Variability in neural excitability and plasticity induction in the human cortex: A brain stimulation study

Variability in neural excitability and plasticity induction in the human cortex: A brain stimulation study

Accepted Manuscript Variability in Neural Excitability and Plasticity Induction in the Human Cortex: A Brain Stimulation Study Brenton Hordacre, Mitch...

2MB Sizes 0 Downloads 45 Views

Accepted Manuscript Variability in Neural Excitability and Plasticity Induction in the Human Cortex: A Brain Stimulation Study Brenton Hordacre, Mitchell R. Goldsworthy, Ann-Maree Vallence, Sam Darvishi, Bahar Moezzi, Masashi Hamada, John C. Rothwell, Michael C. Ridding PII:

S1935-861X(16)30383-7

DOI:

10.1016/j.brs.2016.12.001

Reference:

BRS 987

To appear in:

Brain Stimulation

Received Date: 1 August 2016 Revised Date:

9 November 2016

Accepted Date: 3 December 2016

Please cite this article as: Hordacre B, Goldsworthy MR, Vallence A-M, Darvishi S, Moezzi B, Hamada M, Rothwell JC, Ridding MC, Variability in Neural Excitability and Plasticity Induction in the Human Cortex: A Brain Stimulation Study, Brain Stimulation (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.12.001. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1

Variability in Neural Excitability and Plasticity Induction in the Human Cortex: A Brain

2

Stimulation Study Abbreviated Title: Excitation and Plasticity in the Human Cortex

RI PT

3 4

Authors and Affiliations: Brenton Hordacre1, Mitchell R. Goldsworthy1,2, Ann-Maree Vallence3,

6

Sam Darvishi4, Bahar Moezzi5, Masashi Hamada6, John C. Rothwell7, Michael C. Ridding1

7

1

8

5005, Australia.

9

2

10

3

11

4

12

Australia.

13

5

14

and Mathematical Sciences, University of South Australia, Mawson Lakes 5095, Australia.

15

6

16

Japan.

17

7

18

Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom.

M AN U

The Robinson Research Institute, School of Medicine, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide

Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia. School of Psychology and Exercise Science, Murdoch University, Murdoch 6150, Australia.

TE D

School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide 5005,

Computational and Theoretical Neuroscience Laboratory, School of Information Technology

EP

Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, 113-8655,

Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience and Movement Disorders, UCL Institute of

AC C

19

SC

5

20

Corresponding Author: Michael C Ridding, The Robinson Research Institute, School of

21

Medicine, The University of Adelaide, 77 King William St, North Adelaide, 5006, Australia. Email:

22

[email protected]

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Abstract

24

Background: The potential of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) for both probing human

25

neuroplasticity and the induction of functionally relevant neuroplastic change has received

26

significant interest. However, at present the utility of NIBS is limited due to high response

27

variability. One reason for this response variability is that NIBS targets a diffuse cortical

28

population and the net outcome to stimulation depends on the relative levels of excitability in

29

each population. There is evidence that the relative excitability of complex oligosynaptic circuits

30

(late I-wave circuits) as assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is useful in

31

predicting NIBS response.

32

Objective: Here we examined whether an additional marker of cortical excitability, MEP

33

amplitude variability, could provide additional insights into response variability following

34

application of the continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS) NIBS protocol. Additionally we

35

investigated whether I-wave recruitment was associated with MEP variability.

36

Methods: Thirty-four healthy subjects (15 male, aged 18-35 years) participated in two

37

experiments. Experiment 1 investigated baseline MEP variability and cTBS response.

38

Experiment 2 determined if I-wave recruitment was associated with MEP variability.

39

Results: Data show that both baseline MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment are

40

associated with cTBS response, but were independent of each other; together, these variables

41

predict 31% of the variability in cTBS response.

42

Conclusions: This study provides insight into the physiological mechanisms underpinning NIBS

43

plasticity responses and may facilitate development of more reliable NIBS protocols.

44

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

45

Key Words: Transcranial magnetic stimulation, Theta burst stimulation, Motor evoked

47

potential, Variability, Motor cortex

RI PT

46

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

48

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Introduction

50

Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) can induce neuroplasticity in the human cortex that has

51

similar characteristics to activity-dependent long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term

52

depression (LTD) [1,2]. NIBS-induced neuroplasticity outlasts the stimulation [3-5], is bi-

53

directional based on pattern of stimulation [3-5], and is abolished following administration of

54

NMDA antagonists [6]. Importantly, there are behavioural effects following NIBS. For example,

55

inhibitory NIBS protocols applied to the motor cortex (M1) can degrade motor control [7], and

56

facilitatory NIBS can increase the rate of learning on a ballistic motor task [8]. Inducing LTP- or

57

LTD-like plasticity in the human motor cortex and modifying behaviour would be of clinical

58

value for a range of neurological conditions. However, at present the effects of various NIBS

59

protocols are highly variable [9-14]. This response variability limits the behavioural and clinical

60

usefulness of NIBS.

SC

M AN U

TE D

61

RI PT

49

Several factors contribute to NIBS response variability including age, time of day, attention,

63

history of physical activity and genetics [15]. Additionally, inter-individual differences in the

64

cortical network activated by NIBS can influence the response. The descending volley evoked by

65

single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) consists of a series of components. The

66

earliest of these probably reflects direct activation of the corticospinal output cells and is

67

known as the “direct (D)-wave”. The later components have been termed “indirect (I)-waves”.

68

The early I-waves likely reflect monosynaptic input to corticospinal neurons from layer II/III

69

interneurons, whereas more complex oligosynaptic circuits generate the late I-waves [16].

70

Individuals in whom TMS is more likely to recruit late I-waves respond more strongly to several

AC C

EP

62

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

forms of NIBS [13,17]. The reason for this is unclear but Hamada and colleagues (2013)

72

suggested that the late I-wave generating circuit might be more sensitive to NIBS than the early

73

I-wave generating circuit. Here, we were interested in examining whether variability in baseline

74

motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitude could serve as an indicator of likely neuroplastic

75

response to a NIBS protocol (continuous theta burst stimulation: cTBS). Our reasoning was as

76

follows: the amplitude of MEPs evoked in individuals in whom TMS was more likely to recruit

77

late I-wave generating circuits would be more variable due to the involvement of more complex

78

networks than in individuals in whom TMS was more likely to recruit less complex early I-wave

79

generating circuitry [18]. To explore mechanisms underpinning MEP variability we used

80

multiple TMS coil orientations to examine I-wave recruitment [13]. In summary, the aims of this

81

study were to (1) investigate the relationship between MEP variability and NIBS (cTBS)

82

response, and (2) explore whether I-wave recruitment profile might influence MEP variability.

83

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

71

Material and Methods

85

Subjects

86

A total of 34 healthy subjects (15 male) aged 18-35 years (mean age, 25.0 ± 4.9 years)

87

participated in two experiments. Potential subjects with contraindications for TMS, including

88

metallic implants, a history of seizures and medications known to alter CNS excitability were

89

excluded [19]. Ethical approval was provided by the University of Adelaide Human Research

90

Ethics Committee, and all participants provided written informed consent in accordance with

91

the Declaration of Helsinki.

92

AC C

EP

84

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Electromyography

94

For both experiments, surface EMG was recorded from the right first dorsal interosseous (FDI)

95

muscle using Ag/AgCl electrodes (Ambu, Ballerup, Denmark) with electrodes positioned in a

96

belly-tendon montage. Signals were sampled at 5 kHz (Cambridge Electronic Design 1401,

97

Cambridge, UK), amplified with a gain of 1000, band-pass filtered (20-1000 Hz) (Cambridge

98

Electronic Design 1902 amplifier, Cambridge, UK) and stored for offline analysis (Signal v4.09,

99

Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK).

M AN U

100

SC

RI PT

93

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

102

Single-pulse TMS was applied with a monophasic waveform using a figure-of-eight coil (external

103

wing diameter 90mm) connected to a Magstim 200 stimulator (Magstim Company, Dyfed, UK).

104

For Experiment 1, the coil was positioned tangentially over the left M1, with the handle rotated

105

posterior-laterally approximately 45° to the sagittal plane to induce a posterior-anterior current

106

flow across the hand M1. The optimal coil position for evoking a MEP in the right FDI muscle at

107

rest was located and marked on the scalp using a water-soluble felt tip marker. Rest motor

108

threshold (RMT) for the right FDI was defined as the minimum stimulus intensity required to

109

evoke an MEP with peak-to-peak amplitude ≥50µV in at least five out of 10 consecutive trials in

110

the relaxed FDI.

EP

AC C

111

TE D

101

112

For Experiment 2, MEPs were evoked using three different directions of current flow across the

113

left M1 hand area. Previous studies have demonstrated that by modifying the direction of

114

current flow it is possible to target specific populations of neurons using single pulse TMS.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Posterior-anterior (PA) currents preferentially recruit early I-waves, anterior-posterior (AP)

116

currents recruit late I-waves and lateral-medial (LM) currents at high stimulus intensities evoke

117

D-waves [18,20-23]. In this experiment we evoked MEPs using three different coil orientations

118

to preferentially induce current flow across the hand M1 to investigate late I-waves, early I-

119

waves and D-waves. PA currents were elicited with the handle of the figure-of-eight coil rotated

120

posterior-laterally, approximately 45° to the sagittal plane. AP currents were elicited by placing

121

the coil 180° to the PA current coil position. LM currents were elicited with the handle rotated

122

laterally to a position 90° to the midsagittal line. Active motor threshold (AMT) was measured

123

for PA, AP and LM currents while stimulating at the hotspot determined by PA currents, as

124

previous studies have determined that direction of the current does not influence the position

125

of the hotspot [22,24]. AMT was defined as the lowest intensity to evoke an MEP of ≥ 200µV in

126

at least five out of 10 consecutive trials whilst maintaining a 5-10% maximal voluntary

127

contraction of the FDI. Muscle contraction was monitored visually using a digital oscilloscope

128

with participants able to monitor and adjust muscle contraction to maintain the required 5-10%

129

MVC.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

130

RI PT

115

Continuous theta burst stimulation

132

In Experiment 1, an air-cooled figure-of-eight coil connected to a Magstim Super Rapid

133

stimulator (Magstim Company, Dyfed, UK) was used to apply cTBS with a biphasic pulse

134

waveform (current direction PA-AP) to the optimal site for stimulating the right FDI. The cTBS

135

protocol consisted of 600 pulses applied in bursts of three pulses at 50Hz, repeated at 5Hz for a

AC C

131

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

136

total of 40 seconds [3]. The intensity of stimulation was set to 70% RMT [25,26], assessed prior

137

to cTBS application using the rTMS coil.

