Accepted Manuscript Variability in Neural Excitability and Plasticity Induction in the Human Cortex: A Brain Stimulation Study Brenton Hordacre, Mitchell R. Goldsworthy, Ann-Maree Vallence, Sam Darvishi, Bahar Moezzi, Masashi Hamada, John C. Rothwell, Michael C. Ridding PII:
S1935-861X(16)30383-7
DOI:
10.1016/j.brs.2016.12.001
Reference:
BRS 987
To appear in:
Brain Stimulation
Received Date: 1 August 2016 Revised Date:
9 November 2016
Accepted Date: 3 December 2016
Please cite this article as: Hordacre B, Goldsworthy MR, Vallence A-M, Darvishi S, Moezzi B, Hamada M, Rothwell JC, Ridding MC, Variability in Neural Excitability and Plasticity Induction in the Human Cortex: A Brain Stimulation Study, Brain Stimulation (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.12.001. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
Variability in Neural Excitability and Plasticity Induction in the Human Cortex: A Brain
2
Stimulation Study Abbreviated Title: Excitation and Plasticity in the Human Cortex
RI PT
3 4
Authors and Affiliations: Brenton Hordacre1, Mitchell R. Goldsworthy1,2, Ann-Maree Vallence3,
6
Sam Darvishi4, Bahar Moezzi5, Masashi Hamada6, John C. Rothwell7, Michael C. Ridding1
7
1
8
5005, Australia.
9
2
10
3
11
4
12
Australia.
13
5
14
and Mathematical Sciences, University of South Australia, Mawson Lakes 5095, Australia.
15
6
16
Japan.
17
7
18
Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom.
M AN U
The Robinson Research Institute, School of Medicine, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide
Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia. School of Psychology and Exercise Science, Murdoch University, Murdoch 6150, Australia.
TE D
School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide 5005,
Computational and Theoretical Neuroscience Laboratory, School of Information Technology
EP
Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, 113-8655,
Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience and Movement Disorders, UCL Institute of
AC C
19
SC
5
20
Corresponding Author: Michael C Ridding, The Robinson Research Institute, School of
21
Medicine, The University of Adelaide, 77 King William St, North Adelaide, 5006, Australia. Email:
22
[email protected]
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Abstract
24
Background: The potential of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) for both probing human
25
neuroplasticity and the induction of functionally relevant neuroplastic change has received
26
significant interest. However, at present the utility of NIBS is limited due to high response
27
variability. One reason for this response variability is that NIBS targets a diffuse cortical
28
population and the net outcome to stimulation depends on the relative levels of excitability in
29
each population. There is evidence that the relative excitability of complex oligosynaptic circuits
30
(late I-wave circuits) as assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is useful in
31
predicting NIBS response.
32
Objective: Here we examined whether an additional marker of cortical excitability, MEP
33
amplitude variability, could provide additional insights into response variability following
34
application of the continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS) NIBS protocol. Additionally we
35
investigated whether I-wave recruitment was associated with MEP variability.
36
Methods: Thirty-four healthy subjects (15 male, aged 18-35 years) participated in two
37
experiments. Experiment 1 investigated baseline MEP variability and cTBS response.
38
Experiment 2 determined if I-wave recruitment was associated with MEP variability.
39
Results: Data show that both baseline MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment are
40
associated with cTBS response, but were independent of each other; together, these variables
41
predict 31% of the variability in cTBS response.
42
Conclusions: This study provides insight into the physiological mechanisms underpinning NIBS
43
plasticity responses and may facilitate development of more reliable NIBS protocols.
44
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
45
Key Words: Transcranial magnetic stimulation, Theta burst stimulation, Motor evoked
47
potential, Variability, Motor cortex
RI PT
46
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
48
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Introduction
50
Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) can induce neuroplasticity in the human cortex that has
51
similar characteristics to activity-dependent long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term
52
depression (LTD) [1,2]. NIBS-induced neuroplasticity outlasts the stimulation [3-5], is bi-
53
directional based on pattern of stimulation [3-5], and is abolished following administration of
54
NMDA antagonists [6]. Importantly, there are behavioural effects following NIBS. For example,
55
inhibitory NIBS protocols applied to the motor cortex (M1) can degrade motor control [7], and
56
facilitatory NIBS can increase the rate of learning on a ballistic motor task [8]. Inducing LTP- or
57
LTD-like plasticity in the human motor cortex and modifying behaviour would be of clinical
58
value for a range of neurological conditions. However, at present the effects of various NIBS
59
protocols are highly variable [9-14]. This response variability limits the behavioural and clinical
60
usefulness of NIBS.
SC
M AN U
TE D
61
RI PT
49
Several factors contribute to NIBS response variability including age, time of day, attention,
63
history of physical activity and genetics [15]. Additionally, inter-individual differences in the
64
cortical network activated by NIBS can influence the response. The descending volley evoked by
65
single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) consists of a series of components. The
66
earliest of these probably reflects direct activation of the corticospinal output cells and is
67
known as the “direct (D)-wave”. The later components have been termed “indirect (I)-waves”.
68
The early I-waves likely reflect monosynaptic input to corticospinal neurons from layer II/III
69
interneurons, whereas more complex oligosynaptic circuits generate the late I-waves [16].
