Accepted Manuscript A bio-cultural approach to the study of food choice: The contribution of taste genetics, population and culture Davide S. Risso, Cristina Giuliani, Marco Antinucci, Gabriella Morini, Paolo Garagnani, Sergio Tofanelli, Donata Luiselli PII:
S0195-6663(16)30516-5
DOI:
10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.046
Reference:
APPET 3407
To appear in:
Appetite
Received Date: 4 October 2016 Revised Date:
23 March 2017
Accepted Date: 30 March 2017
Please cite this article as: Risso D.S., Giuliani C., Antinucci M., Morini G., Garagnani P., Tofanelli S. & Luiselli D., A bio-cultural approach to the study of food choice: The contribution of taste genetics, population and culture, Appetite (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.046. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
A bio-cultural approach to the study of food choice: the contribution of taste genetics,
2
population and culture
3
Davide S. Rissoa-b*, Cristina Giulianib*, Marco Antinuccic, Gabriella Morinid, Paolo Garagnanie,
5
Sergio Tofanellic^, Donata Luisellib^
RI PT
4
6 7
a
8
Washington 98195, USA
9
b
SC
Department of Genome Sciences, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle,
Department of Biological, Geological and Environmental Sciences (BiGeA), Laboratory of
Molecular Anthropology and Centre for Genome Biology, University of Bologna, via Selmi 3,
11
40126 Bologna, Italy
12
c
Department of Biology, University of Pisa, Via Ghini 13, 56126 Pisa, Italy
13
d
University of Gastronomic Sciences, Piazza Vittorio Emanuele 9, Bra, Pollenzo 12042, CN, Italy
14
e
Department of Experimental, Diagnostic and Specialty Medicine (DIMES), University of
15
Bologna, Via San Giacomo 12, 40126 Bologna, Italy
M AN U
10
TE D
16
*, ^ These authors contributed equally
18
* Corresponding authors:
[email protected],
[email protected]
20 21 22 23 24 25 26
AC C
19
EP
17
27 28 29
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
30 Abstract
32
The study of food choice, one of the most complex human traits, requires an integrated approach that
33
takes into account environmental, socio-cultural and biological diversity. We recruited 183 volunteers
34
from four geo-linguistic groups and highly diversified in terms of both genetic background and food
35
habits from whom we collected genotypes and phenotypes tightly linked to taste perception. We
36
confirmed previous genetic associations, in particular with stevioside perception, and noted significant
37
differences in food consumption: in particular, broccoli, mustard and beer consumption scores were
38
significantly higher (Adjusted P=0.02, Adjusted P<0.0001 and Adjusted P=0.01, respectively) in North
39
Europeans, when compared to the other groups. Licorice and Parmesan cheese showed lower
40
consumption and liking scores in the Sri Lankan group (Adjusted P=0.001 and Adjusted P<0.001,
41
respectively). We also highlighted how rs860170 (TAS2R16) strongly differentiated populations and was
42
associated to salicin bitterness perception. Identifying genetic variants on chemosensory receptors that
43
vary across populations and show associations with taste perception and food habits represents a step
44
towards a better comprehension of this complex trait, aimed at improving the individual health status.
45
This is the first study that concurrently explores the contribution of genetics, population diversity and
46
cultural aspects in taste perception and food consumption.
47 48 49
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
31
Keywords
51
Food choice, Nutrition, Population Diversity, Taste, Genetics
53 54 55 56 57
AC C
52
EP
50
58 59 60 61 2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1. Introduction
63
Food choice is a highly complex human trait whose study requires an integrated multidisciplinary
64
approach (Anon 2000), which takes into account environmental, socio-cultural and biological factors
65
(Rozin 1982; Turner et al. 2013; Armelagos 2014; Pieroni et al. 2016). Food selection allows humans to
66
fulfill the vital function of nutrition, constituting the deepest connection with the environment, and
67
being a relevant factor to define human communities. During the course of human evolution the sense
68
of taste, together with the other chemical senses smell and chemesthesis, has played a fundamental
69
role in food choice, by ensuring an efficient discrimination between edible sources of nutrients and
70
potentially toxic substances (Glendinning 1994; Breslin 2013). Plant secondary metabolites trigger
71
environmental adaptation, providing defense mechanisms against pathogens and making plants
72
unpalatable to predators. Phenols, flavonoids, terpenes, alkaloids and glucosinolates are in fact bitter or
73
irritant plant compounds: for this reason, variants at genes encoding taste receptors have undergone
74
adaptive changes in relation to eating habits (Fischer et al. 2005; Campbell et al. 2012; Risso et al. 2014a;
75
Perry et al. 2015). Most phytochemicals also exhibit a wide array of biological properties (Kabera et al.
76
2014; Wink 2015) that humans have learnt to exploit for their therapeutic effects (Petrovska 2012). This
77
has added interest to the study of the genetic bases of taste preferences and health status in relation to
78
the introduction of phytochemicals in the diet (Tepper 1998; Dinehart et al. 2006; des Gachons et al.
79
2009; Mennella 2014; Mennella et al. 2015). TAS2R genes, which codify for bitter taster receptors, are
80
also expressed in the gastrointestinal tract and in other extra-oral tissues such as gut, lungs and testis
81
(Finger and Kinnamon 2011), where they modulate systemic functions of tastants either endogenously
82
produced or contained in food (Santa-Cruz Calvo et al. 2015; Clark et al. 2015; Shaik et al. 2016). The
83
most studied taste-related gene is TAS2R38, which encodes the bitter taste receptor mediating the
84
ability to be a “taster” (PAV haplotype) or a “non-taster” (AVI haplotype) for different bitter compounds,
85
including the well-known phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) and 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) (Kim et al. 2003,
86
Boxer and Garneau 2015). Correlations have been identified between PROP/PTC taster status and
87
dietary intake (Turnbull and Matisoo-Smith 2002; Tepper 2008; Feeney et al. 2011). This, in addition to
88
the particular worldwide distribution of TAS2R38 haplotypes, has suggested that natural selection may
89
have been acted on this gene, since the earlier stages of human evolution (Wooding et al. 2004;
90
Campbell et al. 2012; Risso et al. 2016).
