Age-Related Eye Disease and Cognitive Function

Age-Related Eye Disease and Cognitive Function

Journal Pre-proof Age-Related Eye Disease and Cognitive Function: The Search for Mediators Mélanie Varin, MSc, Marie-Jeanne Kergoat, MD, Sylvie Bellev...

477KB Sizes 0 Downloads 90 Views

Journal Pre-proof Age-Related Eye Disease and Cognitive Function: The Search for Mediators Mélanie Varin, MSc, Marie-Jeanne Kergoat, MD, Sylvie Belleville, PhD, Gisele Li, MD, MSc, Jacqueline Rousseau, PhD, Marie-Hélène Roy-Gagnon, PhD, Solmaz Moghadaszadeh, MSc, Ellen E. Freeman, PhD PII:

S0161-6420(19)32111-6

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ophtha.2019.10.004

Reference:

OPHTHA 10952

To appear in:

Ophthalmology

Received Date: 2 July 2019 Revised Date:

30 September 2019

Accepted Date: 2 October 2019

Please cite this article as: Varin M, Kergoat M-J, Belleville S, Li G, Rousseau J, Roy-Gagnon M-H, Moghadaszadeh S, Freeman EE, Age-Related Eye Disease and Cognitive Function: The Search for Mediators, Ophthalmology (2019), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ophtha.2019.10.004. This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of the American Academy of Ophthalmology

1

2

Age-Related Eye Disease and Cognitive Function: The Search for

3

Mediators

4 5

Mélanie Varin, MSc1, Marie-Jeanne Kergoat, MD2, Sylvie Belleville, PhD2, Gisele Li, MD,

6

MSc3,4, Jacqueline Rousseau, PhD2,5, Marie-Hélène Roy-Gagnon, PhD1, Solmaz

7

Moghadaszadeh, MSc3, Ellen E. Freeman, PhD1,3,4,6

8 9 10 11 12 13

1

School of Epidemiology and Public Health, University of Ottawa; 2 Centre de Recherche, Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal; 3 Centre de Recherche, Hôpital MaisonneuveRosemont, Montréal; 4 Department of Ophthalmology, Université de Montréal; 5 School of Rehabilitation, Université de Montréal; 6 Ottawa Hospital Research Institute, Ottawa; Canada

14

Presented at ARVO Minisymposium, May 2019

15

Funded by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP 133560)

16 17 18 19 20

Conflict of Interest: No conflicting relationship exists for any author

21 22 23 24 25 26

Impact Statement 1. We certify that this work is novel 2. The potential impact: Older adults with glaucoma may be at risk for cognitive impairment. 3. This research reports a novel finding on the relationships between glaucoma and three cognitive tests of working memory.

27 28 29 30 31

Corresponding Author: Ellen Freeman, School of Epidemiology and Public Health, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada, [email protected], 613-562-5800 x8439

Running Head: Age-Related Eye Disease and Cognitive Function Word Count: 2,507

1

ABSTRACT

32 33 34

Purpose: Age-related eye disease may be associated with cognitive decline but the scientific

35

literature has not been consistent. Furthermore, no studies have been able to explain the

36

relationship. Our objective was to assess whether older adults with age-related macular

37

degeneration (AMD) or glaucoma performed worse on six cognitive tests compared to older

38

adults with normal vision, and, if so, to understand why.

39

Design: Cross-sectional analysis of hospital-based study (Maisonneuve-Rosemont Hospital

40

ophthalmology clinics, Montreal, Canada)

41

Subjects, Participants, and Controls: 336 adults ages 65 and older with either AMD,

42

glaucoma, or normal vision.

43

Methods: Cognition was measured with six cognitive tests administered orally. Activity levels

44

were measured using the Victoria Longitudinal Study Activity Lifestyle Questionnaire. Visual

45

acuity and visual field were measured. Multiple linear regression was used. Mediation was

46

assessed using structural equation modeling.

47

Main Outcome Measures: Verbal fluency test animal and letter versions, the digit span forward

48

and backward versions, and the logical memory test with immediate and delayed recall

49

Results: People with glaucoma had lower scores on three cognitive tests than the group with

50

normal vision: the digit span forward and backward tests (β=-0.8, 95% CI -1.5, -0.2 and

51

β=-0.7, 95% CI -1.3, -0.1 respectively) and the logical memory test with immediate recall

52

(β=-1.3, 95% CI -2.4, -0.2). Activity levels statistically significantly mediated the relationship

53

between glaucoma and the digit span forward test (P=0.043, percentage of the total effect

54

mediated=17%).