70
Individuals in whom TMS is more likely to recruit late I-waves respond more strongly to several
AC C
EP
62
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
forms of NIBS [13,17]. The reason for this is unclear but Hamada and colleagues (2013)
72
suggested that the late I-wave generating circuit might be more sensitive to NIBS than the early
73
I-wave generating circuit. Here, we were interested in examining whether variability in baseline
74
motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitude could serve as an indicator of likely neuroplastic
75
response to a NIBS protocol (continuous theta burst stimulation: cTBS). Our reasoning was as
76
follows: the amplitude of MEPs evoked in individuals in whom TMS was more likely to recruit
77
late I-wave generating circuits would be more variable due to the involvement of more complex
78
networks than in individuals in whom TMS was more likely to recruit less complex early I-wave
79
generating circuitry [18]. To explore mechanisms underpinning MEP variability we used
80
multiple TMS coil orientations to examine I-wave recruitment [13]. In summary, the aims of this
81
study were to (1) investigate the relationship between MEP variability and NIBS (cTBS)
82
response, and (2) explore whether I-wave recruitment profile might influence MEP variability.
83
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
71
Material and Methods
85
Subjects
86
A total of 34 healthy subjects (15 male) aged 18-35 years (mean age, 25.0 ± 4.9 years)
87
participated in two experiments. Potential subjects with contraindications for TMS, including
88
metallic implants, a history of seizures and medications known to alter CNS excitability were
89
excluded [19]. Ethical approval was provided by the University of Adelaide Human Research
90
Ethics Committee, and all participants provided written informed consent in accordance with
91
the Declaration of Helsinki.
92
AC C
EP
84
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Electromyography
94
For both experiments, surface EMG was recorded from the right first dorsal interosseous (FDI)
95
muscle using Ag/AgCl electrodes (Ambu, Ballerup, Denmark) with electrodes positioned in a
96
belly-tendon montage. Signals were sampled at 5 kHz (Cambridge Electronic Design 1401,
97
Cambridge, UK), amplified with a gain of 1000, band-pass filtered (20-1000 Hz) (Cambridge
98
Electronic Design 1902 amplifier, Cambridge, UK) and stored for offline analysis (Signal v4.09,
99
Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK).
M AN U
100
SC
RI PT
93
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
102
Single-pulse TMS was applied with a monophasic waveform using a figure-of-eight coil (external
103
wing diameter 90mm) connected to a Magstim 200 stimulator (Magstim Company, Dyfed, UK).
104
For Experiment 1, the coil was positioned tangentially over the left M1, with the handle rotated
105
posterior-laterally approximately 45° to the sagittal plane to induce a posterior-anterior current
106
flow across the hand M1. The optimal coil position for evoking a MEP in the right FDI muscle at
107
rest was located and marked on the scalp using a water-soluble felt tip marker. Rest motor
108
threshold (RMT) for the right FDI was defined as the minimum stimulus intensity required to
109
evoke an MEP with peak-to-peak amplitude ≥50µV in at least five out of 10 consecutive trials in
110
the relaxed FDI.
EP
AC C
111
TE D
101
112
For Experiment 2, MEPs were evoked using three different directions of current flow across the
113
left M1 hand area. Previous studies have demonstrated that by modifying the direction of
114
current flow it is possible to target specific populations of neurons using single pulse TMS.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Posterior-anterior (PA) currents preferentially recruit early I-waves, anterior-posterior (AP)
116
currents recruit late I-waves and lateral-medial (LM) currents at high stimulus intensities evoke
117
D-waves [18,20-23]. In this experiment we evoked MEPs using three different coil orientations
118
to preferentially induce current flow across the hand M1 to investigate late I-waves, early I-
119
waves and D-waves. PA currents were elicited with the handle of the figure-of-eight coil rotated
120
posterior-laterally, approximately 45° to the sagittal plane. AP currents were elicited by placing
121
the coil 180° to the PA current coil position. LM currents were elicited with the handle rotated
122
laterally to a position 90° to the midsagittal line. Active motor threshold (AMT) was measured
123
for PA, AP and LM currents while stimulating at the hotspot determined by PA currents, as
124
previous studies have determined that direction of the current does not influence the position
125
of the hotspot [22,24]. AMT was defined as the lowest intensity to evoke an MEP of ≥ 200µV in
126
at least five out of 10 consecutive trials whilst maintaining a 5-10% maximal voluntary
127
contraction of the FDI. Muscle contraction was monitored visually using a digital oscilloscope
128
with participants able to monitor and adjust muscle contraction to maintain the required 5-10%
129
MVC.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
130
RI PT
115
Continuous theta burst stimulation
132
In Experiment 1, an air-cooled figure-of-eight coil connected to a Magstim Super Rapid
133
stimulator (Magstim Company, Dyfed, UK) was used to apply cTBS with a biphasic pulse
134
waveform (current direction PA-AP) to the optimal site for stimulating the right FDI. The cTBS
135
protocol consisted of 600 pulses applied in bursts of three pulses at 50Hz, repeated at 5Hz for a
AC C
131
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
136
total of 40 seconds [3]. The intensity of stimulation was set to 70% RMT [25,26], assessed prior
137
to cTBS application using the rTMS coil.
