Accepted Manuscript Anthony Sclafani: Consummate scientist Joseph R. Vasselli, Gerard P. Smith PII:
S0195-6663(16)30959-X
DOI:
10.1016/j.appet.2016.12.024
Reference:
APPET 3269
To appear in:
Appetite
Received Date: 20 October 2016 Accepted Date: 18 December 2016
Please cite this article as: Vasselli J.R. & Smith G.P., Anthony Sclafani: Consummate scientist, Appetite (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.12.024. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Anthony Sclafani: Consummate Scientist
RI PT
Joseph R Vassellia and Gerard P Smithb
New York Obesity-Nutrition Research Center, Department of Medicine, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
b
Department of Psychiatry, Weill Cornell Medicine, Payne Whitney Westchester, New York-Presbyterian Hospital, 21 Bloomingdale Road, White Plains, NY
TE D
M AN U
SC
a
Address for Correspondence:
AC C
EP
Joseph R. Vasselli, Ph.D. Columbia University Obesity-Nutrition Research Center Russ Berrie Medical Pavilion 1150 St. Nicholas Avenue – Suite 121 New York, NY 10032-3702 Phone: 212 342-6978 Fax: 212 851-5579 Cell: 203 640-4078 E-mail:
[email protected]
Keywords: hyperphagia, VMH syndrome, diet-induced obesity, palatability, carbohydrate appetite, taste preference
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2
Abstract In this article we review the scientific contributions of Anthony Sclafani, with specific
4
emphasis on his early work on the neural substrate of the ventromedial hypothalamic
5
(VMH) hyperphagia-obesity syndrome, and on the development of diet-induced obesity
6
(DIO). Over a period of 20 years Sclafani systematically investigated the neuroanatomical
7
basis of the VMH hyperphagia-obesity syndrome, and ultimately identified a longitudinal
8
oxytocin-containing neural tract contributing to its expression. This tract has since been
9
implicated in mediating the effects of at least two gastrointestinal satiety factors. Sclafani
SC
RI PT
1 2 3
was one of the first investigators to demonstrate DIO in rats as a result of exposure to
11
multiple palatable food items (the “supermarket diet”), and concluded that diet palatability
12
was the primary factor responsible for DIO. Sclafani went on to investigate the potency of
13
specific carbohydrate and fat stimuli for inducing hyperphagia, and in so doing discovered
14
that post-ingestive nutrient effects contribute to the elevated intake of palatable food
15
items. To further investigate this effect, he devised an intragastric infusion system which
16
allowed the introduction of nutrients into the gut paired with the oral intake of flavored
17
solutions, an apparatus her termed the “electronic esophagus”. Sclafani coined the term
18
“appetition” to describe the effect of intestinal nutrient sensing on post-ingestive appetite
19
stimulation. Sclafani’s productivity in the research areas he chose to investigate has
20
been nothing short of extraordinary, and his studies are characterized by inventive
21
hypothesizing and meticulous experimental design. His results and conclusions, to our
22
knowledge, have never been contradicted.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
10
23 24
Abstract Word Count: 245
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 3 25 26
1. Introduction If those of us who know Anthony (Tony) Sclafani were asked to provide a term describing his qualities as an investigator, the term most often used would undoubtedly
28
be some version of “consummate scientist”. This is the character of the man we have
29
come to know through his prolific contributions to the study of feeding behavior and its
30
neurological and physiological bases. His work spans more than four decades of
31
sustained research, and includes studies of hypothalamic hyperphagia, diet palatability,
32
dietary obesity, intestinal bypass, carbohydrate appetite, and more recently flavor-nutrient
33
conditioning and the role of intestinal nutrient receptors in ingestive behavior.
34
Remarkably, Sclafani has made landmark discoveries and original contributions to our
35
knowledge and techniques in all of these areas. His studies are characterized by
36
inventive hypothesizing and meticulous experimental design, and his productivity is
37
nothing short of extraordinary.
SC
M AN U
TE D
38
RI PT
27
Sclafani first became interested in the study of feeding as a result of a seminar he
40
took with Sebastian (“Pete”) Grossman as a beginning graduate student at the University
41
of Chicago in 1966. “In my first year at Chicago I took a seminar course with Grossman
42
in which each student had to review a brain area. I chose the hypothalamus, and among
43
the topics I reviewed (temperature control, aggression, thirst, hunger, etc.) I became most
44
interested in hunger and feeding. Therefore, when I joined Pete’s lab in the second year,
45
that was my area of research.” (Sclafani, personal communication to JRV, July 13, 2016).
46
This was indeed a fortunate development for the study of feeding behavior and its
47
biological bases. The young Sclafani entered the area of feeding behavior when the
48
reigning notion regarding the neurological basis of appetite and body weight regulation
AC C
EP
39
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4 was the Dual Center hypothesis, The hypothesis was based on landmark hypothalamic
50
lesion studies by Hetherington & Ranson (1940) and Anand and Brobeck (1951), and
51
formalized in the now classic motivational review by Eliot Stellar (Stellar, 1954).
52
According to the hypothesis, the lateral hypothalamus (LH), a “hunger center”, is inhibited
53
by signals from the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH), a “satiety center”. Support for this
54
notion came from studies demonstrating that electrical stimulation of these areas results
55
in feeding or satiety, while selective destruction of them leads to aphagia or hyperphagia,
56
respectively. Hyperphagia and obesity resulting from medial hypothalamic damage was
57
termed the VMH syndrome, and the belief at the time was that the ventromedial nucleus
58
(VMN) was the actual locus of feeding-inhibitory impulses. The Dual Center model led to
59
hundreds of research papers many of which, but not all, seemingly supported this
60
hypothesis. At the time Sclafani began his work on the VMH syndrome, however, the
61
hypothesis was already being called into question (Mogenson, 1974). We will review the
62
evolution of his studies, and the ingenious development of his research strategy in
63
addressing this issue, in the next section.
65
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
64
RI PT
49
When one examines the entirety of Sclafani’s research career, one of the most reliable themes which emerges is how issues arising in one area of investigation led him
67
productively into the next. This trend started with his study of the VMH syndrome itself,
68
which as we know is characterized by a dependence on diet palatability. Following
69
extensive studies of finickiness and willingness to work for food in normal and
70
hypothalamic hyperphagic rats, Sclafani introduced the dual lipostat model of hunger and
71
appetite, which integrated seemingly disparate results reported by numerous
72
investigators in this area (Sclafani & Kluge, 1974). This model brought into focus the
AC C
66
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 5 effects of palatable diets in stimulating body weight gain and altering the body weight
74
“setpoint” in an upward direction. A direct result of this hypothesis was his pursuit of the
75
effects of palatable diets on the development of obesity not only in hypothalamic obese
76
but also in normal animals (Sclafani & Berner, 1976), which led ultimately to his landmark
77
studies of dietary obesity. True to form, Sclafani systematically examined the role of age,
78
sex, exercise, diet type, and diet selection on the development of dietary obesity in
79
normal rats, thus expanding his research scope well beyond that of hypothalamic effects.
80
These contributions will also be reviewed in detail below.
SC
RI PT
73
M AN U
81 82 83 84
2.1 Challenging the Dual Center Hypothesis
85
student, was designed to directly test the Dual Center hypothesis by making long bilateral
86
parasagittal transections between the medial and lateral hypothalamus without significant
87
damage to either area. To make the transections, the investigators used a retractable
88
wire “knife” first introduced in this article (Sclafani & Grossman, 1969). The transections
89
resulted in significantly increased food intake and body weight over a 15-day post-
90
surgical period, supporting the notion that inhibitory fibers arising medially, when severed,
91
release feeding excitatory fibers in the lateral hypothalamus, resulting in sustained
92
hyperphagia. It is interesting to note that in this paper, Sclafani and Grossman reported
93
that one knife cut animal also had significant bilateral damage in the posterior
94
hypothalamus (presumably an effect of the surgical procedure), and displayed
95
hyperphagia and weight gain far in excess of the rest of the knife cut animals. In an early
96
clue which presaged Sclafani’s later thinking, the authors cited the possibility that the
97
VMH “may have direct caudal output to the midbrain through the posterior hypothalamus”
AC C
EP
TE D
Sclafani’s first published study with Grossman, conducted while he was a graduate
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 6 98
(Sclafani & Grossman, 1969, p. 536), a possibility originally suggested by Hetherington
99
and Ranson (1942).
100 Sclafani next undertook his dissertation work in Grossman’s laboratory, which
RI PT
101
involved a systematic series of experiments utilizing bilateral VMH electrolytic lesions,
103
bilateral long and short parasagittal knife cuts, and bilateral coronal transections either
104
anterior or posterior to the VMH. An improved version of the knife cut device, labeled the
105
“encephalotome”, was introduced in the article reporting this work (Sclafani, 1971).