2

55

Conclusions: People with glaucoma had lower scores on cognitive tests that might depend on

56

verbal working memory and encoding. If confirmed in longitudinal studies, interventions should

57

be developed that are appropriate for a visually impaired population in order to slow this

58

cognitive decline.

3

59

Evidence is accumulating that visual impairment due to age-related eye disease is related

60

to poorer cognitive function. In particular, studies have found associations between cognitive

61

function and early age-related macular degeneration (AMD)1, late AMD2, glaucoma3-5, and

62

visual impairment6-9. Brain imaging studies have also found that AMD and glaucoma patients

63

have weaker brain functional connectivity compared to controls 10,11. Other studies have not

64

found associations between eye disease or vision and cognition12,13.

65

There are various hypotheses about why eye disease and cognition may be related 14,15.

66

Consistent with the sensory deprivation hypothesis and the disuse hypothesis 14-16, we believe

67

that eye disease can cause cognitive decline due to its effect on intervening variables in the

68

causal pathway. To our knowledge, no studies have attempted to explain the relationship

69

between eye disease and cognitive function by examining potential intervening variables such as

70

activity levels. Performing fewer activities in older age can increase the risk of Alzheimer’s

71

disease or cognitive impairment 17 18 and the loss of vision late in life may lead to a less active

72

lifestyle 19. Besides activity level, other potential intervening variables, also called mediators,

73

could include life space and depressive symptoms. Life space, a measure of the spatial extent of

74

a person over the last month20, has been found to be associated with both eye disease 21 and

75

cognitive function 22, as have depressive symptoms 23,24.

76

Our primary objective was to examine the relationship between two common age-related

77

eye diseases, AMD and glaucoma, and 6 cognitive outcomes that are valid to use even in

78

participants with poor vision because they are orally administered. Our secondary objective was

79

to examine whether activity levels, life space, or depressive symptoms act as mediators.

80 81

METHODS

4

82 83 84

Study design and population

85

Maisonneuve-Rosemont Hospital (Montréal, Québec) ophthalmology clinics from 2016-2018.

86

Three groups of people were recruited: 1) those with late stage AMD (geographic atrophy or

87

neovascular disease) in both eyes with a better eye visual acuity worse than 20/40 (n=111); 2)

88

those with a diagnosis of glaucoma in both eyes with visual field mean deviation worse than or

89

equal to -4dB in their better eye, (n=96). Secondary glaucoma was excluded due to concerns that

90

the cause of the glaucoma could also affect cognitive function (e.g. trauma); 3) Those with

91

normal vision who were not being seen for suspected AMD or glaucoma with visual acuity better

92

than 20/40 in both eyes and visual field mean deviation better than -4dB in both eyes (n=129).

93

Those in this group had conditions not currently causing visual impairment such as early

94

cataract, diabetic retinopathy, posterior vitreous detachment, or ocular hypertension.

95

Participants in this hospital-based cross-sectional study were recruited from the

Participants were excluded from the study if they were under 65, if they could not

96

respond for themselves, or if they scored less than 10 on the Mini-Mental State Examination

97

(MMSE) Blind version. The Blind version of the MMSE omits 8 items that rely on vision and

98

has been validated against the original version 25. A score less than 10 on the MMSE Blind

99

could indicate some form of dementia, Alzheimer’s disease or cognitive impairment and may

100

lead to unreliable self-reported data25,26. Furthermore, people who had received eye surgery in

101

the last 2 months were enrolled after a 2 month delay so that their cognition would not be

102

affected by their recovery.

103

There were 1,010 patients who appeared to meet eligibility criteria from a review of the

104

medical records. Of the 1,010 patients, 535 patients accepted our invitation to be in the study

105

(53%), 394 refused (39%), and 81 (8%) were not capable of responding for themselves. Of the

5

106

535 who accepted, 336 people met the final eligibility criteria after giving the MMSE and

107

measuring their current visual acuity and visual field. Six people were excluded because they

108

scored too low on the MMSE. This study received approval from the Ethics Committee at

109

Maisonneuve-Rosemont Hospital and was conducted according to the tenets of the Declaration

110

of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

111 112

Data Collection

113

Vision and Eye Disease

114

Information on date of diagnosis, type of age-related macular degeneration, type of

115

glaucoma, previous treatment for AMD, and number and name(s) of current glaucoma

116

medications was acquired from participants’ medical charts. Binocular visual acuity was

117

measured using the Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) visual acuity chart

118

at 2 meters. Visual acuity scores were converted to log of the minimum angle of resolution

119

(logMAR). Visual field was measured in each eye using the Humphrey Frequency Doubling

120

Technology perimeter full-threshold testing27-29. Visual field data were considered unreliable

121

and were not used if false positives, false negatives, or fixation losses exceeded 33% of trials. If

122

a reliable visual field measure could not be obtained, the most recent visual field data were used

123

from the medical chart as measured using the Humphrey SITA-standard 24-2 program. Contrast

124

sensitivity was measured in each eye using the Pelli Robson chart at 1 meter 30. The presence of

125

a current lens opacity (cataract) was taken from each participant’s medical chart.