RI PT
138
Experimental Protocol
140
For Experiment 1, subjects attended an afternoon experimental session to determine the
141
relationship between baseline MEP variability and the response to cTBS. Subjects were seated
142
in a comfortable chair with their right upper limb in a relaxed position. At baseline, a total of
143
225 MEPs were evoked over two blocks separated by a short, 2 minute rest interval. Three
144
stimulation intensities were used to examine whether the relationship between MEP variability
145
and cTBS response was influenced by MEP amplitude; the intensities were 120% RMT, 150%
146
RMT and a stimulus intensity set to produce a 1mV MEP (SI1mV). The 120% RMT and SI1mV
147
intensities were selected as they are commonly used to evoke test MEPs prior to plasticity
148
induction protocols [27-29]. The 150% intensity was used to explore the relationship between
149
baseline MEP amplitude variability and plasticity response at larger mean MEP amplitudes. At
150
baseline, a total of 75 TMS pulses at each of the three intensities were delivered randomly with
151
an inter-stimulus interval of 6 sec ± 10%. Following cTBS, 50 TMS pulses at each of the three
152
intensities were delivered randomly (with an inter-stimulus interval of 6 sec ± 10%) from 0-15
153
minutes following cTBS, and again at 20-35 minutes following cTBS; therefore, a total of 300
154
MEPs (100 MEPs for each intensity) were obtained following cTBS (and we grouped these into
155
5-minute blocks: 0-, 5-, 10-, 20-, 25-, 30-min post cTBS). The same stimulation intensities and
156
inter-stimulus intervals were used at baseline and following cTBS.
157
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
139
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Experiment 2 was conducted in the afternoon, >7 days following experiment 1. The purpose of
159
Experiment 2 was to determine if differences between individuals in I-wave recruitment were
160
associated with different levels of MEP variability. Subjects were seated in a comfortable chair
161
with the lateral aspect of the distal phalanx of the right index finger positioned against a force
162
transducer. For both AP and PA coil orientations, 20 MEPs were evoked at 110% AMT with
163
subjects asked to relax their hand every 10 trials to avoid fatigue. For LM coil orientation, 10
164
MEPs were evoked at 150% AMT or 50% of maximum stimulator output (MSO), whichever was
165
greater. Higher stimulus intensities were used for LM currents to increase the likelihood of
166
evoking a D-wave [21]. For all three coil orientations, MEPs were evoked with an inter-stimulus
167
interval of 6 sec ± 10%. MEP onset latency for each trial (for all three coil orientations) was
168
determined automatically with a custom made script to avoid assessor bias (Signal v4.09,
169
Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). In each trial, the onset latency was defined as
170
the time point where the rectified EMG signals exceeded an average plus two standard
171
deviations of the pre-stimulus EMG level 100ms prior to the TMS pulse. AP, PA and LM onset
172
latencies were averaged across trials for each subject. Consistent with the study of Hamada and
173
colleagues (2013), the latency difference between AP and LM evoked MEPs was used as a
174
measure of the relative likelihood of recruiting late I-wave input to corticospinal neurons [13].
176
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
175
RI PT
158
177
Data analysis
178
Normality of data were tested with the Shapiro-Wilk test and where required, logarithmic
179
transformations were performed. Descriptive statistics were used to report experimental
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
variables. To characterise the response to cTBS in Experiment 1, MEP amplitudes were
181
averaged for each stimulus intensity, subject and time point. Trials contaminated with
182
background EMG activity during 100ms prior to the TMS pulse were excluded from the analysis.
183
A 2 x 2 repeated measures ANOVA (factor of ‘TIME’: baseline 1, baseline 2, Post 0 min, Post 20
184
min; factor of ‘INTENSITY’: 120% RMT, 150% RMT, SI1mV) was used to investigate the effect of
185
cTBS on absolute (raw) MEP amplitude. The association between baseline MEP variability
186
(coefficient of variation, CV) and cTBS response (quantified as the grand average of post-cTBS
187
time points normalised to the average baseline) was investigated with Pearson correlations for
188
MEPs evoked at 120% RMT, 150% RMT and SI1mV. To investigate whether MEP amplitude or
189
RMT were associated with baseline MEP variability, we performed Pearson correlations for
190
MEPs evoked at 120% RMT, 150% RMT and SI1mV. To investigate whether the distribution of
191
MEP amplitudes changed following cTBS we analysed the skewness and kurtosis of MEPs
192
evoked at 120% RMT and SI1mV before and after cTBS, split into high and low MEP variability
193
groups (median split), and tested for differences using paired t-tests. For Experiment 2, the
194
association between AP-LM and PA-LM latency differences, and both cTBS response and
195
variability of baseline MEPs was investigated with Pearson correlations for MEPs evoked at
196
120% RMT, 150% RMT and SI1mV. Since it could be argued that the range of MEP latencies
197
evoked by AP currents is due to variation of MEP amplitudes evoked with this coil orientation,
198
we correlated AP MEP amplitude and AP MEP latency. The significance level was set at p ≤ 0.05,
199
and SPSS software was used for all statistical analyses (IBM Corp., Released 2011, IBM SPSS
200
Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0, Armonk, NY, USA).
201
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
180
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Results
203
Experiment 1 – Baseline MEP variability and cTBS response
204
The average RMT was 41.3% MSO (SD 8.3). Baseline neurophysiological measures for each
205
stimulus intensity are reported in Table 1. As expected, there was a main effect of INTENSITY
206
(F(2,66) = 52.3, p < 0.001) with MEPs evoked at 150% RMT larger than those evoked at 120% RMT
207
(95%CI; 1.2-1.8, p < 0.001) and SI1mV (95%CI; 1.2-2.4, p < 0.001). However, there was no main
208
effect of TIME (F(3,99) = 0.86, p = 0.47) or TIME x INTENSITY interaction (F(6,198) = 0.29, p = 0.94)
209
suggesting cTBS did not significantly change MEP amplitude (see figure 1; individual subject
210
cTBS response profiles provided in Supplementary figure 1; tabulated data provided in
211
Supplementary table 1). As there were no significant differences in MEP amplitude across post-
212
cTBS time points, we calculated a grand average cTBS response value for each intensity: MEP
213
amplitudes were normalised to baseline (percentage of the average baseline MEP amplitude),
214
and then averaged across all post-cTBS time-points. There were significant correlations
215
between baseline MEP variability and cTBS response for 120% RMT (r = -0.44, p = 0.01) and
216
SI1mV (r = -0.37, p = 0.03), but not 150 %RMT (p = 0.59) (see figure 2); higher variability in
217
baseline MEP amplitude (at 120%RMT and SI1mV) was associated with a stronger inhibitory
218
response to cTBS. Following correction for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni correction,
219
adjusted level of significance p < 0.017), the association between baseline MEP variability and
220
cTBS response remained significant for 120% RMT. Further investigation of the relationship
221
between baseline MEP variability (120% RMT) and cTBS response revealed that participants
222
with higher baseline MEP variability (median split of baseline MEP variability) had a significant
223
suppression of MEP amplitude following cTBS (post cTBS MEPs 84% of baseline, t(16) = 2.19, p =
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
202
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
0.04). However, participants with low baseline MEP variability did not have a significant
225
facilitation of MEP amplitude following cTBS (post cTBS MEPs 112% of baseline, t(16) = 1.75, p =
226
0.10). Although there were significant associations between the variability and amplitude of
227
MEPs recorded at baseline (120%RMT; r = -0.47, p = 0.01; 150% RMT; r = -0.45, p = 0.01; SI1mV; r
228
= -0.43, p = 0.01), the relationship between baseline MEP variability and cTBS response
229
remained when controlling for baseline MEP amplitude (120%RMT; r = -0.43, p = 0.01; SI1mV; r =
230
-0.35, p = 0.03). As previous, the association between MEP variability and cTBS response
231
remained significant for 120% RMT survived correction for multiple comparisons. There were
232
no significant relationships between RMT and baseline MEP variability for MEPs evoked at all
233
intensities (all p > 0.16). There was no significant difference in distribution of MEP amplitudes
234
from baseline to post cTBS for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT (p = 0.44) or SI1mV (p = 0.42) in both
235
participants with high or low MEP variability (see figure 3).
SC
M AN U
TE D
236
RI PT
224
Experiment 2 – I-wave recruitment and MEP variability
238
Mean AMT, MEP amplitude and onset latencies for PA, AP and LM coil orientations are
239
reported in table 2. The mean AP-LM latency difference was 3.85±1.25 ms and PA-LM latency
240
difference was 1.82±1.12 ms (see figure 4). There was a significant correlation between AP-LM
241
latency difference and grand average cTBS response for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT (r = -0.37, p
242
= 0.03) (figure 4), but not 150% RMT (p = 0.40) or SI1mV (p = 0.27), however the relationship with
243
AP-LM latency difference and cTBS response for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT did not survive
244
correction for multiple comparisons. There were no significant associations between PA-LM
245
latency difference and cTBS response for MEPs evoked at any stimulation intensity (p > 0.54).
AC C
EP
237
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
There were no significant associations between AP-LM or PA-LM latency difference and
247
baseline MEP variability for MEPs evoked at any intensity (all p > 0.33). There was no
248
association between MEP amplitude evoked by AP currents and AP latency (p = 0.83). Since
249
both MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment appear to be independent, but important,
250
factors associated with the cTBS response measured at 120% RMT we investigated the
251
relationship with a multiple regression. The regression model reached significance (R2 = 0.31, p
252
= 0.004), indicating that MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment predict 31% of the variance
253
in cTBS response.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
246
254
Discussion
256
In this study we report significant inter-subject variability in the response to cTBS, which is
257
consistent with recent reports that demonstrate highly variable response profiles following
258
cTBS [13,30]. Indeed, even though care was taken in controlling factors know to influence cTBS
259
response (e.g. pre-activation and time of day) there was no significant group level cTBS
260
response. While progress has been made in understanding the causes of response variability
261
(for review see Ridding and Ziemann [15]), a large component of this variability remains
262
unexplained and this may have contributed to the non-significant group response to cTBS
263
observed in this study. Identifying additional factors contributing to response variability is
264
important to both improve understanding of the physiology underpinning responses to cTBS
265
(and other NIBS protocols) and to direct development and targeting of more robust stimulation
266
protocols. Here, we provide some novel insights into additional factors contributing to cTBS
267
response variability.