106
Sclafani’s primary findings indicated that long parasagittal knife cuts, extending into the
107
anterior hypothalamic area (AHA), produced a VMH syndrome very similar to that
108
produced by VMH lesions in terms of degree of hyperphagia and body weight gain, and
109
in numerous behavioral tests including finickiness, food motivation, irritability, latency to
110
eat, and wood gnawing. In contrast, shorter parasagittal knife cuts limited to the region
111
between the ventromedial nucleus and lateral hypothalamus failed to generate a VMH-
112
like feeding and weight gain syndrome. Since both the long and shorter knife cuts should
113
in theory have severed inhibitory fibers arising medially and coursing to the lateral
114
hypothalamus, this latter observation was not consistent with the Dual Center hypothesis.
115
The coronal knife cuts also had no effect, probably because they were too medially
116
placed to transect hyperphagia-related fibers (Sclafani, 1971).
118
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
117
SC
102
These results prompted Sclafani to suggest a possible “rostrolateral orientation of
119
VMH feeding-related fibers” (Sclafani, 1971, p. 90), a possibility he would pursue avidly in
120
future studies. In fairness, others had also demonstrated VMH-like syndromes following
121
long transections between the medial and lateral hypothalamus (Albert & Storlien, 1969;
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 7 Gold, 1970; Grossman & Grossman, 1971), with Gold questioning the existence of satiety
123
fibers originating in the VMH, since his effective cuts were anterior and lateral to the
124
VMH. This was a very active time in the pursuit of the neuroanatomy of feeding behavior,
125
and a lively and productive competition was developing between numerous laboratories,
126
including those mentioned above.
RI PT
122
127
Sclafani, now working in his own laboratory at Brooklyn College, published three
SC
128
additional reports that raised serious doubts concerning the validity of the Dual Center
130
hypothesis. In the first of these (Sclafani & Maul, 1974), the investigators reasoned that
131
effective parasagittal knife cuts between the medial and lateral hypothalamus,
132
presumably severing inhibitory fibers coursing to lateral hypothalamus, should prevent
133
electrical stimulation of the VMN from inhibiting feeding in the rats, as it did prior to
134
surgery. This was not the case, however, again prompting Sclafani to consider the
135
possibility that rostral or caudal connections from the VMH may mediate its inhibitory
136
feeding effects.
TE D
EP
137
M AN U
129
In a second report (Sclafani, Berner & Maul, 1973), Sclafani and his co-
139
investigators demonstrated that parasagittal hypothalamic knife cuts differing in their
140
lateral plane from the midline differentially affected expression of the VMH syndrome,
141
with cuts at lateral 1.0 mm (just lateral to the VMN) being far more effective in provoking
142
hyperphagia and weight gain that those at lateral 1.5 mm (closer to the boarder of the
143
lateral hypothalamus). In a straightforward interpretation of the Dual Center hypothesis,
144
this result should not have been the case. In an inventive bit of original theorizing,
145
Sclafani reasoned in the third paper (Sclafani, Berner & Maul 1975) that these results
AC C
138
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 8 could still be consistent with the Dual Center hypothesis if some feeding excitatory fibers
147
were located in the perifornical region (between the medial and lateral hypothalamus),
148
while the remainder were located in the lateral hypothalamus (see Fig. 1). Knife cuts at
149
lateral 1.0 mm should then sever all of the medial fibers inhibiting both sets of feeding-
150
excitatory neurons, while knife cuts at lateral 1.5 mm would sever feeding-inhibitory
151
axons on lateral hypothalamic feeding excitatory cells, but in addition sever the feeding-
152
excitatory axons in the perifornical area entering the lateral hypothalamus. Thus
153
parasagittal knife cuts made at lateral 1.5 mm could result in the less pronounced
154
expression of hyperphagia, a result previously observed by the investigators.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
146
155 156
Sclafani further reasoned that if this were the case, then a lateral 1.0 mm cut made following an initial lateral 1.5 mm cut should make no difference in the relatively weak
158
expression of hyperphagia seen in 1.5 mm cut rats. However, as demonstrated in this
159
study (Sclafani et al., 1975), this turned out not to be the case, i.e., a lateral 1.0 mm cut
160
made following a lateral 1.5 mm cut resulted in full hyperphagia and excess body weight
161
gain. In a major departure from existing thinking, Sclafani concluded that “the results of
162
this study…challenge the basic assumption that the VMH inhibits the LH feeding system;
163
and this necessitates a reevaluation of hypothalamic feeding circuitry” (Sclafani et al.,
164
1975, p. 215). It should be noted that Sclafani generated these results in response to his
165
own theorizing and systematic testing, a strategy he used repeatedly in his work.
167 168 169
EP
AC C
166
TE D
157
2.2 Identification of a Longitudinal Feeding Inhibitory Pathway By this point, Sclafani was convinced that the hyperphagia and obesity produced by hypothalamic damage was due to interruption of a longitudinal feeding inhibitory
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 9 pathway. Acknowledging earlier reports demonstrating that overeating and obesity could
171
be produced by coronal knife cuts either anterior or posterior to the hypothalamus (Albert,
172
Storlien, Albert & Mah, 1971; Grossman, 1971; Palka, Leibelt & Critchlow, 1971), Sclafani
173
returned to the use of coronal cuts as an essential strategy in defining the course of a
174
longitudinal tract passing through and beyond the hypothalamus. In designing his next
175
study (Sclafani & Berner, 1977), Sclafani also made use of the strategy of asymmetric
176
knife cuts, as demonstrated earlier by Gold and co-workers (Gold, Quackenbush &
177
Kapatos, 1972; Gold, Jones, Sawchenko & Kapatos, 1977). Sclafani was able to show in
178
this study (Sclafani & Berner, 1977) that unilateral parasagittal hypothalamic knife cuts
179
combined with contralateral posterior hypothalamic or midbrain tegmental coronal cuts
180
resulted in hyperphagia and rapid body weight gain. In a direct comparison of the effects
181
of bilateral parasagittal vs. bilateral coronal posterior hypothalamic knife cuts, both
182
manipulations resulted in highly significant hyperphagia-weight gain effects, although the
183
magnitude of the effects was greater in rats bearing parasagittal knife cuts. Addressing
184
this difference, Sclafani speculated that the parasagittal cuts may have severed more
185
feeding inhibitory fibers than the coronal cuts at their respective anatomical locations.
186
Finally, in results similar to those of the earlier Sclafani et al. (1975) report, bilateral
187
coronal posterior hypothalamic cuts, followed 20 days later by bilateral parasagittal knife
188
cuts at lateral 1.5 mm, resulted in significantly greater hyperphagia and obesity than
189
obtained with the 1.5 mm lateral cuts alone.
191
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
190
RI PT
170
In the Discussion section of this paper Sclafani presented a long and carefully
192
reasoned analysis of the probable course of the feeding-inhibitory tract, utilizing his
193
results and the results of others to chart the probable course of the tract through the
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 10 medial hypothalamic area (Figure 2). Here he states that “..the feeding inhibitory fibers
195
responsible for the hyperphagia syndrome course in the perifornical region of the medial
196
forebrain bundle (MFB), and turn medially just rostral to the ventromedial nucleus”
197
Sclafani & Berner, 1977, p. 1014). After considering possible candidates for the identity of
198
this tract, including ascending noradrenergic and serotonergic fiber systems, he is careful
199
to state that “..it is possible that it is the destruction of a descending fiber system that is
200
responsible for hypothalamic hyperphagia.” (Sclafani & Berner, 1977, p. 1016), citing the
201
existence a descending pathway from the AHA to the midbrain recently identified by
202
Conrad and Pfaff (1975). This was in marked contrast to Gold’s hypothesis that
203
ascending noradrenergic and serotonergic pathways terminating in the paraventricular
204
nucleus (PVN), when transected, result in the overeating-obesity syndrome (Gold, 1973;
205
Kapatos & Gold, 1973).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
194
206
In an early attempt to trace the anatomical course of the longitudinal feeding-
TE D
207
inhibitory tract, Sclafani and colleagues studied axonal fiber degeneration using the Fink-
209
Heimer stain following hyperphagia-inducing parasagittal hypothalamic knife cuts in the
210
medial perifornical region of the hypothalamus (Mufson, Sclafani & Aravich, 1980).
211
Axonal damage radiated both anteriorally and posteriorally from the knife cut locus,
212
starting in the AHA, passing through the perifornical area of the hypothalamus along the
213
course of the medial forebrain bundle (MFB), and ultimately terminating in the midbrain
214
tegmental area. Medially and dorsally, axonal degeneration was seen in the AHA and
215
PVN. Sclafani’s own results, and the results of others (Swanson & Kuypers, 1980;
216
Swanson, Sawchenko, Wiegand & Price, 1980) now focused his attention specifically on
217
the PVN. As noted above, Gold and his co-workers had proposed that the PVN itself
AC C
EP
208
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 11 218
could be the terminus for ascending longitudinal feeding inhibitory fibers (Gold et al.,
219
1977). Moreover Leibowitz, Hammer & Chang (1981) had recently demonstrated that
220
lesions restricted to the PVN induced overeating and obesity in the rat.