126 127

Cognitive Outcomes

6

128

Participants were orally administered six cognitive tests: the one-minute verbal fluency

129

test (letter and category versions), the digit span test (forward and backward versions), and the

130

logical memory test (immediate and 30-minute delayed recall). The tests took approximately 30

131

minutes in total to complete. These tests were chosen because they are valid and reliable

132

cognitive tests that are orally administered in their original validated version. All questionnaires

133

were interviewer-administered in a face-to-face manner. For the one-minute verbal fluency letter

134

test, participants were asked to name as many words as they could that began with the letter p in

135

one minute 31,32. For the one-minute verbal fluency category test, participants were asked to

136

name as many animals as they could in one minute. These tests measure language and retrieval

137

skills. The digit span forward and backward tests were used to assess verbal working memory

138

33

139

in forward or reverse order. The last cognitive test administered was the logical memory test

140

using immediate and 30-minute delayed recall 33. Participants were told a detailed short story

141

and were asked to recall the story immediately and again 30 minutes later. This test examines

142

verbal memory, encoding, and maintenance. Detailed scoring instructions were available. All

143

tests were recorded and were scored by trained personnel.

. Participants were given an increasingly long list of numbers and were asked to repeat the list

144 145 146

Potential Mediators Activities were measured using the Victoria Longitudinal Study Activity Lifestyle

147

Questionnaire 16,34. This questionnaire has 70 items divided into 6 activity categories: physical,

148

self-maintenance, social, hobbies and home maintenance, novel information processing, and

149

passive information processing. The average frequency of participation over the last 2 years is

150

rated on a 9-point scale (never, less than once a year, about once a year, 2 or 3 times a year,

7

151

about once a month, 2 or 3 times a month, about once a week, 2 or 3 times a week, daily). This

152

questionnaire showed good test-retest reliability and construct validity34. This questionnaire was

153

scored by adding up all the activities that were done at least once per month.

154

Depressive symptoms were measured by the Geriatric Depressive Scale Short Form 35

155

and life space was measured using the Life Space Assessment 20. Life space is measured on a

156

scale of 0 to 120 with higher scores indicating greater independent life space.

157 158

Demographics and Health

159

Demographic data such as age, sex, and highest level of education attained were

160

collected. The self-report of a physician’s diagnosis of 13 health conditions (e.g., diabetes, heart

161

disease, stroke, arthritis, asthma, chronic obstructive pulmonary disorder, hypertension,

162

peripheral artery disease, Parkinson’s disease, hearing impairment, depression, back problem,

163

hip fracture) was given by participants. Smoking status was obtained through self-report.

164 165 166

Statistical Analysis To compare the three eye disease groups in preliminary analyses, ANOVA tests were

167

used for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables. The normality of the

168

cognitive outcomes was assessed with normal quantile plots. Multiple linear regression analysis

169

was utilized to examine the relationship between eye disease and the cognitive outcomes

170

adjusting for confounders. Rather than put all 13 comorbid health conditions into the model,

171

health conditions were only entered into the model if they had a P-value<0.1 with at least one

172

cognitive outcome after adjusting for demographic variables, smoking, and cataract. Diabetes,

173

asthma, and hearing impairment had a P-value<0.1 for at least one cognitive outcome.

8

174

Regression analyses were adjusted for covariates such as age, sex, education, diabetes, asthma,

175

hearing impairment, , smoking, and cataract because of their potential importance to cognition or

176

their association with AMD or glaucoma according to prior literature36. We limited our analyses

177

to Caucasian individuals since we did not have enough non-Caucasian people to include them in

178

the analysis (n=14).

179

To determine whether activity level, life space, or depressive symptoms acted as

180

mediators 37 of any relationships, structural equation modeling was used with the maximum

181

likelihood with missing values option. Analyses were done in Stata Version 15.0 (College

182

Station, Texas).

183 184

RESULTS

185

Of the 1,010 patients who we approached to participate in the study, 394 refused (39%).

186

Patients who refused did not differ in age or sex from those who agreed to participate and were

187

eligible (mean age and sex of participants versus refusers, respectively, 78.1 versus 79.0 years,

188

P=0.125 and 58% versus 60% women, P=0.685).

189

The three recruitment groups are compared in Table 1. The AMD and glaucoma groups

190

were older (P<0.001), more likely to be women (P=0.027), and to have less education (P<0.001)

191

than the normal vision group. They also had more depressive symptoms, performed fewer

192

activities, and had reduced life space (P<0.001). Furthermore, as expected, the AMD group had

193

worse visual acuity and the glaucoma group had worse visual field than the normal vision group

194

(P<0.001). Both the AMD and glaucoma groups had worse contrast sensitivity than the normal

195

vision group (P<0.001). The average duration of time since AMD diagnosis was 6 years while

196

the average duration of time since glaucoma diagnosis was 10 years.

9

197

The means and standard deviations of the cognitive tests are presented by eye disease

198

status in Table 2. The scores for the normal vision group were higher than the groups with eye

199

disease for all cognitive tests (P<0.001). For example, the AMD group reported three fewer

200

words that start with the letter p in one minute, on average, than the normal vision group, while

201

the glaucoma group reported just less than 2 fewer words.

202

After adjustment for demographic, health, and lifestyle variables, only the glaucoma

203

group remained statistically significantly associated with any cognitive outcomes (Table 3).

204

Specifically, the glaucoma group had worse digit span forward scores (β=-0.8, 95% CI

205

-1.5, -0.2). In other words, on average, the glaucoma group remembered 0.8 fewer digits than

206

the normal vision group, after adjustment. The glaucoma group also had worse digit span

207

backward scores (β=-0.7, 95% CI -1.3, -0.1), and worse scores on the logical memory test with

208

immediate recall (β=-1.3, 95% CI -2.4, -0.2). After adjustment, AMD was not associated with

209

any cognitive outcomes although it had borderline statistical significance with the digit span

210

backward test (P=0.057). In a sensitivity analysis, we examined whether more severe AMD

211

(with visual acuity worse than 20/60) was associated with any of the cognitive outcomes and it

212

was not. We also ran a sensitivity analysis to see if the AMD results differed by whether a

213

person had neovascular AMD in both eyes, a mix of neovascular AMD in one eye and

214

geographic atrophy (GA) in one eye, or GA in both eyes. Those with neovascular AMD in both

215

eyes had a lower verbal fluency letter score (β==-1.9, 95% CI -3.7, -0.1). No other statistically

216

significant associations were found with AMD type.

217

To explain the relationships identified between glaucoma and cognition, we examined

218

whether any of the following variables acted as mediators: activity level, life space, or

219

depressive symptoms. Activity levels statistically significantly mediated the relationship

10

220

between glaucoma and the digit span forward test. The P-value for the indirect effect was

221

0.043and the percentage of the total effect mediated was 17% (Figure 1). The P-values for the

222

indirect effect of activity level on the verbal digit span backward test and the logical memory test

223

had borderline significance at P=0.073 and P=0.089, respectively. The P-values for the indirect

224

effect of life space on the verbal digit span forward test had borderline statistical significant at

225

P=0.053 while the P-values for the indirect effect of life space on the verbal digit span backward

226

test and the logical memory test were not statistically significant at P=0.113 and P=0.207,

227

respectively. The P-values for the indirect effect of depressive symptoms on the verbal digit

228

span forward test, the verbal digit span backward test, and the logical memory test were also not

229

statistically significant at P=0.194, P=0.351, and P=0.774, respectively.

230

We also ran models with visual acuity and visual field without the eye disease indicator

231

variables. The results were consistent in that visual field in the better eye (which is primarily

232

affected in glaucoma) was statistically significantly associated with the digit span forward

233

(P=0.004) and backward tests (P=0.015) and the logical memory test with immediate recall

234

(P=0.008) . It was also associated with the verbal fluency category score (P=0.030) and the

235

logical memory test with delayed recall (P=0.029). Visual acuity (which is primarily affected in

236

AMD) was not related to any cognitive tests. Finally, we ran models with contrast sensitivity

237

and visual field without the eye disease variables. Contrast sensitivity was not statistically

238

significantly related to any cognitive outcomes.

239

DISCUSSION

240

We have found for the first time that glaucoma patients have worse scores on three

241

cognitive tests that measure verbal memory and verbal working memory, while AMD was not

242

associated with any of the cognitive outcomes. Furthermore, activity levels act as a partial

11

243

mediator of the relationship between glaucoma and the digit span forward test while life space

244

and depressive symptoms did not act as mediators.

245

It is interesting that glaucoma was only related to tests of memory and not the other

246

cognitive tests. Working memory involves the storage and manipulation of information over a

247

short period of time. In addition to working memory, the immediate recall of a story probably

248

also relies on the capacity to process information as the story is being presented. We considered

249

a few possible explanations for our results. We found modest evidence to support that reduced

250

activity levels explain the relationship between eye disease and cognitive function. Reduced

251

activity levels explained a small percentage (17%) of the relationship between glaucoma and the

252

digit span forward test. There may be other mediators for which we did not have data that might

253

explain more of these relationships such as reading difficulty or anxiety.

254

explanation for our findings related to the common cause hypothesis of cognitive aging 15 is that

255

elevated intraocular pressure (IOP) itself could affect memory. One study has shown that

256

induced IOP elevation in rats was associated with an increase in levels of amyloid beta and

257

phosphorylated tau while learning and memory declined 38. It is possible that participants in our

258

study had IOP elevations in the past (before initiation of treatment) with residual optic nerve

259

damage and possibly chronic changes in amyloid, phosphorylated tau levels, learning, and

260

memory. In recent years, glaucoma has been hypothesized to be a neurodegenerative disease 39.

261

Retinal ganglion cell injury can lead to trans-synaptic (transneuronal) anterograde degeneration.

262

Trans-synaptic degeneration has been shown in various optic neuropathies. It is not clear whether

263

the retinal ganglion cell damage seen in glaucoma causes transneuronal neurodegenerative brain

264

changes or whether there is a primary central nervous system pathology 39. Although none of

265

our participants had cognitive impairment, researchers have noted pathological similarities

12

Another possible

266

between conditions like glaucoma and Alzheimer’s disease, which affects working memory and

267

verbal memory in its prodromal stage 40. These neurodegenerative diseases share features like

268

glial reactivity, neuroinflammation, and oxidative stress 41. It is possible that glaucoma and

269

neurodegenerative diseases affecting cognition share common biological pathways and share

270

common risk factors like age and smoking. However, adjusting for risk factors like age and

271

smoking did not diminish the associations between glaucoma and the tests of memory.

272

It was surprising that AMD was not related to any of the cognitive tests given prior

273

research 1,2,42-44 . However, only three studies had a similar design to ours and can be directly

274

compared. First, Rozzini et al compared a late AMD group to a control group and examined a

275

variety of cognitive tests 43. They found that AMD was associated with lower letter fluency

276

scores but that it was not associated with category fluency scores. Second, Woo et al also

277

compared a late AMD group with a control group 44. They found that AMD was associated with

278

category fluency but not the digit span forward or backward tests. Finally, the AREDs study did

279

not find a relationship between AMD severity and cognitive outcomes like letter fluency,

280

category fluency, or the digit span backwards test but they did find associations between visual

281

acuity and letter and category fluency 42. The disparate results between our study and these other

282

studies may have to do with the use of possibly healthier volunteers recruited from outside the

283

clinic in the control population 42,44 or residual confounding for important variables such as age,

284

sex, and education 43,44. It is also possible that one must have an eye disease for a certain period

285

of time to suffer from its cognitive effects. The average duration of time since AMD diagnosis

286

was 6 years in our sample while the average duration of time since glaucoma diagnosis was 10

287

years. Perhaps our AMD patients had not experienced visual acuity loss for long enough to have

288

experienced cognitive changes. About 70% of our patients had wet AMD while 30% had dry.

13

289

There were slightly lower scores on the verbal fluency letter test for AMD and glaucoma

290

patients compared to controls although they were not statistically significant. We had 80%

291

power to detect a difference of 1.9 words on the verbal fluency letter test given our sample size

292

and the variability of the test. Instead, we saw half of that effect size with a difference of 1 word

293

for the glaucoma group and 0.7 words for the AMD group. We would have needed a much

294

larger sample size (376 glaucoma patients and 489 people with normal vision) for those more

295

modestly sized differences to have been statistically significant.

296

Strengths of this study include the involvement of patients with both AMD and glaucoma

297

as well as a group with normal vision recruited from the same ophthalmology clinics, the

298

measurement of visual acuity and visual field, the examination of potential mediators that were

299

tested using structural equation modeling, and the inclusion of multiple cognitive tests that do

300

not rely on vision. A limitation of this study is the lack of brain imaging data. To have had that

301

data on even just a subset of the sample would have been valuable given previous findings of

302

weaker brain functional connectivity in people with AMD and glaucoma 10,11. Another limitation

303

is the lack of data on other sensory deprivations like measured hearing or olfactory impairments

304

although the self-report of hearing impairment was not related to eye disease in our sample.

305

Also, our reference group was being seen for vision problems (e.g. early cataract) that were not

306

yet affecting visual acuity, visual field, or contrast sensitivity. However, we cannot be certain

307

that this group did not have poorer visual functioning on other vision tests like glare sensitivity

308

or motion perception. If our reference group did have vision problems in these other measures,

309

this may have diluted our results. Finally, our data were cross-sectional, which precludes our

310

ability to examine the temporal relationship of the variables and to more rigorously test

311

mediation. Two-year follow-up data will be available in the future.

14

312

To conclude, glaucoma patients had worse scores on three cognitive tests that were

313

related to working memory. Further studies should consider longitudinal designs, incorporate

314

brain imaging, and measure other sensory deprivations as well as vision problems 45. The

315

relevance of this work is that if eye disease does result in cognitive problems, cognitive training

316

exercises 46 could be developed that are appropriate and targeted towards people with vision loss.

317

15

Figure 1: Results from a structural equation model showing the direct and indirect effects of glaucoma on the digit span forward test. The indirect effect is the product of -1.98*0.073=-0.15 (SE=0.072). The direct effect is -0.72 (SE=0.305). The total effect is the addition of the direct and indirect effects. The proportion of the total effect mediated= -0.15 / (-0.72-0.15) =0.17

16

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.

9. 10.

11. 12.

13. 14. 15.

16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22.

Baker ML, Wang JJ, Rogers S, et al. Early age-related macular degeneration, cognitive function, and dementia: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Arch Ophthalmol. 2009;127(5):667-673. Pham TQ, Kifley A, Mitchell P, Wang JJ. Relation of age-related macular degeneration and cognitive impairment in an older population. Gerontology. 2006;52(6):353-358. Harrabi H, Kergoat MJ, Rousseau J, et al. Age-related eye disease and cognitive function. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(2):1217-1221. Tamura H, Kawakami H, Kanamoto T, et al. High frequency of open-angle glaucoma in Japanese patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Sci. 2006;246(1-2):79-83. Su CW, Lin CC, Kao CH, Chen HY. Association Between Glaucoma and the Risk of Dementia. Medicine (Baltimore). 2016;95(7):e2833. Fischer ME, Cruickshanks KJ, Schubert CR, et al. Age-Related Sensory Impairments and Risk of Cognitive Impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2016;64(10):1981-1987. Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Fischer ME, et al. Sensory Impairments and Cognitive Function in Middle-Aged Adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2017;72(8):1087-1090. Zheng DD, Swenor BK, Christ SL, West SK, Lam BL, Lee DJ. Longitudinal Associations Between Visual Impairment and Cognitive Functioning: The Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2018;136(9):989-995. Nael V, Peres K, Dartigues JF, et al. Vision loss and 12-year risk of dementia in older adults: the 3C cohort study. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019;34(2):141-152. Zhuang J, Madden DJ, Duong-Fernandez X, et al. Language processing in age-related macular degeneration associated with unique functional connectivity signatures in the right hemisphere. Neurobiol Aging. 2018;63:65-74. Frezzotti P, Giorgio A, Toto F, De Leucio A, De Stefano N. Early changes of brain connectivity in primary open angle glaucoma. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(12):4581-4596. Hong T, Mitchell P, Burlutsky G, Liew G, Wang JJ. Visual Impairment, Hearing Loss and Cognitive Function in an Older Population: Longitudinal Findings from the Blue Mountains Eye Study. PLoS One. 2016;11(1):e0147646. Ou Y, Grossman DS, Lee PP, Sloan FA. Glaucoma, Alzheimer disease and other dementia: a longitudinal analysis. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2012;19(5):285-292. Salthouse TA. Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. New York: Routledge; 2016. Baltes PB, Lindenberger U. Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: a new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychol Aging. 1997;12(1):12-21. Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA. Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging. 1999;14(2):245-263. Wilson RS, Mendes De Leon CF, Barnes LL, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2002;287(6):742-748. Bassuk SS, Glass TA, Berkman LF. Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Ann Intern Med. 1999;131(3):165-173. Varin M, Kergoat MJ, Belleville S, et al. Age-Related Eye Disease and Participation in Cognitive Activities. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):17980. Baker PS, Bodner EV, Allman RM. Measuring life-space mobility in community-dwelling older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(11):1610-1614. Popescu M, Boisjoly H, Schmaltz H, et al. Age-Related Eye Disease and Mobility Limitations in Older Adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52(19):7168-7174. Silberschmidt S, Kumar A, Raji MM, Markides K, Ottenbacher KJ, Al Snih S. Life-Space Mobility and Cognitive Decline Among Mexican Americans Aged 75 Years and Older. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65(7):1514-1520. 17

23.

24. 25.

26.

27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36.

37.

38. 39.

40. 41. 42. 43.