AC C
EP
TE D
255
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
268
Associations between I-wave recruitment, MEP variability, and cTBS response
270
By using different coil orientations (PA/AP) it is possible to preferentially recruit early and late I-
271
waves. Using this approach, Hamada and colleagues [13] demonstrated a stronger cTBS
272
response in individuals in whom TMS pulses preferentially recruited late I-waves. The current
273
results replicate the findings of Hamada et al. [13], showing that inter-individual differences in
274
late I-wave recruitment is associated with cTBS response. Hamada and colleagues [13]
275
suggested that the association between I-wave recruitment and cTBS-induced plasticity could
276
be due to a greater sensitivity of the late I-wave generating circuit than the early I-wave
277
generating circuit to cTBS.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
269
278
Given the proposed differential sensitivity of the late and early I-wave generating circuits, we
280
examined the relationship between the likelihood of the TMS pulse recruiting late I-waves (with
281
AP-LM latency difference acting as a marker) and MEP variability; we hypothesised that MEPs
282
evoked in individuals in whom TMS was more likely to recruit late I-wave circuits would be
283
more variable than MEPs in individuals in which TMS was more likely to recruit less complex
284
early I-wave circuitry. However, surprisingly, we found no relationship between I-wave
285
recruitment and MEP variability. This suggests that MEP variability is not highly dependent
286
upon engagement of late I-wave circuits.
AC C
EP
TE D
279
287 288
While there was no significant association between I-wave recruitment and MEP variability,
289
there was a significant association between MEP variability and cTBS response; greater MEP
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
variability at baseline was associated with a stronger inhibitory cTBS response. This relationship
291
was significant for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT and was supported by data recorded at the 1mV
292
intensity where the correlation between variability and response approached statistical
293
significance. One possible explanation for this is that high MEP variability is associated with a
294
wider distribution of MEP amplitudes; for example, in individuals with high variability, TMS
295
might evoke more very large MEPs that are closer to the ceiling of the testable range than in
296
individuals with low variability. This could be important because it has been reported that cTBS
297
is more likely to inhibit near-maximal MEPs [31]. Thus it could be that, on average, individuals
298
with high variability are more likely to respond to cTBS because they have more near-maximal
299
MEPs. If this were the case, then we might expect these large MEPs to be inhibited following
300
cTBS, and hence the distribution of MEP amplitudes would change. However, we show that the
301
distribution of MEPs evoked at 120% RMT and SI1mV intensities did not change following cTBS,
302
irrespective of whether individuals demonstrated high or low baseline variability. There was no
303
relationship between baseline variability in MEPs evoked at an intensity of 150% RMT and cTBS
304
response. We suggest this likely reflects the reduced MEP variability when tested at this high
305
intensity.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
306
RI PT
290
307
While the data does show that high variability is associated with a strong inhibitory response to
308
cTBS it also suggests that low variability is associated with a facilitatory response. To explore
309
this further we examined the response to cTBS by splitting the participant data into two groups,
310
namely high and low variability groups. We were able to show that the higher baseline MEP
311
variability group demonstrated a significant suppression of MEP amplitudes following cTBS. In
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
contrast, the low baseline variability group had a non-significant trend towards a facilitatory
313
response to cTBS. This result suggests that the relationship between variability and the
314
response to cTBS might be complex and not one merely based upon the strength of the
315
response. Indeed, differing levels of MEP variability may be associated with the engagement of
316
different mechanisms (LTD/LTD) by the cTBS stimulus. However, this is highly speculative and
317
we suggest this issue requires further investigation, perhaps, by using a facilitatory (LTP-like)
318
non-invasive brain stimulation protocol. Nevertheless, our results suggest late I-wave
319
recruitment and MEP variability are independently associated with cTBS response. When
320
combined, MEP variability and late I-wave recruitment predicted 31% of the cTBS response
321
variability in the current study. Given the extensive range of factors that contribute to
322
variability in NIBS response [15], these two factors account for a major component of cTBS
323
response variability. However, we acknowledge that this estimate, and that reported for several
324
other factors, is likely to be an overestimation given likely interdependence between multiple
325
factors. Further work is required to investigate the interdependence between factors that have
326
been identified as contributing to NIBS response variability.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
327
RI PT
312
We hypothesised that MEP variability would be greatest in individuals in which late I-waves
329
were more likely to be recruited. However, as described above, we were unable to provide
330
support for this hypothesis. What then is the mechanism underpinning MEP variability?
331
Previous studies have demonstrated that MEP amplitude is influenced by intrinsic oscillatory
332
activities recorded using electroencephalography (EEG). For example, for TMS applied near
333
resting motor threshold, MEPs are more likely to be evoked when preceded by low pre-stimulus
AC C
328
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
alpha power [32]. Similarly, larger MEPs have been observed with lower pre-stimulus beta
335
power [33-37]. Furthermore, oscillatory phase of both alpha and beta activities can influence
336
MEP amplitude [37-39]. Therefore, we suggest intrinsic fluctuations in ongoing oscillatory
337
activity are a likely substrate to explain at least some of the MEP variability. How such changes
338
might explain the variability in cTBS response is unclear.