RI PT

138

Experimental Protocol

140

For Experiment 1, subjects attended an afternoon experimental session to determine the

141

relationship between baseline MEP variability and the response to cTBS. Subjects were seated

142

in a comfortable chair with their right upper limb in a relaxed position. At baseline, a total of

143

225 MEPs were evoked over two blocks separated by a short, 2 minute rest interval. Three

144

stimulation intensities were used to examine whether the relationship between MEP variability

145

and cTBS response was influenced by MEP amplitude; the intensities were 120% RMT, 150%

146

RMT and a stimulus intensity set to produce a 1mV MEP (SI1mV). The 120% RMT and SI1mV

147

intensities were selected as they are commonly used to evoke test MEPs prior to plasticity

148

induction protocols [27-29]. The 150% intensity was used to explore the relationship between

149

baseline MEP amplitude variability and plasticity response at larger mean MEP amplitudes. At

150

baseline, a total of 75 TMS pulses at each of the three intensities were delivered randomly with

151

an inter-stimulus interval of 6 sec ± 10%. Following cTBS, 50 TMS pulses at each of the three

152

intensities were delivered randomly (with an inter-stimulus interval of 6 sec ± 10%) from 0-15

153

minutes following cTBS, and again at 20-35 minutes following cTBS; therefore, a total of 300

154

MEPs (100 MEPs for each intensity) were obtained following cTBS (and we grouped these into

155

5-minute blocks: 0-, 5-, 10-, 20-, 25-, 30-min post cTBS). The same stimulation intensities and

156

inter-stimulus intervals were used at baseline and following cTBS.

157

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

139

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Experiment 2 was conducted in the afternoon, >7 days following experiment 1. The purpose of

159

Experiment 2 was to determine if differences between individuals in I-wave recruitment were

160

associated with different levels of MEP variability. Subjects were seated in a comfortable chair

161

with the lateral aspect of the distal phalanx of the right index finger positioned against a force

162

transducer. For both AP and PA coil orientations, 20 MEPs were evoked at 110% AMT with

163

subjects asked to relax their hand every 10 trials to avoid fatigue. For LM coil orientation, 10

164

MEPs were evoked at 150% AMT or 50% of maximum stimulator output (MSO), whichever was

165

greater. Higher stimulus intensities were used for LM currents to increase the likelihood of

166

evoking a D-wave [21]. For all three coil orientations, MEPs were evoked with an inter-stimulus

167

interval of 6 sec ± 10%. MEP onset latency for each trial (for all three coil orientations) was

168

determined automatically with a custom made script to avoid assessor bias (Signal v4.09,

169

Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). In each trial, the onset latency was defined as

170

the time point where the rectified EMG signals exceeded an average plus two standard

171

deviations of the pre-stimulus EMG level 100ms prior to the TMS pulse. AP, PA and LM onset

172

latencies were averaged across trials for each subject. Consistent with the study of Hamada and

173

colleagues (2013), the latency difference between AP and LM evoked MEPs was used as a

174

measure of the relative likelihood of recruiting late I-wave input to corticospinal neurons [13].

176

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

175

RI PT

158

177

Data analysis

178

Normality of data were tested with the Shapiro-Wilk test and where required, logarithmic

179

transformations were performed. Descriptive statistics were used to report experimental

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

variables. To characterise the response to cTBS in Experiment 1, MEP amplitudes were

181

averaged for each stimulus intensity, subject and time point. Trials contaminated with

182

background EMG activity during 100ms prior to the TMS pulse were excluded from the analysis.

183

A 2 x 2 repeated measures ANOVA (factor of ‘TIME’: baseline 1, baseline 2, Post 0 min, Post 20

184

min; factor of ‘INTENSITY’: 120% RMT, 150% RMT, SI1mV) was used to investigate the effect of

185

cTBS on absolute (raw) MEP amplitude. The association between baseline MEP variability

186

(coefficient of variation, CV) and cTBS response (quantified as the grand average of post-cTBS

187

time points normalised to the average baseline) was investigated with Pearson correlations for

188

MEPs evoked at 120% RMT, 150% RMT and SI1mV. To investigate whether MEP amplitude or

189

RMT were associated with baseline MEP variability, we performed Pearson correlations for

190

MEPs evoked at 120% RMT, 150% RMT and SI1mV. To investigate whether the distribution of

191

MEP amplitudes changed following cTBS we analysed the skewness and kurtosis of MEPs

192

evoked at 120% RMT and SI1mV before and after cTBS, split into high and low MEP variability

193

groups (median split), and tested for differences using paired t-tests. For Experiment 2, the

194

association between AP-LM and PA-LM latency differences, and both cTBS response and

195

variability of baseline MEPs was investigated with Pearson correlations for MEPs evoked at

196

120% RMT, 150% RMT and SI1mV. Since it could be argued that the range of MEP latencies

197

evoked by AP currents is due to variation of MEP amplitudes evoked with this coil orientation,

198

we correlated AP MEP amplitude and AP MEP latency. The significance level was set at p ≤ 0.05,

199

and SPSS software was used for all statistical analyses (IBM Corp., Released 2011, IBM SPSS

200

Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0, Armonk, NY, USA).

