Anticipation in sport

Anticipation in sport

Accepted Manuscript Title: Anticipation in sportAnticipation in sport–> Authors: Florian Loffing, Rouwen Ca˜nal-Bruland PII: DOI: Reference: S2352-25...

297KB Sizes 1 Downloads 125 Views

Accepted Manuscript Title: Anticipation in sportAnticipation in sport–> Authors: Florian Loffing, Rouwen Ca˜nal-Bruland PII: DOI: Reference:

S2352-250X(16)30143-9 http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.008 COPSYC 397

To appear in: Please cite this article as: Florian Loffing, Rouwen Ca˜nal-Bruland, Anticipation in sport (2010), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.008 This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1 Current Opinion in Psychology – Special Issue: Sport Psychology 2017

2nd resubmission: 08.03.2017

Anticipation in sport

Florian Loffinga & Rouwen Cañal-Brulandb

a

Institute of Sport Science, University of Oldenburg, Ammerländer Heerstr. 114-118, 26111

Oldenburg, Germany; [email protected] b

Institute of Sport Science, Friedrich-Schiller-University Jena, Seidelstraße 20, 07749 Jena,

Germany; [email protected]

Corresponding author: Rouwen Cañal-Bruland, Institute of Sport Science, Friedrich-Schiller-University Jena, Seidelstraße 20, 07749 Jena, Germany; Email: [email protected]

Abstract Anticipation has become an increasingly important research area within sport psychology since its infancy in the late 1970s. Early work has increased our fundamental understanding of skilled anticipation in sports and how this skill is developed. With increasing theoretical and practical insights and concurrent technological advancements, researchers are now able to tackle more detailed questions with sophisticated methods. Despite this welcomed progress, some fundamental questions and challenges remain to be addressed, including the (relative) contributions of visual

2 and motor experience to anticipation, intra- and interindividual variation in gaze behaviour, and the impact of non-kinematic (contextual or situational) information on performance and its interaction with advanced kinematic cues during the planning and execution of (re)actions in sport. The aim of this opinion paper is to shortly sketch the state of the art, and then to discuss recent work that has started to systematically address open challenges thereby inspiring promising future routes for research on anticipation and its application in practice. What makes players successfully return tennis serves approaching with speeds over 200 km/h (about 125 mph) or hit baseball pitches delivered with speeds over 145 km/h (about 90 mph)? For scientists these are theoretically pertinent questions that have received empirical scrutiny in the field of sports at least over the past four decades (e.g. for reviews, see [1,2]). At the same time, coaches, athletes and others working in the field are interested in novel insights that may help to further enhance performance (e.g. for reviews, see [3,4]). Hence, the aim of psychological research in sports is to provide evidence-based answers both to develop theory and guide practice. The above described situations illustrate the following challenge: The receiver facing the server in tennis or the batter facing the pitcher in baseball is exposed to a highly time-constrained situation. Due to latency in processing sensory information into appropriate motor responses [1,2], responding to the server’s or pitcher’s action only after reliable information (i.e., information with high certainty) about ball flight becomes available (e.g., after the ball is hit by the racket of the opponent) does not allow to be at the right location at the right time and successfully return the serve or hit the pitch. It follows that in such situations players (must) rely on and use other than ball flight information (i.e., information with relatively high uncertainty) to initiate their (re)actions. In other words, players are required to make and act on a “prediction” of the outcome of the observed unfolding event. Here, we summarize and discuss current insights and recent

3 developments in understanding the strategies and mechanisms underlying athletes’ anticipation of their opponents’ action intentions.

Factors contributing to the prediction of action effects in sports Two broad categories of information sources can be differentiated that contribute to the prediction of action effects in sports [5,6]: kinematics of the opponent and non-kinematic (or contextual) sources of information. The efficient pick-up and use of information emanating from these sources may be governed by different factors related to domain-specific (visual and/or motor) expertise. Key findings from recent research on anticipation alongside the respective methodological approaches taken are illustrated below.

Kinematic information An experts’ superiority over their less skilled counterparts in predicting opponents’ action intentions based on the opponent’s kinematics has been identified in early work [7] and confirmed repeatedly since [2,8]. While the kinematics’ relevance for anticipation is intuitive, answering what kinematic cues are used and how the cues are picked up is certainly not trivial. To address this issue, researchers employ and occasionally combine different experimental paradigms such as the temporal [9**,10] or spatial [11,12] manipulation of a video showing an opponent’s action (Figure 1). In addition, an opponent’s movement is visually reduced to its underlying relative motion (point-light displays, Figure 1C, [13-15**]) or researchers track participants’ gaze behaviours [16,17] (e.g. for a review, see [18]).

