Age decisions on familiar and unfamiliar faces

Age decisions on familiar and unfamiliar faces

Behavioural Processes, 24 (1991) 21-35 0 1991 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. 0376.6357/91/$03.50 21 BEPROC 00354 Age decisions on familiar R. Buy...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 65 Views

Behavioural Processes, 24 (1991) 21-35 0 1991 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. 0376.6357/91/$03.50

21

BEPROC 00354

Age decisions on familiar R. Buyer Louvain

University

and unfamiliar

faces **

*, A. Lafalize and M. Distefano

(UCL), Psychology (NECO),

Department,

Unit& de Neuropsychologie

Louvain-la-Neuve,

(Accepted

27 February

Cognitive

Belgium

1991)

Abstract The extraction of age is among the “early visual processes” that operate on faces. Current cognitive models mention this operation, but data on the subject is rare. In the first experiment, a series of familiar and unfamiliar faces were displayed to subjects who were asked to make old/young categorizations. To test how early this operation occurs, the same material was used for familiarity decision. Finally, the stimuli were displayed centrally to 16 subjects and laterally to 32 subjects. The age decision was easier and quicker than the familiarity decision; the familiarity decision was unaffected by the age of the faces, while face familiarity affected the extraction of age. In a second experiment (40 subjects), we manipulated the ambiguity of the apparent age and conditional tasks were used, i.e., the subjects had to process both age and familiarity in a single task. The results suggest that age extraction is an operation that is mandatory for an efficient recognition process. Therefore, it is proposed to dissociate, within the semantic visually derived codes, the processing of age from the processing of sex and race.

Key words:

Age; Cognitive model; Conditional task; Face recognition; Familiarity

Introduction The cognitive models of face recognition include visual operations that precede the recognition stage and can be applied to unfamiliar as well as to familiar faces. Beyond the very first step of “facial decision” (Ellis, 1983, 1986), these operations, among other things,

* UCL-NECO, **

Voie du Roman

This study was presented,

Fransaise,

Paris, 1988.

Pays 20, 1348

Louvain-la-Neuve,

in part, at the meeting

of the Soci&

Belgium. de Neuropsychologie

de Langue

22

age of the extract characteristics concerning the sex, the race, and the approximate derived semantic codes” (Bruce & Young, perceived face. This extraction of “visually 1986) would not seem to be mandatory for face recognition. Two questions arise about these processes. The first is how early these operations occur with regard to the recognition step. The second question concerns their independence of face familiarity. Indeed, it could be that top-down mechanisms apply, so that familiarity could facilitate or inhibit decisions about the sex, age, or race of the face. In addition, we consider the possibility of lateral differences, i.e., the opportunity to demonstrate dissociations in the function of the stimulated portion of the visual field and, accordingly, of the stimulated cerebral hemisphere. However, given the small amount of published data, we also have to examine this question with centrally displayed stimuli. In normal subjects, Bruyer & Schweich (1987) used this neuropsychological procedure and showed that the “race decision” was an early operation and was not lateralized (see also Bruyer & Dujeux, 1988). However, only unfamiliar faces were used, so the effect of familiarity on this decision was not investigated. With regard to gender classification, several studies have shown that this operation was early, non-lateralized, and free of familiarity effects (Bruyer & Dujeux, 1988; Jones, 1979, 1980; Marzi, Tassinari, Tressoldi, Barry & Crabowska, 1985; Sergent, 1985). Finally, to our knowledge, there are no neuropsychological studies with normal subjects on “age classification” of faces. With brain-injured subjects, there are a few cases of prosopagnosia (i.e., a specific face agnosia) in which defects about face classification as a function of age (Beyn & Knyazeva, 1962; Bruyer, Rectem & Dupuis, 1986; Cloning, Cloning, Jellinger & Quatember, 1970; Michel, P&enin & %&off, 1986; Tiberghien & Clerc, 1986; Whiteley & Warrington, 1977), sex (Beyn & Knyazeva, 1962; Tiberghien & Clerc, 1986), or race (Cole & PerezCruet, 1964) have been clinically described. Nevertheless, prosopagnosics are more often than not able to perform these operations (e.g., Sergent & Villemure, 1989) and no pathological cases have been described in which these operations were defective while recognition was preserved. Therefore, there are precede and are independent of recognition. In this view. Tranel, Damasio & Damasio (1988) gender, age, and identity of faces to four severe defective in every task, while the remaining

reasons to suppose that these operations addition, more experimental data support applied recognition tests of expressions, cases of prosopagnosia. One subject was three were deficient for identity only.

Newcombe & Russell (1969) showed that a group of right-brain-injured, non-prosopagnosic subjects was impaired in a task of age and sex categorization of faces. However, the two scores were pooled in the results, and the faces used were only approximations of faces (Mooney faces). De Renzi, Bonacini & Faglioni (1989) have shown that damage to the posterior region of the right hemisphere impairs the classification of faces by age, more than it impairs face recognition. Finally, in a prosopagnosic subject, Sergent & Poncet (1990) found familiarity effects in the episodic recognition of faces whose age changed between encoding and test. The present study focusses on one of these visually derived semantic codes, namely, age. In the first experiment, normal subjects were presented with laterally or centrally displayed faces and asked to make old/young decisions. Two questions were considered, namely, (a) familiar and unfamiliar faces were intermixed in order to detect familiarity effects, and (b) a control recognition condition was provided to test how early is age evaluation. Indeed, little is known about the processing of age and the few published experiments have shown (a) that episodic recognition of faces is possible even if the age is changed between encoding and test, provided the change is not too large (Bruyer & Lafal-

23

ize, 19891, and the performance Seamon,

1982);

is facilitated

(b) that this episodic

encoding provided 1990); and (c) that parameter the three-dimensional

by face familiarity

recognition

is facilitated

(Bruyer

& Lafalize,

by the exposure

1989;

duration

at

the structural age changes are modest (Read, Vokey & Hammersley, the human perceiver can extract age changes by considering a single strain of a cardioidal function (Mark & Todd, 1985>, even on representations (Mark & Todd, 1983; see also Bruce, Burton,

Doyle & Dench, 1989 for both replication and slight qualifications). In the first experiment, we hypothesized that an advantage of familiar over unfamiliar faces in age decision would reflect top-down influence of face familiarity (face recognition units) on visual processes, and that age decision would be faster than familiarity decision. Bruce, Ellis, Gibling & Young (1987) used an elegant paradigm showing categorization was independent of recognition. This study will be presented we applied this methodology in our second experiment on age categorization, attempted

to precise the task effect observed

Experiment

that gender below since in which we

in the first experiment.

I

Method Subjects Forty-eight

(24 female,

24 male) young,

neurologically

intact,

right-handed

adults were

enrolled. There were no left-handed close relatives. They were randomly assigned to two displayed stimuli” condigroups: 16 subjects (8 females) were assigned to the “centrally tion; the remaining 32 took part in the “laterally displayed stimuli” condition (Actually, the lateral presentation was administered to 34 subjects. However, the data of two subjects had to be discarded because they were completely unable to recognize familiar faces in the familiarity decision task). Within each condition, two tasks, age decision tered to each subject in a balanced order. Stimuli A series of was submitted evaluated the 20 & 30; .; selected, with

and familiarity

decision,

were

adminis-

60 black-and-white photographs of familiar (well-known celebrities) faces to 40 judges who evaluated their celebrity on a seven-point scale; they also age on a seven-point scale (1 = less than 20 years old; 2 = between 7 = more than 70). Ten clearly familiar faces (mean score > 5) were then five being clearly “young” (values < 2: mean = 1.92; mean familiarity =

6.021, and five clearly “old” (values > 5: mean = 6.11; mean familiarity = 5.94). Another series of 47 black-and-white photographs of unfamiliar (unknown) faces was submitted to a similar process (n = 26 judges). This material had been collected from people who were invited to supply with two (or more) photographs of the same face taken at an interval of 15 years or more, with the youngest not less than 15 years old. We selected 10 clearly unfamiliar faces (mean score < 21, with five being clearly “young” (values < 2: mean = 1.63; mean familiarity = 1.38) and five clearly “old” (values > 5: mean = 6.22; mean familiarity = 1.39). Several slides were made of these 20 faces. The stimulus width was four degrees of the visual field. When laterally displayed, they were centered four degrees left or right of the fixation point.

24

Experimental design In the central condition, each task (age decision and familiarity decision) involved 60 items, i.e., three displays of each face in random order. In the lateral condition, each task 120 items, i.e., three displays of each face in each hemifield in random order. The subject was asked to respond as quickly as possible by depressing one of two push buttons (index fingers) indicating his/her choice: old/young or familiar/ unfamiliar. The involved

button assignment was balanced across the subjects. For the familiarity decision, the subject was asked to decide if the face was well known due to its episodic recurrence in the course of the or not (irrespective of its “familiarity” experimental session). For the age decision, some examples of old and young faces, not used in the experiment, were shown before beginning. This adaptation was provided since the notions of “old” and “young” can be relative to the current set of stimuli and/or to each subject. Procedure The subject sat facing the screen (viewing distance = 85.5 cm), the head supported on a chin rest. Each trial was sequenced as follows: auditory warning (500 msec) to invite the subject to focus on the central fixation point, delay (1000 msec), displaying of the stimulus during 150 msec, inter-item interval of five sec. The stimuli were back-displayed by projectors controlled by a microcomputer, the response device. The instructions stressed the speed of response. Statistical

as was

design

Central and lateral presentations were analyzed separately, since they were submitted to different subjects and differed in perceptual difficulty. In both cases, errors and correct latencies were studied separately by means of analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Post-hoc analyses of significant interactions were performed by using the Newman-Keuls test (p < 0.05).

Results Central presentation We computed 2 x 2 x 2, within-subject,

ANOVAs.

The factors were the task, the age of

the faces, and the face familiarity. As can be seen in Table 1, there were more errors in the familiarity decision than in the age decision (F(l ,I 5) = 29.38, p < 0.0001) and more errors for the familiar than for the

TABLE 1 Exp. I. Central

presentation:

errors (out

of 15, between

familiarity.