201

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

180

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Results

203

Experiment 1 – Baseline MEP variability and cTBS response

204

The average RMT was 41.3% MSO (SD 8.3). Baseline neurophysiological measures for each

205

stimulus intensity are reported in Table 1. As expected, there was a main effect of INTENSITY

206

(F(2,66) = 52.3, p < 0.001) with MEPs evoked at 150% RMT larger than those evoked at 120% RMT

207

(95%CI; 1.2-1.8, p < 0.001) and SI1mV (95%CI; 1.2-2.4, p < 0.001). However, there was no main

208

effect of TIME (F(3,99) = 0.86, p = 0.47) or TIME x INTENSITY interaction (F(6,198) = 0.29, p = 0.94)

209

suggesting cTBS did not significantly change MEP amplitude (see figure 1; individual subject

210

cTBS response profiles provided in Supplementary figure 1; tabulated data provided in

211

Supplementary table 1). As there were no significant differences in MEP amplitude across post-

212

cTBS time points, we calculated a grand average cTBS response value for each intensity: MEP

213

amplitudes were normalised to baseline (percentage of the average baseline MEP amplitude),

214

and then averaged across all post-cTBS time-points. There were significant correlations

215

between baseline MEP variability and cTBS response for 120% RMT (r = -0.44, p = 0.01) and

216

SI1mV (r = -0.37, p = 0.03), but not 150 %RMT (p = 0.59) (see figure 2); higher variability in

217

baseline MEP amplitude (at 120%RMT and SI1mV) was associated with a stronger inhibitory

218

response to cTBS. Following correction for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni correction,

219

adjusted level of significance p < 0.017), the association between baseline MEP variability and

220

cTBS response remained significant for 120% RMT. Further investigation of the relationship

221

between baseline MEP variability (120% RMT) and cTBS response revealed that participants

222

with higher baseline MEP variability (median split of baseline MEP variability) had a significant

223

suppression of MEP amplitude following cTBS (post cTBS MEPs 84% of baseline, t(16) = 2.19, p =

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

202

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

0.04). However, participants with low baseline MEP variability did not have a significant

225

facilitation of MEP amplitude following cTBS (post cTBS MEPs 112% of baseline, t(16) = 1.75, p =

226

0.10). Although there were significant associations between the variability and amplitude of

227

MEPs recorded at baseline (120%RMT; r = -0.47, p = 0.01; 150% RMT; r = -0.45, p = 0.01; SI1mV; r

228

= -0.43, p = 0.01), the relationship between baseline MEP variability and cTBS response

229

remained when controlling for baseline MEP amplitude (120%RMT; r = -0.43, p = 0.01; SI1mV; r =

230

-0.35, p = 0.03). As previous, the association between MEP variability and cTBS response

231

remained significant for 120% RMT survived correction for multiple comparisons. There were

232

no significant relationships between RMT and baseline MEP variability for MEPs evoked at all

233

intensities (all p > 0.16). There was no significant difference in distribution of MEP amplitudes

234

from baseline to post cTBS for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT (p = 0.44) or SI1mV (p = 0.42) in both

235

participants with high or low MEP variability (see figure 3).

SC

M AN U

TE D

236

RI PT

224

Experiment 2 – I-wave recruitment and MEP variability

238

Mean AMT, MEP amplitude and onset latencies for PA, AP and LM coil orientations are

239

reported in table 2. The mean AP-LM latency difference was 3.85±1.25 ms and PA-LM latency

240

difference was 1.82±1.12 ms (see figure 4). There was a significant correlation between AP-LM

241

latency difference and grand average cTBS response for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT (r = -0.37, p

242

= 0.03) (figure 4), but not 150% RMT (p = 0.40) or SI1mV (p = 0.27), however the relationship with

243

AP-LM latency difference and cTBS response for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT did not survive

244

correction for multiple comparisons. There were no significant associations between PA-LM

245

latency difference and cTBS response for MEPs evoked at any stimulation intensity (p > 0.54).

AC C

EP

237

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

There were no significant associations between AP-LM or PA-LM latency difference and

247

baseline MEP variability for MEPs evoked at any intensity (all p > 0.33). There was no

248

association between MEP amplitude evoked by AP currents and AP latency (p = 0.83). Since

249

both MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment appear to be independent, but important,

250

factors associated with the cTBS response measured at 120% RMT we investigated the

251

relationship with a multiple regression. The regression model reached significance (R2 = 0.31, p

252

= 0.004), indicating that MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment predict 31% of the variance

253

in cTBS response.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

246

254

Discussion

256

In this study we report significant inter-subject variability in the response to cTBS, which is

257

consistent with recent reports that demonstrate highly variable response profiles following

258

cTBS [13,30]. Indeed, even though care was taken in controlling factors know to influence cTBS

259

response (e.g. pre-activation and time of day) there was no significant group level cTBS

260

response. While progress has been made in understanding the causes of response variability

261

(for review see Ridding and Ziemann [15]), a large component of this variability remains

262

unexplained and this may have contributed to the non-significant group response to cTBS

263

observed in this study. Identifying additional factors contributing to response variability is

264

important to both improve understanding of the physiology underpinning responses to cTBS

265

(and other NIBS protocols) and to direct development and targeting of more robust stimulation

266

protocols. Here, we provide some novel insights into additional factors contributing to cTBS

267

response variability.

AC C

EP

TE D

255

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

268

Associations between I-wave recruitment, MEP variability, and cTBS response

270

By using different coil orientations (PA/AP) it is possible to preferentially recruit early and late I-

271

waves. Using this approach, Hamada and colleagues [13] demonstrated a stronger cTBS

272

response in individuals in whom TMS pulses preferentially recruited late I-waves. The current

273

results replicate the findings of Hamada et al. [13], showing that inter-individual differences in

274

late I-wave recruitment is associated with cTBS response. Hamada and colleagues [13]

275

suggested that the association between I-wave recruitment and cTBS-induced plasticity could

276

be due to a greater sensitivity of the late I-wave generating circuit than the early I-wave

277

generating circuit to cTBS.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

269

278

Given the proposed differential sensitivity of the late and early I-wave generating circuits, we

280

examined the relationship between the likelihood of the TMS pulse recruiting late I-waves (with

281

AP-LM latency difference acting as a marker) and MEP variability; we hypothesised that MEPs

282

evoked in individuals in whom TMS was more likely to recruit late I-wave circuits would be

283

more variable than MEPs in individuals in which TMS was more likely to recruit less complex

284

early I-wave circuitry. However, surprisingly, we found no relationship between I-wave

285

recruitment and MEP variability. This suggests that MEP variability is not highly dependent

286

upon engagement of late I-wave circuits.

AC C

EP

TE D

279

287 288

While there was no significant association between I-wave recruitment and MEP variability,

289

there was a significant association between MEP variability and cTBS response; greater MEP

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

variability at baseline was associated with a stronger inhibitory cTBS response. This relationship

291

was significant for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT and was supported by data recorded at the 1mV

292

intensity where the correlation between variability and response approached statistical

293

significance. One possible explanation for this is that high MEP variability is associated with a

294

wider distribution of MEP amplitudes; for example, in individuals with high variability, TMS

295

might evoke more very large MEPs that are closer to the ceiling of the testable range than in

296

individuals with low variability. This could be important because it has been reported that cTBS

297

is more likely to inhibit near-maximal MEPs [31]. Thus it could be that, on average, individuals

298

with high variability are more likely to respond to cTBS because they have more near-maximal

299

MEPs. If this were the case, then we might expect these large MEPs to be inhibited following

300

cTBS, and hence the distribution of MEP amplitudes would change. However, we show that the

301

distribution of MEPs evoked at 120% RMT and SI1mV intensities did not change following cTBS,

302

irrespective of whether individuals demonstrated high or low baseline variability. There was no

303

relationship between baseline variability in MEPs evoked at an intensity of 150% RMT and cTBS

304

response. We suggest this likely reflects the reduced MEP variability when tested at this high

305

intensity.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

306

RI PT

290

307

While the data does show that high variability is associated with a strong inhibitory response to

308

cTBS it also suggests that low variability is associated with a facilitatory response. To explore

309

this further we examined the response to cTBS by splitting the participant data into two groups,

310

namely high and low variability groups. We were able to show that the higher baseline MEP

311

variability group demonstrated a significant suppression of MEP amplitudes following cTBS. In

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

contrast, the low baseline variability group had a non-significant trend towards a facilitatory

313

response to cTBS. This result suggests that the relationship between variability and the

314

response to cTBS might be complex and not one merely based upon the strength of the

315

response. Indeed, differing levels of MEP variability may be associated with the engagement of

316

different mechanisms (LTD/LTD) by the cTBS stimulus. However, this is highly speculative and

317

we suggest this issue requires further investigation, perhaps, by using a facilitatory (LTP-like)

318

non-invasive brain stimulation protocol. Nevertheless, our results suggest late I-wave

319

recruitment and MEP variability are independently associated with cTBS response. When

320

combined, MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment predicted 31% of the cTBS response

321

variability in the current study. Given the extensive range of factors that contribute to

322

variability in NIBS response [15], these two factors account for a major component of cTBS

323

response variability. However, we acknowledge that this estimate, and that reported for several

324

other factors, is likely to be an overestimation given likely interdependence between multiple

325

factors. Further work is required to investigate the interdependence between factors that have

326

been identified as contributing to NIBS response variability.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

327

RI PT

312

We hypothesised that MEP variability would be greatest in individuals in which late I-waves

329

were more likely to be recruited. However, as described above, we were unable to provide

330

support for this hypothesis. What then is the mechanism underpinning MEP variability?

331

Previous studies have demonstrated that MEP amplitude is influenced by intrinsic oscillatory

332

activities recorded using electroencephalography (EEG). For example, for TMS applied near

333

resting motor threshold, MEPs are more likely to be evoked when preceded by low pre-stimulus

AC C

328

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

alpha power [32]. Similarly, larger MEPs have been observed with lower pre-stimulus beta

335

power [33-37]. Furthermore, oscillatory phase of both alpha and beta activities can influence

336

MEP amplitude [37-39]. Therefore, we suggest intrinsic fluctuations in ongoing oscillatory

337

activity are a likely substrate to explain at least some of the MEP variability. How such changes

338

might explain the variability in cTBS response is unclear.

