extended transit fidelity. Furthermore, another choice wascrews made: for Nicholas (1990) state space longalso results in skill skill decay. This Firstly, extended transit period and gives rise to two more challenges. it makes it imposand Foushee (1990) state that space crews for longduration space missions have to be regarded as groups, gives rise to two more challenges. Firstly, it makes it imposalso results in skill decay. This extended transit period duration space missions have to be regarded as groups, gives rise to two more challenges. Firstly, it makes it imposand Foushee (1990) state that space crews for longduration space missions have to be regarded as groups, gives rise to two more challenges. Firstly,in makes impos- duration sible send specialized crews to missions have tothey be regarded instead of individuals. However, continue by stating sible to to send specialized crews upwards upwards init order toit perform gives rise to two more challenges. Firstly,in itorder makes itperform impos- instead instead of ofspace individuals. However, they continue as by groups, stating sible to specialized crews upwards to duration space missions have tothey be regarded as groups, individuals. However, continue by stating sible to send send specialized crews upwards in order orderdelays to perform perform critical repairs. Secondly, the communication that instead individuals. However, they continue by stating of that group performance is dependent on three factors, one critical repairs. Secondly, the communication delays that sible to send specialized crews upwards in orderdelays to perform that group performance is dependent on three factors, one critical repairs. Secondly, the communication that instead of individuals. However, they continue by stating that group performance is dependent on three factors, one critical repairs. Earth Secondly, the communication delays that occur between and Mars might last as long as 40 that group performance is dependent on three factors, one occur between Earth and Mars might last as long as 40 critical repairs. Secondly, the communication delays that occur between Earth and Mars might last as long as 40 that group performance is dependent on three factors, one occur between Earth and Mars might last as long as 40 occur between Earth and might last by asElsevier long as 40All rights reserved. 2405-8963 Copyright © 2019. The Mars Authors. Published Ltd.

Copyright © under 2019 IFAC IFAC 247 Peer review responsibility of International Federation of Automatic Control. Copyright © 2019 247 Copyright © 247 Copyright © 2019 2019 IFAC IFAC 247 10.1016/j.ifacol.2019.12.099 Copyright © 2019 IFAC 247

2019 IFAC HMS 248 Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019

Marc A. Pieters et al. / IFAC PapersOnLine 52-19 (2019) 247–252

being the level of skill of its members. Therefore, in this literature review, it was chosen to focus on the individualastronaut skills, instead of on team skills. 2. TRAINING 2.1 Types of Training On long-duration space missions, astronauts will train before launch, as well as on board. The application of training on board is dual. On one side, it could serve as refresher training, to ensure the astronauts possess the necessary skill to perform their mission critical task. On the other, just-in-time training would occur for low-likelihood events that are time critical. However, because of the radically new architecture of long space missions, a situation where astronauts would perform initial training on board could be imagined. Training type is thus a more useful variable to consider than mission time line. In initial training, high fidelity simulators are often used. In a situation where initial training is performed on board, however, it is less likely that a high fidelity simulator is present due to design restrictions associated with spacecraft. This necessitates investigating the effects of simulator fidelity on training, which is discussed later in this literature review. The goal of refresher training is to ensure skill levels are adequately high to perform tasks after a period of non-use. In ground based refresher training, the original simulators can be used. However, on board the same simulator restrictions apply. Gardlin and Sitterley (1972) propose three levels of complexity in on-board refresher training: The lowest would entail nothing more than a verbal or mental review of the task (sometimes referred to as symbolic rehearsal). The median level would be using the real systems in a safe training mode. Finally, Gardlin states: “Beyond this would be the application of more sophisticated combinations of software, computers, and simulation/training hardware to provide high fidelity reproductions of spacecraft system dynamics and the operational visual environment associated with critical mission operations, phases, and maneuvers.” The third form of training is just-in-time training, which is characterized by an unanticipated task which needs to be trained in a timely manner, for instance by making use of previously learned generalizable skills. An example of such a task is a failure of a critical system which requires an extra vehicular activity to repair. The critical nature of the failure would allow for only a short period (a day, for instance) to train. Barshi and Dempsey (2016) state that the ability to design just-in-time training requires expecting the unexpected. They continue: “Because not all such events can be anticipated in advance, methods for the crew to develop their own training for such occasions must be developed for cases when communication delays prevent the up-link of such training from the ground.” Apart from this being a demanding task on its own, it would have to be executed under time pressure. Thus, from this point of view, a low-fidelity simulation is preferred. Only by allowing the crew such flexibility, could the highest autonomy be reached. Caldwell and Onken (2011) define five levels of autonomy, the highest one being “goal determination”. In long duration space missions, the need for this high level of crew autonomy is especially pertinent. 248