Means

mean

latency

parentheses)

of the correct

in function

responses

in msec and mean

number

of 16 subjects. Age decision

of

of the task, the age of the faces, and the face

Familiarity

decision

Old

Young

Old

Young

Familiar

678 (0.19)

661 (0.56)

831 (4.50)

732 (5.25)

Unfamiliar

699 (I .44)

729 (T.31)

776 (1.69)

783 (I .12)

25 TABLE

2

Exp. I. Lateral presentation: mean latency of the correct responses in msec and mean number of errors (out of 15, between parentheses) in function of the task, the age of the faces, the face familiarity, and the visual field (LVF = left, RVF = right). Means of 32 subjects. Age decision Old Familiar LVF RVF

Unfamiliar LVF RVF

Familiarity decision Young

Old

Young

729 (1.41)

729C1.75)

976C6.28)

936C5.12)

697c1.28)

713 (1.69)

923 (6.25)

863 (5.28)

730 (1.81) 728 (1.78)

795 (4.47)

911 (3.34)

884 (2.37)

818 (4.87)

886C3.06)

927 (2.47)

unfamiliar faces (F(1,15> = 9.83, p < 0.01). Moreover, these two factors interacted significantly (F(1 ,I 5) = 30.97, p < 0.0001). Post-hoc analysis of this interaction revealed a higher number of errors on the familiar faces for the familiarity decision condition than in the remaining three conditions. Thus, the task effect applied to the familiar faces only, and the familiarity effect applied to the familiarity decision only. No other main effect or interaction reached statistical significance. For the correct latencies (Table familiarity decision (F(1 ,I 5) = 9.1,

I), there p < 0.01).

was a significant There was also

advantage of age over a significant interaction

between the age and the familiarity of faces (F(1 ,I 5) = 6.2, p < 0.02). Post-hoc analysis of the interaction revealed that the young familiar faces were processed faster than both the young unfamiliar and the old familiar faces. There was no age effect for the unfamiliar people and no familiarity effect for the old faces. No other main effect or interaction reached

statistical

significance.

Lateral presentation We computed two

2 x 2 x 2 x 2, within-subject,

ANOVAs.

The factors were

the task,

the age of the faces, the face familiarity, and the visual hemifield of presentation. As shown in Table 2, accuracy was better for age than for familiarity decisions (F(1,31) = 42, p < 0.0001) and better for the old than for the young faces (F(1,31) = 5.3, p < 0.05). These two factors, however, interacted significantly (F(1,31) = 16.4, p < 0.001). Post-hoc analysis of this interaction showed that the age-decision advantage was observed for the old faces only. On the other hand, whereas the advantage of the old over the young faces appeared for age decision, the opposite advantage of the young over the old faces was significant in the familiarity decision task. There was also a significant task X familiarity interaction (F(1,31) = 44.4, p < 0.0001; see Fig. I). Post-hoc analysis of this interaction showed that the age-decision advantage was observed only for the familiar faces, and an advantage of the familiar over the unfamiliar faces appeared in the age-decision task, while accuracy was better for the unfamiliar than for the familiar faces in the familiarity decision task. Finally, the two factors involving the kinds of stimuli interacted significantly (F(1,31) = 8.6, p < 0.01). Post-hoc analysis of this age X familiarity interaction showed that old unfamiliar people were better processed than the other three series, which did not differ significantly from each other.

26

01

I

FAMILIAR Fig I. The taskxface

familiarity

UNFAMILIAR

interaction

for errors in Exp. I, lateral presentation.

While the field of presentation did not affect accuracy as a main effect or within interactions, the three remaining factors interacted significantly (F(1,31) = 7.02, p < 0.02). This interaction slightly modifies the effects already noted. (a) With the exception of the young unfamiliar faces, for which familiarity decisions were easier than age decisions, the age decision

was better

(b> An advantage decision when

task (for

the young

unfamiliar familiarity For

performed

than the familiarity

of the unfamiliar

both old and young faces were

faces,

the

decisions

old

faces

the correct

latencies

were

to the age decision. better

processed

faces, the young

(Table

2),

task for every

faces was revealed

faces) and of the familiar

submitted

on familiar

decision

over the familiar

there

than

faces were

was a main

over

Cc> For the

better

of face.

the unfamiliar age decisions

young;

faces on the

conversely,

processed

significant

kind

in the familiarity

for

than the old.

advantage

of the age

(F(1,31)= 46.9, p < 0.0001) and a bias favoring the right over the left field (F(1,31)= 3.5, p = 0.067). Three one-way interactions reached significance. For the first two (task X age: F(1,31) = 7.23, p < 0.01; task X familiarity:

decision

F(1,31)

over the familiarity

= 4.8,

p < 0.05),

significant

for

unfamiliar

faces.

familiarity), third

it can

both the old

be seen

and the young

Secondly,

while

significant

decision

the familiarity

the age decision

interaction

modified

F(1,31)= 14.24, p < 0.001)

since

in

Figure

2,

firstly,

faces as well decision

was affected

was

that

as for

unaffected

by face familiarity

the bias towards a significant

task

field

effect

advantage

was

and the

by age (as well (as well

as by

as by age). The

a main field effect (field

right

950

the

both the familiar

X familiarity:

for the familiar

but

n

-_____~‘~l!JRW

/----¤ w---

900 850 I

o

7oo%zE+-xFig 2. The taskxage

(left)

and taskxfamiliarity

-

FAMILIAR (right)

I

UNFAMILIAR

interactions

lateral presentation.