4 Information pick-up strategies Abernethy and Russell [19-21] pioneered the work on information pick-up strategies underlying successful anticipation using selectively occluded film fragments to determine the differences in anticipation between expert and novice squash and badminton players. More recently, Williams and colleagues [14] manipulated selected body regions (e.g., shoulders, hips, arm-racket area) of an opponent hitting tennis forehand groundstrokes and presented the actions as point-light animations stopping at the moment of racket-ball-contact. Participants viewed the videos on a screen and were asked to predict whether shots would be directed to their left or right. Results revealed that skilled tennis players relied on different body areas (i.e., shoulders, hips, legs, arm-racket), whereas their less skilled counterparts were merely affected by the manipulation of the arm-racket area. Hence, skilled players were supposed to use a ‘global’ information pick-up strategy as opposed to less skilled players who appear to primarily rely on local end-effector information (see also [13]). Similar findings have also been reported for handball goalkeeping [11]. Presenting normal videos of penalty-throws where selected body parts were either removed or shown in isolation (cf. Figure 1B), Loffing and Hagemann demonstrated that skilled and novice goalkeepers were capable of predicting the type of throw (hard vs. soft shot) above chance level solely based on ball-hand information, but only the skilled group clearly benefited from the availability, and suffered from the removal, of distal (throwing arm and the ball) and proximal regions (upper body). The kinematic cues informing skilled anticipation as well as the predictive power of selected local body areas, however, is likely to vary across sports (e.g., cricket [12], badminton [22]) and even between tasks within the same sport (e.g., tennis [23]).

5 Gaze behaviour An efficient information pick-up requires that athletes direct their visual attention to (often by means of gazing at) the most relevant body areas when observing an opponent executing an action. A meta-analysis [8] on eye-tracking research suggests that, compared to novice or less skilled athletes, experts employ a more efficient visual search strategy by directing their gaze to information-rich areas – not necessarily specific body parts but also regions that enable the pickup of information surrounding gaze location (i.e., visual pivot, [24]) – using fewer fixations of longer duration. There is increasing awareness, however, that summary statistics of gaze data may lead to premature conclusions, for example, regarding skill-specificity in gaze behaviour [25]. Likewise, intra- and interindividual variation [26] as well as differences in gaze behaviour as a function of task demands (e.g., in soccer goalkeeping [27]) suggest that more sophisticated analyses and interpretation of gaze data are required in future research.

Training kinematic cue usage to improve anticipation skill Those body areas or segments that have been identified to underpin successful anticipation by means of the above mentioned methods, have been implemented in sport-specific perceptualcognitive trainings. Such interventions have been proven successful in improving novices’ ability to predict opponents’ action outcomes [28,29]. Consequently, it may provide one additional means to foster talent development in sports [3]. However, several issues such as structure of practice, transfer to the field, sustainability of training effects [4] or optimal instruction and feedback techniques to promote both rate of learning and robustness of skill-recall under stress [30,31] have yet to be examined in more detail.

6 Visual and motor experience Visual experience has been identified as an important factor driving the prediction of an opponent’s action intention. In support of this and suggesting specificity in skilled anticipation, the outcome of perceptually less familiar left-handed actions seems harder to predict compared to more commonly exposed right-handed actions in tennis [32], volleyball [33,34] or handball goalkeeping [35], irrespective of an observer’s handedness [32]. However, athletes’ anticipation skill may also benefit from their perceptual-motor expertise, although relative contributions of the respective components are still unknown [10,36,37]. Moreover, there is growing evidence of a particular involvement of the motor system in skilled anticipation [38,39**]. For example, Aglioti et al. [38] showed that elite basketball players (high visuo-motor expertise) made earlier and more accurate predictions about the outcome of basketball free-throws (options: “in”, “out”, “don’t know”) than a group of expert watchers (journalists and coaches; high visual expertise) and novices (no experience in basketball). Moreover, only motor experts (basketball players) were found to specifically display higher corticospinal activation recorded from the hand (abductor digiti minimi) during the observation of “out” as compared to “in” shots occluded shortly after the ball left the hand, suggesting that motor expertise as opposed to visual expertise enables fine-tuned motor resonance that may ultimately facilitate action outcome prediction (see also [40,41]). Albeit promising, recent evidence of a motor system involvement in anticipation in sports stems from situations that either do not require anticipation in the real world (e.g., darts, [39**]) or where (high) time-pressure is absent (basketball free-throw, [38]). Therefore, whether the motor system substantially contributes to the prediction of action effects in (highly) time-constrained dynamic sport situations (e.g., batting in baseball or cricket) is one important question sport psychologists need continue to work on. Providing a clearer picture on this can help establish a

7 differentiated understanding of the link between perception, cognition and action as well as inform sport practitioners about new potentials in improving anticipatory skill.