SC

339

RI PT

334

Similarity to the relationships between movement variability and motor learning

341

Interestingly, there are some parallels between the current results and several recent reports

342

examining motor learning. Greater task related baseline movement variability predicts faster

343

motor learning [40]. Also, Teo and colleagues [8] reported that facilitatory intermittent theta

344

burst stimulation increased movement variability on a subsequent ballistic motor learning task,

345

and that this increase in variability correlated with improved learning. Movement variability

346

allows the individual to explore motor command space and identify optimal motor patterns

347

resulting in greater efficiency during learning. While multiple factors are likely to contribute to

348

variability of movement output, movement execution contributes a large proportion of this

349

variability [41]. The corticospinal system plays a key role in movement execution [42] and

350

variability in movement likely reflects both cortical and spinal influences. Therefore, the MEP

351

variability described here may reflect to some degree the variability seen in movements during

352

learning. While the output measures of behavioural (learning) and neurophysiological studies

353

(cTBS response) are clearly quite different, there might be involvement of a common

354

physiological mechanism, namely activity dependent changes in synaptic strength.

355

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

340

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

When considering these results, it is important to acknowledge some limitations. First, we only

357

studied a population of healthy adults using one common NIBS plasticity-inducing paradigm

358

(cTBS). It is unclear whether these findings are generalizable to wider populations or alternative

359

NIBS paradigms. Second, we have only investigated one potential contributor to MEP

360

variability. It is likely multiple, interacting factors contribute to MEP variability and further

361

studies should seek to provide greater understanding of contributions to this variability. Third,

362

MEP variability is likely due to both cortical and spinal effects [32,43,44]. We did not investigate

363

spinal influences and so the association between MEP variability and the cortically generated

364

cTBS response might be over or under estimated. Finally, although we took care to minimise

365

coil movement during data collection, it is possible that random small coil movements may

366

have contributed to the overall MEP variability. However, we consider it unlikely that this

367

contributed to the reported association between variability and cTBS response and, in fact, may

368

have weakened the relationship by introducing noise. The use of stereotactic navigation

369

techniques may strengthen the findings reported in this study.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

370

RI PT

356

In summary, we provide evidence that MEP variability is an important influence on the cTBS

372

response in healthy adults. Traditionally considered an unwanted characteristic of stochastic

373

nervous system function driven by multiple intrinsic and extrinsic contributions, MEP variability

374

may in fact be an important physiological characteristic to enhance our understanding of

375

cortical network excitability, motor learning and NIBS response. Our results may suggest

376

avenues for developments that improve the reliability of NIBS, for example by employing

AC C

371

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

377

behavioural or external priming procedures to modulate variability in the excitability of the

378

corticospinal system prior to plasticity induction.

RI PT

379

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

380

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

381

Funding: MRG is supported by an NHMRC-ARC Dementia Research Development Fellowship

382

(1102272). AMV is supported by a NHMRC Biomedical Training Fellowship (GNT1088295).

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

383

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

384

Table and Figure Legends

385

Table 1: Baseline neurophysiological measures for each stimulus intensity

387

RMT, resting motor threshold; MSO, maximum stimulator output; mV, millivolts; CV, coefficient

388

of variation; MEP, motor evoked potential.

SC

389

RI PT

386

Table 2: AMT, MEP amplitude and onset latency for MEPs evoked using PA, AP and LM coil

391

orientations. AMT, active motor threshold; MEP, motor evoked potential; MSO, maximal

392

stimulator output; PA, posterior-anterior current direction; AP, anterior-posterior current

393

direction; LM, lateral-medial current direction.

M AN U

390

394

Figure 1: Group average cTBS response for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT, 150% RMT, and SI1mV for

396

both raw MEP amplitudes (top) and MEPs normalised to the mean baseline (bottom). CTBS did

397

not affect MEP amplitude for any stimulation intensity.

TE D

395

EP

398

Figure 2: Correlation between MEP variability at baseline for MEPs evoked at A) 120% RMT, B)

400

150% RMT and C) SI1mV and cTBS response averaged across post cTBS time points. Greater MEP

401

variability was associated with a stronger inhibitory response to cTBS for MEPs evoked at 120%

402

RMT and SI1mV intensities.

AC C

399

403 404

Figure 3: Distribution of MEPs evoked at baseline (top) and following cTBS (bottom) for subjects

405

with low variability (left) and high variability (right) for MEPs evoked at SI1mV. MEP amplitude

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

was normalised to the mean baseline amplitude for each subject. There was no change in

407

distribution of MEPs following cTBS suggesting that cTBS does not target specific networks

408

responsible for either large or small MEPs.

RI PT

406

409 410

Figure 4: PA-LM and AP-LM latency differences for A) individual participants and B) presented

412

as a histogram in 0.5ms bins. The AP-LM latency difference was longer than the PA-LM latency

413

difference. The AP-LM latency difference also appeared more variable across participants

414

compared to the PA-LM latency difference. Figure C) presents the correlation between AP-LM

415

latency difference as a measure of late I-wave efficiency and cTBS response averaged across

416

post cTBS time points for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT. A stronger response to cTBS was

417

associated with a greater AP-LM latency difference.

M AN U

TE D EP AC C

418

SC

411

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

419

Tables

420

Table 1

RI PT

421

Stimulus Intensity 150%RMT

SI1mV

Stimulation Intensity (%MSO, mean (SD))

49.7 (9.6)

62.3 (11.8)

50.9 (11.7)

MEP amplitude (mV, mean (SD))

1.48 (0.9)

3.05 (1.5)

1.18 (0.4)

422 423

425

EP

427

Table 2

35.6 (13.3)

66.5 (24.9)

Current Direction

AMT (%MSO)

MEP amplitude (mV)

Latency (ms)

PA

32.7 (7.4)

0.83 (0.41)

22.6 (1.7)

AP

45.1 (10.7)

0.68 (0.38)

24.5 (2.1)

LM

36.1 (8.7)

5.89 (2.41)

20.7 (1.6)

AC C

426

64.4 (20.7)

TE D

424

M AN U

MEP variability (CV, %, mean (SD))

SC

120%RMT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

428

References

429

[1]

430

dentate area of the unanaestetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol

431

1973;232:357-74.

432

[2]

433

the adult and immature hippocampus. J Neurosci 1993;13:2910-8.

434

[3]

435

human motor cortex. Neuron 2005;45:201-6.

436

[4]

437

frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 1997;48:1398-403.

438

[5]

439

repetitive cortical stimulation in man. Exp Brain Res 1998;122:79-84.

440

[6]

441

stimulation is NMDA receptor dependent. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:1028-32.

442

[7]

443

cortex degrades selective muscle activation in the ipsilateral arm. J Neurophysiol

444

2010;104:2594-602.

445

[8]

446

stimulation enhances subsequent motor learning and increases performance variability. Cereb

447

Cortex 2011;21:1627-38.

RI PT

Bliss TV, Gardner-Medwin AR. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the

SC

Dudek SM, Bear MF. Bidirectional long-term modification of synaptic effectiveness in

M AN U

Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC. Theta burst stimulation of the

Chen R, Classen J, Gerloff C et al. Depression of motor cortex excitability by low-

TE D

Berardelli A, Inghilleri M, Rothwell JC et al. Facilitation of muscle evoked responses after

Huang Y-Z, Chen R-S, Rothwell JC, Wen H-Y. The after-effect of human theta burst

AC C

EP

Bradnam LV, Stinear CM, Byblow WD. Theta burst stimulation of human primary motor

Teo JTH, Swayne OBC, Cheeran B, Greenwood RJ, Rothwell JC. Human theta burst

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

[9]

López-Alonso V, Fernández-del-Olmo M, Costantini A, Gonzalez-Henriquez JJ, Cheeran B.

449

Intra-individual variability in the response to anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin

450

Neurophysiol 2015;126:2342-7.

451

[10]

452

direct current stimulation (tDCS) at varying current intensities. Brain Stimul 2015;8:1130-7.

453

[11]

454

dependency of motor cortical plasticity induced by paired associative stimulation. Exp Brain Res

455

2008;187:467-75.

456

[12]

457

intermittent theta burst stimulation: Implications for rehabilitation and recovery. Brain Stimul

458

2014;7:365-71.

459

[13]

460

networks in driving human motor cortical plasticity. Cereb Cortex 2013;23:1593-605.

461

[14]

462

invasive brain stimulation protocols and motor learning in healthy adults. Neurosci Lett

463

2013;549:151-6.

464

[15]

465

invasive brain stimulation in healthy subjects. J Physiol (Lond) 2010;588:2291-304.

466

[16]

467

2012;5:512-25.

468

[17]

469

stimulation of the motor cortex. Brain Stimul 2014;7:468-75.

RI PT

448

Chew T, Ho KA, Loo CK. Inter- and intra-individual variability in response to transcranial

M AN U

SC

Müller-Dahlhaus JF, Orekhov Y, Liu Y, Ziemann U. Interindividual variability and age-

Hinder MR, Goss EL, Fujiyama H et al. Inter- and intra-individual variability following

TE D

Hamada M, Murase N, Hasan A, Balaratnam M, Rothwell JC. The role of interneuron

EP

Vallence AM, Kurylowicz L, Ridding MC. A comparison of neuroplastic responses to non-

AC C

Ridding MC, Ziemann U. Determinants of the induction of cortical plasticity by non-

Di Lazzaro V, Profice P, Ranieri F et al. I-wave origin and modulation. Brain Stimul

Wiethoff S, Hamada M, Rothwell JC. Variability in response to transcranial direct current

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

[18]

Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Saturno E et al. The effect on corticospinal volleys of reversing

471

the direction of current induced in the motor cortex by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp

472

Brain Res 2001;138:268-73.

473

[19]

474

update. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;122:1686.

475

[20]

476

human motor cortex: surface EMG and single motor unit responses. J Physiol (Lond)

477

1989;412:449-73.

478

[21]

479

latency of surface EMG and single motor unit responses in the first dorsal interosseous muscle.

480

Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;93:138-46.

481

[22]

482

activation of different I waves by transcranial magnetic stimulation with a figure-of-eight-

483

shaped coil. Exp Brain Res 1997;113:24-32.

484

[23]

485

volleys evoked by transcranial stimulation in conscious humans. J Physiol (Lond) 1998;508:625-

486

33.

487

[24]

488

train, monophasic and biphasic repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the

489

human motor cortex. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:605-13.

RI PT

470

Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A. Screening questionnaire before TMS: An

M AN U

SC

Day BL, Dressler D, Maertens de Noordhout A et al. Electric and magnetic stimulation of

Werhahn KJ, Fong JKY, Meyer BU et al. The effect of magnetic coil orientation on the

TE D

Sakai K, Ugawa Y, Terao Y, Hanajima R, Furubayashi T, Kanazawa I. Preferential

AC C

EP

Di Lazzaro V, Restuccia D, Oliviero A et al. Effects of voluntary contraction on descending

Arai N, Okabe S, Furubayashi T, Terao Y, Yuasa K, Ugawa Y. Comparison between short

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

[25]

Gentner R, Wankerl K, Reinsberger C, Zeller D, Classen J. Depression of human

491

corticospinal excitability induced by magnetic theta-burst stimulation: evidence of rapid

492

polarity-reversing metaplasticity. Cereb Cortex 2008;18:2046-53.

