Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
Age-related decline of auditory function in the chinchilla (Chinchilla laniger) Sandra L. McFadden a *, Pierre Campo b , Nicola Quaranta c , Donald Henderson ;
a
a
Center for Hearing and Deafness, Hearing Research Laboratories, Department of Communicative Disorders, 215 Parker Hall, SUNY at Bu¡alo, Bu¡alo, NY 14214, USA
b
INRS, Service de Physiologie de l'Environnement, Avenue de Bourgogne, 54501 Vandoeuvre, France
c
Department of Audiology and Otology, University of Bari, Bari, Italy
Received 14 January 1997; revised 28 May 1997; accepted 30 May 1997
Abstract
The aim of this study was to examine the functional consequences of aging in the chinchilla, a rodent with a relatively long life span and a range of hearing similar to that of humans. Subjects were 21 chinchillas aged 10^15 years, and 23 young controls. Thresholds were determined from auditory evoked potentials (EVPs), and outer hair cell (OHC) functioning was assessed by measuring 2f1 f2 distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs). Six cochleas from 11^12-year-old animals were examined for hair cell loss and gross strial pathology. The results show that the chinchilla exhibits a small but significant decline of auditory sensitivity and OHC functioning between 3 and 15 years of age, with high-frequency losses exceeding and growing more rapidly than low-frequency losses. Compared to rodents with shorter life spans, the chinchilla has a rate of loss that is more similar to that of humans, which could make it a valuable model for understanding the etiology of human presbycusis.
3
Presbycusis; Age-related hearing loss; Auditory evoked potential; Distortion product otoacoustic emission; Cochlear pathology Keywords :
1. Introduction
Presbycusis refers to a constellation of age-related auditory de¢cits that include a loss of hearing sensitivity and a decreased ability to understand speech, particularly in the presence of background noise (Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging, 1988). Once hearing loss begins to occur in adulthood, it tends to become more pronounced and accelerated with each passing decade, with high-frequency losses exceeding low-frequency losses at all ages (Corso, 1963; Glorig and Nixon, 1962; Jerger, 1973; Hinchcli¡e, 1959; Pearson et al., 1995 ; Spoor, 1967). Data from large populations screened for noise exposure and otologic disease (e.g., International Organization for Standardization; Pearson et al., 1995) show small ( 5 dB) but statisti-
6
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 (716) 829-2001; Fax: +1 (716) 829-2980; E-mail:
[email protected]
cally signi¢cant losses of sensitivity as early as age 20^ 30 in males. However, losses are likely to become clinically signi¢cant, i.e. to exceed 20 dB hearing level (HL), only after age 50. Beginning around age 70^75, clinically signi¢cant losses can be seen in males at low frequencies ( 4 kHz) as well as high frequencies (4^8 kHz), with losses at 8 kHz exceeding 50 dB HL. Age-related hearing de¢cits in humans have commonly been attributed to histopathological changes in the cochlea, including a loss of sensory cells, atrophy of the stria vascularis, and a loss of spiral ganglion cells (Bredberg, 1968; Johnsson and Hawkins, 1972; Nadol, 1980 ; Schuknecht, 1989 ; Schuknecht and Gacek, 1993). However, presbycusis also involves changes in the central auditory system that may be either independent of cochlear pathology or secondary to it, as shown by numerous studies with animals (e.g. Boettcher et al., 1993, 1995, 1996; Caspary et al., 1995 ; McFadden and Willott, 1994a,b; Walton et al., 1995 ; Willott,
0378-5955 / 97 / $17.00 ß 1997 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved PII S 0 3 7 8 - 5 9 5 5 ( 9 7 ) 0 0 0 9 9 - 3
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
6
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
115
1984, 1996a,b ; Willott et al., 1988, 1991, 1992, 1993).
olds can decline signi¢cantly (Mills et al., 1990). The
The etiological mechanisms and functional consequen-
C57 mouse is essentially deaf long before reaching the
ces of age-related changes throughout the auditory sys-
end of its 2-year life expectancy (Willott, 1991), with
tem are beginning to be elucidated as a result of re-
thresholds
search
exceeding
80
dB
SPL
by
15^18
months
and
(Henry, 1983 ; Hunter and Willott, 1987). In contrast,
complexity of changes that can and do occur with aging
the average gerbil, whose life expectancy is approxi-
(see Willott, 1991 for a review), no one animal is likely
mately 36 months (Adams and Schulte, 1997 ; Schmiedt
to serve as an ideal model of human presbycusis. In-
et al., 1990), is likely to have relatively minor losses
stead, a full understanding of presbycusis will require
(typically
research using a wide variety of animal models and
(Boettcher et al., 1993, 1995, 1996 ; Mills et al., 1990 ;
diverse experimental approaches.
Schmiedt et al., 1990).
using
animal
models.
Given
the
range
Two of the best characterized models of human pres-
less
than
30
dB)
even
at
36^38
months
Potentially important di¡erences in pattern, magni-
and
tude, and rate of age-related hearing loss can be ob-
Schulte, 1997 ; Boettcher et al., 1993, 1995, 1996 ; Grat-
served among humans, C57 mice, and Mongolian ger-
ton et al., 1995, 1996 ; Hellstrom and Schmiedt, 1990 ;
bils. To facilitate comparisons among the three species,
Mills et al., 1990, 1997 ; Schmiedt et al., 1990, 1996 ;
Fig. 1 shows average hearing loss of 12-month-old C57
Schulte and Schmiedt, 1992 ; Tarnowski et al., 1991)
mice, 36-month-old gerbils, and human males aged 50^
and the C57 mouse (e.g. Henry, 1983 ; Hunter and Wil-
80 years. It is important to note for these comparisons
lott, 1987 ; McFadden and Willott, 1994a,b ; Mikaelian,
that the current life expectancy of the human male liv-
1979 ; Walton et al., 1995 ; Willott, 1984, 1991, 1996a,b ;
ing in the USA is 72 years (National Center for Health
Willott et al., 1988, 1991, 1992, 1993, 1994, 1995).
Statistics), compared to 24 months for the C57 mouse
These two rodent species have speci¢c features that
and 36 months for the Mongolian gerbil. Thus, in terms
make them valuable as models of human presbycusis
of percent of life span achieved (the usual metric for
(see Mills et al., 1990 ; Willott, 1991), and most previous
comparing hearing loss over time between animals and
research has focused on the similarities between them
humans ; see Willott, 1991), 12-month-old C57 mice can
and humans. However, di¡erences may be instructive to
be considered to correspond to 36-year-old males, and
consider as well, as they could provide important clues
36-month-old gerbils to 72-year-old males.
bycusis
are
the
Mongolian
gerbil
(e.g.
Adams
regarding the etiology of presbycusis in humans.
As shown in Fig. 1, hearing loss in human males is
The gerbil has an audibility curve similar to that of
typically characterized by high-frequency loss that ex-
humans, with greatest sensitivity around 4 kHz (Mills et
ceeds and grows more rapidly than low-frequency loss.
al., 1990 ; Ryan, 1976). The C57 mouse has an audibil-
The C57 mouse exhibits an audiometric pattern similar
ity curve that is shifted upward by several octaves, with
to this, although the a¡ected frequencies are shifted
greatest sensitivity around 16 kHz (Henry and Chole,
upward by several octaves. In contrast, the gerbil ex-
1980 ;
Willott,
hibits a £at loss of hearing from 1 to 16 kHz. At 12
Walton et al., 1995). The
months of age, the average C57 mouse has developed
Mongolian gerbil is an outbred strain, and is therefore
losses ranging from 20 dB at 2 and 4 kHz, to 45 dB at
genetically diverse, like the human. As a result, varia-
32 kHz. At 36 months of age, the average gerbil has lost
bility in the magnitude of hearing loss that gerbils de-
approximately 20 dB of sensitivity at octave frequencies
velop over their life span is extremely high, with some
from 1 to 16 kHz. Thus, if the hearing curve for the
animals exhibiting no signi¢cant loss between 16^18
C57 mouse is shifted downward by two octaves, then
months (the age at which hearing loss is likely to begin)
12-month-old
C57
and 36 months, and others exhibiting losses that exceed
quantitatively
similar
the limits of measurement (Mills et al., 1990, 1996a,b).
(Fig. 1). Gerbils at 36 months of age are most like
In contrast to gerbils and humans, all individuals of the
70^80-year-old males in the magnitude of their low-fre-
C57 strain are genetically identical. As a result, any
quency loss, but most like 50-year-old males in the
di¡erences that are observed among individuals can
magnitude
be attributed to environmental factors and/or interac-
points in their life span, i.e. near the end of the life
tions between environmental factors and speci¢c, ho-
expectancy, gerbils have approximately 20^40 dB less
mogeneous genetic backgrounds (Erway et al., 1993 ;
high-frequency loss than 70-year-old males. The di¡er-
Erway and Willott, 1996 ; Erway and Willott, 1996 ;
ences will become even larger in humans and gerbils
Willott et al., 1995).
that live beyond their average life expectancy.
1994a ;
Li
and
Borg,
1991 ;
Mikaelian, 1979 ;
McFadden
and
of
mice to
are
both
human
high-frequency
qualitatively
males
loss.
At
in
their
and 60s
comparable
Hearing loss in C57 mice typically begins in young
The di¡erences in magnitude of loss between the ro-
adulthood, around 1^2 months of age (Henry, 1983 ;
dent models and humans are not simply due to di¡er-
Hunter and Willott, 1987 ; Li and Borg, 1991). In con-
ences in chronological age. Rather, they result from
trast, the gerbil typically maintains normal hearing sen-
fundamental di¡erences in the rate of decline among
sitivity until 16^18 months of age, after which thresh-
the species, that may be relevant for understanding
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
116
The di¡erences in onset, pattern, magnitude, and rate of hearing loss among humans, gerbils and C57 mice imply di¡erences in underlying etiological mechanisms. Cochlear pathology and hearing loss in the C57 mouse are hypothesized to result from a single gene (Erway et al., 1993 ; Erway and Willott, 1996). It is likely that genetic factors also contribute to age-related pathology
and
hearing
loss
in
gerbils
(Adams
and
Schulte, 1997 ; Mills et al., 1996b) and humans (Schuknecht, 1974 ; Willott, 1996a), with the genes either acting directly (e.g. by triggering degenerative processes at particular periods in the life span), or indirectly (e.g. by a¡ecting susceptibility to environmental in£uences, the most obvious ones being ototoxic drugs and noise) (Erway and Willott, 1996 ;
Steel et al., 1983 ;
Willott,
1996a). The fast rate of decline in quiet-aged gerbils that are not exposed to noise or ototoxic chemicals is consistent with direct gene actions. On the other hand, the slow rate of age-related decline typically seen in humans suggests that indirect gene actions or other factors may be involved. Thus, one hypothesis to acFig. 1. Comparison of average hearing loss in 36-month-old Mon-
count for di¡erences between humans, gerbils and C57
golian gerbils, 12-month-old C57 mice, and human males aged 50^
mice in the rate of loss is that age-related threshold
80 years. Values for gerbils are based on mean age-related threshold shifts
reported
in
¢ve
recent
studies
by
Mills
and
colleagues
shifts in the gerbil and C57 mouse primarily re£ect
(Boettcher et al., 1993, 1995, 1996 ; Mills et al., 1990, 1997). Values
direct gene actions, whereas shifts in humans depend
shown for C57 mice are based on thresholds reported by Henry
on more complex interactions between genetics and a
(1983), Li and Borg (1991), McFadden and Willott (1994a), and Mi-
life time of exposure to environmental noise and vari-
kaelian (1979). Values shown for human males were derived from
ous ototoxic agents.
Spoor (1967) (open symbols) and the more recent Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging data from a population screened for noise
The hypothesis is consistent with the notion that
exposure and otologic disease, reported by Pearson et al. (1995)
many degenerative changes associated with aging result
(¢lled symbols).
from the accumulation of cellular damage over time (Cohen, 1988 ; Hay£ick, 1977, 1985). Cellular damage could be caused by processes such as free radical for-
the etiological mechanisms underlying their age-related
mation or waste-product accumulation, that are associ-
pathologies. The 10^60 dB of hearing loss observed in
ated both with normal cellular function and with re-
males at age 70 develops over a chronological time
sponses of the system to environmental stresses (Beal,
period of 30^50 years, depending on frequency (Pearson
1995 ; Harman, 1986). It is possible that the progression
et al., 1995). Low-frequency losses occur at rates be-
of presbycusis seen in humans re£ects time-related ac-
tween 0.2 and 0.8 dB HL/year, and high-frequency
cumulation of damage caused by normal (genetically
losses occur at rates between 0.8 and 2 dB HL/year.
determined) metabolic processes, even in the absence
The £at 20 dB of hearing loss seen in 36-month-old
of exposure to extrinsic agents such as acoustic over-
gerbils develops over a chronological time period of
stimulation and ototoxic chemicals, that increasingly
1.5^1.7 years (i.e. between 16^18 months and 36 months
compromises cellular adaptive or compensatory mech-
of age), which translates to a rate of 12^13 dB HL/year.
anisms (Cotman and Peterson, 1989).
Hearing loss occurs most rapidly in C57 mice, with
The
importance
of
time-related
accumulation
of
losses shown in Fig. 1 developing over a chronological
damage versus time-independent, genetically triggered
period of 0.8^0.9 years (i.e. from 1^2 months to 12
degenerative processes or other factors in the etiology
months), which translates to rates ranging from 22^56
of presbycusis is di¤cult, if not impossible, to evaluate
dB HL/year, depending on frequency. Thus, when the
with current rodent models. One species that might be
rate of hearing loss is referenced to chronological aging,
able to provide insight into the importance of chrono-
it is seen to occur slowly (up to 2 dB HL/year) in hu-
logical aging is the chinchilla (Chinchilla
mans, rapidly (12^13 dB HL/year) in Mongolian ger-
outbred rodent species that, like the Mongolian gerbil
bils, and very rapidly (22^56 dB HL/year) in C57 mice.
(Mills et al., 1990), has a range of hearing similar to
With respect to the chronological rate of age-related
that of humans (He¡ner and He¡ner, 1991 ; Miller,
decline in auditory sensitivity, neither the C57 mouse
1970). Although the average life expectancy of the chin-
nor the gerbil is an adequate model for the human.
chilla has not been clearly established, as it has for
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
laniger),
an
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
117
short-lived laboratory rodents such as mice and gerbils,
cated next to a cemetery in a rural area of Rochester,
there is no question that it exceeds 8 years. Literature
MN. The noise history of the animals was not docu-
from
Chinchilla
mented by the breeder, but it is unlikely that they were
Ranch) states that the average life span of the chinchilla
exposed to unusually loud or damaging levels of noise
is 8^12 years, with some animals living to 22 years.
during their life times. Nevertheless, it is possible that
Bohne and colleagues (Bohne et al., 1990 ; Sun et al.,
some, or even all, of the decrements observed in the
1994) have reported that the life span of the chinchilla
aged animals were related to their noise exposure his-
is 15^20 years, and Clark (1984) lists the life span as
tory, even if noise levels were not particularly high
12^20 years. Thus, the chinchilla provides an unusual
(Schmiedt and Schulte, 1992). We do not consider this
opportunity to examine the in£uence of endogenous
a problem for two reasons. First, exposure to environ-
and exogenous factors linked to chronological aging.
mental noises may be one trigger of the cellular meta-
one
commercial
breeder
(Moulton
We are unaware of any published studies describing
bolic processes that produce age-related hearing loss in
the hearing sensitivity of aged chinchillas. However,
humans and some animals. Second, based on the ob-
histological data obtained by Bohne et al. (1990) sug-
servation by Bohne et al. (1990) that cochlear histopa-
gest that chinchillas may exhibit a slow progression of
thology of commercially bred aged chinchillas was not
age-related hearing loss that parallels that of the hu-
di¡erent from that of colony-raised aged chinchillas, it
man. Bohne et al. observed morphological changes in
is unlikely that di¡erent results would have been ob-
21 cochleas from chinchillas aged 8^19.2 years that
tained using colony-reared animals. The aged animals we obtained were 10 (n = 2), 11
were qualitatively similar to those seen in the temporal bones of aging humans. Beginning after approximately
(n = 12), 12 (n = 4), 14 (n = 2), and 15 (n = 1) years old,
3 years of age, chinchillas manifest sensory, neural and
with a mean age of 11.6 years (S.D. = 1.3). The precise
strial cochlear pathology that becomes progressively
ages of the young animals were not known. However,
worse with age. At the same time, the histological
as none of the 23 young animals was older than 3 years,
changes observed by Bohne were relatively minor. If
cochlear pathology was assumed to be minimal or ab-
the histological changes are associated with signi¢cant
sent in this group (Bohne et al., 1990 ; Bhattacharyya
functional de¢cits, then the chinchilla may provide an
and Dayal, 1985).
important perspective on the importance of time-related
Each animal was deeply anesthetized with ketamine
accumulation of damage in the etiology of presbycusis.
(0.56 mg/kg) and acepromazine (36 mg/kg), and chronic
This study describes EVP thresholds and input/out-
recording electrodes were surgically implanted in the
32
f ) distortion product
left and/or right inferior colliculus (IC). A small hole
otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) for 21 aged chinchil-
was drilled in the dorsal cranium overlying the IC, and
las (10^15 years) and 23 young controls (
a
put (I/O) functions of cubic (2f1
9
3 years).
tungsten
electrode
was
stereotaxically
lowered
The cubic DPOAE is the largest and most commonly
through the hole while the surgeon monitored sound-
measured distortion product generated by the cochlea
evoked electrical activity on audio and video monitors.
in response to stimulation by two primary tones, f1 and
When the electrode had been advanced to a depth that
f2 . Cubic DPOAEs appear to depend on outer hair cell
produced clear, large-amplitude EVPs, it was cemented
(OHC) functioning (Brownell, 1990 ; Trautwein et al.,
to the skull with cyanoacrylic adhesive and dental ce-
1996), and have been used to assess the condition of the
ment. A second electrode was implanted in the rostral
OHC system in both aged (Lonsbury-Martin et al.,
cranium just below the dura mater to serve as the
1991) and noise-damaged (Avan et al., 1996 ; Hamernik
ground lead for EVP recording. Ten young and six
et al., 1996) ears. In addition to the two physiological
aged animals were implanted with a single electrode
measures,
animals
in the left IC ; the rest were implanted bilaterally. Fol-
were examined for evidence of hair cell loss and gross
lowing surgery, animals were allowed to recover in the
stria
cochleas
from
six
11^12-year-old
de-
quiet animal colony for 1^3 weeks before testing. All
scribes the results of suprathreshold tests of auditory
procedures regarding the use and care of the animals
function performed on a subset of these young and
were reviewed and approved by the University at Buf-
aged animals.
falo Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and
vascularis
pathology.
A
companion
paper
conformed to state and federal guidelines for the humane treatment of animals. 2. Methods
2.2. Stimuli and procedures for measuring evoked potential thresholds
2.1. Subjects aged chinchillas
All testing was conducted in a sound-attenuating
from a commercial breeder
booth (Industrial Acoustics Corp.) lined with sound-
(Moulton Chinchilla Ranch), whose facilities are lo-
absorbing foam panels. An animal was placed in a
Subjects were 23 young and 21 (Chinchilla
laniger) obtained
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
118
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
padded restraining yoke that held its head at a constant orientation within a calibrated sound ¢eld. All animals were tested while awake. Test stimuli were 10 ms tones (5 ms cosine rise/fall ramps, 10/s rate) at 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 8 and 16 kHz, generated digitally by a signal processing board (Spectrum Signal Processing TMS320C25) housed in an IBM-compatible personal computer (PC). Signals were converted to analog voltages by a 16-bit D/A converter on the board, and routed through computer-controlled attenuators and impedance matching transformers to a loudspeaker (Realistic 401197) located at a distance of approximately 38 cm in front of the animal's head. Output from the recording electrode was ampli¢ed (U20 000), ¢ltered (10^3000 Hz), and routed to an A/D converter on a separate board in the computer. Tone bursts were presented in ascending order of frequency and intensity (5 dB steps) until clearly de¢ned suprathreshold responses were obtained. Each animal was tested on three separate occasions, and the three threshold estimates were averaged for a stable estimate of sensitivity as a function of frequency. To determine thresholds, stored waveforms were visually analyzed by three independent raters, each of whom was blind as to the age of the animal. Threshold at each frequency was de¢ned as the mid-point between the lowest level at which a clear response was elicited and the next lower level, in a 5 dB step, at which no response could be discerned. The average of estimates made by the three raters was used as the threshold measure for each animal. 2.3. Stimuli and procedures for obtaining DPOAE input/output functions
DPOAEs were measured in a sound-attenuating booth (Industrial Acoustics Corp.) lined with soundabsorbing foam panels, using a low noise microphone (Etymotic ER10B). Animals were placed in a customdesigned restraining device and tested while awake. The two tones used to elicit the DPOAE were generated by separate digital signal processing boards (Spectrum Signal Processing TMS320C25) in a PC, low-pass ¢ltered (TDK HFL0030, roll-o¡ 90 dB between 20 and 24 kHz), attenuated by custom-designed computer-controlled attenuators, ampli¢ed, and delivered from sound sources (Etymotic ER2) coupled to the microphone through a narrow tube. Output of the microphone was processed by a third signal processing board in the PC. Microphone output was digitized by a 16-bit A/D converter and sampled for 500 ms at a sampling rate of 31 kHz. Measurements made in a hard-walled cavity indicated that the distortion in the measurement system was less than 2 dB SPL for primary tone levels of 80 dB SPL. I/O functions for f2 primaries of 1.2, 2.4, 3.6, 4.8, 7.2,
9.6, and 12 kHz were recorded in 5 dB steps from 0 to 70^80 dB SPL, using an f2/f1 ratio of 1.2, and a level di¡erence of 15 dB (L1 s L2). Parameters for the primary tones were selected on the basis of a parametric study (unpublished data) conducted with young normal-hearing animals, showing that the largest amplitude responses are typically elicited using a f2 /f1 ratio of 1.2 and a level di¡erence of 10^15 dB. Three I/O functions were obtained from each ear of each animal and averaged. 2.4. Histology
Two young and six 11^12-year-old animals were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg i.p.) and decapitated. The rest of the animals were subsequently used in another experiment, and their cochleas were therefore unavailable for histological examination. The cochleas were quickly removed from the temporal bone, the ossicles were removed, and the round window membranes were perforated. The cochleas were gently perfused through the round window membranes with 2.5% glutaraldehyde in veronal acetate bu¡er (pH 7.3^7.4), stored in cold ¢xative for a minimum of 12 h, post-¢xed with 1% osmium tetroxide bu¡er for 1 h, then rinsed in veronal acetate bu¡er. The cochleas were dissected in 70% EtOH, beginning at the apex and proceeding toward the base. After the bony capsule had been removed, the stria vascularis and spiral ligament were carefully separated from the rest of the cochlea using a 31-gauge syringe needle as a knife. The three sections of strial tissue were either mounted in glycerin on glass microscope slides and coverslipped as a surface preparation, or embedded in resin and sectioned in 4^5 Wm sections for comparisons of tissue thickness. Dissection of the organ of Corti was completed by removing Reissner's membrane and the tectorial membrane, and then dissecting the tissue away in three turns. The organ of Corti sections were mounted in glycerin on glass microscope slides and coverslipped. Strial tissue samples from young and aged cochleas were observed with a light microscope (Zeiss Axioskop). Specimens were evaluated for qualitative di¡erences in stria vascularis thickness, extent of vascularization, diameter of blood vessels, and accumulation of pigmented granules. No attempt was made to quantify the di¡erences in stria vascularis between young and aged specimens in the small sample available. Surface preparations of the organ of Corti from the aged animals were evaluated using a Nomarski di¡erential interference contrast microscope (Zeiss). The number of missing inner hair cells (IHCs) and OHCs in each 0.24 mm segment of the organ of Corti was determined for each subject, and individual cochleograms were constructed to show the percentage of
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
hair cells missing as a function of distance from the apex. Then, an average cochleogram was constructed to show percent loss within each 10% section of the cochlea for the 6 aged ears. The standard reference values for cochleograms in our lab are averages of hair cell counts obtained from cochleas of 9 young (approximately 6-month-old) chinchillas.
119
2.5. Data analyses
Table 1 Summary of results from linear regression analyses of threshold as a function of age Frequency (kHz) R Slope Intercept P 0.5 0.4106 0.84 15.75 0.0005 1 0.4500 1.10 7.55 0.0001 2 0.4160 1.09 4.27 0.0004 4 0.4490 0.96 33.13 0.0001 8 0.6429 2.00 36.48 6 0.0001 16 0.6150 2.10 30.20 6 0.0001
Data analyses were geared toward answering the following questions: (1) Are EVP thresholds of aged animals elevated relative to those of young animals? (2) If so, what is the rate of sensitivity loss in older animals? (3) Are DPOAE I/O functions of aged animals depressed relative to those of young animals? Mean thresholds of the young and aged groups were compared using one-way ANOVAs at each frequency. Linear regression analyses were used to determine the rate of threshold elevation as a function of age at each frequency. For the linear regression analyses, all young animals were assumed to be 3 years of age. Thus, the regressions spanned a 12-year range, from 3 to 15 years of age. DPOAE data were compared by computing the 95% con¢dence interval for the young animals. All statistical tests were evaluated using a 0.05 criterion of signi¢cance.
variability, one-way ANOVAs indicated that thresholds of aged (mean = 11.6 years; S.D. = 1.3) chinchillas were signi¢cantly higher than those of young controls at all frequencies (all P values 6 0.001). Elevations were greater at high frequencies (8 and 16 kHz) than at lower frequencies. Linear regression analyses were performed to quantify the relationship between age and loss of sensitivity in our sample. The results are summarized in Table 1. Age accounted for a signi¢cant proportion (up to 41% at 8 kHz) of the variance in threshold data at every frequency. Loss of sensitivity at low frequencies (0.5^4 kHz) occurred at rates between 0.84 and 1.1 dB HL/ year, whereas losses at 8 and 16 kHz occurred at rates of 2.0 and 2.1 dB HL/year, respectively. 3.2. DPOAE input/output functions
3. Results
3.1. Evoked potential thresholds
Mean EVP thresholds of aged chinchillas (n = 21) and young controls (n = 23) are shown in Fig. 2. The shaded regions, representing the 95% con¢dence interval for each group, show that variability was greater among the older group of animals. Despite considerable
Fig. 2. Mean evoked potential thresholds (dB SPL) of young (n =23) and aged (n = 21) animals as a function of frequency. Shaded regions represent 95% con¢dence intervals for each group.
Mean DPOAE input/output functions for f2 frequencies of 1.2, 2.4, 3.6, 4.8, 7.2, 9.6 and 12 kHz are shown in Fig. 3. The thin line represents the mean values for 17 young normal-hearing chinchillas, and the hatched area shows the 95% con¢dence interval. The thick solid line represents the mean values for 15 aged (mean = 12.2 years) animals. The noise £oor, represented by the lowlevel (0^20 dB input levels) portion of the I/O function, was approximately 38 dB at the lowest frequency (f2 = 1.2 kHz), and 312 dB or less at all higher frequencies. DPOAE threshold was de¢ned as the input level at which the amplitude of the DPOAE exceeded the noise £oor by at least 3 dB. DPOAE thresholds of young animals ranged from 35 dB at f2 = 1.2 kHz to 20 dB at f2 = 12 kHz. Thresholds of aged animals were 5^10 dB higher than thresholds of young animals at most frequencies. DPOAE amplitudes of aged animals were signi¢cantly lower than amplitudes of young animals at all frequencies tested. This was true both for mean data (shown in Fig. 3) and for functions from individual aged animals, i.e. every aged animal tested had decreased DPOAE amplitudes relative to young controls. Furthermore, amplitudes of DPOAEs from aged animals were reduced at all input levels above threshold.
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
120
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
Fig. 3. Input/output functions for 2f1 3f2 DPOAE at seven frequencies. The thin solid line represents the mean amplitude for young animals (n=17), and the hatched region around the mean represents the 95% con¢dence interval. The thick solid line represents means for the aged animals (n = 15).
For example, at an input level of 60 dB SPL, mean amplitudes of DPOAEs of young animals were between 7 and 11 dB higher than amplitudes of aged animals.
3.3. Cochlear histopathology
Mean cytocochleograms for six cochleas from ani-
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
Fig. 4. Mean cytocochleograms of six aged (11-12-year-old) chinchillas, showing percent hair cell loss as a function of distance along the basilar membrane (10% segments, measured from the apex of the cochlea). A: Outer hair cell losses shown separately for each row. B: Inner hair cell losses.
mals aged 11^12 years are shown in Fig. 4. OHC loss (A) and IHC loss (B) are expressed in percent loss for each 10% segment of the organ of Corti from apex to
121
base, relative to nine cochleas from young animals (see Section 2). The average length of the six aged cochleas was 19.85 ( þ 0.9) mm, compared to our mean reference length of 19.45 ( þ 0.7) mm for young ears. For the six aged cochleas, OHC losses in rows 2 and 3 exceeded losses in row 1 (Fig. 4A), particularly in the apical half of the cochlea, where di¡erences were in the order of 5^10%. OHC losses exceeded 15% only for rows 2 and 3 in the extreme apex (0^10% distance from apex) and for all three rows in the base (90^ 100% distance from apex). The largest OHC lesions were observed in the base, with OHC losses (averaged across rows) ranging from 6% to 39% for individual animals (mean = 28.5%). IHC losses did not exceed 6% in any region of the cochlea. Losses in the apical half of the cochlea ranged from 0 to 2.5%, whereas losses in the base ranged from 0.3% to 5.3%. As with OHCs, IHC losses were greatest in the extreme base and apex of the cochlea. The greatest loss of IHCs (5.3%) occurred at a location 90^100% distance from the apex, followed by losses of 1.8^2.5% in the apical 20% of the cochlea. Fig. 5 illustrates the appearance of the organ of Corti in the basal turn of an 11-year-old cochlea. Phalangeal scars have formed in areas of missing OHCs in all three rows and pigmented granules, which may be lipofuscin or melanin, have accumulated throughout the organ of Corti. In stria vascularis of aged animals, the most obvious di¡erence from young animals was increased size and incidence of pigmented granules, as illustrated in Fig. 6. Other qualitative di¡erences between young and aged animals included reduced vascularization and decreased diameter of blood vessels, and thinning of the strial tissue.
Fig. 5. Surface preparation of organ of Corti from the basal turn of an 11-year-old cochlea. Several OHC are missing in rows 1 and 2 (4 and 3, respectively), and phalangeal scars have formed in their place. Note the widespread distribution of pigmented granules, which may be lipofuscin or melanin, throughout the organ of Corti.
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
122
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
Fig. 6. Surface preparations of the stria vascularis from a young chinchilla (A, top) and an 11-year-old chinchilla (B, bottom). Darkly stained inclusions are larger and much more numerous in the older specimens.
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
123
4.2. Hair cell loss
4. Discussion
4.1. Age-related loss of auditory sensitivity: comparisons with gerbils and C57 mice
Pure tone thresholds of aged chinchillas were signi¢cantly elevated at all frequencies between 0.5 and 16 kHz, with threshold elevations at 8 and 16 kHz exceeding those at lower frequencies. Thus, age-related loss of sensitivity in the chinchilla is characterized by an audiometric pattern that resembles the pattern exhibited most frequently by humans with presbycusis, i.e. greater losses for high frequencies than for low frequencies (Corso, 1963; Glorig and Nixon, 1962; Hinchcli¡e, 1959; Pearson et al., 1995; Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging, 1988). In this regard, the chinchilla appears to be more similar to the C57 mouse than to the gerbil (see Fig. 1) and other rodent models of presbycusis that exhibit greater apical pathology and low-frequency hearing loss than basal pathology and high-frequency loss (Adams and Schulte, 1997; Willott, 1991). Compared to the C57 mouse, however, the chinchilla has a range of hearing that is more similar to that of humans (He¡ner and He¡ner, 1991; Miller, 1970), which may enhance its usefulness as a model for understanding some functional consequences of age-related cochlear pathology. Mean threshold shifts in our group of animals aged 10^15 years (mean = 11.6 years, S.D. = 1.3) were relatively small, ranging from 6.3 to 15.3 dB HL, depending on frequency (Fig. 2). Based on the rates shown in Table 1, chinchillas at the upper end of their life span estimate at 20 years would be expected to have losses of 13^17 dB at low frequencies (0.5^4 kHz), and 31^32 dB at high frequencies (8 and 16 kHz). Neither the measured values for 12-year-old chinchillas nor the projected values for 20-year-old chinchillas are much di¡erent from losses measured in 36-month-old gerbils or 12month-old C57 mice. However, unlike these short-lived rodents, the chinchilla has a rate of loss that approximates the rate seen in human males. Regression analyses indicated that auditory sensitivity declined at rates of 0.8^2 dB HL/year in chinchillas, versus 0.2^2 dB HL/ year for human males. Similarities in the pattern and rate of hearing loss between chinchillas and humans could re£ect similar etiological mechanisms. Thus, the chinchilla may be an excellent model for exploring the etiology, as well as the functional consequences, of agerelated hearing loss in humans. Obviously, given the time and expense involved in raising chinchillas to `old age,' they are not a practical substitute for shortlived species such as mice and gerbils for most studies of aging. However, our data suggest that despite their practical limitations, the chinchilla may be the most appropriate rodent model for elucidating the etiology of human presbycusis.
The inner and outer hair cell losses we observed in chinchillas aged 11^12 years (Fig. 3) were slightly less than would be expected from losses reported by Bohne et al. (1990). OHC losses reported by Bohne for 12 animals aged 8^11.5 years were approximately 13% in the apex (1^22% distance from apex), 7% in the 3 kHz region (51^72% distance from apex), 7% in the 6 kHz region (64^85% distance from apex), and 8% in the base (80^100% distance from apex). In Bohne's oldest age group, which consisted of nine animals aged 11.5^19.2 years, OHC losses had increased to 18%, 12%, 15% and 25%, respectively. Outer hair cell losses of our 11^12year-old animals would be expected to fall between these two sets of values. However, OHC losses in roughly equivalent regions (0^20%, 50^70%, 60^80%, and 80^100% distances) amounted to 12%, 4%, 4% and 17%, respectively. IHC losses were also slightly smaller in our sample. However, these di¡erences are minor, as both studies observed less than 7% IHC loss in all four regions of the cochlea, irrespective of age group. Despite relatively minor di¡erences in the magnitude of hair cell loss, the present study and the study by Bohne et al. (1990) provide a consistent picture of the pattern of age-related hair cell degeneration in the chinchilla. Between young adulthood (approximately 3 years) and 8^10 years, IHC and OHC losses are minor, not exceeding 6% in any region of the cochlea. At this time, losses are slightly greater in the apex than in the base. After 8^10 years, however, OHC losses begin to accelerate, particularly in the base of the cochlea. In our sample of 11^12-year-old chinchillas, OHC losses in the basal half exceeded losses in the apical half by nearly 10%. 4.3. Relationship between anatomical and physiological measures
The present data indicate that aging in the chinchilla is associated with a general decline of auditory function. Presumably, these functional de¢cits result from progressive cochlear pathology, such as hair cell loss. However, the magnitude of the age-related loss of sensitivity we observed was greater than would be expected on the basis of hair cell losses alone. Poor correlations between hair cell loss and degree of hearing loss have been noted in noise-exposed animals as well (Arehole et al., 1989; Boettcher et al., 1992; Shone et al., 1991). One possibility to account for the discrepancy is that threshold sensitivity is being a¡ected by other degenerative changes in the cochlea, such as neural degeneration and/or strial pathology (Adams and Schulte, 1997; Gratton et al., 1995; Schulte and Schmiedt, 1992). Our qualitative impressions of the stria vascularis in aged
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
124
chinchillas
support
this
notion.
A
second
possibility,
then one obvious implication for humans is that adher-
though
ing to good hearing conservation practices which min-
present, may be functionally abnormal. A third possi-
imize exposure to noise and ototoxic chemicals can re-
bility is that the central auditory system has undergone
duce the magnitude of age-related hearing loss.
not
mutually
exclusive,
is
that
hair
cells,
changes with age that are largely independent of peripheral pathology. Our DPOAE results (Fig. 2) are consistent with the hypothesis DPOAE
that
aging
thresholds
a¡ects
were
slightly
OHC elevated
Acknowledgments
functioning. (5^10
dB),
Supported in part by the Center for Hearing and
and amplitudes were signi¢cantly decreased at all fre-
Deafness
quencies tested despite relatively minor losses of OHCs.
grant
University
of
Bu¡alo,
5RO10H0115214
at
to
D.H.
and
Our data do not permit a determination of the source
like to thank Kimin Kim for assistance in data collec-
of the DPOAE de¢cits. However, several possibilities
tion and DPOAE analysis, Marty Howard for assist-
can be considered. First, DPOAE de¢cits could be re-
ance in data collection, and Vincenzo Sallustio and Da-
lated to decreased endolymphatic potential (EP) associ-
lian Ding for preparation of histological specimens.
The
by
NIDCD
authors
would
ated with strial pathology. Studies with the Mongolian gerbil have shown a strong relationship between pathologic
changes
threshold
in
the
elevations
lateral
wall,
(Boettcher
et
decreased al.,
1995 ;
EP,
and
Gratton
et al., 1995 ; Schulte and Schmiedt, 1992 ; Schmiedt et al., 1990). Second, it may be that many surviving OHCs in aged animals are functionally abnormal. Third, it is possible that the DPOAE de¢cits are related to agerelated
changes
in
the
olivocochlear
e¡erent
system,
References Adams, J.C., Schulte, B.A., 1997. Histopathologic observations of the aging gerbil cochlea. Hear. Res. 104, 101^111. Arehole, S., Salvi, R.J., Saunders, S.S., Gratton, M.A., 1989. Evokedresponse forward-masking functions in chinchillas with noise-induced permanent hearing loss. Audiology 28, 92^110. Avan, P., Bon¢ls, P., Loth, D., 1996. E¡ects of acoustic overstimula-
which has been shown to in£uence OHC electromotility
tion on distortion-product and transient-evoked otoacoustic emis-
and otoacoustic emissions (Mountain, 1980 ; Siegel and
sions. In : Axelsson, A. et al. (Eds.), Scienti¢c Basis of Noise-In-
Kim, 1982), rather than changes in the OHCs them-
duced Hearing Loss. Thieme, New York, pp. 65^81. Beal, M.F., 1995. Aging, energy, and
selves.
oxidative
stress in neurode-
generative diseases. Ann. Neurol. 38, 357^366.
4.4. Implications for understanding human presbycusis
Bhattacharyya, T.K., Dayal, V.S., 1985. Age-related cochlear hair cell loss in the chinchilla. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 94, 75^80. Boettcher,
The results of the present study show that the aging chinchilla loses auditory sensitivity at rates very similar to those seen in humans. The similarities between chinchillas and humans in the rate of loss suggest that sim-
F.A.,
Mills,
J.H.,
Dubno,
J.R.,
Schmiedt,
R.A.,
1995.
Masking of auditory brainstem response in young and aged Mongolian gerbils. Hear. Res. 89, 1^13. Boettcher, F.A., Mills, J.H., Norton, B.L., 1993. Age-related changes in auditory evoked potentials of gerbils. I. Response amplitudes. Hear. Res. 71, 137145.
ilar etiological mechanisms may be contributing to age-
Boettcher, F.A., Mills, J.H., Swerdlo¡, J.L., Holley, B.L., 1996. Audi-
related hearing loss in the two species. Thus, the chin-
tory evoked potentials in aged gerbils : responses elicited by noises
chilla could o¡er insights into human presbycusis that are not possible with other rodent models such as gerbils and C57 mice.
separated by a silent gap. Hear. Res. 102, 167^178. Boettcher, F.A., Sprongr, V.P., Salvi, R.J., 1992. Physiological and histological
changes
associated
with
the
reduction
in
threshold
shift during interrupted noise exposure. Hear. Res. 62, 217^236.
Because the chinchillas used in our study were not raised in a low-noise environment, the possibility that age-related changes were due to exposure to environmental noises cannot be ruled out. However, exposure to similar levels of noise could be an important factor
Bohne, B.A., Gruner, M.M., Harding, G.W., 1990. Morphological correlates of aging in the chinchilla cochlea. Hear. Res. 48, 79^92. Bredberg, G., 1968. Cellular pattern and nerve supply of the human organ of Corti. Acta Otolaryngol. Suppl. 236, 1^135. Brownell, W.E., 1990. Outer hair cell electromotility and otoacoustic emissions. Ears Hear. 11, 82^92.
contributing to hearing loss in humans as well (Kryter,
Caspary, D.M., Milbrandt, J.C., Helfert, R.H., 1995. Central auditory
1983, 1985 ; Mills and Going, 1982). The chinchilla of-
aging : GABA changes in the inferior colliculus. Exp. Gerontol.
fers a means for assessing the importance of chronic exposure to noise and other ototoxic agents in the etiology of age-related hearing loss. For example, if hear-
30, 349^360. Clark, J.D., 1984. Biology and diseases of other rodents. In : Fox, J.G., Cohen, R.J., Loew, F.M. (Eds.), Laboratory Anim.l Medicine, Academic Press, New York.
ing losses are greater in chinchillas raised in noise than
Cohen, G.D., 1988. The Brain in Human Aging. Springer, New York.
in chinchillas raised in quiet, this would suggest that a
Corso, J.F., 1963. Age and sex di¡erences in pure-tone thresholds.
signi¢cant proportion of human presbycusis might be due to chronic exposure to environmental noise. If age-
Arch. Otolaryngol. 77, 385^405. Cotman, C.W., Peterson, C., 1989. Aging in the nervous system. In : Siegel, G.J. et al. (Eds.), Basic Neurochemistry : Molecular, Cellu-
related hearing loss is associated with exposure to noise
lar, and Medical Aspects (4th edn.). Raven, New York, pp. 523^
and other ototoxic agents over an individual's life time,
540.
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
125
Erway, L.C., Willott, J.F., 1996. Genetic susceptibility to noise-in-
loss : E¡ects of changing the azimuthal location of a continuous
duced hearing loss in mice. In : Axelsson, A. et al. (Eds.), Scienti¢c
noise masker on responses to contralateral tones. Hear. Res. 78,
Basis of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss. Thieme, New York, pp. 56^ 64.
132^148. Mikaelian, D.O., 1979. Development and degeneration of hearing in
Erway, L.C., Willott, J.F., Archer, J.R., Harrison, D.E., 1993. Genet-
the C57/bl6 mouse : Relation of electrophysiologic responses from
ics of age-related hearing loss in mice. I. Inbred and F1 hybrid
the round window and cochlear nucleus to cochlear anatomy and
strains. Hear. Res. 65, 125^132.
behavioral responses. Laryngoscope 34, 1^15.
Glorig, A., Nixon, J., 1962. Hearing loss as a function of age. Laryngoscope 72, 1596^1610.
Miller, J.D., 1970. Audibility curve of the chinchilla. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 48, 513^523.
Gratton, M.A., Smyth, B.J., Schulte, B.A., Vincent, D.A., Jr., 1995. Na,K-ATPase activity declines in the cochlear lateral wall of quietaged gerbils. Hear. Res. 83, 33^40.
Mills, J.H., Going, J.A., 1982. Review of environmental factors a¡ecting hearing. Environ. Health Perspect. 44, 119^127. Mills, J.H., Boettcher, F.A., Dubno, J.R., 1997. Interaction of noise-
Gratton, M.A., Schmiedt, R.A., Schulte, B.A., 1996. Age-related decreases in endocochlear potential are associated with vascular abnormalities in the stria vascularis. Hear. Res. 102, 181^190.
induced permanent threshold shift and age-related threshold shift. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 101, 1681^1686. Mills, J.H., Boettcher, F.A., Dubno, J.R., Schmiedt, R.A., 1996a.
Hamernik, R.P., Ahroon, W.A., Lei, S.-F., 1996. The cubic distortion
Psychophysical and evoked response studies of aged subjects :
product otoacoustic emissions from the normal and noise-dam-
Masking by low-pass noise. In : Axelsson, A. et al. (Eds.), Scien-
aged chinchilla cochlea. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 1003^1012.
ti¢c Basis of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss. Thieme, New York,
Harman, D., 1986. Free radical theory of aging : role of free radicals
pp. 181^192.
in the origination and evolution of life, aging, and disease proc-
Mills, J.H., Lee, F.-S., Dubno, J.R., Boettcher, F.A., 1996b. Interac-
esses. In : Johnson, J.E. Jr., Walford, R., Harman, D., Miquel, J.
tions between age-related and noise-induced hearing loss. In : Ax-
(Eds.), Free Radicals, Aging, and Degenerative Diseases. Liss,
elsson, A. et al. (Eds.), Scienti¢c Basis of Noise-Induced Hearing
New York.
Loss. Thieme, New York, pp. 193^212.
Hay£ick, L., 1977. The cellular basis for biological aging. In : Finch,
Mills, J.H., Schmiedt, R.A., Kulish, L.F., 1990. Age-related changes
C.E., Hay£ick, L. (Eds.), Handbook of the Biology of Aging. Van
in auditory potentials of Mongolian gerbil. Hear. Res. 46, 201^
Nostrand Reinhold, New York.
210.
Hay£ick, L., 1985. Theories of biological aging. In : Maddox, G.L. (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Aging. Springer, New York.
Mountain,
D.C.,
1980.
Changes
in
endolymphatic
potential
and
crossed olivocochlear bundle stimulation alters cochlear mechan-
He¡ner, R.S., He¡ner, H.E., 1991. Behavioral hearing range of the chinchilla. Hear. Res. 52, 13^16.
ics. Science 210, 71^72. Nadol, J.B. Jr., 1980. The aging peripheral hearing mechanism. In :
Hellstrom, L.I., Schmiedt, R.A., 1990. Compound action potential input/output functions in young and quiet-aged gerbils. Hear. Res. 50, 163^174.
Beasley, D.S., Davis, G.A. (Eds.), Aging Communication Processes and Disorders. Grune and Stratton, Orlando, FL, pp. 63^85. Pearson, J.D., Morrell, C.H., Gordon-Salant, S., Brant, L.J., Metter,
Henry, K.R., 1983. Aging and audition. In : Willott, J.F. (Ed.), Audi-
E.J., Klein, L.L., Fozard, J.L., 1995. Gender di¡erences in a lon-
tory Psychobiology of the Mouse. CC Thomas, Spring¢eld, IL,
gitudinal study of age-associated hearing loss. J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
pp. 470^493.
97, 1196^1205.
Henry, K.R., Chole, R.A., 1980. Genotypic di¡erences in behavioral, physiological and anatomical expressions of age-related hearing loss on the laboratory mouse. Audiology 19, 369^383.
Ryan, A., 1976. Hearing sensitivity of the Mongolian gerbil,
unguiculatus.
Meriones
J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 59, 1222^1226.
Schmiedt, R.A., Mills, J.H., Adams, J.C., 1990. Tuning and suppres-
Hinchcli¡e, R., 1959. The threshold of hearing as a function of age. Acoustica 9, 303^308.
sion in auditory nerve ¢bers of aged gerbils raised in quiet or noise. Hear. Res. 45, 221^236.
Hunter, K.P., Willott, J.F., 1987. Aging and the auditory brainstem
Schmiedt, R.A., Schulte, B.A., 1992. Physiologic and histopathologic
response in mice with severe or minimal presbycusis. Hear. Res.
changes in quiet- and noise-aged gerbil cochleas. In : Dancer, A.L.,
30, 207^218.
Henderson, D., Salvi, R.J., Hamernik, R.P. (Eds.), Noise-Induced
International Organization for Standardization, 1990. Acoustic-determination of occupational noise exposure and estimation of noiseinduced hearing impairment, ISO 1999. ISO, Geneva.
Hearing Loss. Mosby Year Book, St. Louis, MO, pp. 246^256. Schmiedt, R.A., Mills, J.H., Boettcher, F.A., 1996. Age-related loss of activity of auditory-nerve ¢bers. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 2799^2803.
Jerger, J., 1973. Audiological ¢ndings in aging. Adv. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 20, 115^124.
Schuknecht, H., 1974. Pathology of the Ear. Harvard Press, Cambridge, MA.
Johnsson, L.-G., Hawkins, J.E., 1972. Sensory and neural degenera-
Schuknecht, H., 1989. Pathology of presbycusis. In : Goldstein, J.C.,
tion with aging, as seen in microdissections of the human inner
Kashima, H.K., Koopmann, C.F. (Eds.), Geriatric Otolaryngol-
ear. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 81, 193^197.
ogy. B.C. Decker, Toronto, Ont., pp. 40^44.
Kryter, K.D., 1983. Presbycusis, sociocusis and nosocusis. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 73, 1897^1916.
Schuknecht, H., Gacek, M., 1993. Cochlear pathology in presbycusis. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. Suppl. 158, 1^16.
Kryter, K.D., 1985. The E¡ects of Noise on Man. Academic Press, New York.
Schulte, B.A., Schmiedt, R.A., 1992. Lateral wall Na,K-ATPase and endocochlear potential decline with age in quiet-reared gerbils.
Li, H.S., Borg, E., 1991. Age-related loss of auditory sensitivity in two mouse genotypes. Acta Otolaryngol. 111, 827^834.
Hear. Res. 62, 46^56. Shone, G., Altschuler, R.A., Miller, J.M., Nuttall, A.L., 1991. The
Lonsbury-Martin, B.L., Cutler, W.M., Martin, G.K., 1991. Evidence
e¡ect of noise exposure on the aging ear. Hear. Res. 56, 173^178.
for the in£uence of aging on distortion-product otoacoustic emis-
Siegel, J.H., Kim, D.O., 1982. E¡erent neural control of cochlear
sions in humans. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 89, 1749^1759.
mechanics ? Olivocochlear bundle stimulation a¡ects cochlear bio-
McFadden, S.L., Willott, J.F., 1994a. Responses of inferior colliculus neurons in C57BL/6J mice with and without sensorineural hearing loss : E¡ects of changing the azimuthal location of an unmasked pure-tone stimulus. Hear. Res. 78, 115^131.
mechanical nonlinearity. Hear. Res. 6, 245^248. Spoor, A., 1967. Presbycusis values in relation to noise induced hearing loss. Int. Audiol. 6, 48^57. Steel, K., Niaussat, M.M., Bock, G.R., 1983. The genetics of hearing.
McFadden, S.L., Willott, J.F., 1994b. Responses of inferior colliculus neurons in C57BL/6J mice with and without sensorineural hearing
In : Willott, J.F. (Ed.), Auditory Psychobiology of the Mouse. CC Thomas, Spring¢eld, IL, pp. 341^394.
HEARES 2854 11-11-97
S.L. McFadden et al. / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 114^126
126
Sun, J.C., Bohne, B.A., Harding, G.W., 1994. Is the older ear more susceptible to noise damage ? Laryngoscope 104, 1251^1258.
tory cortex associated with sensorineural hearing loss in adult
Tarnowski, B.I., Schmiedt, R.A., Hellstrom, L.I., Lee, F.S., Adams, J.C., 1991. Age-related changes in cochleas of Mongolian gerbils. Hear. Res. 54, 123^134. Selective inner hair cell loss does not alter distortion product otoacoustic emissions. Hear. Res. 96, 71^82.
mice. J. Comp. Neurol. 321, 666^678. Willott, J.F., Erway, L.C., Archer, J.R., Harrison, D.E., 1995. Genetics of age-related hearing loss in mice. II. Strain di¡erences and
Walton, J.P., Frisina, R.D., Meierhans, L.R., 1995. Sensorineural hearing loss alters recovery from short-term adaptation in the C57BL/6 mouse. Hear. Res. 88, 19^26.
e¡ects of caloric restriction on cochlear pathology and evoked response thresholds. Hear. Res. 88, 143^155. Willott, J.F., Parham, K., Hunter, K.P., 1988. Response properties of
Willott, J.F., 1984. Changes in frequency representation in the auditory system of mice with age-related hearing impairment. Brain Res. 309, 159^162.
inferior colliculus neurons in middle-aged C57 mice with presbycusis. Hear. Res. 37, 15^28. Willott, J.F., Parham, K., Hunter, K.P., 1991. Comparison of the
Willott, J.F., 1991. Aging and the Auditory System : Anatomy, Physiology, and Psychophysics. Singular, San Diego, CA. neuroscience perspective. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 7, 141^151. mouse. In : Salvi, R., Henderson, D., Coletti, V., Fiorino, F. York, pp. 297^316.
and
Regeneration.
Thieme,
Hear. Res. 53, 78^94. Willott, J.F., Bross, L.S., McFadden, S.L., 1994. Morphology of the
Willott, J.F., 1996b. Auditory system plasticity in the adult C57BL/6J Plasticity
auditory sensitivity of neurons in the cochlear nucleus and inferior colliculus neurons of young and aging C57BL/6J and CBA/J mice.
Willott, J.F., 1996a. Anatomic and physiologic aging : A behavioral
Auditory
C57BL/6J mice. J. Comp. Neurol. 329, 402^411. Willott, J.F., Bross, L.S., McFadden, S.L., 1992. Morphology of the dorsal cochlear nucleus in young and aging C57BL/6J and CBA/J
Trautwein, P., Hofstetter, P., Wang, J., Salvi, R., Nostrant, A., 1996.
(Eds.),
Willott, J.F., Aitkin, L.M., McFadden, S.L., 1993. Plasticity of audi-
New
inferior colliculus in C57BL/6J and CBA/J mice across the lifespan. Neurobiol. Aging 15, 175^183. Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging, 1988. Speech understanding and aging. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 859^895.
HEARES 2854 11-11-97