Non-kinematic (contextual or situational) information Players’ actions are embedded in sport- and task-specific contexts. They constantly change dependent on, for instance, game score, field position or sequences of consecutive actions in tennis rallies. Also, athletes often know their opponents from previous competitions and/or performance profiles. Although work published as early as in the late 1970s hinted at the fact that these exemplar non-kinematic sources of information can be used to guide anticipatory behaviour [42,43], systematic research on their contribution to anticipation in sport is limited (for a recent opinion article arguing along the same lines, see [6]). Knowledge of likelihoods or probabilities about the occurrence of certain events had early been shown to quite strongly influence a player’s anticipatory movements, even before their opponent’s movement information becomes available. These promising research ideas and findings, for unknown reasons, however remained dormant until an influential paper on anticipation in squash appeared in 2001 [9**]. More specifically, in an on-court experiment Abernethy and colleagues found that even before the opponent had initiated their movements, experts performed above chance and better than less-skilled players in anticipating the outcome of the actions that were yet to be executed. The authors concluded that experts must have relied on probabilistic information unrelated to kinematics, dubbed situational probabilities, and identified the opponents’ court position to be a likely candidate to convey such information. That paper has inspired a handful of researchers across the globe to broaden their research focus both to develop theory and guide practice on skilled anticipation in sports [6]. The rather isolated work of different research groups led to the identification of several (non-kinematic)

8 sources of information that may affect anticipatory performance in the sport domain [44**]. These sources of probabilistic information include, for instance, an opponent’s court position [9**,15**,45], action patterns associated with certain game scores [46,47], knowledge of action preferences [48*,49] or of general action occurrence [12,50], familiarity with a particular opponent [51] or action [52] or sequences of action outcomes [47,53]. For example, in tennis, Farrow and Reid [46] identified game score as one source that may provide situational probability information and thereby facilitate anticipation. Participants were asked to predict the location of serves presented as video as soon as they felt confident knowing it. Game score was manipulated in a way that it provided advance probability information about shot direction of the first service in a game. Across twelve simulated service games, older but not younger players picked up on and used the information conveyed by game score as indicated by progressively earlier (but constantly correct) predictions. Relying on situational probability, however, may not be beneficial under all circumstances. Mann and colleagues [48*] exposed two groups of skilled handball goalkeepers to penalty takers that either had or did not have strong preference to throw towards a particular corner of a goal. Results revealed that if, in the test situation, penalty takers behaved according to the previously shown action preference, anticipatory performance improved. Conversely, if penalty takers did not behave according to their previously shown action preference, goalkeepers’ performance declined (see also [49]). Although these findings may appear trivial in some respects, they illustrate the need for a differentiated view on the facilitating versus hindering impact of situational variables on anticipation (see also [47,53]). As another example, Cañal-Bruland et al. [50] analysed the movement patterns of baseball batters facing fastballs (fast type of pitch) and change-ups (a slower type of pitch) to examine to what degree following the ‘sitting on a fastball’ strategy (i.e., to always expect a fastball) impacts

9 on batters’ movement responses. Results showed that in cases where change-ups were not successfully hit, batters had failed to adjust (i.e., slow down) their movement initiation patterns, but rather showed movement patterns that were typical for responses to fastballs. Hence, expectations (e.g., informed by instructions or action strategies) can influence motor responses in highly time-constrained situations [47]. One important question relates to how situational probabilities can be optimally communicated to athletes [54].

Conclusions As discussed in this paper, players use and rely on both kinematic and non-kinematic sources when planning and executing their (re)actions. A central question for future research pertains to how these sources interact with respect to their respective contribution to the prediction of action effects [6,55]? Models on sensorimotor control may suggest a solution (e.g., Bayesian integration, [56]), but evidence originating from more direct testing in a sport-related setting is scarce. Recent work indicates that information related to kinematics and context may be weighted differently during anticipatory processing, resulting in a dynamic updating of expectations in the course of an opponent’s unfolding movement [15**,45]. The relative contribution of kinematic vs. contextual sources to anticipation, however, may further vary as a function of several factors such as task constraints [57], performance pressure [58**] or game situation [55]. In addition, future research needs to address pertinent methodological questions such as whether video-based testing may fall short when trying to capture skilled anticipation, and hence whether it is necessary to consider real-life testing [59] that includes the true actions under scrutiny [60,61]. Further promising routes that may fundamentally improve our understanding include studying skill transfer between domains or tasks [62] and scrutinizing individual differences in more detail [63,64].

10 Funding. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of Interest. We have no potential conflict of interest to report.

11 References 1. Yarrow K, Brown P, Krakauer JW: Inside the brain of an elite athlete: The neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2009, 10:585-596. 2. Müller S, Abernethy B: Expert anticipatory skill in striking sports: A review and a model. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 2012, 83:175-187. 3. Loffing F, Hagemann N, Farrow D: Perceptual-cognitive training: The next piece of the puzzle. In Routledge Handbook of Talent Identification and Development in Sport. Edited by Baker J, Cobley S, Schorer J, Wattie N: Routledge; in press. 4. Broadbent DP, Causer J, Williams AM, Ford PR: Perceptual-cognitive skill training and its transfer to expert performance in the field: Future research directions. European Journal of Sport Science 2015, 15:322-331. 5. Buckolz E, Prapavesis H, Fairs J: Advance cues and their use in predicting tennis passing shots. Canadian Journal of Sport Sciences 1988, 13:20-30. 6. