Behavioural
Processes,
0 1990 Elsevier
BEPROC
235
22 (1990) 235-250
Science Publishers
B.V. 0376.6357/90/$03.50
00326
of movements
An analysis
Apodemus
CNRS, Laboratoire
in its home range
syhticus Simon
de Neurosciences
of the wood mouse
Benhamou
Fonctionnelles,
13402 Marseille
Chdex 9, France
(Accepted 30 August 1990)
Abstract The aim of this study was to quantify the movements of the wood mouse Apodemus sylvaticus in its home range. The movements were recorded using a new tracking method: it consisted of tracking a visually tagged wood mouse by a telescope from an observation tower and automatically recording the successive positions of this telescope (corresponding to the successive positions of the wood mouse in the field). Statistical analysis of 51 recorded movements, involving 10 wood mice, was carried out. The characteristics of the general movement patterns fitted a first order correlated random walk model. Furthermore, the detailed structure of paths was quantified. An analysis of variance showed that an individual factor was involved in metric
and temporal
movement
characteristics
but not in the statistical
structure
of
the paths. Lastly, examination of all the movements of 6 wood mice, with for which at least 4 different paths were recorded, revealed their space use patterns within their home ranges.
Key words:
Wood mouse; Movement;
Space use pattern; Tracking
Introduction Foraging behaviour largely determines how animals use available space. Precise spatio-temporal analysis of movement patterns in the field therefore constitutes a fundamental approach to the study of this behaviour: in a given sedentary species, this analysis provides characteristic parameters on strategies used by individuals foraging in their home ranges. Although spacing patterns have been studied extensively in mammals and birds (review in Brown & Orians, 1970; Waser & Wiley, 19BO), only a few field studies, such as those by Cody (1971,1974) and Smith (1974a,b) on
236 birds
and
centrated study
Siniff
and
Jessen
(1969)
on spatio-temporal
aimed
to
and
analysis
conduct
Macdonald
(1980)
of movement
an analysis
of
this
on
patterns
kind
on
mammals,
in the
the
have
field.
wood
The
mouse
con-
present
Apodemus
s ylvaticus. A close-cropped In a previous their
home
ranges
The
present
study
characteristics (1)
lawn
study
was
Bovet
& Benhamou
to
movements
space
the
in which
to
mice
the
tracks
son,
1977;
the
weight
enough
& Benhamou,
wood of
random
walk
mouse’s
wood
in
1989).
movement
be
from
one
developed for
(Banhamou
& Benhamou,
might
mice
model
to be useful
mechanisms
model
home
patterns shown
Bovet
& Bovet,
1990).
by
theoretical 1989;
Here
useful
for
analysing
of the
wood
mice
it
was
actual
ranges.
of the
movements
of movement
located
characteristics of
of
animals
in the
of the use
data about
field
lawn
wood
patterns
1985),
the animal
1978;
were mice
between
considered. are individually
exhibited
to not
by the
animals
imprecision
disturb
or marked
1966
wood
faeces,
1984;
of this
method
of the
recordings
& Freeman, distances,
used
to locate
Mineau
is limited
& Madi-
because
when tracking
does
continuous
large
been
1977;
a new
or locating
Lemen over
has
mice’s of small
1975a,b)
Recording
path
Madison,
movements
wood tracks
in Twigg,
paths.
et al.,
it. Consequently,
mice’s
and
animals’
1964;
the
punctual
radio-tracking
& Hartline,
wood
record
an individual’s
Although
the
to
Recording
Duplantier
the practicability
and
record
necessary
the
to record
(Rawson
of transmitters
was
in Sanderson,
information.
1983,
it
of toe clipped
(Jones,
in the field
to precisely
space
of precision.
(review
animals
in the
the
view, level
precise
static
Wolton,
from
developed
(Jamon
the
methods). to move
ranges. point
to use
rodents
already
1990;
this
characteristics
sufficiently
only
al,
movement
home
by dyed
was
structure parts
correlated
orientation
et
as the footprints
is tricky
small
area (see observed
are not.
labelled
and affords
and
a satisfactory
such
left
model
in their
the
practical
provide
1985)
the
with
radioactive not
as study
mice were
quantify
movement
order
whether
mice
only
their
movements
use
which
analysis within
mammals,
used
wood
to another
to precisely
This
detailed
and which
From
was
field, shrub
general
Benhamou
of wood
to determine
(4)
the
determine
to analyse
controlled
shrubs
one
fit the first
(1988).
1989;
attempted
(3)
would
some
Benhamou,
shrubs,
from
designed
whether
to another
(2)
with
in a similar
by piloting
to determine
of
out
in order:
shrub studies
sprinkled
carried
working method
of far was
in the field.
Met hods
Principle The used
of the recording
recording by Jamon
an observation angular (0).
method
was
& Benhamou tower
positions
Corresponding
with
method adapted (1989).
a telescope
of the ball-and-socket positions
of the
(and
considerably
It consisted placed joint animal
improved)
of tracking
on a tripod in the
from
each wood
and recording
horizontal
can be determined
(6)
a method
mouse
from
the successive
and vertical by trigonometry,
planes on
237 condition that it moves on a flat area. Each position of the wood mouse on the field (X, Y) then corresponded to a recorded position of the joint: X = OM . cos 6 and Y = OM . sin 6, with OM = OT . tan 8 where OT is the observation
height (at which the telescope
is positioned)
and OM is
the distance from which the animal is observed. Thus, an animal’s path can be perfectly reconstructed from the successively recorded positions of the joint. A similar recording principle was used by Tyack (1981) to determine the position of whales using a theodolite
from the top of a cliff.
Visual tagging of the wood
mouse
As established by Brown (1956a), Baumler (1975), Greenwood (1978), and Wolton (1983), the wood mouse is a nocturnal rodent. Direct observations were conducted at night using Buchler’s (1976) technique: the animal was tagged with a small bulb (0.4 ml) containing cyalume (l/4 reagent, 3/4 solvent), a chemiluminescent product that allows observations for 4 hours at distances over 100 m. To facilitate changing of bulbs, the male part of a press-stud was sewed (under anesthesia) onto the neck of the wood mouse and the female part was glued onto the bulb. The sewing held for about 15 days. It was then possible to sew another press-stud on the animal. The bulb was filled with cyalume and afterwards set on the animal, just before its movements were recorded. The bulb weighed about 1 g (< 5% of the body weight). Each bulb was used only once. The cyalume technique did not disturb the behaviour of the animal, as previously noted by Buchler (1976), Jamon & Bovet (1987) and Jamon & Benhamou (1989). The telescope (15 X 50) used to track the tagged wood mouse was equipped with a radio-luminescent nocturnal movements.
Recording
reticle,
so that one could focus on the animal
during
its
technique
The telescope was set on the joint and placed on a steady tripod at the top of an observation tower, strongly guyed to the soil, at an observation height of 10.84 m. Successive
positions
of the joint
corresponding
to the tracked wood mouse’s
succes-
sive positions in the field were recorded by two precision potentiometers fixed to the joint (Fig. la): the angle B was measured by the vertical potentiometer and the angle S was measured by the horizontal potentiometer. The mechanical multiplication (by 6) of the vertical movements of the joint considerably improved the precision of the vertical angular measurement which is of major importance because of the “perspective effect”, while the mechanical demultiplication of the horizontal movements (by 0.875) ensure to avoid the blind area of the potentiometer (between 355 and 360 degrees). The angular values of 13and 6, as measured by the potentiometers, were digitalized by a 12-bit analogical-numerical converter. The time factor was given in seconds by an electronic clock. The data were stored in the central memory of a microcomputer. Every time the wood mouse was centered with the telescope, its spatio-temporal position was coded, using a switch (Fig. 1 b). The electronic working of the system was under the general control of a computer program. After each recording session, the
238
data were cassette The
conveyed
recorder.
maximum
around
of 100
central system
error
position
on other
at a distance
the
whole
electronic
the actual
depended
from
The
memory was
storage
such
to a radius
observed
as the sighting,
on the
in the field
(1 bit) corresponded
of an animal
factors,
for
supplied
from
magnetic
by three
tape of a
12 V batteries.
of approximately
a distance
of 100
so that the actual
error
25 cm
m. Accuracy
was about
1 m
m.
0b Fig.
1. Scheme
of the tracking
reticle;
WI,
vertical
and horizontal
sion
WI-W2,
W2,
W3, W3-W4,
analogical-numerical
W4:
(a) and recording
notched
potentiometers; respectively); converter;
EC:
wheels VB, 5:
HB:
96, 16,
vertical
switch
electronic
micro-computer;
(b) system.
with
T: telescope; 21,
24
teeths,
and horizontal
controlling clock;
CR: cassette
VIA:
the versatile
recorder.
R: radio-luminescent respectively;
notched recording interface
belts
VP,
HP:
(transmis-
system;
ANC:
adapter;MC:
239
Study
area and timing
The field work was conducted, from July to December 1984, at the Biological Station of Tour du Valat, in the Camargue (Southern France). The field was a flat flat horizontal close-cropped lawn sprinkled with shrubs (Phillyrea angustifolia; density: 85 shrubs/ha). This lawn was delimited by meadows and cultivated fields to 180 m in width (N-S) and 300 m in length (E-W). Wood mice caught by Sherman live traps were individually marked by ear-tagging. Trapping began in July using a 200 by 160 m trapping grid and recording began in August, Only sedentary wood mice were studied. Sedentariness was established when one week in a definite area a wood mouse was trapped for at least (Andrzejewski
& Wierzbowska,
1961; Bovet, 1963).