91
Nonetheless, correlations between genetic variations and food preferences have been recognized hard
92
to detect, since each food is constituted by several different chemical compounds that may be able to
93
activate different receptors (Meyerhof et al. 2010; Roudnitzky et al. 2011, 2015). In addition, food
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
62
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
choice is the result of the integration with other sensory inputs in the brain, and it is further complicated
95
by different cultural backgrounds such as learning, memory and emotion (Bertino et al. 1983; Shepherd
96
2012; Williams et al. 2016). For these reasons, food choice and habits have been recently explored
97
under a different perspective, where the co-variation of taste phenotypes and genotypes is addressed
98
within the context of population diversity in eating behaviors (Kumanyika 2008; Robino et al. 2014;
99
Pirastu et al. 2015). Under this approach, inter-populations variability is taken into account when
RI PT
94
performing genotype-phenotype association studies investigating the relationships between SNPs,
101
structural variations, and taste perception (Campbell et al. 2012, 2014; Allen et al. 2013; Roudnitzky et
102
al. 2015, 2016).
103
In this explorative study we explored an integrated approach and recruited a total number of 183
104
volunteers belonging to four different geographical regions and highly diversified in terms of both
105
genetic background and food habits: Italy, North Europe, Maghreb and Sri Lanka. After collecting bitter,
106
sweet and umami taste-phenotypes using both natural and synthetic compounds, a food habits
107
questionnaire on common bitter, sweet and umami-tasting foods was presented to the participants. A
108
panel of 37 SNPs located in 14 genes involved in taste perception was also analyzed in order to explore
109
potential associations between genetic variants, food habits, taste perception and population
110
differences.
111
TE D
M AN U
SC
100
2. Materials and Methods
113
2.1. Studied population
114
An overall number of 183 individuals were recruited and enrolled in the study. Subjects (81 females and
115
102 males with an average age of 42.71 ± 15.89) did not report any food allergies, were not following
116
any prescribed diet or using drugs that might interfere with taste perception. Most of the participants
117
(N=111) were Italians, with the remaining subjects coming from the Maghreb region (N=18), Sri Lanka
118
(N=26) and Northern Europe (N=28) but recruited in Italy. Detailed information of samples included in
119
the study is shown in Table 1. A written informed consent was obtained from all the volunteers and all
120
the experimental protocols were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Ethics Committee of
121
the University of Bologna and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000.
AC C
EP
112
122 123 4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
124 125 Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants in the four analyzed populations.
127 128
B.M.I. Body mass index.
Population
N (males/females)
Mean age (SD)
Italy
111 (55/56)
47.64 (16.06)
Maghreb
18 (18/0)
38.56 (13.80)
N. Europe
28 (6/22)
21.50 (6.35)
21.50 (2.93)
Sri Lanka
26 (23/3)
38.59 (13.12)
25.43 (2.34)
All
183 (102/81)
42.71 (15.89)
24.14 (3.47)
131 132 133
24.27 (3.52)
25.48 (3.31)
SC
130
Mean B.M.I. (SD)
M AN U
129
RI PT
126
134 2.2 DNA collection
136
Saliva samples were collected from participants using Oragene saliva collection kits (Genotek Inc.
137
Kanata, Ontario, Canada). DNA was extracted according to the manufacturer’s protocol and checked for
138
quantity/quality by spectrophotometric analyses (GeneQuantTMRNA/DNA Calculator, Amersham
139
Biosciences, UK). We genotyped 37 SNPs selected from a panel of 14 taste-related genes (Table 2) using
140
the SequenomMassARRAY technology (Sequenom, San Diego, CA, USA).
141
PCR fragments were analyzed by MALDI-TOF mass spectrometry (Gabriel et al. 2009) and spectrograms
142
were checked individually, in order to evaluate the presence of calling errors. As additional quality
143
control, we checked that no significant deviations from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p-value<0.001)
144
were present in the analyzed SNPs and that their call rate was >0.95. Primer sequences are shown in
145
Supplementary Table 1.
147 148
EP
AC C
146
TE D
135
149 150
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
151 152 Table 2. List of the selected genes and single nucleotide polymorphisms.
Gene
Product function
SNPs
Chromosome
TAS2R1
Bitter receptor
rs2234233
5
Bitter receptor
rs2234001
7
Bitter receptor
rs11610105, rs3741843, rs7138535
12
TAS2R16
Bitter receptor
rs860170
7
TAS2R38
Bitter receptor
rs10246939, rs1726866, rs713598
7
TAS2R50
Bitter receptor
rs10772397, rs1376251
CA6
Carbonate dehydratase
rs2274333
GNAT3
Bitter, sweet and umami taste transduction
rs6467192, rs6467217, rs1524600, rs1107657, rs940541
TRPM5
Taste transduction
141478574
11088981, 11091432, 11091693 122635024
141672604, 141672705,141673345 11138683, 11138852
1
9017204
7
80107798, 80138178, 80138303, 80150018, 80150594
M AN U
12
rs2301696, rs800344, rs800345, rs2301698
2426984, 2429653, 2429733, 2437425, 2444094
11
rs2301699, rs800347, rs800348, rs2074234, rs886277 rs7534618, rs4920564, rs35874116, Sweet receptor
TAS1R2
9629529
SC
TAS2R4 TAS2R14
Positions (GRCh37)
RI PT
153
2430597, 2430865, 2432964, 2439767 19170078, 19179104, 19181393
1
rs9701796, rs3935570, rs4073538, rs4920566 Sweet and Umami receptor
rs35424002
GLUT2
Glucose transporter
rs5400
TAS1R1
Umami receptor
rs34160967
mGluR1
Glutamate receptor
TE D
TAS1R3
rs6923492
154 155
19186129, 19167371, 19172341, 19179824
1
1269986
3
170732300
1
6635306
6
146755324
2.3 Food habits questionnaire
157
Participants completed a questionnaire on individual liking and consumption about 12 common foods:
158
seven bitter-tasting (“Broccoli”, “Radicchio”, “Artichoke”, “Arugula”, “Licorice”, “Mustard” and “Beer”),
159
three sweet-tasting (“Date”, “Honey” and “Torrone”) and two umami-tasting foods (“Parmesan cheese”
160
and “Bouillon cube”). Subjects were asked to rate each item on a 3-point liking scale: “Dislike” (score 1),
161
“Sort of” (score 2) and “Like extremely” (score 3). In addition, these items were rated on a 3-points
162
consumption scale: “Yearly” (score 1), “Monthly” (score 2) and “Weekly” (score 3). A “Never tasted”
163
(score 0) option was also included. The liking and consumption scores were then pooled together to
164
have a single 1-to-6 liking + consumption score. Supplementary Table 2 summarizes mean and standard
165
deviation for each item score. Lastly, weight (in kg) and height (in m) were collected in order to calculate
166
the body mass index (BMI).
167
2.4. Assessment of taste phenotypes
AC C
EP
156
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Taste perception phenotypes were collected for the three examined taste qualities (e.g. umami, sweet
169
and bitter). Volunteers were asked to refrain from eating and drinking for at least three hours before the
170
beginning of the session and to rinse their mouth with room temperature deionized water before each
171
tasting step. Subjects were informed that they may receive stimuli eliciting more than one taste quality.
172
They were asked to hold the presented solutions in their mouth for ten seconds and subsequently
173
choose among different tastes such as bitter, sweet, umami and/or tasteless, as well as to rate their
174
perceived intensity separately on multiple Labeled Magnitude Scales (LMSs, Green et al. 1996), one for
175
each taste quality.
176
For the umami taste phenotypes, two cups containing different concentrations (3.13 mM and 12.5 mM,
177
respectively) of monosodium glutamate dissolved in water (MSG, Sigma Aldrich S.r.l.) were presented to
178
the subjects to estimate individual’s MSG thresholds. According to the recognized solution, they were
179
classified in MSG non-sensitives (no recognition of umami taste), medium-sensitives (recognition of the
180
umami taste only in the second tasting) and sensitives (recognition of umami taste in both tastings).
181
Regarding sweet taste, volunteers were asked to give their preference for a series of three cups
182
containing different sucrose (Sigma Aldrich S.r.l) water solutions (200, 400 and 800 mM, respectively) on
183
a seven-point category scale ranging from “Dislike extremely” to “Like extremely” (Supplementary
184
Figure 1). Subjects were then classified in sucrose non-likers, medium-likers and likers, after analysis of
185
the “liking-curve” of different sucrose tastings (Lundgren et al. 1978).
186
Lastly, bitter taste-related phenotypes were collected by quantifying the ability of tasting three different
187
bitter compounds (salicin, PROP and stevioside) using the LMS. A single water solution of stevioside 1.26
188
mM (Nastevia, Stevia Italia s.r.l. Italy) was presented to the subjects. The PROP-taster status was
189
assessed using cotton swabs dipped in 50 mM 6-n-propylthiouracil solution (Sigma Aldrich S.r.l.)
190
modifying the protocol for filter paper discs described in Zhao et al. (2003) as previously shown (Risso et
191
al. 2014b). Cotton swabs dipped in supra-threshold solutions of salicin 15mM (Sigma Aldrich S.r.l) were
192
also given to the participants. LMS scores were then used as quantitative variables for logistic and linear
193
regression analyses.
194
2.5. Genetic and Statistical analyses
195
Arlequin v.3.5 (Excoffier et al. 2010) was used to compute summary statistics such as FST values,
196
estimated heterozygosity (EH), gene diversity, number of polymorphic loci and the analysis of molecular
197
variance (AMOVA). FST values and MAFs were then compared to the ones calculated in population-
198
matching groups of the 1000 Genomes Project (1KGenomes) (1000 Genomes Project Consortium et al.
199
2015) and the Human Genome Diversity Project (HGDP) (Cann et al. 2002). Distributions of food habit
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
168
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
scores and differences in taste perception were evaluated though the analysis of variance (ANOVA)
201
using R statistical analysis software (http://www.R-project.org/). Tukey’s post-hoc tests were run to
202
evaluate differences between means. Since the number of subjects in the Italian group was 4 to 6 times
203
bigger than the other groups, all the statistical analyses were replicated on a subsample of 30 individuals
204
obtained by 1000 bootstrap resampling. Due to the size of the other population samples, we were
205
unable to replicate the genotype-phenotype associations independently in each groups. For this reason,
206
we explored genotype-phenotype associations and interactions between genotypes and food habits
207
scores through logistic regressions in PLINK v.1.07 (Purcell et al. 2007), using sex, age, BMI and ethnicity
208
as covariates in the entire group (N=183). When multiple tests were performed under the same null
209
hypothesis, the statistical significance was also adjusted according to the Bonferroni correction (i.e.
210
Adjusted P= nominal P-value x number of individual tests).
211
3. Results
212
3.1. Genetic variability
213
The analyzed SNPs showed similar diversity patterns across the different groups: the overall pairwise FST
214
values were indeed low (average value of 0.024 with a standard deviation of 0.01), but significant in all
215
the comparisons (Supplementary Table 3 A-B). Average gene diversity (+/- SD), number of polymorphic
216
sites and estimated heterozygosity were indeed similar across different populations (Supplementary
217
Table 4). This means that most of the variance is between individuals within populations (98%) rather
218
than among populations (2%). As shown in Figure 1, the distribution of the calculated FST values
219
highlights the overall low level of differentiation of these SNPs across the studied populations.
220
However, three SNPs, namely rs1376251 (TAS2R50), rs800345 (TRPM5) and rs860170 (TAS2R16),
221
showed modest to intermediate differentiation in our sample, falling in the last 5th percentile of the FST
222
distribution. Table 3 shows the the observed minor allele frequencies at these SNPs and those calculated
223
from public datasets are comparable with TAS2R50 rs1376251 C, TRPM5 rs800345 T and TAS2R16
224
rs860170 G alleles found to be more frequent in populations of Asian ancestry. In the same way, the
225
calculated pairwise FST values better highlighted the different distribution of the analyzed SNPs among
226
our population groups (Supplementary Figure 2).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
200
227 228 229
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
230 Figure 1. Distribution of the analyzed SNPs according to pairwise FST values (X-axis) and Minor Allele
232
Frequencies (MAFs). Arrows indicate the SNPs showing the highest FST values, falling in the upper 5th
233
percentile of the distribution (dashed horizontal line).
RI PT
231
234 235
SC
236 237
M AN U
238 239 240 241 242
Table 3. Allele frequencies of the highest differentiated SNPs with the highest FST values in our groups
244
(shown in italics) and population-matched groups from the 1000 Genomes project Phase III
245
(1KGenomes) and Human Genome Diversity Project (HGDP).
246
MZB, Mozabite from Algeria (HGDP); EUR, Europeans (1KGenomes); TSI, Tuscans from Italy
247
(1KGenomes); STU, Sri Lankan Tamil from the UK (1K Genomes).
EP
TE D
243
rs860170 (TAS2R16)
rs800345 (TRPM5)
rs1376251 (TAS2R50)
A
G
C
T
C
T
AC C
Population MZB
0.82
0.18
0.87
0.13
0.50
0.50
Maghreb
0.86
0.14
0.83
0.17
0.50
0.50
EUR
0.69
0.31
0.92
0.08
0.64
0.36
N. Europe
0.67
0.33
0.89
0.11
0.43
0.57
TSI
0.69
0.31
0.93
0.07
0.58
0.42
Italy
0.67
0.33
0.92
0.08
0.59
0.41
STU
0.63
0.37
0.70
0.30
0.33
0.67
Sri-Lanka
0.59
0.41
0.65
0.35
0.77
0.23
248 249 9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
250 3.2. Genotype-phenotype association
252
Three statistically significant associations were found between SNPs and food habit ratings. TAS2R38
253
rs10246939, rs1726866 and rs713598 were found to be associated with Broccoli score (Adjusted P-
254
values=0.02, 0.03 and 0.04 respectively) in all the studied populations. In particular, the taster haplotype
255
(PAV) was inversely correlated to the reported Broccoli score (R=-0.71, P<0.05).
256
PROP perceived bitterness was, as expected, associated with the TAS2R38 PAV taster haplotype in all
257
the studied populations (Adjusted P=1.05E-15). The CCG genotype (PAV haplotype) was in fact more
258
frequent in individuals who rated higher scores of PROP bitter perception (Supplementary Figure 3A).
259
The perceived bitterness of stevioside was positively associated with TAS2R4 rs2234001 G allele. When
260
separating individuals who could perceive stevioside bitterness (N=135) from individuals who couldn’t
261
(N=48), rs2234001 G allele was in fact more abundant in the former (55% vs 34%; OR=2.69, Adjusted P=
262
1.24E-03). In the same way, TAS2R4 rs2234001 CC homozygotes (N= 48) showed lower average levels
263
(30.83 +/- 4.74) of perceived stevioside bitterness when compared to GG homozygotes (49.10 +/- 4.16)
264
(Adjusted P-value= 4.95E-03; Supplementary Figure 3B). The perception of salicin bitterness was
265
associated with TAS2R16 rs860170 A allele (Adjusted P=0.01). At the genotypic level, a notable trend can
266
be indeed observed, with average salicin bitterness levels decreasing from 55.15 +/- 25.45 (AA
267
genotype) to 39.37 +/- 24.07 (GG genotype) (Adjusted P=0.01; Supplementary Figure 3C). Lastly, we
268
failed to detect any evidence of correlation between sweet and umami taste-related gene
269
polymorphisms and the perception of sucrose and umami solutions (all P’s>0.05) as well as between
270
CA6 rs2274333 and perceived PROP bitterness (P>0.05). A summary of the results is reported in
271
Supplementary Figure 4.
272
3.3 Population-based food preferences
273
Most of the liking/consumption scores showed a similar distribution across populations. However,
274
significant different food habit scores were found. Table 4 recapitulates these findings, showing how the
275
score of three bitter foods (broccoli, mustard and beer) was significantly higher in North Europeans
276
(5.18 +/- 1.04, 4.79 +/- 1.66 and 4.54 +/- 1.59, respectively) when compared to Maghrebis (4.50 +/- 1.46,
277
2.33 +/- 1.72 and 3.17 +/- 2.31, respectively), Lankans (4.31 +/- 1.52, 2.58 +/- 1.83 and 2.65 +/- 1.85,
278
respectively) and Italians (4.08 +/- 1.40, 2.85 +/- 1.65 and 4.11 +/- 1.68, respectively) (Adjusted P=0.02,
279
Adjusted P=7.40E-06 and Adjusted P=0.01, respectively). In addition, one bitter item (licorice) showed
280
lower ratings in the Sri Lankan group (1.77 +/- 1.30) with respect to Maghrebis (3.33 +/- 2.16), North
281
Europeans (3.89 +/- 1.71) and Italians (3.44 +/- 1.47) (Adjusted P=0.001). Parmesan cheese showed
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
251
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
lower scores in Lankans (3.91 +/- 1.77) and Maghrebis (3.72 +/- 1.81), with respect to Italians (5.24 +/-
283
1.19) and North Europeans (5.25 +/- 0.80) (Adjusted P=3.00E-03). No significant correlations (All P-
284
values > 0.05) between BMI and food preferences were observed (Supplementary Figure 5).
285
Table 4. Liking/consumption scores showing significant differences across population samples.
RI PT
282
All (SD)
Italians (SD)
Maghreb (SD)
North-Europe (SD)
Sri Lanka (SD)
Adjusted P-value
Broccoli
4.32 (1.41)
4.08 (1.40)
4.5 (1.46)
5.18 (1.04)
4.31 (1.52)
0.02
Beer
3.87 (1.84)
4.11 (1.68)
3.17 (2.31)
4.54 (1.59)
2.65 (1.85)
0.01
Licorice
3.26 (1.68)
3.44 (1.47)
3.33 (2.16)
3.89 (1.71)
1.77 (1.30)
0.001
Mustard
3.06 (1.79)
2.85 (1.65)
2.33 (1.72)
4.79 (1.66)
2.58 (1.83)
7.40E-06
Parmesan cheese
4.92 (1.42)
5.24 (1.19)
3.72 (1.81)
5.25 (0.80)
3.91 (1.77)
3.00E-03
M AN U
286
SC
Food
3.4. Population-based taste phenotypes
288
The distribution of MSG perception varied across different groups. Maghrebis and Lankans, in fact,
289
showed a lower percentage of MSG sensitives (6% and 8%, respectively) when compared to Italians and
290
North Europeans (44% and 64%, respectively; P=1.72E-08). Conversely, a higher percentage of MSG non-
291
sensitives was noted in Maghrebis and Lankans (39% and 46%, respectively), in comparison to Italians
292
and North Europeans (13% and 7%, respectively; P=1.72E-08) (Figure 2A).
293
Regarding sucrose preference, we found more non-likers in the North European group (71%), in
294
comparison to Maghrebis (44%), Italians (36%), and Lankans (27%) (P=0.01; Figure 2B).
295
Bitter taste perception varied across populations as well: the perceived bitterness of stevioside was in
296
fact higher in the North European group (60.54 +/- 22.11), with lower levels in Lankans (39.62 +/- 21.53),
297
Italians (38.60 +/- 25.22) and Maghrebis (14.67 +/- 20.67) (P=0.001). However, the perceived levels of
298
salicin and PROP bitterness did not vary across different groups (P=0.61 and P=0.38, respectively).
300 301
EP
AC C
299
TE D
287
302 303
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
304
Figure 2. Monosodium-Glutamate perception (A) and sucrose preference (B) in the examined
305
populations. Standard deviation values in the Italian group are based on 1000 bootstrap replicates of 30
306
individuals. * indicates statistical significance.
RI PT
307 308 309
SC
310 311
M AN U
312 313 314 315
317 318 319
EP
TE D
316
4. Discussion
321
In the present study, we propose an integrated approach where the variance of genotypes and
322
phenotypes of human taste chemoreception is partitioned into geo-linguistic groups. Genotypes of 37
323
taste-related loci showed that three SNPs at TAS2R50 (rs1376251), TRPM5 (rs800345) and TAS2R16
324
(rs860170) genes had particular high levels of differentiation among our studied populations. The MAFs
325
of these SNPs were indeed found to be much higher in populations of Asian ancestry. We did not find
326
any correlations between two of these SNPs and the investigated phenotypes, neither biological nor
327
cultural, although a recent study showed that TAS2R50 rs1376251 is associated with a decreased
328
dietary fiber and vegetable intake (Schembre et al. 2013). We cannot exclude the possibility that the
AC C
320
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
lack of association can be linked to the limited number of samples considered in this study, highlighting
330
the importance of further investigations on potential associations between these two SNPs and taste
331
phenotypes.
332
Other studies reported an association between this SNP, myocardial infarction and coronary heart
333
disease, suggesting a possible relationship between this polymorphism and unbalanced food habits
334
(Shiffman et al. 2008; Akao et al. 2012). These correlations are however still debated, since an
335
independent study of 3657 patients with myocardial infarction and 1211 controls failed to replicate this
336
association (Koch et al. 2011). TRPM5 rs800345 was also found to be significantly associated with pre-
337
diabetic phenotypes (Ketterer et al. 2011), indicating a potential link between genetic variability across
338
populations and glucose tolerance.
339
When examining potential associations between taste genetic variants and food habits, we found a
340
significant association between TAS2R38 SNPs and broccoli consumption, in agreement with previous
341
results (Niewind et al. 1988; Jerzsa-Latta et al. 1990) and, more generally, with an inverse relationship
342
between TAS2R38-mediated sensitivity to bitterness and vegetable intake (Dinehart et al. 2006; Duffy et
343
al. 2010, Colares-Bento et al. 2012). As expected from the literature, we found that higher scores of
344
PROP bitterness perception were associated with the TAS2R38 PAV taster haplotype (Kim et al. 2003;
345
Duffy et al. 2004; Allen et al. 2013; Fischer et al. 2014). However, we could not confirm the association
346
between CA6 rs2274333 and perceived PROP bitterness (Padiglia et al. 2010), thus supporting more
347
recent studies (Feeney and Hayes 2014; Barbarossa et al. 2015). The perceived bitterness of stevioside
348
was positively associated with TAS2R4 rs2234001 G allele, replicating previous results from our group on
349
a lower sample (Risso et al. 2014b) and in part confirming data published by a recent study (Hayes et al.
350
2011). Hayes and colleagues showed that individuals carrying one or two copies of a specific more
351
responsive haplotype containing this SNP (i.e. TGAG: rs765007, rs224001, rs2234012, rs2227264)
352
experienced twice as much coffee bitterness when compared to individuals homozygous for the less
353
responsive haplotype (CCGT). However, it is important to note that this less responsive haplotype was
354
also found to be associated with higher bitterness ratings of both ethanol and capsaicin (Nolden et al.
355
2016), indicating the need to replicate these findings in order to elucidate the role of this SNP/haplotype
356
in bitterness perception, in particular considering genetic ancestry, ethnicity, gender and age.
357
On the contrary, the association with TAS2R14 rs3741843 reported in the same study was not
358
confirmed here (Risso et al. 2014b). The bitterness elicited by stevioside was far higher in North
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
329
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Europeans than in the Maghreb population, alerting caution towards a non population-oriented
360
marketing of Stevia rebaudiana extracts.
361
More generally, it is worth noticing that the peaks of frequency difference among groups and the
362
associations with taste phenotypes involved different SNPs, suggesting that present-day genetic
363
variance was not caused by unbalanced positive selection upon the investigated phenotype patterns.
364
The only exception is rs860170 (TAS2R16), with its ancestral allele (A) being highly predominant in north
365
African individuals and associated with salicin bitterness perception. This result is in line with those of
366
Soranzo and colleagues (2005) detecting signatures of positive selection at TAS2R16 surveying the
367
geographic pattern of its variants. Interestingly, an haplotype including TAS2R16 rs860170-A was found
368
associated with longevity in humans (Campa et al. 2012), thus opening new prospects on the interplay
369
between human evolution, taste perception and health. In this paper the authors have hypothesized
370
that salicin could have similar effects to aspirin (acetylsalicylic acid), acting as an anti-inflammatory
371
agent and therefore favoring healthy ageing. This consideration is also important for human
372
evolutionary studies and we speculate that the rs860170 derived allele could increase the tolerance
373
level for salicin bitterness and other similar substances used as analgesic, anti-inflammatory, and
374
antipyretic. Inflammation and foods that modulate the release of pro-inflammatory mediators might be
375
considered the mechanism crucial for both human evolution and human pathology (chronic
376
inflammation) (Franceschi and Campisi 2014; Straub 2014). The link between inflammation, ageing and
377
nutrition has been addressed (Szarc vel Szic et al. 2015) and most plant-derived dietary phytochemicals
378
able to modulate oxidative stress and inflammatory signaling through epigenetic mechanism are also
379
taste-active. However, the role and contribution of taste perception in this process still needs to be
380
elucidated.
381
In this paper, we reported for the first time population-related differences in taste perception and
382
eating habits in native groups with well-defined cultural and geographic features.
383
Broccoli, mustard and beer liking/consumption scores were significantly higher in North Europeans
384
when compared to the other groups. In addition, licorice and Parmesan cheese showed lower
385
consumption and liking scores in the Sri Lankan group. Similarly, Lankans showed a lower percentage of
386
MSG sensitives: this is in agreement with previous observations (Singh et al. 2010) that studied
387
variations in umami taste perception in Europeans. Lastly, we here reported more sucrose non-likers in
388
Northern Europe, in comparison to African or Asian populations, supporting previous studies observing
389
that life experiences may contribute to ethnic differences in sweet preferences (Pepino and Mennella
390
2005). Other authors reported in fact that cultural practices common in Africa, such as feeding sugar
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
359
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
water to infants, resulted in an increased sweet preferences during the second year of life of the child
392
(Beauchamp and Moran 1984). This indicates that traditions, experience and historical background could
393
have a significant impact on food preferences from the first period of life (Kobayashi and Kennedy 2002,
394
Mennella 2014). This study, in accordance with some previous ones, suggests that taste responsiveness
395
could be related to the ethnic background (Ahn et al. 2011; Williams et al. 2016). However, an adequate
396
classification from an ethno-anthropological point of view is still needed in order to avoid misleading
397
interpretations.
RI PT
391
398 5. Conclusion
400
Identifying genetic variants on chemosensory receptors that vary across populations and show
401
associations with taste perception and food habits is of high importance. Although based on a small
402
number of samples, this exploratory and multidisciplinary study constitutes an innovation in the fields,
403
since it represents an attempt to explore and combine different aspects (genetics, population variability
404
and phenotypic associations) that together might have an impact in determining individuals’ food
405
preferences.
406
Conflict of Interest
407
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
408
Source of funding
409
This work was supported by the Fondazione Del Monte ‘‘Taste Genetic Geography’’ Grant to DL and by
410
University of Pisa ex60% grants to ST.
411
Acknowledgments
412
The authors would like to thank the ‘‘Slow Food Communities,’’ Silvano Zaccone, Luisella Verderi and all
413
the people who agreed to participate to this study.
414
References
415 416
1000 Genomes Project Consortium et al. 2015. A global reference for human genetic variation. Nature. 2015;526(7571):68-74
417
Ahn, Y.-Y. et al. 2011. Flavor network and the principles of food pairing. Scientific reports, 1, p.196
418 419
Allen AL, McGeary JE, Knopik VS, Hayes JE. 2013. Bitterness of the non-nutritive sweetener acesulfame potassium varies with polymorphisms in TAS2R9 and TAS2R31. Chem Senses. 38(5):379-89
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
399
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Akao H et al. 2012. KIF6, LPA, TAS2R50, and VAMP8 genetic variation, low density lipoprotein cholesterol lowering response to pravastatin, and heart disease risk reduction in the elderly. Atherosclerosis. 2012 Feb;220(2):456-62
423 424
Allen AL, McGeary JE, Knopik VS, Hayes JE. 2013. Bitterness of the non-nutritive acesulfame potassium varies with polymorphisms in TAS2R9 and TAS2R31. Chem Senses 38(5):379–389
425 426
Armelagos, G.J. 2014. Brain evolution, the determinates of food choice, and the omnivore's dilemma. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 54, pp.1330-1341
427 428 429
Barbarossa, I.T. et al. 2015. The gustin (CA6) gene polymorphism, rs2274333 (A/G), is associated with fungiform papilla density, whereas PROP bitterness is mostly due to TAS2R38 in an ethnically-mixed population. Physiology & behavior, 138, pp.6–12
430 431
Beauchamp, G.K. and Moran, M. 1984. Acceptance of sweet and salty tastes in 2-year-old children. Appetite, 5(4), pp.291–305
432 433