222
RI PT
221 In their next study Aravich & Sclafani (1983) addressed the issue of whether PVN and VMH damage produced similar syndromes of hyperphagia and obesity. If true, this
224
would support the notion of a feeding inhibitory tract originating in the PVN which, when
225
interrupted at either locus, would result in the hypothalamic hyperphagia syndrome. In a
226
direct comparison of the effects of VMH knife cuts and PVN lesions, the authors found
227
that the feeding, body weight, and behavioral effects of the two types of damage were
228
remarkably similar, although parasagittal VMH knife cuts resulted in greater hyperphagia
229
and body weight gain. The authors concluded that “..the neuronal substrate responsible
230
for the hypothalamic hyperphagia syndrome is not confined to the paraventricular
231
nucleus, but is more diffusely organized in the medial and anterior hypothalamus”
232
(Aravich & Sclafani, 1983, p. 980). However, based on the fact that most effective
233
damage to the PVN was localized to the caudal parvocellular area of the nucleus, which
234
is rich in oxytocin-expressing cell bodies, the authors hypothesized that a descending
235
oxytocin-containing pathway from the PVN, when severed, is most likely responsible for
236
the hyperphagic effects specifically of PVN damage (Sclafani & Aravich, 1983).
238
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
237
SC
223
In an ambitious set of studies designed to define the caudal path of the
239
longitudinal feeding-inhibitory tract, and identify its neurotransmitter substrate, Sclafani
240
and his graduate student Annette Kirchgessner utilized asymmetrical knife cuts
241
consisting of combinations of parasagittal, coronal, and oblique transections
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 12 (Kirchgessner & Sclafani, 1988). The investigators found that unilateral parasagittal knife
243
cuts in the hypothalamus combined with contralateral coronal knife cuts in either the
244
ventrolateral pons or ventrolateral medulla resulted in significant hyperphagia and weight
245
gain. This was also the case for unilateral parasagittal knife cuts combined with
246
contralateral oblique knife cuts directly under the dorsal vagal complex. Importantly, the
247
combination of bilateral parasagittal hypothalamic knife cuts with a unilateral coronal
248
ventrolateral pontine cut produced no greater feeding effect than the parasagittal
249
hypothalamic knife cuts alone. This indicates that the asymmetrical knife cuts were
250
effective because they transected the same feeding-inhibitory fibers on both sides of the
251
brain. It remained true, however, that the asymmetrical knife cut conditions did not
252
generate as great a degree of hyperphagia and weight gain as bilateral parasagittal
253
hypothalamic knife cuts, suggesting that the asymmetrical cuts did not sever all of the
254
feeding-inhibitory fibers involved in the VMN hyperphagia syndrome. The authors
255
concluded that the caudal course of the PVN-feeding inhibitory pathway is through
256
posterior hypothalamus along the MFB, then passing through the ventrolateral pons and
257
medulla (probably in the hindbrain reticular formation), with a sharp dorsomedial turn
258
beyond this point towards the dorsal vagal complex. Although not demonstrated in this
259
study, the authors hypothesized that many of the feeding-inhibitory fibers terminate in the
260
nucleus of the solitary tract (NST) and dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve (DMX),
261
components of the dorsal vagal complex which modulate orosensory, viscerosensory,
262
and autonomic information in relation to feeding.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
242
263 264 265
In their final study, Sclafani and Kirchgessner utilized combinations of unilateral and bilateral hypothalamic and ventral pontine knife cuts, and immunocytochemical
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 13 analysis, to map the entire course of the PVN-hindbrain feeding-inhibitory tract, and
267
identify the neurotransmitter mediating its effects (Kirchgessner, Sclafani & Nilaver,
268
1988). The investigators first used horseradish peroxidase (HRP) applied to the site of a
269
unilateral coronal ventrolateral pontine reticular knife cut to identify HRP-labeled neurons
270
rostral to the cut (labeling results from the retrograde transport of HRP by cut axons to
271
the parent cell nucleus). Anteriorally, HRP-labeled neurons were found in the PVN,
272
particularly in the oxytocin-rich caudal parvocellular region, and in other anterior and
273
forebrain regions not involved in feeding, but none were found in the VMN. Posteriorally,
274
HRP-labeled neurons were found in the NST, although this labeling could be attributed to
275
the retrograde transport of HRP to ascending afferent neurons from the NST (an effect
276
consistent with later results, as detailed below).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
266
277
To investigate whether oxytocin is the mediating signal in the PVN-hindbrain
279
feeding inhibitory tract, and identify its caudal terminus, the investigators utilized an
280
immunoperoxidase oxytocin staining technique, and traced the effects of bilateral and
281
unilateral cuts at the hypothalamic and midbrain pontine levels. With this procedure,
282
transection of oxytocin-containing axons results in an accumulation of the neuropeptide in
283
stumps proximal to oxytocin-expressing cell bodies, and severe depletion of the
284
neuropeptide in stumps distal to the cut, including at the terminal synapse. Following the
285
knife cuts made in this study, a heavy concentration of oxytocin immunoreactive (OT-IR)
286
activity was found in transected axons and cell bodies in both the magnocellular and
287
parvocellular divisions of the PVN. In contrast, marked depletion of OT-IR was seen in
288
axon fibers along the entire course of the PVN-hindbrain tract, including in the posterior
AC C
EP
TE D
278
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 14 289
hypothalamus and midbrain and up to, but not including, NST cell bodies (Kirchgessner
290
et al., 1988).
291 The authors concluded that both medial hypothalamic and ventral pontine
RI PT
292
hyperphagia-inducing knife cuts transect a longitudinally coursing oxytocin-containing
294
tract from the PVN to the NST/DMX complex in the hindbrain, and interruption of this tract
295
is a major contributor to the VMH hyperphagia-obesity syndrome (see Fig. 3). Thus
296
almost 20 years after starting his investigation of the neural substrate underlying the VMH
297
hyperphagia-obesity syndrome, Sclafani and his co-workers successfully identified a
298
major pathway involved in its expression. The authors were careful to conclude, however,
299
that “The fact that PVN lesions are less hyperphagia-promoting than are MH knife cuts
300
indicates that the PVN-hindbrain pathway is only part of the anatomical circuit involved in
301
the MH hyperphagia syndrome” (Kirchgessner & Sclafani, 1988, p. 527). They suggest
302
that the VMH hyperphagia-obesity syndrome may result from two or more independent
303
neural systems that separately promote these effects, one PVN-associated, and the other
304
consisting of fiber projections from the VMN which mediate insulin release, and result in
305
hyperinsulinemia when severed (Kirchgessner & Sclafani, 1988).
307
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
306
SC
293
2.3 Current Status of the PVN-Hindbrain Pathway
308
One major bit of unfinished business pointed out by Sclafani and colleagues in the
309
discussion of his 1988 histology study was the issue of the physiological role of the PVN-
310
hindbrain pathway in feeding (Kirchgessner et al., 1988). Although a role in mediating
311
satiety can be assumed for this tract, based on the effects of its transection, no direct
312
evidence for its normal function existed at the time. This issue has been specifically
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 15 addressed in a recent series of studies by several investigators. In the first of these
314
reports, oleoylethanolamide (OEA), an endogenous small-intestinal lipid amide that
315
signals feeding inhibition via the vagus nerve, was shown to selectively activate neurons
316
in the PVN and NTS (Rodriguez de Fonseca, Navarro, Gómez, Escuredo, Nava et al.,
317
2001). Further work identified the central release of oxytocin in the PVN in response to
318
peripheral OEA administration as the mediator of this satiety effect, and demonstrated
319
that blockade of central oxytocin receptors by the intracerebroventricular infusion of an
320
oxytocin antagonist prevented the anorectic effects of OEA (Gaetani, Fu, Cassano,
321
Dipasquale, Romano et al., 2010). In further support of an PVN-NTS oxytocin-mediated
322
satiety pathway Grill and colleagues (Ong, Alhadeff & Grill, 2015) showed that
323
microinjection of oxytocin into the fourth ventricle or medial NTS of rats dose-dependently
324
inhibits food intake. Finally, in a study investigating the nature of ascending stimulation
325
from the NST which activates PVN oxytocin neurons Romano, Potes, Tempesta,
326
Cassano, Cuomo et al. (2015) demonstrated that chemical lesion of A2 noradrenergic
327
cell bodies in the NST, which provide direct excitatory input to the PVN, prevents OEA-
328
induced satiety in rats. Expression of oxytocin in the PVN was virtually eliminated in rats
329
receiving the NST A2 cell body lesions. These results demonstrated that an ascending
330
noradrenergic NST-PVN pathway mediates the activation of PVN-oxytocin satiety
331
signaling. Thus, experimental interruption of either the ascending noradrenergic or
332
descending oxytocinergic PVN-NST pathway can induce a satiety deficit.
334
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
333
RI PT
313
Additional support for the notion of a satiety function for PVN-NTS interconnections is
335
found in work by Rinaman and colleagues (Rinaman, Hoffman, Dohanics, Le, Stricker &
336
Verbalis, 1995), who demonstrated that the gut peptide cholecystokinin (CCK) activates
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 16 primarily noradrenergic neurons in the NTS and ventrolateral medulla that project to
338
oxytocin-expressing regions of the PVN, presumably mediating CCK satiety effects. That
339
CCK activates oxytocin-expressing neurons in the PVN had earlier been demonstrated
340
by Verbalis (Verbalis, Stricker, Robinson & Hoffman, 1991). Thus at least two intestinal
341
satiety factors may share use of the PVN-hindbrain pathway. Finally, recent studies in
342
mice have demonstrated that both oxytocin and oxytocin receptor knock-out lead to adult-
343
onset obesity, albeit without the expression of hyperphagia (Camerino, 2009; Takayanigi,
344
Kasahara, Onaka, Takahashi, Kawada & Nishimori, 2008). However, a study in which
345
oxytocin expression was inhibited or antagonized specifically in the hypothalamus of mice
346
demonstrated both increased food intake and body weight gain (Zhang, Bai, Zhang,
347
Dean, Wu, Li, Guariglia, Meng & Cai, 2011). Indeed, these and other studies have
348
provided a rationale for the potential use of oxytocin inhibitors as a treatment for obesity
349
(Blevins & Baskin, 2015).
SC
M AN U
TE D
350
RI PT
337
351 352 353
3.1 Sclafani and Diet-Induced Obesity
354
productive, Sclafani expanded it in the early 1970s to investigate diet-induced obesity
355
(DIO). When asked about this, he replied, “In my early studies of hypothalamic obesity I
356
was very interested in dietary finickiness and included palatable (high fat and sweetened
357
condensed milk) and unpalatable (powdered chow, quinine chow, cellulose) diets in my
358
experiments. In 1971 or 1972 I met Stanley Schachter and his then student, Judy Rodin,
359
at Columbia. They were writing about the similarities of obese humans and VMH rats
360
which further sparked my interest in comparing hypothalamic obese and dietary obese
361
animals. Schachter, as you may recall, was one of the founders of the Appetitive
AC C
EP
Although his research program on VMH hyperphagia and obesity was very
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 17 362
Seminar and it was my contacts with him that got me invited to the inaugural meeting of
363
the Seminar” (A. Sclafani, personal communication to GPS, April 27, 2016).
364 At first, Sclafani was unsuccessful in producing DIO: “In our hypothalamic studies
RI PT
365
we fed rats the then standard high-fat diet (33% Crisco, 67% powdered chow) that
367
induced enhanced weight gains in the VMH rats but not much weight gain in the controls
368
(Sclafani, Springer & Kluge, 1976). As I recall we then tried a higher fat diet (~60%) like
369
that used in the Peckman, Entenmen & Carroll (1962) study cited in our 1976 cafeteria
370
study, but it also produced relatively little weight gain. That prompted us to go all out with
371
supermarket foods. I had previously used sweetened condensed milk diets and Fruit
372
Loops in prior studies so suspected that these types of foods would be more effective
373
than Crisco-chow diets” (A. Sclafani, personal communication to GPS, April 27, 2016).
M AN U
SC
366
375
TE D
374
The supermarket, cafeteria diet consisted of a high-fat diet (33% Crisco fat, 67% Purina powder), sweetened condensed milk (Magnolia brand mixed with water, 1:1) and
377
a variety of other palatable supermarket foods including chocolate chip cookies, salami,
378
cheese, banana, marshmallows, milk chocolate, and peanut butter. At least 7 different
379
foods were available at any one time and the menu was changed frequently except that
380
Purina pellets, high-fat diet, and milk were always available (Sclafani & Springer, 1976).
381
This palatable diet worked (Figure 4).
AC C
382
EP
376
383
When the manuscript reporting these results was submitted to Physiology &
384
Behavior in July, 1974, however, the reviewers were not convinced. They suggested that
385
the development of obesity under the conditions of restricted, solitary space and a very
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 18 palatable diet had been shown before (Ingle, 1949) and was commonplace in livestock
387
and agricultural businesses (A. Sclafani, personal communication to GPS, April 27,
388
2016). Sclafani and Springer responded by repeating their experiment in rats housed in
389
isolation but also in group conditions. When female rats housed singly were switched
390
from a chow diet to the palatable supermarket diet, they gained weight at a faster rate
391
than chow-fed controls, with the difference in weight gain between the two groups
392
significant by day 10, and increasing up to day 60 (Figure 5). The authors observed that
393
“The 53% weight increase displayed by the experimental group after 60 days on the diet
394
is greater than that observed in previous studies using adult rats and high fat diets”
395
(Sclafani and Springer, 1976, p. 463).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
386
396 397
Non-systematic observation revealed that the rats ignored the chow and overate the supermarket foods, but intake of the numerous food items could not be measured
399
reliably. When the supermarket foods were removed and only chow pellets and water
400
were available, the body weights of the obese rats decreased to control values. When
401
the supermarket foods were returned to the rats, they again became obese (Figure 5).
402
Sclafani and Springer (1976) made a strong inference from this tight correlation:
403
“…although the postingestive effects may be of importance to some aspects of the
404
syndrome, it is the orosensory properties of the diet that appear to be primarily
405
responsible for the development of dietary obesity” (Sclafani & Springer, 1976, p. 463).
EP
AC C
406
TE D
398
407
In the same study (Sclafani & Springer, 1976), groups of female rats were
408
maintained in “complex environments that consisted of 3 large, wire mesh cages stacked
409
on top of each other. The doors of the cages were replaced with a single clear Plexiglas
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 19 door containing wire mesh ramps which connected the 3 levels. The upper two levels of
411
the environment had perforated metal floors, while the bottom level contained a metal
412
pan filled with wooden shavings. Wooden, metal, and at times, plastic objects were
413
located in the upper two levels (Figure 6). Under these enriched environmental
414
conditions, rats fed the supermarket diet gained significantly more body weight than rats
415
fed Purina chow, but an identical amount of body weight as rats fed the palatable
416
supermarket diet and maintained in isolated cages (Figure 7). This demonstrated that the
417
opportunity for increased physical activity and sensory and social stimulation experienced
418
by the rats in the enriched environment did not influence the degree of DIO stimulated by
419
the supermarket diet, an issue raised by the reviewers.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
410
420 421
The supermarket diet did not change the ad lib activity of DIO rats. Access to running wheels reduced the weight gain produced by the supermarket diet, but it did not
423
prevent the development of DIO (Figure 7). The gain of body weight of individual rats fed
424
the supermarket diet for 60 days, whether maintained in isolation or in the enriched
425
environment, was not only larger than rats fed Purina chow, but was also more varied
426
(85-200 g for supermarket vs. 10-80 g for chow (Figure 8). The variable susceptibility of
427
rats fed the supermarket diet to gain body weight observed by Sclafani & Springer (1976)
428
was similar to what had been observed in humans (Garrow,1978) and in rats given
429
palatable, high-calorie diets (Schemmel, Mickelson & Tolgay, 1969).
431
EP
AC C
430
TE D
422
The accurate measurement of intake of some of the foods in the supermarket diet
432
was not possible, so videotape analysis was undertaken. It showed that rats offered the
433
supermarket diet “sampled more than one food item during most of their meals.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 20 434
Furthermore, their number of meals appeared normal and thus, their overeating was
435
accomplished by increases in meal size” (Sclafani and Springer, 1976, p. 469).
436 When 24-hour intake of plain milk and quinine-adulterated milk (0.03%) was
438
measured in a 3-day intake test, DIO female rats had significantly smaller intakes of
439
unadulterated milk and quinine milk than chow-fed rats (Figure 9, left two panels). The
440
larger reduction of intake of quinine-adulterated milk in DIO rats was interpreted as
441
finickiness. This was consistent with Maller’s earlier report that DIO rats were finicky to
442
cellulose adulteration of their diet (Maller, 1964).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
437
443
In the same study (Sclafani and Springer, 1976), DIO rats maintained at 80 or 85
445
% of their ad lib body weights bar pressed less for food than control rats (Figure 5, and
446
Figure 9, extreme right panel). Thus, DIO rats were apparently less motivated to eat than
447
chow-fed rats. The results of a test of the effect of 4 days of food deprivation on wheel
448
running were consistent with this interpretation. Chow-fed rats rapidly increased their
449
wheel running activity under food deprivation, while the DIO rats maintained their activity
450
at baseline levels (Sclafani & Springer, 1976). This effect was replicated in a separate
451
study utilizing male DIO rats (Sclafani and Rendel, 1978), in which the DIO group did not
452
increase wheel running activity in response to food deprivation until day 9, while control
453
rats were more active as early as day 2.