SC
339
RI PT
334
Similarity to the relationships between movement variability and motor learning
341
Interestingly, there are some parallels between the current results and several recent reports
342
examining motor learning. Greater task related baseline movement variability predicts faster
343
motor learning [40]. Also, Teo and colleagues [8] reported that facilitatory intermittent theta
344
burst stimulation increased movement variability on a subsequent ballistic motor learning task,
345
and that this increase in variability correlated with improved learning. Movement variability
346
allows the individual to explore motor command space and identify optimal motor patterns
347
resulting in greater efficiency during learning. While multiple factors are likely to contribute to
348
variability of movement output, movement execution contributes a large proportion of this
349
variability [41]. The corticospinal system plays a key role in movement execution [42] and
350
variability in movement likely reflects both cortical and spinal influences. Therefore, the MEP
351
variability described here may reflect to some degree the variability seen in movements during
352
learning. While the output measures of behavioural (learning) and neurophysiological studies
353
(cTBS response) are clearly quite different, there might be involvement of a common
354
physiological mechanism, namely activity dependent changes in synaptic strength.
355
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
340
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
When considering these results, it is important to acknowledge some limitations. First, we only
357
studied a population of healthy adults using one common NIBS plasticity-inducing paradigm
358
(cTBS). It is unclear whether these findings are generalizable to wider populations or alternative
359
NIBS paradigms. Second, we have only investigated one potential contributor to MEP
360
variability. It is likely multiple, interacting factors contribute to MEP variability and further
361
studies should seek to provide greater understanding of contributions to this variability. Third,
362
MEP variability is likely due to both cortical and spinal effects [32,43,44]. We did not investigate
363
spinal influences and so the association between MEP variability and the cortically generated
364
cTBS response might be over or under estimated. Finally, although we took care to minimise
365
coil movement during data collection, it is possible that random small coil movements may
366
have contributed to the overall MEP variability. However, we consider it unlikely that this
367
contributed to the reported association between variability and cTBS response and, in fact, may
368
have weakened the relationship by introducing noise. The use of stereotactic navigation
369
techniques may strengthen the findings reported in this study.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
370
RI PT
356
In summary, we provide evidence that MEP variability is an important influence on the cTBS
372
response in healthy adults. Traditionally considered an unwanted characteristic of stochastic
373
nervous system function driven by multiple intrinsic and extrinsic contributions, MEP variability
374
may in fact be an important physiological characteristic to enhance our understanding of
375
cortical network excitability, motor learning and NIBS response. Our results may suggest
376
avenues for developments that improve the reliability of NIBS, for example by employing
AC C
371
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
377
behavioural or external priming procedures to modulate variability in the excitability of the
378
corticospinal system prior to plasticity induction.
RI PT
379
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
380
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
381
Funding: MRG is supported by an NHMRC-ARC Dementia Research Development Fellowship
382
(1102272). AMV is supported by a NHMRC Biomedical Training Fellowship (GNT1088295).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
383
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
384
Table and Figure Legends
385
Table 1: Baseline neurophysiological measures for each stimulus intensity
387
RMT, resting motor threshold; MSO, maximum stimulator output; mV, millivolts; CV, coefficient
388
of variation; MEP, motor evoked potential.
SC
389
RI PT
386
Table 2: AMT, MEP amplitude and onset latency for MEPs evoked using PA, AP and LM coil
391
orientations. AMT, active motor threshold; MEP, motor evoked potential; MSO, maximal
392
stimulator output; PA, posterior-anterior current direction; AP, anterior-posterior current
393
direction; LM, lateral-medial current direction.
M AN U
390
394
Figure 1: Group average cTBS response for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT, 150% RMT, and SI1mV for
396
both raw MEP amplitudes (top) and MEPs normalised to the mean baseline (bottom). CTBS did
397
not affect MEP amplitude for any stimulation intensity.
TE D
395
EP
398
Figure 2: Correlation between MEP variability at baseline for MEPs evoked at A) 120% RMT, B)
400
150% RMT and C) SI1mV and cTBS response averaged across post cTBS time points. Greater MEP
401
variability was associated with a stronger inhibitory response to cTBS for MEPs evoked at 120%
402
RMT and SI1mV intensities.
AC C
399
403 404
Figure 3: Distribution of MEPs evoked at baseline (top) and following cTBS (bottom) for subjects
405
with low variability (left) and high variability (right) for MEPs evoked at SI1mV. MEP amplitude
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
was normalised to the mean baseline amplitude for each subject. There was no change in
407
distribution of MEPs following cTBS suggesting that cTBS does not target specific networks
408
responsible for either large or small MEPs.
RI PT
406
409 410
Figure 4: PA-LM and AP-LM latency differences for A) individual participants and B) presented
412
as a histogram in 0.5ms bins. The AP-LM latency difference was longer than the PA-LM latency
413
difference. The AP-LM latency difference also appeared more variable across participants
414
compared to the PA-LM latency difference. Figure C) presents the correlation between AP-LM
415
latency difference as a measure of late I-wave efficiency and cTBS response averaged across
416
post cTBS time points for MEPs evoked at 120% RMT. A stronger response to cTBS was
417
associated with a greater AP-LM latency difference.
M AN U
TE D EP AC C
418
SC
411
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
419
Tables
420
Table 1
RI PT
421
Stimulus Intensity 150%RMT
SI1mV
Stimulation Intensity (%MSO, mean (SD))
49.7 (9.6)
62.3 (11.8)
50.9 (11.7)
MEP amplitude (mV, mean (SD))
1.48 (0.9)
3.05 (1.5)
1.18 (0.4)
422 423
425
EP
427
Table 2
35.6 (13.3)
66.5 (24.9)
Current Direction
AMT (%MSO)
MEP amplitude (mV)
Latency (ms)
PA
32.7 (7.4)
0.83 (0.41)
22.6 (1.7)
AP
45.1 (10.7)
0.68 (0.38)
24.5 (2.1)
LM
36.1 (8.7)
5.89 (2.41)
20.7 (1.6)
AC C
426
64.4 (20.7)
TE D
424
M AN U
MEP variability (CV, %, mean (SD))
SC
120%RMT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
428
References
429
[1]
430
dentate area of the unanaestetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol
431
1973;232:357-74.