2.2 Modeling Training Kim et al. (2013) state: “To better address the issues related to learning and long-term retention, it is necessary to predict the learners future cognitive states.” In order to do so, it is necessary to construct a cognitive model of the human. This section discusses a common taxonomy: Rasmussen’s S-R-K [Rasmussen (1983)]. The manner in which the definitions are used, deviates slightly from the original intention by Rasmussen. Rather than identifying different levels of how one operator might execute a certain task based on skill progression, here the definitions are used to point to a division in task types. This ties directly into how the operator internally processes these tasks. Several authors have proposed models of representing learning in humans. Kim et al. (2013) provide an excellent overview in their work. Fitts and Melton (1964), for instance, proposed three levels in the learning process. This is built upon by Anderson (1982), who also proposed a three-level taxonomy for cognitive skill development. Rasmussen (1983) formulated the Skills, Rules and Knowledge model. All these models have the division of three stages in common. Firstly, “acquiring declarative and procedural knowledge”, secondly, “consolidating the acquired knowledge” and thirdly, “tuning the knowledge towards overlearning” [Kim et al. (2013)]. As a subject is learning a new skill, he or she progresses through these stages. The initial stage of learning involves generating the first mental processes in executing a task. Afterwards, these processes are interconnected in order to form a memory of executing the task. When this is completed, a stage called overlearning is commenced, where the connections harden and muscle memory continues to strengthen. Focusing on Rasmussen’s S-R-K, the first level is skillbased behavior, which comprises of behavior where a task is performed without exerting laborious mental effort [Rasmussen (1983)]. In an experimental research environment, a tracking task is a typical skill-based task. In real life, an example of a tracking task is to follow another aircraft in a formation flight, or following a flight director. The second level is rule-based behavior, where the essence is the presence of a stored rule which acts as a “feedforward control” in achieving a certain goal. The rules may have been formed by the operator himself, but may also have been communicated in a different manner. The separation between rule-based and skill-based tasks is not always distinct, as a person might move from applying rules to learning the corresponding skill. The third and final level is called knowledge-based behavior. Here, Rasmussen argues that performance is fully goal controlled. The goal is formally known and a mental model is used to attain the goal. This level represents new situations where an operator has to exert mental effort to achieve a solution. The taxonomy will be used later in the discussion on the effects of simulator fidelity on retention and generalizablity. 3. SKILL DECAY 3.1 Relevant Variables There is some notion as to why skills decay. However, the question of how they decay remains pertinent. Several studies have been performed to identify the variables