EP
AC C
454
TE D
444
455
The longer latency to increase running during food deprivation was interpreted as
456
further evidence that DIO rats had reduced motivation to eat. When the latencies to run
457
during food deprivation were expressed as percent loss of body weight, however, the
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 21 difference in the latencies disappeared -- both groups increased activity when body
459
weights fell to 75-85% of pre-deprivation body weight (Sclafani and Rendel, 1978).
460
Rather than reduced motivation to eat, the authors reinterpreted their results to indicate
461
that body weight must fall to a critical percentage of predeprivation body weight before
462
activity increases during a fast.
463
465
3.2 A New Animal Model of Human Obesity
SC
464
RI PT
458
Note that the results with DIO rats were similar to previous results in hypothalamic obese rats: both kinds of rats were finicky to quinine-adulterated diets, bar pressed less
467
for food, and had longer latencies to increase activity when food deprived. Noting that
468
the same syndrome had been reported in obese humans, Sclafani concluded, “In view of
469
the fact human obesity is only rarely associated with hypothalamic damage, but is
470
associated with the availability of palatable foods and sedentary activity levels, dietary
471
obesity may be a more appropriate model for human obesity than is the hypothalamic
472
syndrome” (Sclafani and Springer, 1976, p.470).
474
TE D
EP
473
M AN U
466
Sclafani pursued the characteristics of this new animal model of human obesity. He found that DIO did not appear until after postnatal day 60, the percentage weight gain
476
on the cafeteria diet was significantly more in females than in males, and prior experience
477
with DIO did not affect its subsequent development (Sclafani and Gorman, 1977).
478 479
AC C
475
Abdominal vagotomy in DIO and hypothalamic obesity was investigated next. Its
480
effect depended on the diet: Abdominal vagotomy abolished the hypothalamic syndrome
481
in rats given chow, decreased it in rats given 20% sucrose solution, but did not change
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 22 hyperphagia and obesity in rats given cookies, sweet milk, and high-fat ration in addition
483
to chow. When the same obesogenic diet was given to brain-intact rats to produce DIO,
484
however, abdominal vagotomy decreased the gain of body weight significantly. These
485
differential results were the first evidence that the syndromes of hypothalamic and diet-
486
induced obesity were not identical (Sclafani, Aravich, and Landman,1981).
RI PT
482
487
Given the tight correlations between diet and obesity in DIO rats, and his belief
SC
488
that diet-induced hyperphagia was the primary cause of DIO, Sclafani began to
490
investigate the efficacy of high-carbohydrate and high-fat diets to produce hyperphagia
491
and obesity. In male rats, a sucrose solution (32%) produced obesity, but not
492
hyperphagia, while a Polycose solution (hydrolyzed corn starch, 32%) produced obesity
493
and hyperphagia (Sclafani and Xenakis, 1984a). When 32% sucrose was given to
494
female rats in addition to a composite or self-selected diet, however, it also produced
495
hyperphagia and obesity (Ackroff and Sclafani, 1988).
TE D
497
The form of Polycose was also important. A 32% solution of Polycose produced
EP
496
M AN U
489
hyperphagia and obesity in female rats, but Polycose had neither effect when it was
499
given as a powder or as a powder mixed in the chow diet (Sclafani and Xenakis, 1984b).
500
When Polycose was presented as a gel, it was also more potent than when it was
501
presented as a powder (Sclafani, Vigorito & Pfeiffer, 1988).
502 503
AC C
498
To determine if the palatable taste of sucrose or Polycose solutions contributed to
504
the hyperphagia, adult female rats were fitted with chronic intragastric catheters and
505
given ad lib access to chow and a drinking fluid that was paired with intragastric infusions
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 23 of 32% Polycose (Sclafani, Lucas, and Ackroff, 1996). Drinking Polycose (2%) plus 0.2%
507
saccharin under these conditions produced significantly more intake and body weight
508
gain than drinking 2% Polycose with bitter tasting 0.03% sucrose octaacetate. Thus,
509
when the oral taste was not preferred, intragastric Polycose produced a much smaller
510
increase of intake and had no effect on bodyweight. The authors concluded that the oral
511
palatability of carbohydrate solutions was a major determinant of hyperphagia and
512
obesity.
SC
RI PT
506
513
Complex results also occurred with high-fat diets. Rats fed chow and a separate
515
source of fat consumed more fat and total calories, and gained more body weight when
516
the fat source was vegetable shortening or emulsified corn oil than pure corn oil (Lucas,
517
Ackroff, and Sclafani, 1989) .When corn oil or vegetable shortening were presented as
518
gels, however, they increased intake and body weight. The hyperphagia was also
519
greater when chow was the alternative food than when separate sources of carbohydrate
520
and protein were available. Because the orosensory properties of the fat sources did not
521
account for the differential hyperphagia, it appeared that long- term fat selection and
522
caloric intake were influenced primarily by postingestive reinforcing factors.
524
TE D
EP
AC C
523
M AN U
514
This conclusion was apparently supported when rats self-infused significantly
525
more isocaloric high-fat liquid diet (HF) into the stomach than high carbohydrate liquid
526
diet (HC) (Lucas, Ackroff, and Sclafani (1998).
527
infusion of HF diet could also be explained by a reduced satiating effect of HF (Lucas and
528
Sclafani, 1999). The relative contribution of postoral reinforcing and satiating effects of
529
HF intragastric infusions in rats remains to be determined. Recent experiments in mice
However, the increased gastric self-
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 24 demonstrate the importance of the concentration of infused fat in the form of Intralipid for
531
the stimulation of intake and the formation of conditioned preferences (Ackroff and
532
Sclafani, 2014)*. The threshold concentration of Intralipid for stimulating intake and
533
conditioned preferences was 3.2%. Unfortunately, the threshold for a satiating effect was
534
not measured.
RI PT
530
535
Sclafani’s final two papers on DIO appeared in 2010 and 2012. The first used
SC
536
molecular genetic techniques in mice to investigate sugar-induced hyperphagia and
538
obesity in 4 strains of mice polymorphic for Tas1r3, the gene that codes for the T1R3
539
sugar taste receptor. The major results were that Tas1r3 genotype did not predict sugar-
540
induced hyperphagia and that susceptibility to sugar-induced obesity varied with strain,
541
and the concentration and type of sugar (Glendining, Breinager, Kyrillou, Lacuna, Rocha
542
& Sclafani, 2010). The last paper used knockout mice that differed in their sensitivity to
543
Polycose and sucrose. The results suggested: (1) sugar solutions must be highly
544
palatable to cause carbohydrate-induced obesity in mice, and (2) palatability could also
545
produce increased nutrient utilization because obesity could occur with little or no
546
hyperphagia (Glendining, Gilman, Zamer, Margolskee, & Sclafani, 2012).
EP
TE D
M AN U
537
548 549
AC C
547 3.3 Summary
Although Sclafani did not discover DIO, his early results in the 1970s convinced
550
many other investigators that DIO was a robust phenomenon accessible to experiment
551
and that it was a better animal model of human obesity than hypothalamic hyperphagia.
552
The experimental demonstration of DIO shattered the prevailing view that food intake in
553
rats under experimental conditions was tightly controlled by the caloric deficit that
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 25 554
developed between meals. This was the basis of Adolph’s famous dictum, “Rats eat for
555
calories” (Adolph, 1947).
556 Sclafani interpreted the importance of diet to mean that hyperphagia, not
RI PT
557
metabolic effects, was the primary effect of an obesogenic diet. The oral effects of
559
palatable, carbohydrate diets were adequate stimuli for the syndrome of hyperphagia and
560
obesity. In the case of high-fat diets, however, Sclafani suggested, but did not prove, that
561
postoral stimuli of ingested fat were the adequate stimuli for fat-induced hyperphagia and
562
obesity.
M AN U
SC
558
563 564 565
4.1 Conclusion
Sclafani continued his study of the stimulatory effects of sweet and fat stimuli on appetite. This led to his groundbreaking demonstrations of flavor-nutrient conditioning
567
based on the postingestive effects of nutrients delivered directly to the stomach and
568
proximal small intestine. This work was made possible by his introduction of the
569
“electronic esophagus” for rats and mice, which enabled him to pair the oral intake of
570
flavored solutions with the simultaneous infusion of nutrients directly to the gut (Elizalde &
571
Sclafani, 1990). In a major contribution to the lexicon of the feeding literature, Sclafani
572
coined the term “appetition” to describe the effect of intestinal nutrient sensing on post-
573
ingestive appetite stimulation (Sclafani, 2013). In so doing, he introduced a novel feeding-
574
stimulatory mechanism which promises to be a powerful explanatory concept in our
575
understanding of how palatable foods increase caloric intake and lead to excess body
576
weight gain, not only as a function of their oral taste qualities, but also as a result of
577
reinforcing effects mediated by small intestinal nutrient sensors.