FlGI DECISION

for correct

latencies

in Exp. I,

27

not the unfamiliar faces emerged. Complementarily, faster than the unfamiliar in the right field only.

the familiar

faces were processed

Discussion One purpose of this experiment was to decide whether age processing is earlier than face recognition. It seems that a positive answer can be given. Indeed, this expected kind of result appeared for latencies on both centrally and laterally displayed faces, and a similar pattern appeared for accuracy. Therefore, these results tend to support the theoretical models that locate the age decision in the cognitive stages (“visual processing”) that precede the recognition stages (e.g., Bruce & Young, 1986). One could argue that even though the same faces have been used in both tasks the task difficulty was not identical, since the stimuli were selected on the criteria of the ease to be discriminated for age. In this respect, it should be mentioned, firstly, that the stimuli were also selected on the basis of the ease to be discriminated for familiarity. Secondly, this facility of age discrimination will be manipulated in the second experiment. Another purpose of this study concerned the relative dependency between these two stages of processing, i.e., whether familiarity affects decisions on age. Provided the task demands were hard enough i.e., the lateral mode of presentation it appeared that familiarity influenced the age decision, while the speed of familiarity decision was unaffected by age. This observation suggests top-down effects of the face recognition units on the decisions taken at the visual processing level. That is to say, when faces were recognizable, the decision on age was more easily (or quickly) taken than when they were not. In a companion paper using an episodic recognition procedure, we (Bruyer & Lafalize, 1989) noted that it was almost impossible to recognize unfamiliar faces strongly modified by age, while such a decision was much easier for familiar faces (see also Seamon, 1982). It was suggested that only the familiar faces activated semantic nodes about the person seen (Bruce & Young, 1986) so that the abstraction of age appearance became useless. Another interpretation, however, could be that face recognition units are “instance based”, i.e., they can vary in age or expression and, in this way, there is no need to resort to any semantic node effect. Finally, lateral differences emerged favoring the right visual hemifield that were due to familiar faces, irrespective of the task. Such a left hemispheric superiority has already been reported when known faces are involved, as compared to the “classical” right hemisphere advantage for unfamiliar faces. It has also been shown that this asymmetry results more from familiarity than from access to name representation (Umilta, Brizzolara, Tabossi & Fairweather, 1978). This result could also suggest that the left-hemisphere advantage in the processing of familiar faces emerges at the “visual processing” stage of the stimuli before the recognition stage. Since this laterality effect did not interact with the task, the lateral mode of presentation was not used in the second experiment. In short, the data collected in Exp.1 suggest that age decision precedes the recognition, and is affected - at least in some conditions - by face familiarity, in a top-down manner. However, as will be developed below, we have to check for possible methodological (task constraints) bias of this asymmetrical effect: age categorization was not required to take the familiarity decision. Exp. II has been planned to envisage this possibility. Moreover and more generally, even if an operation is faster than and is influenced by another one, it

28 remains to determine whether the two operations function sequentially

or in parallel. Exp.

II was designed to solve this question.

Experiment

II

The studies of Bruyer & Dujeux (19881, Jones (1979, 19801, Marzi et al. (1985) and Sergent (1985) have shown that gender classification of faces was independent of face familiarity. However, Bruce (I 986) showed that face familiarity facilitated sex categorization as we did in Exp. I regarding age categorization (Obviously, there is a lack of clinical and experimental data supporting the contention that sex and age [and race] classifications resort to identical mechanisms, even Bruyer & Dujeux [I 9881 suggested that Additional experiments are required to tions differ on at least one major point:

though authors tend to make such an analogy. sex and race categorizations should be dissociated. test this point. In addition, sex and age categorizathe former refers to a discrete dichotomy, while the

latter taps a continuous scale). Moreover, Bruce (1986) suspected that this familiarity effect was particularly evident when the gender of the face was someone ambiguous. This has since been confirmed (Bruce et al., 1987). This may be linked to our results showing that familiarity influences age decisions provided that perceptual conditions are difficult enough. According to Bruce, this result is not consistent with a strict perceptual hierarchy where one process should be completed before the next one begins. In the study of Bruce et al. (19871, it was questioned whether this lack of hierarchical relationship could have resulted from methodological biases, i.e., sex categorization was not a mandatory step for face recognition. Therefore, the authors designed a second experiment in which sex and familiarity were made to be relevant to each other, i.e., in the disjunctive condition, the subjects had to decide if the face seen was male (female) or known (unknown) and, in the conjunctive condition, they had to decide if the face was male (female) and known (unknown). According to the authors, the conjunctive condition provided a strong test of the model: if gender classification precedes the familiarity decision, “then a factor which slows up the sex judgement should also slow up conjunction decision. Such a consequence would be unlikely faces

in

decision

parallel

with

their

was unaffected

model

was claimed

In our second

familiarity”

(p.

by face ambiguity,

in a system which processed the sex of

516).

while

In

their

experiment,

the disjunctive

the

decision

conjunctive

was. The

parallel

to be supported.

experiment,

we used the disjunctive

et al. for age and familiarity

decisions.

We

and conjunctive

also tested

conditions

the age decision

of Bruce

and the familiarity

alone to search for effects of ambiguity. Furthermore, since, in Exp. I, the effects of interest were found when the task was difficult (lateral presentation), then, in Exp. II, we had the opportunity to examine the effect of another kind of perceptual difficulty by using

decision

ambiguous faces. Contrary to the opinion of Bruce et al. (19871, ambiguity for parallel young

does

processing.

(disjunction)

(by pressing and

not affect the response

such

we consider that, if age a result

a subject

has to decide whether

to give a positive

response,

he/she

button)

old (conjunction).

fore,

in the disjunctive

were

conjunctive,

and negative

conditions,

when

the other

Indeed,

in both

The

condition,

while,

answers

if the face is neither same

in reverse

positive

answers

in the conjunctive

were

disjunctive.

nor young,

i.e.,

for the conjunctive were

condition,

still

argue or

has also to give a negative response

familiar

applies

would

the face is familiar

disjunctive, positive

if it is unfamiliar condition.

There-

and negative answers

answers

were

conjunctive,

29

Method Subjects The experiment was submitted selected as in Exp. I.

to 40 (20

female, 20

male) young, normal adults,

Stimuli We used 16 faces from a set of 50 faces that had been submitted to ten judges for two tasks. In the familiarity judgment, they were asked to evaluate the celebrity on a seven-point scale: we selected faces with a mean evaluation greater than five (familiar faces) and less than three (unfamiliar faces). In the age judgment, the same faces had to be categorized in four classes: “very young”, “relatively young”, “elderly”, and “very old”. The judges had to sort the faces into four classes and corrections were authorized. We retained those faces that received the same classification by at least seven judges. The resulting set of stimuli was formed with eight familiar and eight unfamiliar faces. Each subset was composed of four young and four old people with two unambiguous (very young or very old) and two ambiguous (relatively young or elderly) faces. Four main sets of four faces resulted: familiar ambiguous, familiar unambiguous, unfamiliar ambiguous, and unfamiliar unambiguous. The mean familiarity ratings of these four sets were 6.05, 5.97, 1.94 and 1.78, respectively. Experimental

design

Ten slides of each face were prepared. Therefore, for each of the two between-subject conditions (below), 160 trials resulted, 40 for each of the four sets of faces. Sixteen subjects were randomly submitted to the disjunctive condition. They had to press one button when the face seen was either young (old) or familiar (unfamiliar), and the other button in the other cases (index fingers were used). The four possible disjunctions were crossed with four subjects for each. The button assignment was also balanced across the subjects within each subsample. Sixteen other subjects were submitted to the conjunctive condition. They had to push one button when the face seen was young (old) and familiar (unfamiliar) and the other button in the other cases. The four possible conjunctions were crossed with four subjects for each. The button assignment was also balanced across the subjects within each subsample. The remaining eight subjects were given “single tasks”. As in Exp. I, they had to decide whether the face was that of a young or an old person and whether it was familiar or unfamiliar, in two successive conditions. The order of conditions was balanced across the subjects, as was the button assignment. Procedure The procedure was identical to that of Exp.1, with two modifications: the instructions for the disjunctive and conjunctive conditions (see above) and the exposure duration of the stimulus which lasted until a response was given.

Results Single tasks There were 5.4% errors (Table 3) and the analysis revealed only that familiar faces were better processed than unfamiliar faces (F(1,7) = 12.7, p < 0.01). The correct latencies

30 TABLE 3 Exp. II. Single tasks: mean of 40, between Means

latency

parentheses)

of the correct

in function

responses in msec and mean number

of the task, the face familiarity,

of errors (out

and the ambiguity

of age.

of eight subjects. Age decision

Familiarity

decision

Familiar

Unfamiliar

Familiar

Unfamiliar

Ambiguous

958 (I ,251

1007

(5.00)

870 (0.62)

1057

(3.50)

Unambiguous

878 (0.87)

834 (1.37)

814 (2.25)

1022

(2.50)

llOO1050,-

n

FAMILIARITY DECISION

looo~950 I’ AGE DECISION

0 900 I’ 850

.A

-

,

800 * UNFAMILIAR

FAMILIAR Fig. 3. The task X familiarity

(Table

3) were

submitted

to

interaction

for correct

a within-subject,

latencies

in Exp. II, single tasks.

2 x 2 x 2 ANOVA.

The

factors

were

familiarity, age ambiguity, and task. There appeared a main significant advantage of familiar over unfamiliar faces (F(1,7) = 13.16, p < 0.01) and of unambiguous over ambiguous faces (F(1,7) = 24.55, p < 0.002). The task X familiarity interaction was significant (F(1,7) = 13.26, p < 0.01; see Fig. 3), i.e., familiarity decisions on familiar faces were faster than the other three conditions, and familiarity judgments than the remaining three conditions (the face familiarity

on unfamiliar faces were slower did not affect age evaluations).

There was a strong bias toward a task x ambiguity interaction (F(1,7) = 4.95, p = 0.06): age decisions on unambiguous faces were si,gnificant/y faster than in the remaining three conditions (see Fig. 4). -

lOOOr

n 0

900 g50:j

850

k

n

FPlMl LIARITY DECISION

0 AGE DECISION

1

800. AMBIGUOUS Fig. 4. The taskxambiguity

interaction

UNAMBIGUOUS for correct

latencies

in Exp. II, single tasks.

31 TABLE 4 Exp. Il. Conditional (out

tasks: mean latency

of 40, between

parentheses)

age. Means of 16 subjects

of the correct

in function

responses (msec) and mean number

of the task, the face familiarity

of errors

and the ambiguity

of

per task.

Disjunctive

task

Conjunctive

task

Familiar

Unfamiliar

Familiar

Unfamiliar

Ambiguous

945 (7.25)

1033

869 (4.44)

896 (8.00)

Unambiguous

911 (4.62)

849 (2.82)

855 (5.19)

(12.19)

933 (6.06)

Conditional tasks The correct latencies (Table 4) were studied by means of a mixed 2 x 2 X 2 ANOVA. The between-subject factor was the condition (conjunctive vs. disjunctive), and the within-subject factors were face familiarity and age ambiguity. Familiar faces were processed faster than unfamiliar faces (F(1,30) = 4.41, p < 0.05), and unambiguous faces were judged faster than ambiguous faces (F(1,30) = 8.41, p < 0.01). Errors (Table 4) were not rare (15.8%) and were submitted to the same ANOVA as latencies. Errors were less frequent on familiar than on unfamiliar faces (F(1,30) = 4.38, p < 0.05) and on unambiguous than on ambiguous faces (F(1,30) = 9.05, p < 0.01). Ideally, the statistical analyses should have included the kind of response as a factor. However, such a design was virtually impossible since, in the disjunctive task, each subject gave positive responses to the familiar and unfamiliar faces but half of them gave negative responses to the familiar faces and the other half to the unfamiliar faces. In the conjunctive task, a mirror image applied. One manner to solve this technical difficulty was to compute an index of ambiguity. We computed an index on correct latencies, for each subject, each kind of face familiarity, and each kind of response (conjunctive responses, i.e., positive responses in the conjunctive task and negative responses in the disjunctive task, vs disjunctive responses, i.e., positive responses in the disjunctive task and negative responses in the conjunctive task). The index was computed as (A - nA)/(A + nA), where A and nA represent the mean correct latency for the ambiguous and the unambiguous faces respectively. The four indexes (see Fig. 5) were tested against zero, i.e., against the absence of ambiguity effect: the ambiguity effect was significant for disjunctive responses to unfamiliar (0.034: t(31) = 2.3, p < 0.025) but not to familiar faces (0.009), and for

0,4T

Fig. 5. The index

of ambiguity

DISJUNCTIVE

for correct

(disjunctive,

latencies

conjunctive)

COWJNCTIVE

in Exp. II, conditional and the face familiarity.

tasks, according

to the task

32

conjunctive responses to both familiar (0.350: (0.103: t(15> = 3.92, p < 0.001 I

t(l5)

=

2.7, p < 0.01) and unfamiliar faces

In considering the single tasks, two conclusions derived from the Exp. I should be revised. On the one hand, age decisions were faster than familiarity decisions in certain conditions only, i.e., when the unfamiliar faces were considered (either ambiguous or not) and when easy, unambiguous faces were processed (either familiar or not). Two speculative comments can be proposed, which would require additional experiments. Firstly, the task difficulty could explain that age decision was not faster than familiarity decision on ambiguous faces: indeed, faces were selected as easily discriminable in the familiarity dimension while, by definition, ambiguous faces were selected as not easily discriminable in the age dimension. Secondly, age decision was not faster than familiarity decision when familiar faces were seen: this could suggest that the recognition is unavoidable and that the response (age decision) is mandatorily delayed up to the activation of a face recognition unit. We acknowledge, however, that this was not observed in Exp. I or in other studies dealing with gender classification or expression analysis. On the other hand, when both the ambiguous and the unambiguous faces were mixed together, the effect of face familiarity on age decision disappeared. It could be, therefore, that the suggested top-down effects of familiarity on visual, earlier processes was due only to the easy, unambiguous faces like those used in Exp. I (see Fig. 2, right panel, vs. Fig. 3). This observation is contrary to what is argued by Bruce (1986) and Bruce et al. (1987). They suggest that gender classification is facilitated by face familiarity when the gender is ambiguous. This constitutes another argument supporting dissociations within the “visually derived semantic codes” (Bruce & Young, 1986). The present controversy suggests a dissociation between sex attribution and age categorization, just as the study of Bruyer & Dujeux (I 988) suggested a dissociation between sex attribution and race categorization. The single tasks revealed two other points. Firstly, as expected, age judgment was affected by the ambiguity, i.e., for ambiguous faces, age decisions were as slow as familiarity decisions. Secondly, it confirmed that the familiarity decisions are not influenced by age. Whatever the kind of task and for both dependent variables, the pattern emerging from the conditional tasks was clear: the familiar faces were processed better than the unfamiliar and the unambiguous faces better than the ambiguous. Thus, there was no sign of a difference between the two tasks, which was to be expected since, in both cases, 75% of stimuli gave rise to “disjunctive decisions” and one quarter of trials should have induced “conjunctive judgments”. The ambiguity of age affected both conditional tasks. Thus, when the subject could “choose” either the apparent age or the familiarity of faces (disjunctive responses), he/she voted for age and was therefore affected by the age ambiguity. Such a choice would be plausible if one recalls that, in general, age decisions are easier and faster than familiarity decisions. However, when the subject was forced to process both age and familiarity (conjunctive responses), he/she was still affected by the ambiguity. Finally, the analysis of the ambiguity index supports a sequential model in which age decision is an early visual stage within the series of operations aimed at recognizing the face.