During the recording session, only a few traps were used. They were checked in the morning. One wood mouse was selected from the trapped wood mice. Because it is more important in movement analysis to have numerous data available on the same animal than to have only some data for a large number of animals, priority was always given to the wood mouse whose movements had been recorded most often. The selected wood mouse was kept in its trap until night. A cyalume bulb was then tagged onto its neck, and it was released at the same place where it was trapped. The procedure then consisted of tracking the wood mouse until 4 hours had elapsed (lifetime of cyalume). Recordings were made during sive days of a lunar month, to ensure good conditions mouse.
moonless nights, for 15 succesfor detecting the tracked wood
Data processing Recorded data were first transferred from magnetic tape to the micro-computer, and afterwards, to a powerful computer by means of a communication interface (RS232).
A computer
program
was
then
used
to decode the
recordings,
i.e. to
reconstitute the paths as sequences of points (X, Y, t). With each recording, path length (L), travel time (T), time spent in shrubs (TS), percentage of time spent in shrubs (%TS = 100. TS/T) and mean speed of movements between shrubs
(V = L/(T-TS))
were computed.
As explained above, two analysis levels of wood mice’s paths were used, because of the heterogeneity of the environment: -Level 1: analysis of the general movement patterns of wood mice from one shrub to another. It was tested if these patterns, represented by the locations of the successively visited shrubs, could fit the first order correlated random walk model. Movements line Bovet
between
which
& Benhamou
successive mean
successively
segments, steps
(1988). are
and a standard
S = o/Jm(P),
where
than 1.2 rad (Bovet corresponding tion
(a)
and
The
shrubs
were
to as steps, model
therefore
according
assumed
that
randomly
drawn
in a wrapped
deviation
u. Then
the sinuosity
m(P) is the mean step length, & Benhamou,
percentage mean
visited
are referred
vector
of path length
1988). length (r,
Step
changes normal of the
number expresses
(N), N
of
mean
the
given
direction
distribution paths
. m(P)/L),
by straight
terminology
step
with
a null as
that u is less length
and standard
concentration
in
between
can be computed
under the condition
(%L = 100.
which
approximated to the
of
(m(P)), deviaangular
240 values
around
changes -Level
the angular
of direction 2: analysis
only
the
of
were
were
after
rediscretization
analysed
by
Bovet
correlated movements
in the
Note
that
were
it is possible
always
the
the
of the distributions
However, made
obviously
shrub
account
length
level
apparatus
lengths,
of
at this
second
oriented,
during
These
the recording.
of direction to
use
level
which
to
obtained
the
the
method
first
analysis,
order
because
constituted
an external
model.
deviation
(a)
the dispersion but
not
of an angular of angular
a wrapped
analyis,
in which
considered.
according
attempt
rad for
the shrubs,
were
no
in the
(r) when
to u < 1.2
second
lawn
of changes
step
was
into
corresponding
case for
1981) between
the
distribution
to use the standard
of the mean vector
large (r > 0.5,
(1988).
is not taken
paths
on
constant
model
lawn
of the
by the tracking
the
different
& Benhamou
which
Batschelet,
located
discretized
with walk
1972;
structure
movements
by computing
random
constraint instead
the
arbitrarily
They given
Mardia,
computed.
of the detailed
parts
movements
mean;
were
normal
for
the
distribution
values
is not too
distribution);
first
level
it was
analysis
(see
below). Finally, tics
by examining
which
space
all the
are individually
use
patterns
were
movements
of the wood
controlled
were
identified
determined
from
the
mice,
movement
by analyses
time
spent
characteris-
of variance,
by the
wood
and the
mice
in the
shrubs.
Results The (C,
analysis
F, G, H,
given (i.e.
was
in Fig. most
2. Wood
usual)
out
mice
speed
greater
than
1). This
difference
the
carried
on 51 recordings
I, and J) and four
the
females
spent
speed
ranged
between
56% from
shrubs
was due to irregular
of IO wood
B, D, and
on average
of movement
mean
(A,
of their which
time was
movements
including
examples
0.5 to 2 m/s,
(V)
slow
mice,
E). Three
in shrubs. that
males
The
much
IO m/min mice
are
modal
is therefore
about
the wood
six
of paths
(Table
performed
in
lawn.
First level analysis Movements wood
between lengths
successively
1972;
Batschelet,
than
43
another
shrubs
constitute
the
general
of movements
accounts
paths.
1981),
distribution
only
the successive
5. Using it was
differ
for
67%
of changes
(given
changes
that
from
in
framework
by straight average
the
were
(1960;
line
of
the
of direction,
test
between
of changes
of
the
segments total
path
distribution
of the observed of
to show whereas
Jupp & Mardia
significant 37 paths
successive
of direction
see Stephens, of changes
normal
to that
the
found
also
distribution
a wrapped
using
of direction
the number
test
by r) equal
Furthermore, 2 paths
where
Kuiper’s
found
at p = 0.10
a concentration
Batschelet, between
of the
1981),
not significantly of
to
schematization
out on 43 recordings
to or greater
out
The
visited
analysis
was carried
and with
shrub
1).
equal
38
one
movements.