AC C
EP
TE D
566
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 26 578 579
As noted above, Sclafani’s productivity in the research areas he chose to investigate has been nothing short of extraordinary. Table 1 presents a distribution of
581
published articles among major subject areas investigated by Sclafani and his colleagues
582
as of July, 2016. Sclafani’s publications, including research articles and reviews, total
583
over 300 currently. Research areas have been listed roughly in order of appearance in
584
Sclafani’s investigative career. There is inevitably some overlap of areas, but for our
585
purposes the articles are categorized on the basis of their primary research focus. It is
586
remarkable that in over 40 years of experiments on hypothalamic pathways, DIO, and
587
nutrient preferences and conditioning, his experimental results and interpretations were
588
never contradicted.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
580
589 590
Sclafani acknowledges the good fortune of having mentored many dedicated undergraduate, graduate and postdoctoral students in his laboratory who contributed
592
significantly to his research productivity. Not the least of these is his colleague and close
593
friend Karen Ackroff, who joined his laboratory in 1987 as a postdoctoral fellow, and
594
remained for 30 years as his co-investigator and co-director of his laboratory. Over this
595
period, Ackroff and Sclafani jointly authored 76 research articles. The experience of
596
working with Sclafani was described by Ackroff recently: “When I was finishing my
597
dissertation work and trying to write it up, my mentor George Collier provided a wonderful
598
distraction by giving me a stack of manuscripts to read. They were all focused on the
599
maltodextrin Polycose, recounting the various ways Tony Sclafani and his colleagues
600
sought to understand this unusual carbohydrate that animals treated as neither sugar nor
601
starch. Tony had asked George to review them prior to submission for a special issue of
AC C
EP
TE D
591
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 27 Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, and George in turn asked me. I recall my main
603
response as ‘why doesn’t he just come out and say it? There must be a ‘Polycose’
604
receptor! “When I heard that Tony had advertised for a postdoc, I spoke to him at the
605
next Columbia Appetitive Seminar. Perhaps my credentials as a Collier student were
606
good enough for him. And, I knew his Polycose work!”
607
RI PT
602
“I started in the lab just as Tony and his students were developing the “electronic
609
esophagus”, the system that infused rats intragastrically whenever they licked a drinking
610
spout. This was another mark of Tony’s science – to devise apparatus that supports
611
creative approaches to the physiology underlying the behavior. Tony had begun
612
developing computer controls and interfacing them with the relay rack equipment that had
613
been the standard for behavioral research for so long. With my background from the
614
Collier lab, I became the second computer programmer, modifying the code for new kinds
615
of experiments. The lab was full of post-docs, graduate and undergraduate students, and
616
technicians running studies all day. Tony led by example: he was in the lab early every
617
day, and usually came in on weekends. Although there might be a dozen or more studies
618
in progress, he kept track of them all and discussed them frequently with the
619
experimenters. The most satisfying time in the lab was spent reviewing the data and
620
evaluating possible explanations and next steps, devising plans for the current study and
621
those to follow.”
623
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
622
SC
608
“Tony’s leadership of his lab always conveyed his scientific values: focus on the
624
problem, devise creative ways to get the answers, and report the information clearly. His
625
hundreds of publications and many invited talks attest to the respect his work has earned.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 28 626
His lab was an amazing place to conduct great research, and I was fortunate to be able
627
to stay there for my entire postgraduate career” (K. Ackroff, personal communication, to
628
JRV, September 14, 2016).
630
RI PT
629 No description of Sclafani and his contributions would be complete without
mention of his ability to train and motivate his students. Paul Aravich, who earned his
632
doctoral degree with Sclafani in December of 1982, described what it was like being a
633
graduate student of Tony’s: “Tony is one of the most remarkable scientists I have ever
634
met. In my dissertation 33 years ago I stated that ‘I have never met, nor do I expect to
635
meet, a more dedicated and cautious scientist.’ Those words are as true now as then. He
636
accepted a kid from the coal fields of Pennsylvania who did not see the ocean until he
637
went to college. As such, he changed my life and the life of my wife, Michele, in more
638
ways than we can count. He taught me how to think critically and to have a broad
639
integrated systems approach. He taught me to be an investigator-educator and gave me
640
significant undergraduate teaching responsibilities along with significant research
641
responsibilities. In the short run balancing these responsibilities was problematic; in the
642
long run balancing them was an invaluable experience.
644
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
643
SC
631
“Tony said, ‘I don’t require you to work 7-days a week, but sometimes the
645
experiments do.’ As such, no one worked harder than him. Despite that, he regularly
646
helped me do knife cut surgeries and brain cannulations. Tony taught me the thrill of the
647
conception of an idea; the thrill of the delivery of an idea; and the sometimes more than 9
648
months of hard labor in between. He taught me that the scientist is the messenger and
649
never the message. When I became a postdoc and a research associate, I appreciated to
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 29 650
an even greater extent the exceptional training he gave me and the amazing access I
651
had to his wisdom and expertise.” (P. Aravich, personal communication to JRV, August 6,
652
2016).
654
RI PT
653 In Sclafani’s long and distinguished academic career, now spanning almost 50 years, there has been no shortage of honors and accolades. In addition to his
656
appointment as Distinguished Professor of Psychology at Brooklyn College in 1994, he
657
was awarded the Tow Professorship at Brooklyn College (1993-1995), an honor
658
bestowed upon “faculty whose talents and accomplishments are of a particularly high
659
order”. In addition to numerous RO1 awards, Sclafani won two consecutive Senior
660
Research Scientist Awards from NIMH for the study of carbohydrate appetite and obesity
661
(1992-2000), and was then honored with an NIH Merit Award over the 10-year period
662
2001–2011. NIH Merit Awards recognize “superior competence and outstanding
663
productivity in pursuit of research in areas of special importance”, and fewer than 5% of
664
NIH investigators receive this honor. Sclafani was one of the first elected presidents of
665
the newly formed North American Association for the Study of Obesity in 1989 (now The
666
Obesity Society). He played a major hand in founding the Society for the Study of
667
Ingestive Behavior, and was elected president of the society in 1992. He cites
668
participation in these groups as having had a major impact on his research career,
669
fostering fruitful collaborations with many distinguished scientists.
671 672 673
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
670
SC
655
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 30
*Footnote
675
There are a number of papers by Sclafani on acquired food preference by oral-
676
postingestive nutrient conditioning not discussed here (see review by Sclafani and
677
Ackroff, 2012 and chapter by Myers in this Festschrift).
678
RI PT
674
Acknowledgements
680
The authors would like to thank Dr. Carol Maggio for her assistance in preparation of the
681
manuscript, and her insightful review of the text. We also thank Drs. Karen Ackroff and
682
Paul Aravich for their willingness to share their experiences in working with Dr. Sclafani.
683
JRV would like to gratefully acknowledge the training and guidance Tony provided during
684
his years as a postdoctoral fellow in the Sclafani laboratory at Brooklyn College (1974-
685
1976).
M AN U
SC
679
TE D
686 References
688
Ackroff, K., &, Sclafani, A. (1988). Sucrose-induced hyperphagia and obesity in rats fed
689
a macronutrient self-selection diet. Physiology & Behavior, 44, 181-187.
690
Adolph, E.F. (1947). Urges to eat and drink in rats. American Journal of Physiology,
691
151, 110-125.
692
Albert D.J., & Storlien, L.H. (1969). Hyperphagia in rats with cuts between the
693
ventromedial and lateral hypothalamus. Science, 165, 599-600.
694
Albert, D.J., Storlien, L.H., Albert, J.G., & Mah, C.J. (1971). Obesity following
695
disturbance of the ventromedial hypothalamus: a comparison of lesions, lateral cuts, and
696
anterior cuts. Physiology & Behavior, 7, 135-141.
AC C
EP
687
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 31 Anand, B.K. & Brobeck, J.R. (1951). Hypothalamic controls of food intake in rats and
698
cats. Yale Journal of Biological Medicine, 24, 123-139.
699
Aravich, P.F., & Sclafani, A. (1983). Paraventricular hypothalamic lesions and medial
700
hypothalamic knife cuts produce similar hyperphagia syndromes. Behavioral
701
Neuroscience, 97, 970-983.
702
Conrad, L.C.A., & Pfaff, D.W. (1975). Axonal projections of medial preoptic and anterior
703
hypothalamic neurons. Science, 190, 1112-1114.
704
Elizalde, G., & Sclafani, A. (1990). Flavor preferences conditioned by intragastric
705
Polycose infusions: A detailed analysis using an electronic esophagus preparation.
706
Physiology & Behavior, 47, 63-77.
707
Gaetani, S., Fu, J., Cassano, T., Dipasquale, P., Romano, A., Righetti, L., Cianci, S.,
708
Laconca, L., Giannini, E., Scaccianoce, S., Mairesse, J., Cuomo, V., & Piomelli, D.
709
(2010). The fat-induced satiety factor oleoylethanolamide suppresses feeding through
710
central release of oxytocin. Journal of Neuroscience., 30, 8096-8101.
711
Garrow, J.S. (1978). The regulation of energy expenditure in man. In G.A. Bray (Ed.),
712
Advances in Obesity Research: II. (pp. 200-210). London: Newman.