Popescu ML, Boisjoly H, Schmaltz H, et al. Explaining the Relationship between Three Eye Diseases and Depressive Symptoms in Older Adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(4):23082313. John A, Patel U, Rusted J, Richards M, Gaysina D. Affective problems and decline in cognitive state in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2019;49(3):353-365. Busse A, Sonntag A, Bischkopf J, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. Adaptation of dementia screening for vision-impaired older persons: administration of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). J Clin Epidemiol. 2002;55(9):909-915. Gross AL, Rebok GW, Unverzagt FW, Willis SL, Brandt J. Cognitive predictors of everyday functioning in older adults: results from the ACTIVE Cognitive Intervention Trial. The journals of gerontology Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences. 2011;66(5):557-566. Bailey IL, Bullimore MA, Raasch TW, Taylor HR. Clinical grading and the effects of scaling. Investigative ophthalmology & visual science. 1991;32(2):422-432. Ferris FL, 3rd, Kassoff A, Bresnick GH, Bailey I. New visual acuity charts for clinical research. American journal of ophthalmology. 1982;94(1):91-96. Anderson AJ, Johnson CA. Frequency-doubling technology perimetry. Ophthalmol Clin North Am. 2003;16(2):213-225. Elliott DB, Sanderson K, Conkey A. The reliability of the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity chart. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1990;10(1):21-24. M L. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995. Ratcliff G, Dodge H, Birzescu M, Ganguli M. Tracking cognitive functioning over time: ten-year longitudinal data from a community-based study. Appl Neuropsychol. 2003;10(2):76-88. Wechsler D. The Wechsler Memory Scale-III. San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. Jopp DS, Hertzog C. Assessing adult leisure activities: an extension of a self-report activity questionnaire. Psychol Assess. 2010;22(1):108-120. Burke WJ, Roccaforte WH, Wengel SP. The short form of the Geriatric Depression Scale: a comparison with the 30-item form. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 1991;4(3):173-178. Plassman BL, Williams JW, Jr., Burke JR, Holsinger T, Benjamin S. Systematic review: factors associated with risk for and possible prevention of cognitive decline in later life. Ann Intern Med. 2010;153(3):182-193. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51(6):1173-1182. Yuan Y, Chen Z, Li L, et al. High intraocular pressure produces learning and memory impairments in rats. Brain Res. 2017;1675:78-86. Lawlor M, Danesh-Meyer H, Levin LA, Davagnanam I, De Vita E, Plant GT. Glaucoma and the brain: Trans-synaptic degeneration, structural change, and implications for neuroprotection. Surv Ophthalmol. 2018;63(3):296-306. Belleville S, Sylvain-Roy S, de Boysson C, Ménard M. Characterizing the memory changes in persons with mild cognitive impairment. Progress in Brain Research. Vol 169: Elsvier; 2008. Sivak JM. The aging eye: common degenerative mechanisms between the Alzheimer's brain and retinal disease. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013;54(1):871-880. Clemons TE, Rankin MW, McBee WL. Cognitive impairment in the Age-Related Eye Disease Study: AREDS report no. 16. Arch Ophthalmol. 2006;124(4):537-543. Rozzini L, Riva M, Ghilardi N, et al. Cognitive dysfunction and age-related macular degeneration. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2014;29(3):256-262.

18

44. 45.

46.

Woo SJ, Park KH, Ahn J, et al. Cognitive impairment in age-related macular degeneration and geographic atrophy. Ophthalmology. 2012;119(10):2094-2101. Whitson HE, Cronin-Golomb A, Cruickshanks KJ, et al. American Geriatrics Society and National Institute on Aging Bench-to-Bedside Conference: Sensory Impairment and Cognitive Decline in Older Adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66(11):2052-2058. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288(18):2271-2281.

19

Table 1: Descriptive characteristics of the study population Glaucoma Mean (SD) or % n=96 78.1 (7.9)

Normal Vision Mean (SD) or % n=129 72.5 (5.8)

P-value

Age, Years

AMD Mean (SD) or % n=111 83.1 (7.2)

Sex Women

67.6%

56.3%

49.6%

0.027

Education, Years

10.2 (4.2)

11.3 (4.2)

12.8 (4.1)

<0.001

Binocular visual acuity, logMAR

0.6 (0.4)

0.2 (0.2)

0.1 (0.1)

<0.001

Visual field in better eye, MD in dB

-3.3 (3.8)*

-8.5 (6.8)

1.2 (1.9)

<0.001

Log Contrast Sensitivity in Better Eye

1.1 (0.4)

1.4 (0.3)

1.7 (0.2)

<0.001

Cataract

28.4%

28.9%

39.9%

0.165

Smoking Never smoker Former smoker Current smoker

38.1% 52.4% 9.5%

37.8% 54.1% 8.1%

44.9% 48.5% 6.6%

0.739

Diabetes

25%

20%

28%

0.397

Asthma

15.5%

12.6%

14.7%

0.716

34%

31%

29%

0.853

Activity Levels

18.2 (6.2)

22.2 (6.7)

25.8 (5.1)

<0.001

Life Space

48.7 (20.4)

61.0 (23.5)

73.2 (19.6)

<0.001

Hearing Impairment

<0.001

Depressive 3.4 (2.7) 2.7 (2.9) 2.0 (2.0) <0.001 Symptoms MD=mean deviation, dB=decibels, SD=standard deviation, AMD=age-related macular degeneration *38 participants with AMD were missing visual field data.