Bertino M. Beauchamp G.K. Jen K.C. 1983. Rated taste perception in two cultural groups. Chemical Senses. 8, pp.3-15
434 435
Boxer EE, Garneau NL. Rare haplotypes of the gene TAS2R38 confer bitter taste sensitivity in humans. Springerplus. 17;4:505
436 437
Breslin, P.A.S. 2013. An Evolutionary Perspective on Food and Human Taste. Current Biology, 23(9), pp.R409–R418
438
Campa, D. et al. 2012. Bitter taste receptor polymorphisms and human aging. PloS one, 7(11), p.e45232
439 440
Campbell, M.C. et al. 2012. Evolution of functionally diverse alleles associated with PTC bitter taste sensitivity in Africa. Molecular biology and evolution, 29(4), pp.1141–1153
441
Cann, HM. et al. 2002. A human genome diversity cell line panel. Science. 2002;296(5566):261-2
442 443
Clark, A.A. Dotson, C.D. Elson, A.E.T. Voigt, A. Boehm, U. Meyerhof, W. Steinle N.I. Munger, S.D. 2015. TAS2R bitter taste receptors regulate thyroid function. FASEB J. Jan;29(1):164-72
444 445
Colares-Bento FC et al. 2012. Implication of the G145C polymorphism (rs713598) of the TAS2r38 gene on food consumption by Brazilian older women. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 54(2):e13-8
446 447
des Gachons, C.P. Beauchamp, G.K. and Breslin, P.A. 2009. The genetics of bitterness and pungency. Detection and its impact on phytonutrient evaluation. Ann N Y Acad Sci. Jul;1170:140-4
448 449
Dinehart ME, Hayes JE, Bartoshuk LM, Lanier SL, Duffy VB. 2006. Bitter taste markers explain variability in vegetable sweetness, bitterness, and intake. Physiol Behav. 28;87(2):304-13
450 451
Duffy, V.B. et al. 2004. Bitter receptor gene (TAS2R38), 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) bitterness and alcohol intake. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research, 28(11), pp.1629–1637
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
420 421 422
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Duffy et al. 2010. Vegetable Intake in College-Aged Adults Is Explained by Oral Sensory Phenotypes and TAS2R38 Genotype. Chemosens Percept. 1;3(3-4):137-148
454 455
Excoffier L, Laval G, Schneider S. 2007. Arlequin (version 3.0): an integrated software package for population genetics data analysis. Evol Bioinform Online 23(1):47–50
456 457
Feeney E, O'Brien S, Scannell A, Markey A, Gibney ER. 2011. Genetic variation in taste perception: does it have a role in healthy eating? Proc Nutr Soc. 70(1):135-43
458 459
Feeney, E.L. and Hayes, J.E. 2014. Exploring associations between taste perception, oral anatomy and polymorphisms in the carbonic anhydrase (gustin) gene CA6. Physiology & behavior, 128, pp.148–154
460
Finger T.E. and Kinnamon S.C. 2011. Taste isn’t just for taste buds anymore. F1000 Biol Rep. 3: 20
461 462
Fischer, A. et al. 2005. Evolution of bitter taste receptors in humans and apes. Molecular biology and evolution, 22(3), pp.432–436
463 464
Fischer, M.E. et al. 2014. The associations between 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) intensity and taste intensities differ by TAS2R38 haplotype. Journal of nutrigenetics and nutrigenomics, 7(3), pp.143–152
465 466 467 468 469
Franceschi, C. and Campisi, J. 2014. Chronic inflammation (inflammaging) and its potential contribution to age-associated diseases. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. Jun;69 Suppl 1:S4-9
470
Glendinning JI. 1994. Physiol Behav. Dec;56(6):1217-27
471 472
Green BG, Dalton P, Cowart B, Shaffer G, Rankin K, Higgins J. 1996. Evaluating the 'Labeled Magnitude Scale' for measuring sensations of taste and smell. Chem Senses. 21(3):323-34
473 474 475
Hayes JE, Wallace MR, Knopik VS, Herbstman DM, Bartoshuk LM, Duffy VB. 2011. Allelic variation in TAS2R bitter receptor genes associates with variation in sensations from and ingestive behaviors toward common bitter beverages in adults. Chem Senses. 36(3):311-9
476 477
Jerzsa-Latta, M. Krondl, M. and Coleman, P. 1990. Use and perceived attributes of cruciferous vegetables in terms of genetically-mediated taste sensitivity. Appetite, 15(2), pp.127–134
478 479 480
Kabera, J.N. Semana, E. Mussa, A.R. and He, X. 2014. Plant secondary metabolites: biosynthesis, classification, function and pharmacological properties. Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology 2, pp.377-392
481 482
Ketterer, C. et al. 2011. Genetic variation within the TRPM5 locus associates with prediabetic phenotypes in subjects at increased risk for type 2 diabetes. Metabolism. 2011;60(9):1325-33
483 484
Kim, U. et al. 2003. Positional cloning of the human quantitative trait locus underlying taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide. Science (New York, N.Y.), 299(5610), pp.1221–1225
M AN U
SC
RI PT
452 453
AC C
EP
TE D
Gabriel S, Ziaugra L, Tabbaa D. 2009. SNP genotyping using the Sequenom MassARRAY iPLEX platform. Curr Protoc Hum Genet. doi:10.1002/0471142905.hg0212s60
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Kobayashi, C. and Kennedy, L.M. 2002. Experience-induced changes in taste identification of monosodium glutamate. Physiology & behavior, 75(1–2), pp.57–63
487 488
Koch, W. et al. 2011. Variations of specific non-candidate genes and risk of myocardial infarction: a replication study. International journal of cardiology, 147(1), pp.38–41
489 490
Kumanyika, S.K. 2008. Environmental influences on childhood obesity: ethnic and cultural influences in context. Physiology & behavior, 94(1), pp.61–70
491 492
Lundgren, B et al., 1978. Taste discrimination vs hedonic response to sucrose in coffee beverage. An interlaboratory study. Chemical Senses, 3 (3): 249-265
493 494
Mennella J.A. 2014. Ontogeny of taste preferences: basic biology and implications for health. Am J Clin Nutr 99(suppl), pp.704S–711S
495 496
Mennella, J.A. Bobowski, N.K. 2015. The sweetness and bitterness of childhood: Insights from basic research on taste preferences. Physiol Behav. Dec 1;152(Pt B):502-7
497 498
Meyerhof, W. et al. 2010. The molecular receptive ranges of human TAS2R bitter taste receptors. Chemical senses, 35(2), pp.157–170
499 500