713
Glendining, J.L., Breinager, L., Kyrillo, E., Kacuna, K., Rocha, R., & Sclafani, A. (2010).
714
Differential effects of sucrose and fructose on dietary obesity in four mouse strains.
715
Physiology & Behavior, 101, 331-343.
716
Glendining, J.L., Gilman, J., Zamer, H., Margolskee, R.F., & Sclafani, A. (2012). The
717
role of T1r3 and Trpm5 in carbohydrate-induced obesity in mice. Physiology & Behavior,
718
107, 50-58.
719
Gold, R.M. (1970). Hypothalamic hyperphagia produced by parasagittal knife cuts.
720
Physiology & Behavior, 5, 23-25.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
697
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 32 Gold, R. M. (1973). Hypothalamic obesity: the myth of the ventromedial nucleus.
722
Science, 182, 488-490.
723
Gold, R.M., Jones, A.P., & Sawchenko, P.E. (1977). Paraventricular area: critical focus
724
of a longitudinal neurocircuitry mediating food intake. Physiology & Behavior, 18, 1111-
725
1119.
726
Gold, R.M., Quackenbush, P.M., & Kapatos, G. (1972). Obesity following combination of
727
rostrolateral to VMH cut and contralateral mammillary area lesion. Journal of
728
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 79, 210-218.
729
Grossman, S.P. (1971). Changes in food and water intake associated with an
730
interruption of the anterior or posterior fiber connections of the hypothalamus. Journal of
731
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 75, 23-31.
732
Grossman, S.P., & Grossman, L. (1971). Food and water intake in rats with parasagittal
733
knife cuts medial or lateral to the lateral hypothalamus. Journal of Comparative and
734
Physiological Psychology, 74, 148-156.
735
Hetherington, A.W. & Ranson, S.W. (1940). Hypothalamic lesions and adiposity in the
736
rat. Anatomical Record, 78, 149-172.
737
Hetherington, A. W., & Ranson, S.W. (1942). The spontaneous activity and food intake
738
of rats with hypothalamic lesions. American Journal of Physiology, 136, 609-817.
739
Ingle, D.J. (1949). A simple means of producing obesity in the rat. Proceedings of the
740
Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 72, 604-605.
741
Kapatos, G., & Gold, R.M. (1973). Evidence for ascending noradrenergic mediation of
742
hypothalamic hyperphagia. Pharmacology, Biochemistry &. Behavior, 1, 81-87.
743
Kirchgessner, A.L., & Sclafani, A. (1988). PVN-hindbrain pathway involved in the
744
hypothalamic hyperphagia-obesity syndrome. Physiology & Behavior, 42, 517-528.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
721
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 33 Kirchgessner, A.L., Sclafani, A., & Nilaver, G. (1988). Histochemical identification of a
746
PVN-hindbrain feeding pathway. Physiology & Behavior, 42, 529-543.
747
Leibowitz, S.F., Hammer, N.J., & Chang, K. (1981). Hypothalamic paraventricular
748
nucleus lesions produce overeating ad obesity in the rat. Physiology & Behavior, 27,
749
1031-1040.
750
Lucas, F., Ackroff, K., & Sclafani, A. (1989). Dietary fat-induced hyperphagia in rats as a
751
function of fat type and physical form. Physiology & Behavior, 45, 937-946.
752
Lucas, F., Ackroff, K., & Sclafani, A. (1998). High-fat diet preference and overeating
753
mediated by postingestive factors in rats. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory,
754
Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 275: R1511-R1522.
755
Lucas, F., & Sclafani, A. (1999). Differential reinforcing and satiating effects of
756
intragastric fat and carbohydrate infusions in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 66, 381-388.
757
Maller, O. (1964). The effect of hypothalamic and dietary obesity on taste preference in
758
rats. Life Sciences, 3, 1281-1291.
759
Mogenson, G.N. (1974). Changing views of the role of the hypothalamus in the control
760
of ingestive behaviors. In Lederis, K & Cooper, K.E. (Eds.) Recent Studies of
761
Hypothalamic Function (268-293). Basil: Karger.
762
Mufson, E.J., Sclafani, A., & Aravich, P.F. (1980). Fiber degeneration associated with
763
hyperphagia-inducing knife cuts in the hypothalamus. Experimental Neurology, 67, 633-
764
645.
765
Ong, Z.Y., Alhadeff, A.L., & Grill, H.J. (2015). Medial nucleus tractus solitarius oxytocin
766
receptor signaling and food intake control: the role of gastrointestinal satiation signal
767
processing. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative
768
Physiology, 308, R800-R806.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
745
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 34 Palka, Y., Liebelt, R.A., & Critchlow, V. (1971). Obesity and increased growth following
770
partial or complete isolation of ventromedial hypothalamus. Physiology & Behavior, 7,
771
187-194.
772
Paxinos, G., & Watson, C. (1982). The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. New York:
773
Academic Press.
774
Peckman, S.C., Entenmen, C., & Carroll, H.W. (1962). The influence of a hypercaloric
775
diet on gross body and adipose tissue composition in the rat. Journal of Nutrition, 77,
776
187-197.
777
Rinaman, L., Hoffman, G.E., Dohanics, J., Le, W.W., Stricker, E.M., & Verbalis, J.G.
778
(1995). Cholecystokinin activates catecholaminergic neurons in the caudal medulla that
779
innervate the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus in rats. Journal of Comparative
780
Neurology, 360, 246-256.
781
Rodriguez de Fonseca, F., Navarro, M., Gómez, R., Escuredo, L., Nava, F., Fu, J.,
782
Murillo-Rodriguez, E., Gluffrida, A., LoVerme, J., Gaetani, S., Kathuria, S., Gall, C., &
783
Piomelli, D. (2001). An anorectic lipid mediator regulated by feeding. Nature, 414, 209-
784
212.
785
Romano, A., Soares Potes, C., Tempesta, B., Cassano, T., Cuomo, V., Lutz, T., &
786
Gaetani, S. (2013). Hindbrain noradrenergic input to the hypothalamic PVN mediates the
787
activation of oxytocinergic neurons induced by the satiety factor oleoylethanolamide.
788
American Journal of Physiology. Endocrinology and Metabolism , 305, E1266-E1273.
789
Schemmel, R., Mickelson, O., & Tolgay, Z. (1969). Dietary obesity in rats: Influences of
790
diet, weight, age, and sex on body composition. American Journal of Physiology, 216,
791
373-379.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
769
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 35 Sclafani, A. (1971). Neural pathways involved in the ventromedial hypothalamic lesion
793
syndrome in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 77, 70-96.
794
Sclafani, A. (2013). Gut-brain nutrient signaling: appetition vs. satiation. Appetite, 71,
795
454-458.
796
Sclafani, A., & Ackroff, K. (2012). Role of gut nutrient sensing in stimulating appetite and
797
conditioning food preferences. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative
798
and Comparative Physiology, 302, R1119-R1133.
799
Sclafani, A., Aravich, P.F., & Landman, M. (1981). Vagotomy blocks hypothalamic
800
hyperphagia on a chow diet and sucrose solution, but not on a palatable mixed diet.
801
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95, 720-734.
802
Sclafani, A., & Berner, C.N. (1976). Influence of diet palatability on the meal taking
803
behavior of hypothalamic hyperphagic and normal rats. Physiology & Behavior, 16, 355-
804
363.
805
Sclafani, A., & Berner, C.N. (1977). Hyperphagia and obesity produced by parasagittal
806
and coronal hypothalamic knife cuts: further evidence for a longitudinal feeding inhibitory
807
pathway. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 1000-1018.
808
Sclafani, A., Berner, C.N., & Maul, G. (1973). Feeding and drinking pathways between
809
medial and lateral hypothalamus in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological.
810
Psychology, 85, 29-51.
811
Sclafani, A., Berner, C.N., & Maul, G. (1975). Multiple knife cuts between the medial and
812
lateral hypothalamus in the rat: a reevaluation of hypothalamic feeding circuitry. Journal
813
of Comparative and Physiological. Psychology, 88, 210-217.
814
Sclafani, A., & Gorman, A.N. (1977). Effects of age, sex, and prior body weight on the
815
development of dietary obesity in adult rats. Physiology & Behavior, 18, 1021-1026.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
792
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 36 Sclafani, A., & Grossman, S.P. (1969). Hyperphagia produced by knife cuts between the
817
medial and lateral hypothalamus in the rat. Physiology & Behavior., 4, 533-537.
818
Sclafani, A., & Kluge, L. (1974). Food motivation and body weight levels in hypothalamic
819
hyperphagic rats: a dual lipostatic model of hunger and appetite. Journal of Comparative
820
and Physiological Psychology, 86, 28-46.
821
Sclafani, A., Lucas, F., & Ackroff, K. (1996). The importance of taste and palatability in
822
carbohydrate-induced overeating in rats. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory,
823
Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 271, R1197-R1202.