(Table
Statistical steps
from
mice’s
1965;
of direction with
null
distribution, (1980;
correlation did not show
was
Mardia, did mean with also
see
at p = 0.05 any such
Fig. 2. Three
examples
of paths of wood
mice C, E, and H. Shrubs
circles and the small square in the center represents delimited
in the north and the south
to a width
about 100 m beyond the limits
correlation that
the
centered
at p = 0.10. changes
on 0, that
However,
the
of all recordings
this
was How,
then,
changes
a null
next
step
correlation shrub
mean, existed
and
the
preferentially
Second
level
Since
or less
chose
the first
length: 240 m).
possible
to reject
in a wrapped
order
was
too
sinuosity
choose
differ
correlated large
their
of
with
to
it.
the
i.e.
hypothesis distribution
random
(r -C 0.5,
paths
the
normal
sinuosity.
the
next
significantly
be said in the
at p = 0.10
distance
that
the
direction
shrub?
from shrub
of the
walk (T > 1.2
model. rad)
In the cases
was
in
where
computed
cannot
chosen
therefore
visited
shrubs
Because
a wrapped
last
41 out of 43 paths It
the successively
to
be
However,
between
that
on the basis
to make
its
no significant
the size
said
of
distribution
by a mouse
step. be
the distribution normal
of the choosen the
of their
wood
mice
apparent
size.
analysis
the schematization
visited
mice
not
it can first
more
seem
drawn
values
the
by small
The study area was
‘12.
did the wood
was
with
rad),
rad/m did
not
us to be able to compute
(a < 1.2
and 0.44
of direction
with
sively
for
possible 0.22
it does
of angular
tower.
of 180 m, but went on to the east and the west
drawn here (represented
are randomly
is compatible
dispersion
66%
between
Therefore,
of direction
are represented
the observation
shrubs
of movements
accounts
for
67%
by straight in
average
line of
segments
the
total
between path
succes-
lengths,
the
242 difference line
between
therefore
movements
the actual
in the
to 5 m) in order nhamou,
1988).
observed
after
lawn
This
walks,
movements
did
decrease
with
the existence
equal
TABLE
a step
is
the
case search
length
than
of the
5. The
A
L
2
T
195.0
2 4
to 0. This
goal
distributions root
order the
of
expected,
values
oriented
the
correlated
was
However,
1
& Be-
of
(ok
movements,
component. value
distribution did
given
were
of the standard of changes
the number
mean
chart
of direction
analysed
deviations
not differ
1981)
was
of direction
significantly
in Batschelet,
after a, are
of direction
of changes
from
with
0 at
43 out
of
r
%L
02
59.0
0.36
(91.9)
(22.1)
(55.1)
(35.4)
(0.26)
422.5
195.0
40.0
(4.2) 3.9
53.5
0.28
0.79
(13.9)
(4.2) 23.3
(1.3) 7.7
(13.4)
(0.01)
(0.11)
57.0
0.27
0.74
(14.0)
(1.3) 9.0
(6.9) 71.9
(0.08)
(0.13)
0.45
0.56
(6.9) 13.8
(17.6)
(0.17)
(0.23)
69.5
0.52
0.58
(3.5) 75.5
(0.43)
(0.11)
175.1 (40.8)
0.56 (0.19)
213.5
82.9
55.4
(51.6)
(21 .O)
196.0
27.2
46.0 (14.1)
558.5
(8.9) 137.2
26.5
(0.8) 8.1
0.48
0.57
(318.9)
(71.7)
(8.1) 5.7
(3.5) 59.2
(0.08) 0.31
(0.00) 0.74
(2.7) 7.7
(17.7)
(0.26)
(0.24)
71.6
0.41
0.57
(14.2)
(0.26)
(0.14)
(26.9) 2
182.7
85.4
(10 6) 54.2
(139.0)
(68.3)
(16.7)
337.7
145.7
61.6
6 7
(59.0)
(19.3)
99.7
40.2
84.3
(5.1) 19.6
69.2
0.56
0.47
(34.4)
(19.4)
(5.7) 15.8
(4.7) 65.7
(0.19)
(0.14)
78.7
(9.4) 67.8
0.38
0.68
(60.3)
(25.7)
(14.9)
(7.3)
(0.15)
(0.22)
(247.2) 6 6
137.5 (88.7)
wood mouse;
RN: number
percent of time spent in shrubs;
of paths recorded;
general
changes of direction
L: path length (m); T: travel time (mn); %TS:
V: mean speed in the lawn (m/min);
by general movement for
V
(150.4) 2
direction
%TS
6.6
967.5
14
represented
characteristics
49.0
(143.6)
R=2m.
square
Bovet
93.8
(150.6)
of
these
the
R (from
1
RN
WM:
strictly
where
angular
interval
Mean values (and SD) of the main movement WM
with
(see
first
The
lengths.
lengths
uR of with
close
oriented.
R = 2 m. The
analysis
the confidence
remained goal
of changes
45 out of the 51 recordings
to or greater
(from
as
were
of a random
1. Statistical
p = 0.05
means
shrubs
step
than
happen
and a straight
path
of direction
slowly
what
lawn
total
different
deviations
more to
on the
these
of changes
of the distributions
given was
angular
taken of
five
standard
R, contrary
R
with
out with
the
33%
with
increased
the
rediscretization in Table
to
distribution
that
between
characteristics
carried
the
length
whereas
of the paths
average
rediscretized
showed
step
because
The
were
rediscretization
random
indicating
on
to analyse
rediscretization
not
structure
corresponded
patterns;
movement
for movements
%L: percent of path length
r: mean vector length of the distribution
patterns;
a,: standard
deviation
of
in the lawn (rad) after rediscretization
the with
of changes
distribution
of
a step length
243
45 paths, but the normal distribution assumption was rejected at p = 0.05 paths, although the distribution shapes were symetrical and unimodal. deviations ranged from 0.26 to 1.15 rad, which points to the existence of random component. Furthermore the assumption that no correlation existed successive changes of direction was rejected at p = 0.05 with 11 paths, which high as might be expected with strictly goal oriented movements lead to an alternation of rediscretization step length) which appears as successive changes of direction. This also points component
Movement
with 28 Standard a major between is not as
oriented movements: indeed, such right and left turns (whatever the an important correlation between to the existence of a random search
in the path taken in the lawn.
variability
analysis
The results of analysis of the variability of given movement characteristics allowed us to test the hypothesis that the values of these characteristics obtained with individual value.
wood mice are drawn from
parent populations
Taking each wood mouse as an independent showed the existence of an “individual wood
with
the same expected
data group, an analysis of variance mouse” factor contributing to the
metric and temporal movement characteristics: indeed the homogeneity hypothesis was rejected at p = 0.05 for L, T, %TS, and V. On the other hand, this factor could not be shown at p = 0.10 in the case of characteristics depending on the statistical structure of paths: %L, r and a2 (Table 2).
Space use pattern The home ranges of the six wood mice on which at least four recordings were obtained were represented by the elliptical home range model at the 0.95 probability level (Jennrich & Turner, 1969; Mazurkiewicz, 1969,197O; see also Benhamou 1988), from the times spent by these wood mice in the shrubs (Table 3).
& Jamon,
TABLE 2 Variance
analysis
of the main
movement
characteristics.
L
T
%TS
V
%L
r
F
11.75
4.07
3.40
2.23
1.00
0.83
1.13
df
9-41
9-41
9-41
9-41
9-41
9-33
9-35
P
-C 0.0005
< 0.001
-=C0.005
< 0.05
> 0.10
> 0.10
> 0.10
0,
F
20.10
4.69
5.30
3.25
1.29
1.24
1.51
df
5-37
5-37
5-37
5-37
5-37
5-29
5-31
P
< o.ooo5
-C 0.005
-=z0.001
< 0.025
> 0.10
> 0.10
> 0.10
Upper with ratio;
part gives variance the 6 wood df: degrees
analysis
mice on which of freedom;
results
on the 10 wood
at least four
p: probability
recordings level.
mice,
were
whereas
obtained
lower
part deals
only
(see Table 1). F: variance
244
L Fig. 3. Home
ranges and paths of wood mice C, D, C, ti, I, and ). Home
by the elliptical
home range model
at the 0.95 probability
level
The
range limits size
are drawn
of the represented
field is 240 m bv 180 m.
It can be seen the
bivariate
ellipitical able
home home
were
this
representation
distribution
range model
heterogeneity
The They
that
normal
range similar
of
was
areas size
were to
(Fig.
between those
1700
obtained
ranges
is accurate
‘position
not confirmed,
of the environment is
of home animals
in
in particular
space
enough, assumed
because
even by
if
the
of the consider-
3). and 14000 in
the
mL, with
same
a mean
environment
of 6500 by
m’.
trapping
‘(%p)
UO!l3aJ!p
3-M
ayl
0)
UO!PlaJ
U!
S!XL’
JO!PLU
aL#
40
UO!lPKla!JO
:a0
Sl@JJl
Nkl
!asnow
S!Xe-!UlaS
:WM
syled
WM
3iavi
a8ueJ aluoH
POOM
q3e.3 Aq uayel
y(W)
:Ny
~00~
se sys!JaPeJey3
11
40 Jaqwnu
(asnow
I I
1
a~# Aq paleur!lsa
11
sqied
9
H
Sll le>!ld!lla
pue 3X
9
3
:XA
‘258
L
auioq
pue 11 ‘11 !P~PJOMJ
lelol
865
9
3
a
$0 saleu!p~oo>
((~1) sJwJ.33
L’ZLP
P9f z
PL
:Sll
‘(~1) y@~al
6’8SZ
S’ LPZ
9601
P
Al!h!l3e
lelol
:VdH
61
5’605
Z’Z 1s
6’6 LO 1
686 z
away
PL
Z’6LZ
O’fL9
OL8f
pue (u!u~) awy
LZ
CL
L‘OOL
1’1911
eaJe ague
51
LP-
S’O81
8’6P9
au~11 leloj f69
EL-
LL-
Ly !(mu1)
LZZP
ZL-
1s -
pue
6f
LSOP
8158
S-
8f-
u! wads
PP
L6fL
:Za
PL
6P
9s
L08fL
sqnJc(s
PLOf
LS
pue Jo!ew
EL LP LZ
86
lie u! (u!w)
Jou!w
ZL 99Lb
VtlH
&ueJ
3X
SP 18
3A
FS Zi?l
~6.0 ayi
ie lapolu
9s-
t10
‘lahal &!l!qeqoJd
F
246 (jamon,
1982,
p = 0.02
between
1987).