33

General

Discussion

and Conclusion

The extraction of age precedes the familiarity decision, at least for unfamiliar or unambiguous faces. Face familiarity affects the judgment of age, at least when unambiguous faces are employed. The single tasks could suggest that age ambiguity does not influence the familiarity decision, but the conditional tasks show that it does. This leads us to conclude that age evaluation is a visual operation that precedes and is mandatory for an efficient recognition process, and that the output of the recognition stage can affect, in a top-down way, the evaluation of the apparent age (at least when the evaluation is easy). In other words, since familiarity was considered as celebrity in the present study, the subjects could respond more easily to the age question concerning familiar faces because they already knew the age: this is precisely the meaning of a familiarity effect. This statement is paradoxical, since it suggests that an operation (age decision) that must be completed before the next one begins (familiarity decision) can be influenced back by this next operation. This paradox reveals the weakness of strictly serial models, but it disappears when a more dynamic, cascade model, framework is accepted (McClelland, 1979). Gender categorization, age evaluation, and race classification are often taken as examples of semantic codes that are derived from the visual processing of faces, familiar or not 1986) and, in this respect, they differ from the “deep” semantic (Bruce & Young, representations, like activities or addresses, which are not visually derivable and concern derived semantic codes”, is conceived as familiar faces only. This box, the “visually activated only when the task requires it and is considered not necessary for efficient recognition and these visual computations function in parallel with the visual operations leading to recognition. As regards gender categorization, the data of Bruce (I 986; Bruce et al., 1987) seem to support this view. However, as regards age evaluation, our data does not support this view. We suggest that these three operations should not be maintained in a single box. Firstly, there are cases of prosopagnosia (reviewed in the Introduction) in which one of these operations is defective while the others are not. Secondly, the work of Bruce and her coworkers indicates that sex categorization operates independently from the recognition process and, according to Roberts & Bruce (1988), both operations depend on different facial features. On the other hand, our present data indicate that age evaluation is dependent on the recognition process. Thirdly, Bruyer & Dujeux (1988) suggested that sex classification should be dissociated from race categorization. Finally, the age decision seems to be affected by face familiarity while the other operations are not. Therefore, it is proposed that the age decision differs from the other visually derived semantic processes. Such a dissociation is plausible for structural reasons. Sex and race are definitive semantic properties while age is not. Moreover, the structural encoding preceding the access to face recognition units is conceived as the extraction of invariant properties of the faces (Bruce, 1988; Bruce & Young, 1986), i.e., the face must be recognized irrespective of its distance, pose or expression. In this respect, one can imagine that the structural encoding elaborates a representation independent of the apparent age. Finally, this apparent age must be kept independent of facial expression (and facial actions), since it denotes slow and irreversible modifications, which is not the case with facial expressions.

References Beyn,

E.S.

and Knyazeva, G.R., 1962. The

Neurosurgery,

and Psychiatry,

25: 154-I

58.

problem

of prosopagnosia.

Journal

of Neurology,

34 Bruce, V., 1986.

Influence of familiarity

Bruce, V., 1988.

Recognizing Faces. Erlbaum,

Bruce, V., Burton,

London,

H., Cibling,

Further

Perception and Psychophysics,

F. and Young, A., 1987.

15: 387-397.

154 pp.

M., Doyle, T. and Dench, N., 1989.

growth in three dimensions. Bruce, V., Ellis,

on the processing of faces. Perception, experiments

on the perception of

46: 528-536.

Parallel processing of the sex and familiarity

of

faces. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41: 51 O-520. Bruce, V. and Young, A., 1986. 305-327. Bruyer, R. and Dujeux,

J.M.,

Langage et I’Homme, Bruyer,

Understanding 1988.

face recognition.