2019 IFAC HMS Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019

Marc A. Pieters et al. / IFAC PapersOnLine 52-19 (2019) 247–252

which might influence the retention of skill over time. For instance, Schendel et al. (1978) suggest the following variables: “degree of proficiency attained by the learner during initial training; amount and kind of refresher training; transfer of skills from one task to another; interfering activities; scheduling of practice during training; use of parttask versus whole-task training methods, and; introduction of extra test trials prior to final testing”. In a meta-analysis by Arthur, Jr. et al. (1998) all variables influencing skill decay were divided into two categories: methodological or task-related. “Here, the task-related variables are inherent characteristics of the task and are not amenable to modification by the researcher”, as stated by Villado et al. (2013), which is one of the studies put forward in Arthur, Jr. et al. (1998). The meta-analysis used 53 articles. The first objective was to find the correlation between the length of the retention interval and the amount of skill decay. A positive correlation was found; longer intervals result in more decay. This seems like a rather trivial result. However, the strength of the decay varied over different studies, which points to other influencing variables. One of those influencing variables is the degree of overlearning, although its precise effects are not fully clear. One of the studies used in the meta analysis concluded that “overlearning does not prevent, but only somewhat reduces, decrements of performance with time” [Hammerton (1963)]. Arthur, Jr. et al. (1998) concluded that although some correlation indeed exists, the current literature only supports a limited range of overlearning and consequently, only a limited effect is observed. Overlearning thus turns out to be a variable of which the exact effects remain under discussion. Apart from initial training and time, task characteristics also play a role. The meta-analysis hypothesized that open-loop tasks would display less decay than closed-loop tasks, which is a result that follows from other sources on skill decay [Farr (1986)]. However, the results of the meta-analysis indicate a finding that is in contrast with this hypothesis: closed-loop tasks are retained better. This might be due to some contamination effects over the different studies used however. Another variable is the nature of the task; physical or cognitive tasks. The results show that physical tasks show less decay than cognitive tasks. Driskell et al. (1994) argue that this might be due to the fact that even though mental action allows for the construction of words and images to aid recall, it does not provide any direct feedback on performance. Physical tasks are more likely to provide this feedback in the form of visual or tactile knowledge of the results and thus, a higher performance is easier to attain. The meta-analysis hypothesized that natural tasks would display less decay than artificial tasks, which would favor using as realistic tasks as possible in training. The results marginally favoured this statement, although the difference was small. The difference could be explained by varying levels of motivation for learning the tasks. In general, participants express more motivation to master tasks that appear to be natural [Arthur, Jr. et al. (1998); Stefanidis et al. (2005)]. This conclusion, however, is based on a questionnaire, which inherently features some subjec-

249

249

tivity. However, perhaps there is some subconscious truth in the question of motivational differences. Finally, the meta-analysis found a negative correlation between the original learning and retention environment; if the two differed more, more decay was present. This relates directly to generalizability, where a certain skill must be adapted to fit a new task. However, this conclusion was drawn from only four data points. 3.2 Modelling Skill Decay The decay of skill is more than simply a function of time [Arthur, Jr. et al. (2013)]. The variables identified in the previous section attest to this. Modelling the decay of skill is a daunting task. Nevertheless, many have tried. It started with Ebbinghaus (1885), who noticed that a decay curve is most often negatively accelerated: the curve falls most quickly immediately after initial training. McGeoch and Irion (1952) attempted constructing these curves as well: their work focused on reciting words from memory. These are just two examples of research in decay of memory. More examples can be found, such as Wixted and Carpenter (2007); Wixted and Ebbesen (1991); Meeter et al. (2005); Bahrick (1992); Baldwin et al. (1976). Most of these studies suggest that, although it is possible to construct a decay curve for specific types of tasks, there is no such thing as a universal skill decay curve [Naylor and Briggs (1961)]. There are several speculated methods of creating limited skill decay curves; for example, as illustrated by Gronlund and Kimball (2013), who discuss using computational modelling to predict skill decay curves as a function of some of the variables identified in the previous section. However, the authors did not identify any work into the actual construction of these curves using a well-defined systematic approach. Baldwin and Ford (1988) go as far as defining five possible scenarios of “maintenance of training curves”, four of which show possibilities along which skill can decay and a final one which indicates a skill growth after training, because the skill is used extensively. A point to note in their classification is that there is no period of non-use. Rather the term “maintenance” is coined, which indicates that the skills could also be maintained by executing them in operational environments. Furthermore, skill decay is only classified, and no quantifications are proposed in Baldwin and Ford’s literature study. Farr (1986) stated 11 issues for further research and development in his 1986 report on skill retention. The construction of skill decay curves was one of them, but firstly he stated that it is necessary to define measures to quantitatively describe this phenomenon. Arthur, Jr. et al. (2013) look back on these needs and conclude that “not much empirical research has been devoted to the study of knowledge and skill retention outside the more basic, cognitive-experimental work on memory and motor learning”. Even on these two subgroups, the research and its applicability is limited. Two reasons are put forward: Firstly, experiments in retention of skill are logistically very challenging, as long retention periods are involved. Secondly, the need for understanding skill decay in detail is not present, as it is commonly regarded as “a matter of interference with retrieval processes rather than sheer

2019 IFAC HMS 250 Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019

Marc A. Pieters et al. / IFAC PapersOnLine 52-19 (2019) 247–252

non-use”. The detailed working mechanisms of skill decay are not fully understood, which impedes the issue to gain a relevance in the scientific community. It thus appears to be the case that in order to fully understand skill decay, there is a need for a clearly structured approach which would shed light on the underlying processes. With this approach, skill decay curves could be developed, which in turn find many applications in practical operations. 4. SIMULATOR FIDELITY Training simulators come in many different varieties, each placing importance on different aspects of representing the real world, leading to different levels of fidelity. Some fidelities commonly found in literature are physical, psychological, behavioral, and face fidelity. Each of them describes some of the effects the simulator might have on the operator. For a long time, the paradigm in designing simulator training was to facilitate as high fidelity on all aspects as possible. Caird (1996) described this with the statement: “For decades, the naive but persistent theory of fidelity has guided the fit of simulation systems to training.” This notion was especially pertinent in the design of training simulators. However, in recent times a shift towards a deeper understanding of the different types of fidelity is observed [Roscoe (1991)]. Some simulators might focus on a specific type of fidelity and hence this choice is not trivial anymore in the design. This section attempts to correlate the type of fidelity a simulator might have on the retention variables identified before. The knowledge level in S-R-K assumes that a situation is completely new and thus no specific training of it can be performed. Rather, attention must be paid to set up training procedures to allow for high grades of generalizability. As Dahlstrom et al. (2009) put it: “Crews can effectively counter many threats by replicating or slightly varying the technical skills learned during their training.” He continues by arguing that to achieve such creativity of solution, a lower fidelity simulator would be beneficial, since it shifts focus from procedural knowledge to the pure skill-based task. This can subsequently be varied to counter unforeseen circumstances. For the middle level of the S-R-K taxonomy, rule-based behavior, there exists evidence that high physical fidelity simulators accommodate retention of the task for at least a year [Boet et al. (2011)]. Also with low fidelity simulation or even without any simulation at all, these kind of tasks can remain on high performance levels. Without simulation, for instance, a technique called “symbolic rehearsal” is suggested by Kluge and Frank (2014) and Kluge et al. (2015). This is a technique where a person internally visualizes performing a skill, without actually doing it. They conclude that symbolic rehearsal effectively supports knowledge retention, but skill retention in a lesser manner. Procedure-based tasks can be practiced and maintained in several ways, using simulators of varying fidelity levels, with little effort over longer-spread periods. O’Hara (1990) proposes 30minute refresher training every six months. This specific recommendation follows from an experiment with marine cadets; their watch standing skills were tested over a prolonged period of time with no use. After quick refresher 250

training, though, their performance levels could quickly be increased again. Grimsley (1969) found no difference in retention between subjects trained on a low-fidelity simulator and subjects trained on a high-fidelity one. In this research, subjects had to execute a procedural task. It appears that rule-based tasks require relatively low levels of simulator fidelity to maintain. On the skill-based task level, several studies suggest that practice refresher interventions support skill retention in the best way. The experiments treated in Kluge et al. (2015) used a retention period of two weeks (or one week with an intervention in the middle). Three types of retention interventions were investigated: skill practice, skill testing and symbolic rehearsal. In skill practice, a practice session like in the initial training was repeated. In skill testing, a performance test was used to retain the skill and finally symbolic rehearsal was investigated. The results show that both skill practice and testing support retention. Symbolic rehearsal could not fully prevent skill decay. In another study on the topic, Sauer et al. (2000) were not able to identify an influence between two different training methods (limited task-focused knowledge versus detail process-focused knowledge). A conclusion was drawn that continuous skill-based control tasks showed little skill decay over the 8 month retention period. The control task in this study was a process control task of a life support system of a spacecraft. It thus mainly dealt with lowfrequency control. In Merbah and Meulemans (2011), the step to motor skills is made. There are some suggestions to increase retention and generalizability by performing the training phase in a random order; instead of treating one subject per session, instructors are urged to mix it up. During the training phase participants will perform worse, but the skill is retained for longer and is more generalizable. Roscoe (1991) notes on the retention of motor skills: “Research has shown that innovations in training strategies, in some cases involving intentional departures from reality, can have stronger effects than high simulator fidelity on the resulting quality of pilot performance”. Caird (1996) adds to this: “There is some evidence from flight simulation that higher levels of fidelity have little or no effect on skill transfer and reductions in fidelity actually improve training. Reductions of complexity may aid working memory and attention as skills and knowledge are initially acquired.” To conclude this discussion on simulator fidelity and skill decay, it appears that rule and knowledge based tasks can be retained for prolonged periods of times using frequent practice sessions of various nature. Use could be made of low-fidelity simulators or of mental techniques such as symbolic rehearsal, as entailed by Gardlin and Sitterley (1972). For skill-based tasks, however, the story is somewhat more complicated. There is evidence that continuous motor tasks are retained for prolonged periods of time [Casner et al. (2014)]. However, Mulder et al. (2004) stress the need for frequent refreshing training with accurate training simulators. Prophet (1976) warns that many manual control tasks that are executed in flight or in space missions are more complex and therefore feature more decay than the simple motor skills. He supports this statement with the results of several investigations. Firstly, Hammerton (1963) found a significant decrement

2019 IFAC HMS Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019

Marc A. Pieters et al. / IFAC PapersOnLine 52-19 (2019) 247–252

in control skills after a six month period. In the same fashion, Sitterley and Berge (1972) and Sitterley (1974) found a large decay after a three month retention period, with errors being 2-3 times as large as at the end of training. Sitterley even used a complex spaceflight control task in one of his experiments, the other one being based on visually landing an aircraft. Furthermore, Cotterman and Wood (1967) found similar results in their 1967 study on lunar landing manual control skills. These works show varying results across experiments and tasks. A more thorough investigation into the fidelity for the retention of manual control skills is thus needed. 5. DISCUSSION Considering the previous findings uncovered with this literature study, several research gaps were identified. Firstly, as became clear in Section 3 there is a need for skill decay functions to be developed using an analytic and structured approach. Secondly, the link between simulator fidelity and skill decay functions is of interest. Section 4 shows that there is a need for research into fidelity of simulators for manual control tasks. Typical space mission tasks falling into this category are landing or maneuvering a spacecraft, or driving a planetary rover. Multiple sources suggested that this kind of skill is subject to strong decay, more so than simple manual motor tasks [Prophet (1976); Hammerton (1963); Sitterley (1974); Sitterley and Berge (1972); Cotterman and Wood (1967)]. Adding to the problem are sources stating that this kind of skill is best trained in high fidelity simulators [Mulder et al. (2004)]. This is challenging, because of the constrained mass and power budgets commonly found in spacecraft. In this vein, an experiment building on the quantification of the skill decay functions is suggested. By setting up the decay functions for a task executed on different types of simulators, the link between fidelity and retention can be established. Thirdly, the generalizability of skills is a relevant topic for future research since it directly relates to just-intime training. To handle unanticipated situations and make the astronaut-machine system more resilient, this is paramount. Initial skills must be sufficiently generalizable to facilitate just-in-time training. Farr (1986) states: “We need an operational definition of task complexity that will inform us of the memorability of the total task, as well as its components taken individually. We further require a means for deriving an index, preferably a quantitative one, that we can effectively use for twin purposes: (a) for determining the ease of learning the task; and (b) for predicting the decay rate of the task or any of its major components.” A cybernetic approach would provide a highly useful tool when conducting research into skill decay functions and generalizability, for it allows to model a human operator in an analytic and structured manner [Pool and Zaal (2016)]. REFERENCES Anderson, J.R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89(4), 369–406. doi:10.1037/ 0033-295X.89.4.369. 251