432
[2]
433
the adult and immature hippocampus. J Neurosci 1993;13:2910-8.
434
[3]
435
human motor cortex. Neuron 2005;45:201-6.
436
[4]
437
frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 1997;48:1398-403.
438
[5]
439
repetitive cortical stimulation in man. Exp Brain Res 1998;122:79-84.
440
[6]
441
stimulation is NMDA receptor dependent. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:1028-32.
442
[7]
443
cortex degrades selective muscle activation in the ipsilateral arm. J Neurophysiol
444
2010;104:2594-602.
445
[8]
446
stimulation enhances subsequent motor learning and increases performance variability. Cereb
447
Cortex 2011;21:1627-38.
RI PT
Bliss TV, Gardner-Medwin AR. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the
SC
Dudek SM, Bear MF. Bidirectional long-term modification of synaptic effectiveness in
M AN U
Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC. Theta burst stimulation of the
Chen R, Classen J, Gerloff C et al. Depression of motor cortex excitability by low-
TE D
Berardelli A, Inghilleri M, Rothwell JC et al. Facilitation of muscle evoked responses after
Huang Y-Z, Chen R-S, Rothwell JC, Wen H-Y. The after-effect of human theta burst
AC C
EP
Bradnam LV, Stinear CM, Byblow WD. Theta burst stimulation of human primary motor
Teo JTH, Swayne OBC, Cheeran B, Greenwood RJ, Rothwell JC. Human theta burst
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
[9]
López-Alonso V, Fernández-del-Olmo M, Costantini A, Gonzalez-Henriquez JJ, Cheeran B.
449
Intra-individual variability in the response to anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin
450
Neurophysiol 2015;126:2342-7.
451
[10]
452
direct current stimulation (tDCS) at varying current intensities. Brain Stimul 2015;8:1130-7.
453
[11]
454
dependency of motor cortical plasticity induced by paired associative stimulation. Exp Brain Res
455
2008;187:467-75.
456
[12]
457
intermittent theta burst stimulation: Implications for rehabilitation and recovery. Brain Stimul
458
2014;7:365-71.
459
[13]
460
networks in driving human motor cortical plasticity. Cereb Cortex 2013;23:1593-605.
461
[14]
462
invasive brain stimulation protocols and motor learning in healthy adults. Neurosci Lett
463
2013;549:151-6.
464
[15]
465
invasive brain stimulation in healthy subjects. J Physiol (Lond) 2010;588:2291-304.
466
[16]
467
2012;5:512-25.
468
[17]
469
stimulation of the motor cortex. Brain Stimul 2014;7:468-75.
RI PT
448
Chew T, Ho KA, Loo CK. Inter- and intra-individual variability in response to transcranial
M AN U
SC
Müller-Dahlhaus JF, Orekhov Y, Liu Y, Ziemann U. Interindividual variability and age-
Hinder MR, Goss EL, Fujiyama H et al. Inter- and intra-individual variability following
TE D
Hamada M, Murase N, Hasan A, Balaratnam M, Rothwell JC. The role of interneuron
EP
Vallence AM, Kurylowicz L, Ridding MC. A comparison of neuroplastic responses to non-
AC C
Ridding MC, Ziemann U. Determinants of the induction of cortical plasticity by non-
Di Lazzaro V, Profice P, Ranieri F et al. I-wave origin and modulation. Brain Stimul
Wiethoff S, Hamada M, Rothwell JC. Variability in response to transcranial direct current
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
[18]
Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Saturno E et al. The effect on corticospinal volleys of reversing
471
the direction of current induced in the motor cortex by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp
472
Brain Res 2001;138:268-73.
473
[19]
474
update. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;122:1686.
475
[20]
476
human motor cortex: surface EMG and single motor unit responses. J Physiol (Lond)
477
1989;412:449-73.
478
[21]
479
latency of surface EMG and single motor unit responses in the first dorsal interosseous muscle.
480
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;93:138-46.
481
[22]
482
activation of different I waves by transcranial magnetic stimulation with a figure-of-eight-
483
shaped coil. Exp Brain Res 1997;113:24-32.
484
[23]
485
volleys evoked by transcranial stimulation in conscious humans. J Physiol (Lond) 1998;508:625-
486
33.
487
[24]
488
train, monophasic and biphasic repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the
489
human motor cortex. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:605-13.
RI PT
470
Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A. Screening questionnaire before TMS: An
M AN U
SC
Day BL, Dressler D, Maertens de Noordhout A et al. Electric and magnetic stimulation of
Werhahn KJ, Fong JKY, Meyer BU et al. The effect of magnetic coil orientation on the
TE D
Sakai K, Ugawa Y, Terao Y, Hanajima R, Furubayashi T, Kanazawa I. Preferential
AC C
EP
Di Lazzaro V, Restuccia D, Oliviero A et al. Effects of voluntary contraction on descending
Arai N, Okabe S, Furubayashi T, Terao Y, Yuasa K, Ugawa Y. Comparison between short
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
[25]
Gentner R, Wankerl K, Reinsberger C, Zeller D, Classen J. Depression of human
491
corticospinal excitability induced by magnetic theta-burst stimulation: evidence of rapid
492
polarity-reversing metaplasticity. Cereb Cortex 2008;18:2046-53.