824
Sclafani, A., & Maul, G. (1974). Does the ventromedial hypothalamus inhibit the lateral
825
hypothalamus? Physiology & Behavior, 12, 157-162.
826
Sclafani, A. & Rendel, A. (1978). Food deprivation-induced activity in dietary obese,
827
dietary lean, and normal-weight rats. Behavioral Biology, 24, 220-228.
828
Sclafani, A., & Springer, D. (1976). Dietary obesity in adult rats: Similarities to
829
hypothalamic and human obesity syndromes. Physiology & Behavior, 17, 461-471.
830
Sclafani, A., Springer, D., & Kluge, L. (1976). Effects of quinine adulterated diets on the
831
food intake and body weight of obese and non-obese hypothalamic hyperphagic rats.
832
Physiology & Behavior, 16, 631-640.
833
Sclafani, A., Vigorito, M, & Pfeiffer, C.L. (1988). Starch-induced overeating and
834
overweight in rats. Influence of starch type and form. Physiology & Behavior, 42, 409-
835
415.
836
Sclafani, A., & Xenakis, S. (1984a). Sucrose and polysaccharide induced obesity in the
837
rat. Physiology & Behavior, 32, 169-174.
838
Sclafani, A., & Xenakis, S. (1984b). Influence of diet form on the hyperphagia-promoting
839
effect of polysaccharide in rats. Life Sciences, 8, 491-508.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
816
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 37 Stellar, E. (1954). The physiology of motivation. Psychological Reviews, 61, 5-22.
841
Swanson, L.W., & Kuypers, H.G.J.M. (1980). The paraventricular nucleus of the
842
hypothalamus: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and organization of projections to the
843
pituitary, dorsal vagal complex and spinal cord as demonstrated by retrograde
844
fluorescence double-labeling methods. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 194, 555-570.
845
Swanson, L.W., Sawchenko, P.E., Wiegand S.J., & Price, J.L. (1980). Separate neurons
846
in the paraventricular nucleus project to the median eminence and to the medulla or
847
spinal cord. Brain Research, 197, 207-212.
848
Verbalis, J.G., Stricker, E.M., Robinson, A. G., & Hoffman, G.E. (1991). Cholecystokinin
849
activates c-fos expression in hypothalamic oxytocin and corticotrophin-releasing hormone
850
neurons. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 3, 205-213.
M AN U
851
856 857 858 859 860 861 862 863
EP
855
AC C
854
TE D
852 853
SC
RI PT
840
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 38 864 865 866
Table 1. Anthony Sclafani Research Areas and Number of Publications, 1969-2016
RI PT
867 Hypothalamic Pathways
15
Dietary Responsiveness and Finickiness
SC
Dietary Obesity and It’s Metabolic Effects
28
11
Intestinal Bypass Effects on Feeding and Obesity
10
Carbohydrate Appetite and Sweet Taste Responsivity
51
TE D
M AN U
Role of the Vagus Nerve and Autonomic Nervous System in the VMH Syndrome
Flavor/Nutrient Preferences and Conditioning
870 871 872 873 874
AC C
869
EP
Intestinal Nutrient Receptors and Appetition
868
28
119
40
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 39 875 876 877
Figure Legends
RI PT
878 Figure 1. Anatomical model proposed by Sclafani et al. (1973) to explain differential
880
feeding effects of 1.0 and 1.5 knife cuts. A knife cut at L 1.0 mm would sever feeding-
881
inhibitory projections to both sets of feeding excitatory neurons, while a knife cut at L 1.5
882
mm would sever feeding inhibitory projections only to LH feeding-excitatory neurons,
883
while also transecting feeding-excitatory projections to the LH. (VMH = ventromedial
884
hypothalamus; LH = Lateral Hypothalamus F = fornix; I = feeding inhibitory neuron E =
885
feeding excitatory neuron). (Adapted with permission from Sclafani et al. 1975, Vol. 88(1),
886
p. 211, Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, American Psychological
887
Assn.).
M AN U
TE D
888
SC
879
Figure 2. Horizontal sections through the hypothalamus illustrating knife cuts that are
890
effective in producing hyperphagia and obesity (Effective Cuts, blue), and those that are
891
not effective (Ineffective cuts, red), based on the findings of Albert, Gold, Grossman,
892
Paxinos, Sclafani, and others. The position of these cuts suggests that the pathway
893
whose destruction is responsible for the hyperphagia syndrome either originates in the
894
preoptic-anterior hypothalamic area (PO-AH) and descends to the mid- or hindbrain, or
895
ascends from the caudal brainstem to terminate in the PO-AH area. (Adapted with
896
permission from Sclafani & Berner, 1977, Vol. 91(5), p. 1014, Journal of Comparative &
897
Physiological Psychology, American Psychological Assn.).
898
AC C
EP
889
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 40 Figure 3. Schematic representation of the “feeding” pathway implicated in the MH
900
hyperphagia-obesity syndrome. The pathway appears to originate in the PVN and
901
surrounding tissue, and projects laterally and caudally with at least some of the fibers
902
extending to the medulla. The NST and/or DX are likely termination sites for the pathway.
903
Illustrated are a unilateral MH cut and contralateral vM and vP cuts. This horizontal
904
section was adapted from Paxinos and Watson, 1982. (AP = area postrema; DX = dorsal
905
motor nucleus of the vagus; MH = medial hypothalamus; MPO = medial preoptic area;
906
NST = nucleus of the solitary tract; PVN = paraventricular nucleus; vM = ventral medulla;
907
vP = ventral pons; VMN = ventromedial nucleus). (Reprinted with permission from
908
Kirchgessner & Sclafani, 1988, Vol. 42(6), p. 526, Physiology & Behavior, Elsevier Press)
M AN U
SC
RI PT
899
909
Figure 4. DIO in a female rat after 3-4 months on the supermarket cafeteria diet
911
(provided by Professor Anthony Sclafani).
912
TE D
910
Figure 5. Body weight as a function of days and diet in experimental (EXP) and (CON)
914
groups of female rats (lower left of figure). The EXP group was switched to the cafeteria
915
diet at the interval above the arrowhead. Graph in the upper left shows bar pressing for
916
food reward by the two groups of rats as a function of a fixed ratio schedule. Graph on
917
the right shows decreased body weight gain as a result of pelleted chow diet in EXP and
918
quinine adulteration in CON. The graph in the lower middle illustrates food intake by EXP
919
and CON during the 3-day quinine- adulterated-diet test (Reprinted with permission from
920
Sclafani and Springer, 1976, Vol. 17(3), p. 462, Physiology & Behavior, Elsevier Press).
921
AC C
EP
913
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 41 922
Figure 6. Enriched group environment in which supermarket cafeteria diet also produced
923
DIO. (Reprinted with permission from Sclafani and Springer, 1976, Vol. 17(3), p. 466,
924
Physiology & Behavior, Elsevier Press).
RI PT
925 Figure 7. Mean body weight gains of groups fed supermarket (S) or Purina chow (P)
927
diets and housed in enriched (E), isolated (I), or activity (A) cages. Weight gains based
928
on body weights of rats on (Day 10) just prior to their transfer to the different housing
929
conditions. On Day 0, E-S, IS, and A-S groups were given supermarket diet. On Day 65,
930
indicated by the arrows, I-S and I-P groups were placed in the activity wheels becoming
931
groups IA-S and IA-P, while A-S and A-P groups were returned to isolated cages
932
becoming AI-S and AI-P groups. (Reprinted with permission from Sclafani and Springer,
933
1976, Vol. 17(3), p. 467, Physiology & Behavior, Elsevier Press).
M AN U
SC
926
934
Figure 8. Frequency histogram of weight gains displayed by 26 experimental (EXP) and
936
26 control (CON) rats after 60 days on the supermarket or Purina chow diets. (Reprinted
937
with permission from Sclafani and Springer, 1976, Vol. 17(3), p. 470, Physiology &
938
Behavior, Elsevier Press).
EP
AC C
939
TE D
935
940
Figure 9. Twenty-four hour milk intake (3 left panels) and bar pressing responses
941
(extreme right panel) of dietary obese (filled columns) and control (empty columns)
942
female rats offered plain milk and 0.03% quinine adulterated milk. DIO and chow-fed
943
control rats were reduced to 85% of their ad libitum body weight for the bar-pressing test.
944
(Reprinted with permission from Sclafani and Springer, 1976, Vol. 17(3), p. 464,
945
Physiology & Behavior, Elsevier Press).
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Figure 1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 43
Figure 2
INEFFECTIVE CUTS
RI PT
EFFECTIVE CUTS
A-8
PO
A-7
AH
AH
M AN U
A-6 VM
SC
PO
VM
A-5 A-4
L-1
L-0
AC C
EP
TE D
L-2
L-2
L-1
L-0
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 44
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Fig. 3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 45
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Fig. 4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 46
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Fig. 5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 47
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Figure 6.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 48
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Fig. 7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 49
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Fig. 8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 50
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Fig. 9