Table 2: Descriptive statistics for the six cognitive outcomes. Verbal Fluency Letters

Verbal Fluency Category

Digit Span Forward

Digit Span Backward

Immediate Story Recall

Delayed Story Recall

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD)

Eye Disease Group Normal Vision AMD Glaucoma

13.9 (5.2) 11.0 (5.6) 12.1 (5.0)

16.7 (5.2) 13.3 (5.0) 14.6 (5.6)

11.7 (2.0) 10.4 (2.4) 10.4 (2.6)

6.6 (2.3) 5.2 (2.0) 5.6 (2.4)

11.1 (4.1) 8.2 (4.8) 8.6 (4.0)

8.7 (4.5) 6.0 (4.6) 6.6 (3.7)

Possible Range

0-Unlimited

0-Unlimited

0-16

0-14

0-25

0-25

Observed Range

1-27

2-32

3-16

1-13

0-21

0-19

<0.001

<0.001

<0.001

P-value* <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 SD=standard deviation, AMD=age-related macular degeneration * P-value derived from ANOVA test.

Table 3: Multiple linear regression results of relationship between eye diseases and the six cognitive outcomes. Verbal Fluency Letters

Group Normal Vision AMD Glaucoma Age Female Sex Education Smoking Never Former Current Cataract Diabetes Asthma Hearing Impairment

Verbal Fluency Category

Digit Span Forward

β

95% CI

β

95% CI

β

0.0 -0.7 -1.0 -0.1 1.2 0.4

Ref -2.3, 0.8 -2.4, 0.4 -0.2, -0.0 0.0, 2.4 0.3, 0.5

0.0 0.2 -0.2 -0.3 1.0 0.2

Ref -1.3, 1.8 -1.7, 1.2 -0.4, -0.2 -0.2, 2.1 0.1, 0.4

0.0 Ref -0.1 -0.8, 0.6 -0.8 -1.5, -0.2 -0.1 -0.1, -0.0 0.4 -0.1, 0.9 0.2 0.1, 0.2

1.0 0.9 -1.4 -0.3 -1.2 -0.3 -0.8

-0.3, 2.1 -3.6, 0.8 -1.6, 1.0 -2.5, 0.1 -1.8, 1.3 -2.0, 0.5

1.0 -0.1 -0.4 -0.0 -0.5 1.4 0.4

-1.2, 1.1 -2.5, 1.7 -1.3, 1.2 -1.7, 0.8 -0.1, 2.9 -0.8, 1.6

1.0 0.3 0.1 -0.6 -0.2 -0.4 -0.5

AMD=age-related macular degeneration, CI=confidence interval

95% CI

-0.2, 0.9 -0.8, 1.1 -1.1, -0.0 -0.8, 0.3 -1.0, 0.3 -1.0, 0.0

Digit Span Backward

Delayed Story Recall

β

95% CI

β

95% CI

0.0 Ref -0.6 -1.3, 0.0 -0.7 -1.3, -0.1 -0.0 -0.1, 0.0 0.3 -0.2, 0.7 0.2 0.2, 0.3

0.0 -0.4 -1.3 -0.1 0.3 0.3

Ref -1.6, 0.9 -2.4, -0.2 -0.2, -0.1 -0.6, 1.2 0.2, 0.4

0.0 -0.2 -1.0 -0.2 0.3 0.2

Ref -1.5, 1.1 -2.1, 0.2 -0.2, -0.1 -0.6, 1.3 0.1, 0.3

1.0 0.1 1.0 -0.1 -0.3 -0.1 -0.3

1.0 1.0 0.3 -0.9 -0.6 1.9 -0.3

0.1, 1.9 -1.4, 2.0 -1.9, 0.1 -1.6, 0.4 0.6, 3.1 -1.3, 0.6

1.0 0.9 -0.0 -0.9 -0.7 2.3 -0.6

-0.1, 1.9 -1.8, 1.8 -1.9, 0.2 -1.7, 0.4 1.0, 3.6 -1.7, 0.4

β

95% CI

Immediate Story Recall

-0.4, 0.6 0.1, 1.9 -0.6, 0.5 -0.8, 0.3 -0.8, 0.5 -0.8, 0.2