British

Sex and race categorization

Journal of Psychology,

of laterally

displayed

77:

faces. Le

23: 99-107.

R. and Lafalize,

A., 1989.

Stimulus-age

effect on the episodic recognition of familiar and

unfamiliar faces. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive (European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology),

9:

391-400. Bruyer,

R. and Schweich, M., 1987.

of laterally displayed unknown Bruyer,

Race categorization and face recognition stages in the processing faces. Cortex, 23: 415-429.

R., Rectem, D. and Dupuis,

M., 1986.

Various kinds of face familiarity

and a short report on

a case of prosopagnosia. Psychologica Belgica, 26: 221-225. Cole, M. and Perez-Cruet,

J., 1964.

Prosopagnosia.

Neuropsychologia,

De Renzi, E., Bonacini, M.D. and Faglioni, P., 1989. at assessing the age of a face. Neuropsychologia, Ellis,

H., 1983.

Academic Press, London,

I., Gloning, K. and Quatember,

Neuropsychologia, Jones, B., 1979.

Erlbaum, R., 1970.

Hillsdale

Functions

pp. 33-64.

Processes underlying face recognition. In: R. Bruyer (Editor),

Face Perception and Facial Expression. Cloning,

27: 839-848.

The role of the right hemisphere in face perception. In: A. Young (Editor),

of the Right Cerebral Hemisphere. Ellis, H., 1986.

2: 237-246.

Right posterior brain-damaged patients are poor

The Neuropsychology

of

(New Jersey), pp. 1-27.

A case of “Prosopagnosia”

with necropsy findings.

8: 199-204.

Sex and visual field effects on accuracy and decision making when subjects classify

male and female faces. Cortex, 15: 551-560. Jones, B., 1980.

Sex and handedness as factors in visual field organization for a categorization task.

Journal of Experimental Mark,

L.S.

and Todd,

Psychophysics,

Psychology:

J.T.,

1983.

geometric invariants. Tassinari,

Describing perceptual information

Perception and Psychophysics,

J.L.,

Perception

and

D., Tressoldi,

1979.

about human growth in terms of

37: 249-256.

P., Barry, C. and Grabowska, A., 1985.

in face perception tasks of different cognitive requirement, McClelland,

6: 494-500.

33: 193-196.

Mark, L.S. and Todd, J.T., 1985. Marzi, CA.,

Human Perception and Performance,

The perception of growth in three dimensions.

On the time-relations

Hemispheric

Human Neurobiology,

of mental processes:

asymmetry

4: 15-20.

an examination

of systems

of

processes in cascade. Psychological Review, 86: 287-330. Michel,

F.,

Perenin,

M.T.

and S&off,

unilaterale occipito-temporale Newcombe,

F. and Russell,

W.R.,

lesions of the right hemisphere. Read, J.D.,

Experimental T.

Psychology:

and Bruce,

Perception, Seamon, J.D.,

1986.

Prosopagnosie

1969.

R., 1990.

sans hemianopsie

apres lesion

545-549.

Neurosurgery,

and Psychiatry,

32: 73-81.

Changing photos of faces: effects of exposure

on recognition and the accuracy-confidence relationship.

Learning, Memory and Cognition,

V.,

142:

Dissociated visual perceptual and spatial deficits in focal

Journal of Neurology,

Vokey, J.R. and Hammersley,

duration and photo similarity Roberts,

E.,

droite. Revue Neurologique,

1988.

Feature

saliency

Journal of

16: 870-882.

in judging the sex and familiarity

of faces.

examination of a natural phenomenon.

American

17: 475-481. 1982.

Dynamic facial recognition:

Journal of Psychology, 95: 363-381. Sergent, J., 1985.

Influence of task and input factors on hemispheric involvement in face processing.

Journal of Experimental

Psychology:

Human Perception and Performance,

11: 846-861.

35 Sergent, J. and Villemure, 112:

J.D.,

1989.

Prosopagnosia in a right hemispherectomized

patient. Brain,

975-995.

Sergent, J. and Poncet, M., patient. Brain, 113: Tiberghien,

989-l

1990.

G. and Clerc, I., 1986.

Neuropsychology

From covert to overt recognition of faces in a prosopagnosic

004. The cognitive locus of prosopagnosia. In: R. Bruyer (Editor),

of Face Perception and Facial Expression.

Erlbaum,

Hillsdale

The

(New Jersey), pp.

39-62. Tranel,

D., Damasio, A. and Damasio, H., 1988.

Intact recognition of facial expression,

age in patients with impaired recognition of face identity. Neurology, Umilta, C., Brizzolara,

D., Tabossi,

the cerebral hemispheres: mance VII. Wiley, Whiteley,

P. and Fairweather,

familiarity

H., 1978.

gender, and

38: 690-696.

Factors affecting face recognition in

and naming. In: J. Requin (Editor),

Attention

and Perfor-

New York, pp. 363-374.

A.M. and Warrington,

E.K., 1977.

study of three cases. Journal of Neurology,

Prosopagnosia: a clinical, psychological, and anatomical Neurosurgery,

and Psychiatry,

40: 395-403.