251

Arthur, Jr., W., Day, E.A., Bennett, Jr., W., and Portrey, A.M. (2013). Individual and Team Skill Decay: The Science and Implications for Practice (Applied Psychology Series). Routledge. Arthur, Jr., W., Winston Bennett, J., Stanush, P.L., and McNelly, T.L. (1998). Factors that influence skill decay and retention: A quantitative review and analysis. Human Performance, 11(1), 57–101. doi:10.1207/ s15327043hup1101 3. Bahrick, H.P. (1992). Stabilized memory of unrehearsed knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 121(1), 112–113. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121. 1.112. Baldwin, R.D., Cliborn, R.E., and Foskett, R.J. (1976). The acquisition and retention of visual aircraft recognition skills. Technical report, U. S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences. Baldwin, T. and Ford, J.K. (1988). Transfer of training: A review and directions for future research. Personnel Psychology, 41, 63–106. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1988. tb00632.x. Barshi, I. and Dempsey, D.L. (2016). Evidence report: Risk of performance errors due to training deficiencies. Technical report, National Aeronautics and Space Administration, Lyndon B. Johnson Space Center, Houston, Texas. Boet, S., Borges, B.C.R., Naik, V.N., Siu, L.W., Riem, N., Chandra, D., Bould, M.D., and Joo, H.S. (2011). Complex procedural skills are retained for a minimum of 1 yr after a single high-fidelity simulation training session†. BJA: British Journal of Anaesthesia, 107(4), 533–539. doi:10.1093/bja/aer160. Caird, J. (1996). Persistent issues in the application of virtual environment systems to training. In Proceedings Third Annual Symposium on Human Interaction with Complex Systems. HICS’96, 124–132. IEEE Comput. Soc. Press. doi:10.1109/HUICS.1996.549502. Caldwell, B. and Onken, J. (2011). Modeling and analyzing distributed autonomy for spaceflight teams. In 41st International Conference on Environmental Systems, 1– 8. American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics, Portland, Oregon. doi:10.2514/6.2011-5135. Casner, S.M., Geven, R.W., Recker, M.P., and Schooler, J.W. (2014). The retention of manual flying skills in the automated cockpit. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 56(8), 1506–1516. doi:10.1177/0018720814535628. Cotterman, T.E. and Wood, M.E. (1967). Retention of simulated lunar landing mission skills: A test of pilot reliability. Technical report, Aerospace Medical Research Laboratories, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio. URL http://www.dtic.mil/cgi-bin/ GetTRDoc?Location=U2&doc=GetTRDoc.pdf&AD= AD0817232. Dahlstrom, N., Dekker, S., van Winsen, R., and Nyce, J. (2009). Fidelity and validity of simulator training. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 10(4), 305– 314. doi:10.1080/14639220802368864. Driskell, J.E., Copper, C., and Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4), 481–492. doi:10.1037/ 0021-9010.79.4.481.

2019 IFAC HMS 252 Tallinn, Estonia, Sept. 16-19, 2019

Marc A. Pieters et al. / IFAC PapersOnLine 52-19 (2019) 247–252

Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. Teachers College, Columbia University, New York. Farr, M.J. (1986). The long-term retention of knowledge and skills: a cognitive and instructional perspective. Technical report, Institute for Defense Analysis. Fitts, P.M. and Melton, A.W. (1964). Perceptual-motor skill learning. Categories of human learning, 47, 381– 391. Gardlin, G.R. and Sitterley, T.E. (1972). Degradation of learned skills. a review and annotated bibliography. Technical report, Boeing Co., Seattle, WA, United States. URL https://ntrs.nasa.gov/archive/nasa/ casi.ntrs.nasa.gov/19730001425.pdf. Grimsley, D.L. (1969). Acquisition, retention, and retraining: Effects of high and low fidelity in training devices. Technical report, George Washington Univ., Alexandria, VA. Human Resources Research Office. Gronlund, S.D. and Kimball, D.R. (2013). Remembering and forgetting: from the laboratory looking out. Book Chapter in ”Individual and Team Skill Decay: The Science and Implications for Practice (2013)”. Hammerton, M. (1963). Retention of learning in a difficult tracking task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(1), 108–110. doi:10.1037/h0040296. Kim, J.W., Ritter, F.E., and Koubek, R.J. (2013). An integrated theory for improved skill acquisition and retention in the three stages of learning. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 14(1), 22–37. doi:10. 1080/1464536X.2011.573008. Kluge, A. and Frank, B. (2014). Counteracting skill decay: four refresher interventions and their effect on skill and knowledge retention in a simulated process control task. Ergonomics, 57(2), 175–190. doi:10.1080/ 00140139.2013.869357. Kluge, A., Frank, B., Maafi, S., and Kuzmanovska, A. (2015). Does skill retention benefit from retentivity and symbolic rehearsal? – two studies with a simulated process control task. Ergonomics, 59(5), 641–656. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2015.1101167. Mars Architecture Steering Group (2009). Human exploration of mars: Design reference architecture 5.0. Technical report, NASA Headquarters. McGeoch, J.A. and Irion, A.L. (1952). The psychology of human learning. Longmans, Green & Co., 2 edition. URL http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/ 1952-05377-000. Meeter, M., Murre, J.M.J., and Janssen, S.M.J. (2005). Remembering the news: Modeling retention data from a study with 14,000 participants. Memory & Cognition, 33(5), 793–810. doi:10.3758/BF03193075. Merbah, S. and Meulemans, T. (2011). Learning a motor skill: Effects of blocked versus random practice a review. Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 15. doi:10.5334/pb-51-1-15. Mulder, M., Van Paassen, M.M., and Boer, E.R. (2004). Exploring the roles of information in the manual control of vehicular locomotion: From kinematics and dynamics to cybernetics. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 13(5), 535–548. doi:10.1162/ 1054746042545256. Naylor, J.C. and Briggs, G.E. (1961). Long-term retention of learned skills: a review of the literature. Columbus: Ohio State University, Laboratory of Aviation Psychol252

ogy. Nicholas, J.M. and Foushee, H.C. (1990). Organization, selection, and training of crews for extended spaceflight - findings from analogs and implications. Journal of Spacecraft and Rockets, 27(5), 451–456. doi:10.2514/3. 26164. O’Hara, J.M. (1990). The retention of skills acquired through simulator-based training. Ergonomics, 33(9), 1143–1153. doi:10.1080/00140139008925319. Pool, D.M. and Zaal, P.M.T. (2016). A cybernetic approach to assess the training of manual control skills. 13th IFAC/IFIP/IFORS/IEA Symposium on Analysis, Design, and Evaluation of Human-Machine Systems. doi:10.1016/j.ifacol.2016.10.588. Prophet, W.W. (1976). Long-Term Retention of Flying Skills: A Review of the Literature. Alexandria VA: Human Resources Research Organization (ADA036077). Rasmussen, J. (1983). Skills, rules, and knowledge; signals, signs, and symbols, and other distinctions in human performance models. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, SMC-13(3), 257–266. doi:10. 1109/tsmc.1983.6313160. Roscoe, S.N. (1991). Simulator qualification: Just as phony as it can be. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 1(4), 335–339. doi:10.1207/s15327108ijap0104 6. Sauer, J., Hockey, G.R.J., and Wastell, D.G. (2000). Effects of training on short- and long-term skill retention in a complex multiple-task environment. Ergonomics, 43(12), 2043–2064. doi:10.1080/00140130010000893. Schendel, J.D., Shields, J.L., and Katz, M.S. (1978). Retention of motor skills: Review. Technical report, U.S. Army - Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences, Virginia. Sitterley, T.E. (1974). Degradation of learned skills. static practice effectiveness for visual approach and landing skill retention. Technical report, Boeing Aerospace Co., Seattle, WA, United States. URL https://ntrs.nasa. gov/search.jsp?R=19740024447. Sitterley, T.E. and Berge, W.A. (1972). Degradation of learned skills. effectiveness of practice methods on simulated space flight skill retention. Technical report, Boeing Co., Seattle, WA, United States. URL https:// ntrs.nasa.gov/search.jsp?R=19730001426. Stefanidis, D., Korndorffer, J.R., Sierra, R., Touchard, C., Dunne, J.B., and Scott, D.J. (2005). Skill retention following proficiency-based laparoscopic simulator training. Surgery, 138(2), 165–170. doi:https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.surg.2005.06.002. Villado, A.J., Day, E.A., Arthur, Jr., W., Boatman, P.R., Kowollik, V., Bhapatkar, A., and Bennett, Jr., W. (2013). Complex command-and-control simulation task performance following periods of nonuse. Book Chapter in ”Individual and Team Skill Decay: The Science and Implications for Practice (2013)”. Wixted, J.T. and Carpenter, S.K. (2007). The wickelgren power law and the ebbinghaus savings function. Psychological Science, 18, 133–134. doi:https://doi.org/10. 1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01862.x. Wixted, J.T. and Ebbesen, E.B. (1991). On the form of forgetting. Psychological Science, 2(6), 409–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00175.x.