493
[26]
494
LTD-like plasticity in human motor cortex: A matter of preceding motor activation. Brain Stimul
495
2014;7:864-70.
496
[27]
497
estimating 50% of the maximal motor evoked potential. Clin Neurophysiol 2015;126:2337-41.
498
[28]
499
within- and between-session reliability of TMS measures of corticospinal excitability.
500
Neuroscience 2016;320:205-9.
501
[29]
502
stroke. Cereb Cortex 2010;20:1523-8.
503
[30]
504
protocols induces long-lasting neuroplastic changes in the human motor cortex. Eur J Neurosci
505
2012;35:125-34.
506
[31]
507
changes in corticospinal excitability following theta burst stimulation of the human primary
508
motor cortex. Clin Neurophysiol 2016;127:740-7.
509
[32]
510
activity determines cortical excitability in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:284-8.
RI PT
490
SC
Goldsworthy MR, Müller-Dahlhaus F, Ridding MC, Ziemann U. Inter-subject variability of
M AN U
Pitcher JB, Doeltgen SH, Goldsworthy MR et al. A comparison of two methods for
Goldsworthy MR, Hordacre B, Ridding MC. Minimum number of trials required for
TE D
Di Lazzaro V, Profice P, Pilato F et al. Motor cortex plasticity predicts recovery in acute
EP
Goldsworthy MR, Pitcher JB, Ridding MC. The application of spaced theta burst
AC C
Goldsworthy MR, Vallence AM, Hodyl NA, Semmler JG, Pitcher JB, Ridding MC. Probing
Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Gerloff C, Hummel FC. Spontaneous locally restricted EEG alpha
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
[33]
Lepage JF, Saint-Amour D, Théoret H. EEG and neuronavigated single-pulse TMS in the
512
study of the observation/execution matching system: Are both techniques measuring the same
513
process? J Neurosci Methods 2008;175:17-24.
514
[34]
515
motor system excitability in overlapping neuronal populations. Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:492-
516
501.
517
[35]
518
influence of pre-TMS oscillations and corticomuscular coherence on motor-evoked potentials.
519
Cereb Cortex 2014;24:1708-19.
520
[36]
521
potential amplitude. Clin EEG Neurosci 2006;37:247-51.
522
[37]
523
and corticospinal synchronization influence TMS-evoked motor potentials. J Neurophysiol
524
2014;111:513-9.
525
[38]
526
beta-band synchronization entails rhythmic gain modulation. J Neurosci 2010;30:4481-8.
527
[39]
528
markers of corticospinal excitability in the resting brain. BioMed Res Int 2014;2014.
529
[40]
530
variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability. Nat Neurosci
531
2014;17:312-21.
RI PT
511
SC
Mäki H, Ilmoniemi RJ. EEG oscillations and magnetically evoked motor potentials reflect
M AN U
Schulz H, Übelacker T, Keil J, Müller N, Weisz N. Now i am ready - Now i am not: The
Zarkowski P, Shin CJ, Dang T, Russo J, Avery D. EEG and the variance of motor evoked
TE D
Keil J, Timm J, Sanmiguel I, Schulz H, Obleser J, Schonwiesner M. Cortical brain states
EP
Van Elswijk G, Maij F, Schoffelen JM, Overeem S, Stegeman DF, Fries P. Corticospinal
AC C
Berger B, Minarik T, Liuzzi G, Hummel FC, Sauseng P. EEG oscillatory phase-dependent
Wu HG, Miyamoto YR, Castro LNG, Olveczky BP, Smith MA. Temporal structure of motor
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
532
[41]
Van Beers RJ, Haggard P, Wolpert DM. The role of execution noise in movement
533
variability. J Neurophysiol 2004;91:1050-63.
534
[42]
535
2000;23:393-415.
536
[43]
537
reveals rapid shifts in motor cortical excitability during the human sleep slow oscillation. J
538
Neurosci 2012;32:243-53.
539
[44]
540
stimulation: Report of an IFCN committee. Clin Neurophysiol 2012;123:858-82.
RI PT
Sanes JN, Donoghue JP. Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci
SC
Bergmann TO, Molle M, Schmidt MA et al. EEG-guided transcranial magnetic stimulation
M AN U
Groppa S, Oliviero A, Eisen A et al. A practical guide to diagnostic transcranial magnetic
541
AC C
EP
TE D
542
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Highlights
RI PT
SC
M AN U
•
TE D
•
EP
• •
Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) offers significant opportunities to examine plasticity in the human brain. There is some evidence that NIBS might be useful for inducing functionally beneficial change in a variety of neurological/psychiatric conditions. However, at present the utility is NIBS is somewhat limited due to high response variability. Key to harnessing the potential of NIBS is to understand more fully the factors contributing to this variability. This variability might, at least in part, be determined by the relative excitability of different cortical networks activated by NIBS. This study provides new insights into the influence of cortical excitability on the response to a commonly employed NIBS protocol and may open up avenues to improve response reliability.
AC C
•