Furthermore,
the mean
range area. A similar by Wolton reflect
(1985)
result
amount
looks
at all
portions
overlap.
network
of pathways.
genity
was
paths
might The
networks
the
figure
of
that
found
to
and the size
by Brown
suggested wood
that
(1956b)
exist
at
of its home
by trapping
the home
the shrubs all
mouse,
wood
of this
showing
had common
each
indicate
structure
of the environment,
Furthermore,
obtained
which
was
mouse
range
and
area might
of movement.
the
This
correlation
of each wood
previously
by radio-tracking,
the mean
If one
a significant
path length
network
51
move
may
corresponding the
it can
mice
be seen more
be favoured
together
(Fig.
several
less
along
by the
to the junctions
paths
that
or
a
hetero-
of the network.
4) points
that
these
portions.
Discussion The
hypothesis
trails
has
O’Farrel,
put
1965;
Rathbun,
1979;
move
on to
with
1967;
as
with
Frame,
pattern
suggested
1982),
Barry
pathways
Peters
has
by
or
in
clearly study
mice
piloting
and
1979;
been this
wood
by visual
(1980)
1959;
Peters,
always
mice
whether
move
scent-marked
(Pearson,
1975;
not
wood
however,
& Francq
or
species
& Mech,
but
exhibited
pathways by
of
numerous
,1975;
It is not clear,
of scentmarked
another,
a network
Thompson,
use
hypothesis.
a network
on
connection
1982;
space
this
move
in
& Davis,
Pulliainen, The
compatible
mammals
forward
Adams
demonstrated.
shrub
that
been
from
Drickamer
is
actually one
& Stuart
(1984). The
general
associated spent
(56%
for wood
on average) against
of movements from
one
on the lawn
shrub
findings
exhibited home they for
ranges were but
centres
(Jamon
mice
was
quite
different
In most dispersion of only
at the
of
patterns
cases,
paths.
that
order
might
mice
a significant
of
changes
of
to another
showed
correlated
random
possible normal
of the distributions, This
to
from
as possible
were
ranges
speed
time
in the
were
associated
of the released
same
the
same
towards
All with
movements within
not explore
extent
lawn.
environment
using did
0.5 to
their when
procedure their
home
their
activity
in
general
1989).
one shrub
statistically
home
case, the wood
mean
ranged
lawn
in the
the
The as fast
organization
exhibited
and
between
lawn
of their
they
places etc.)
speed
the
in the
where
of their
distributions
in a wrapped
of most
from
movements
of the first
it was
drawn some
from
44%
taken
favourable
lengths.
were of time
arthropods,
in the lawn
path on
the general study,
(seeds,
the usual
travel
on average
this
periphery
& Benhamou,
analysis
in
in the latter
their
not
movements
Furthermore,
wood
assumptions
randomly
lawn.
the
oriented
Statistical basic
spent
and tracking:
ranges,
movement
They
that these
total
whereas
to another
proportion
are indeed
movements
of these did
one shrub
by the high
of energy
hand,
was 10 m/mn,
to another.
released
release
to 33% mice
of the
by
the other
wood
from
Shrubs
sources
the
indicate
an exploitation
On
mice
as indicated therein.
various
on average
Consequently,
these
animals
both
predators.
of the wood
of shrubs,
by the
providing
corresponded
2 m/s.
patterns
the exploitation
mice,
protection shrubs
movement
with
to show
distribution however,
that
direction walk
characteristics
model
changes
centered I was
observed
that their
fairly
of direction
on 0. Because
able to compute
be due to the heterogeneity
well:
fit the indeed, appeared
of the great the sinuosity
of the environment
which
Fig. 4. Drawing of ali the 51 recorded paths taken by all 10 wood represented Field is 240 by 180 m.
mice. The size of the
led the wood mice to walk great distances between successively visited shrubs, m on average. Wood mice therefore need to make large turns in order to restrict
i.e. 9 their
movements within their home ranges (6500 m2 in average). On the other hand, although movements in the lawn were shrub directed, they contained a iarge residual random search component, probably due to the exploitation of the lawn. The results of movement variability analysis showed that the metric and temporal characteristics of the wood mice’s movements, but not the characteristics depending on the statistical path structure, were sensitive to an “individual factor”. This confirms the hypothesis put forward in Covet & Benhamou (‘l988) that the random component of paths might constitute a general adaptation of movement behaviour to the stochasticity of the environment: unlike metric and temporal movement characteristics, this component did not depend on an individual factor but was imposed by the environment. Various habitats have been colonized by the wood mouse. This study showed how the movements can be influenced by a “semidesertic” environment (i,e. an open area sprinkled with shrubs). The movement and space use patterns exhibited by the waod mice in this study are similar to those of other rodents species living in this type of environment. They are ~robabi~ different, however, mice living in a more homogenous environment.
from patterns
exh~b;ted by wood
248
Acknowledgements I thank field
Pierre
work
Bovet,
Marc Jamon
was carried
out
with
and Marilyn
the kind
Cranjon
permission
for their
invaluable
help.
du Valat”
Biological
of the “Tour
The
Station.
References Adams,
L. & Davis, SD.,
Andrzejewski, small
rodents
Theriol., Barry,
in a defined
anatomy of home range. J. Mamm.,
T., 1961. An attempt at assessing
forest
48: 529-536.
the duration
area and the rate of interchange
between
of residence individuals.
of
Acta
5: 1533172
R.E. & Francq,
Peromyscus
Batschelet, Baumler,
1967. The internal
R. & Wierzbowska,
E.N.,
1980.
E., 1981. Circular W., 1975. Activity
Benhamou,
S., 1989.
ranges. J. theor. Benhamou, Anim.
Ortentation
J. Mamm.,
leucopus.
statistics
Biol.,
in biology.
of some small
An olfactory
to
landmarks
within
the
preferred
habitat
by
61: 292-303. Academic press,
mammals
orientation
in the field.
model
London. Acta Theriol.,
for mammals’
20: 365-377.
movements
in therr
home
139: 379-388.
S. & Bovet,
P., 1989.
How
animals
use their
environment:
a new
look
at kinesis.
Behav., 38: 375-383.
Benhamou,
S. & Jamon, M., 1988. Analysing
spatial relationships
from trapping
data. J. Mamm.,
69: 828-831. Benhamou,
S., SauvP, J-P. & Bovet, P., 1990. Spatial memory
and limitation
of the egocentric
Bovet, J, 1963. Observations mus
Bovet,
coding process.
sur la sedentariti.
en Camargue. Terre
sylvaticus)
P. & Benhamou,
S.,
1988.
random walk model. J. theor. Bovet, P. & Benhamou,
Biol.,
et le domaine
efficiency
145: 1-12.
vital du mulot
sylvestre
(Apode
et Vie, 17: 266-279.
Spattal Biol.,
in large scale movements:
J. theor.
analysis
of animals’
movements
using
a correlated
131: 419-433.
S., 1990. Optimal
sinuosity
in central place foraging
movements.
Anim.
Behav., in press. Brown,
J.L. & Orians,
C.H.,
1970.
Spacing patterns
field
experiments
in mobile
animals.
Ann.
Rev. Ecol. Syst.,
1:
2399262. Brown,
L.E.,
1956a.
and Microtus.
Clethrionomys,
Brown, Buchler,
L.E., 1956b. Movements E.R.,
1980.
Evidence
on
Proc. Zool.
the Sot.
of some British
activity
of
the
small
mammals
Apodemus,
Lond., 126: 549-564.
small
mammals.
for the use of a scent
post
J. Anim.
by Myotis
Ecol., 25: 54-71. J. Mamm.,
lucifugus.
61:
525-528. Cody, M.L., 1971. Finch flocks Cody, M L., 1974. Optimization Drickamer,
L.C. & Stuart,
navigation. Duplantier, rescentes
Am. Midl.
in the Mohave desert. Theor.
Pop. Biol.,
2: 142-158.
in ecology. Science, 183: 1156-1164.
J., 1984.
Peromyscus:
snow
tracktng
and
possible
cues
used
for
Nat., 111: 202-204.
J.M., Cassaing,
J., Orsini,
P. and Croset,
pour I’analyse des d&placements
H.,
1984.
des petits rongeurs
Utilisation
des
poudres
dans la nature. Mammalia,
fluo48:
293-298. Frame, L.H.,
1975. Mouse
Greenwood,
P.J., 1978.
wood mouse
runways.
Timing
Apodemus
Pactfic Discovery,
of activity
sylvaticus
Jamon, M , 1982. Capacites d/orientation en Camargue. These
28: 12-15.
of the bank vole
in a deciduous
Clethrionomys
woodland.
et exploitation
de 3eme cycle en Neurosciences,
Oikos,
glareolus,
de I’espace chez Apodemus Universite
and the
31: 123-127.
Aix-Marseille
sylvaticus
II
249 Jamon, M., 1987. populations. I.N.R.A.,
Le partage de I’espace chez In:
R. Campan & F. Spitz
le mulot
(editeurs),
et son
rBle
Organisation
dans
sociale
la dynamique chez
des
les vertebr+s.
Paris, pp. 167-180.
Jamon, M. & Benhamou,
S., 1989. Orientation
Jamon, M. & Bovet, P., 1987. Possible wood mice. Behav. Processes, Jennrich,
and movement
released inside and outside
mus sylvaticus)
R.I. & Turner,
a familiar
patterns
of wood
mice (Apode-
area. J. Comp. Psychol.,
use of environmental
gradients
103: 54-61.
in orientation
by “homing”
15: 93-107.
F.B., 1969. Measurement
of non-circular
home range. J. theor.
Biol.,
22:
227-237. Jones, N.B.,
1978. The use of a fluorescent
rodents.
Int. Biodeter.
Bull.,
pigment
Jupp, P.E. & Mardia, K.V., 1980. A general correlation regression Kuiper,
problems.
N.H.,
Biometrika,
1960. Tests
for tracing the movements
of commensal
14: 61-64. coefficient
for directional
data and related
67: 163-173.
concerning
random points
on a circle.
Konikl.
Nederl.
Akad. Wet.
A,
63: 38-47. Lemen, C.A., & Freeman,
P.W.,
1985. Tracking
animals
with
fluorescent
pigments:
a new tech-
nique. J. Mamm., 66: 134-136. MacDonald,
D.W.,
Madison,
1980. The red fox, Vulpes vulpes,
Z. Tierspsychol.,
terrestris.
D.M.,
1977. Movements
revealed by radiotelemetry. Mardia, K.V., 1972. Statistics Mazurkiewicz,
as a predator upon earthworms,
Lumbricus
42: 171-200. and habitat use among interacting
Can. FieldNat., of directional
Peromyscus
as
leucopus
91: 273-281,
data. Academic press,
M., 1969. Elliptical
modification
M., 1970.
of home
London.
of home range pattern.
Bull.
Acad. Pol. Sci., 17,
427-431. Mazurkiewicz, method. Mineau,
Bull.
Analysis
range directions
based on the catch-mark-release
Acad. Pol. Sci., 18: 465-468.
P&Madison,
D.,
1977.
Radiotracking
of
Peromyscus
leucopus.
Can.
J. Zool.,
55:
465-468. O’Farrel,
M.J., 1965. Home
range and ecology of snowshoe
hares in interior
Alaska.
J. Mamm.,
46: 408-418. Pearson,
O.P.,
169-l
1959.
A traffic
survey
of Microtus-Reithrodontomys
Peters, R.P., 1979. Mental maps in wolf territoriality. and ecology of wolves. Peters,
runways.
J. Mamm.,
40:
80.
R.P. & Mech, L.D.,
Pulliainen,
Z. Saugetierk.,
P.H.,
Science, 146: 1596-l C.C.,
1966.
in wolves.
(editor),
The behavior
pp. 119-152. Am. Scient.,
63: 628-637.
in the pine marten Martes martes in finnish
G.B., 1979. The social structure
Sanderson,
In: E.K. Klinghammer
New-York,
forest
lapland in
47: 91-99.
20 PP. Rawson, K.S. & Hartline, omyscus.
Press,
1975. Scent-marking
E., 1982. Scent marking
winter. Rathbun,
Garland STPM
The
1964.
and ecology of elephantshrews.
Telemetry
of homing
behavior
Advances in ethology, by the deermouse,
Per-
597.
study
of mammal
movements-a
review.
J. Wildl.
Manage.,
30:
215-235. Siniff,
D.B. & Jessen,
CR.,
1969. A simulation
model
of animal
movement
patterns.
Adv. Ecol.
Res., 6: 185-219. Smith,
J.N.M.,
analysis Smith,
1974a. The food searching behaviour
of search paths. Behaviour,
J.N.M.,
1974b. The food searching
ness of search patterns. Stephens,
M.A.,
1965.
The
Behaviour,
of two European thrushes.
I Description
and
48: 276-302. behaviour
of two European
thrushes.
II The adaptive-
49: l-61,
goodness-of-fit
statistic
VN:
distribution
and significance
point.
Biometri ka, 52: 309-321. Thompson, lepida
S.D.,
1982. Spatial utilization
lepida.
J. Mamm., 63: 570-581.
and foraging behavior of the desert woodrat
Neotoma
250 Twigg,
C.I., 1975a. Finding
Twigg,
C.I., 1975b. Marking
mammals,
Tyack,
P., 1981.
Interactions
P.M. &Wiley,
R.H.,
1979.
Marler & J.C. Vandenbergh behavior and communication. Wolton,
Mammal
between
nearby. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Waser,
their signs or remains.
mammals.
singing
Review,
Mammal
Review,
5: 71-82.
5: 101-106.
Hawarran
humback
whales
and conspecrfics
8: 105-116. Mechanism
(editors), Plenum
and evolution
Handbook Press,
R.J., 1983. The activity of free-ranging
of
of behavioral
New-York,
spacing
in
neurobiology,
animals. vol
in:
P.
3 Social
pp. 159-223.
wood mice Apodemus
sylvaticus.
J. Anim.
Ecol.,
52: 781-794. Wolton,
R.J., 1985. The rangrng behaviour
radro-tracking.
J. Zool.
A, 206: 203-224.
of wood
mice, Apodemus
sylvaticus,
as revealed by