Niewind, A. Krondl, M. and Shrott, M. 1988. Genetic influences on the selection of brassica vegetables by elderly individuals. Nutrition Research, 8(1), pp.13–20
501 502
Nolden AA, McGeary JE, Hayes JE. Differential bitterness in capsaicin, piperine, and ethanol associates with polymorphisms in multiple bitter taste receptor genes. Physiol Behav. 2016 Mar 15;156:117-27
503 504 505
Padiglia et al. 2010. Sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil is associated with gustin (carbonic anhydrase VI) gene polymorphism, salivary zinc, and body mass index in humans. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010 Sep;92(3):539-45
506 507
Pepino, M.Y. and Mennella, J.A. 2005. Factors contributing to individual differences in sucrose preference. Chemical senses, 30 Suppl 1, pp.i319-320
508 509
Perry, G.H. et al. 2015. Insights into hominin phenotypic and dietary evolution from ancient DNA sequence data. Journal of human evolution, 79, pp.55–63
510 511
Petrovska, B. B. 2012. Historical Review of Medicinal Plants’ Usage.” Pharmacognosy Reviews 6 (11), pp.1-5
512 513
Pieroni, A. Pawera L. and Shah G.M. 2016. Gastronomic Ethnobiology. In: Albuquerque, U.P. Alves, R.R.N. (Eds.) Introduction to Ethnobiology, 2016, Springer, New York, ISBN 978-3-319-28153-7, pp. 53-62
514 515 516
Pirastu, N. et al. 2015. Genome-wide association analysis on five isolated populations identifies variants of the HLA-DOA gene associated with white wine liking. European journal of human genetics: EJHG, 23(12), pp.1717–1722
517 518
Purcell, S. et al. 2007. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American journal of human genetics, 81(3), pp.559–575
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
485 486
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Risso, D. Tofanelli, S. et al. 2014a. Genetic variation in taste receptor pseudogenes provides evidence for a dynamic role in human evolution. BMC evolutionary biology, 14, p.198
521 522
Risso, D. Morini, G. et al. 2014b. Genetic signature of differential sensitivity to stevioside in the Italian population. Genes & Nutrition, 9(3)
523 524
Risso, D. et al. 2016. Global diversity in the TAS2R38 bitter taste receptor: revisiting a classic evolutionary PROPosal. Sci Rep. 6: 25506
525 526
Robino, A. et al. 2014. A population-based approach to study the impact of PROP perception on food liking in populations along the Silk Road. PloS one, 9(3), p.e91716
527 528
Roudnitzky, N. et al. 2011. Genomic, genetic and functional dissection of bitter taste responses to artificial sweeteners. Human molecular genetics, 20(17), pp.3437–3449
529 530
Roudnitzky, N. et al. 2015. Receptor Polymorphism and Genomic Structure Interact to Shape Bitter Taste Perception T. Gojobori, ed. PLOS Genetics, 11(9), p.e1005530
531 532
Roudnitzky N, et al. 2016. Copy Number Variation in TAS2R Bitter Taste Receptor Genes: Structure, Origin, and Population Genetics. Chem Senses. pii: bjw067
533 534 535
Rozin P. 1982. Human food selection: the interaction of biology, culture and individual experience, in L.M. Barker (Ed) The Psychobiology of Human Food Selection, AVI Publishing, Westport, CT, pp.225269
536 537
Santa-Cruz Calvo, S. and Egan, J. M. 2015. Endocrinology of taste. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 2015 Apr;11(4):213-27)
538 539 540
Shaik FA, Singh N, Arakawa M, Duan K, Bhullar RP, Chelikani P. 2016. Bitter taste receptors: extraoral roles in pathophysiology. The International Journal of Biochemistry & Cell Biology doi:10.1016/j.biocel.2016.03.011
541 542
Schembre, S.M. et al. 2013. Variations in bitter-taste receptor genes, dietary intake, and colorectal adenoma risk. Nutrition and cancer, 65(7), pp.982–990
543
Shepherd G. Neurogastronomy. New York: Columbia University Press; 2012
544 545
Shiffman, D. et al. 2008. Association of gene variants with incident myocardial infarction in the Cardiovascular Health Study. Arteriosclerosis, thrombosis, and vascular biology, 28(1), pp.173–179
546 547
Singh, P.B. Schuster, B. and Seo, H.-S. 2010. Variation in umami taste perception in the German and Norwegian population. European journal of clinical nutrition, 64(10), pp.1248–1250
548 549 550
Straub, R.H. 2014. TRPV1, TRPA1, and TRPM8 channels in inflammation, energy redirection, and water retention: role in chronic inflammatory diseases with an evolutionary perspective. J Mol Med, 92, pp.925–937
551 552
Tepper BJ. 6-n-Propylthiouracil: a genetic marker for taste, with implications for food preference and dietary habits. Am J Hum Genet. 1998 Nov;63(5):1271-6
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
519 520
19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Tepper, B.J. 2008. Nutritional implications of genetic taste variation: the role of PROP sensitivity and other taste phenotypes. Annual review of nutrition, 28, pp.367–388
555 556 557
Turnbull, B. and Matisoo-Smith, E. 2002. Taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil predicts acceptance of bitter-tasting spinach in 3-6-y-old children. The American journal of clinical nutrition, 76(5), pp.1101– 1105
558 559
Turner, B.L. Thompson, A.L. 2013. Beyond the Paleolithic prescription: incorporating diversity and flexibility in the study of human diet evolution. Nutrition Reviews Vol. 71(8):501–510
560 561 562
Szarc vel Szic, K. Declerck, K. Melita Vidaković, M. and Vanden Berghe, W. 2015. From inflammaging to healthy aging by dietary lifestyle choices: is epigenetics the key to personalized nutrition? Clinical Epigenetics, 7:3
563 564
Williams, J.A. et al. 2016. Exploring Ethnic Differences in Taste Perception. Chemical senses Jun;41(5):449-56
565 566
Wink, M. 2015. Modes of action of herbal medicines and plant secondary metabolites. Medicines 2, pp. 251-286; doi:10.3390/medicines2030251
567 568
Wooding, S. et al. 2004. Natural selection and molecular evolution in PTC, a bitter-taste receptor gene. American journal of human genetics, 74(4), pp.637–646
569 570
Zhao L, Kirkmeyer S, Tepper B. 2003. A paper screening test to assess genetic taste sensitivity to 6-n propylthiouracil. Physiol Behav 78:625–633
SC
M AN U
TE D EP AC C
571
RI PT
553 554
20