493

[26]

494

LTD-like plasticity in human motor cortex: A matter of preceding motor activation. Brain Stimul

495

2014;7:864-70.

496

[27]

497

estimating 50% of the maximal motor evoked potential. Clin Neurophysiol 2015;126:2337-41.

498

[28]

499

within- and between-session reliability of TMS measures of corticospinal excitability.

500

Neuroscience 2016;320:205-9.

501

[29]

502

stroke. Cereb Cortex 2010;20:1523-8.

503

[30]

504

protocols induces long-lasting neuroplastic changes in the human motor cortex. Eur J Neurosci

505

2012;35:125-34.

506

[31]

507

changes in corticospinal excitability following theta burst stimulation of the human primary

508

motor cortex. Clin Neurophysiol 2016;127:740-7.

509

[32]

510

activity determines cortical excitability in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:284-8.

RI PT

490

SC

Goldsworthy MR, Müller-Dahlhaus F, Ridding MC, Ziemann U. Inter-subject variability of

M AN U

Pitcher JB, Doeltgen SH, Goldsworthy MR et al. A comparison of two methods for

Goldsworthy MR, Hordacre B, Ridding MC. Minimum number of trials required for

TE D

Di Lazzaro V, Profice P, Pilato F et al. Motor cortex plasticity predicts recovery in acute

EP

Goldsworthy MR, Pitcher JB, Ridding MC. The application of spaced theta burst

AC C

Goldsworthy MR, Vallence AM, Hodyl NA, Semmler JG, Pitcher JB, Ridding MC. Probing

Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Gerloff C, Hummel FC. Spontaneous locally restricted EEG alpha

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

[33]

Lepage JF, Saint-Amour D, Théoret H. EEG and neuronavigated single-pulse TMS in the

512

study of the observation/execution matching system: Are both techniques measuring the same

513

process? J Neurosci Methods 2008;175:17-24.

514

[34]

515

motor system excitability in overlapping neuronal populations. Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:492-

516

501.

517

[35]

518

influence of pre-TMS oscillations and corticomuscular coherence on motor-evoked potentials.

519

Cereb Cortex 2014;24:1708-19.

520

[36]

521

potential amplitude. Clin EEG Neurosci 2006;37:247-51.

522

[37]

523

and corticospinal synchronization influence TMS-evoked motor potentials. J Neurophysiol

524

2014;111:513-9.

525

[38]

526

beta-band synchronization entails rhythmic gain modulation. J Neurosci 2010;30:4481-8.

527

[39]

528

markers of corticospinal excitability in the resting brain. BioMed Res Int 2014;2014.

529

[40]

530

variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability. Nat Neurosci

531

2014;17:312-21.

RI PT

511

SC

Mäki H, Ilmoniemi RJ. EEG oscillations and magnetically evoked motor potentials reflect

M AN U

Schulz H, Übelacker T, Keil J, Müller N, Weisz N. Now i am ready - Now i am not: The

Zarkowski P, Shin CJ, Dang T, Russo J, Avery D. EEG and the variance of motor evoked

TE D

Keil J, Timm J, Sanmiguel I, Schulz H, Obleser J, Schonwiesner M. Cortical brain states

EP

Van Elswijk G, Maij F, Schoffelen JM, Overeem S, Stegeman DF, Fries P. Corticospinal

AC C

Berger B, Minarik T, Liuzzi G, Hummel FC, Sauseng P. EEG oscillatory phase-dependent

Wu HG, Miyamoto YR, Castro LNG, Olveczky BP, Smith MA. Temporal structure of motor

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

532

[41]

Van Beers RJ, Haggard P, Wolpert DM. The role of execution noise in movement

533

variability. J Neurophysiol 2004;91:1050-63.

534

[42]

535

2000;23:393-415.

536

[43]

537

reveals rapid shifts in motor cortical excitability during the human sleep slow oscillation. J

538

Neurosci 2012;32:243-53.

539

[44]

540

stimulation: Report of an IFCN committee. Clin Neurophysiol 2012;123:858-82.

RI PT

Sanes JN, Donoghue JP. Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci

SC

Bergmann TO, Molle M, Schmidt MA et al. EEG-guided transcranial magnetic stimulation

M AN U

Groppa S, Oliviero A, Eisen A et al. A practical guide to diagnostic transcranial magnetic

541

AC C

EP

TE D

542

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Highlights

RI PT

SC

M AN U



TE D



EP

• •

Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) offers significant opportunities to examine plasticity in the human brain. There is some evidence that NIBS might be useful for inducing functionally beneficial change in a variety of neurological/psychiatric conditions. However, at present the utility is NIBS is somewhat limited due to high response variability. Key to harnessing the potential of NIBS is to understand more fully the factors contributing to this variability. This variability might, at least in part, be determined by the relative excitability of different cortical networks activated by NIBS. This study provides new insights into the influence of cortical excitability on the response to a commonly employed NIBS protocol and may open up avenues to improve response reliability.

AC C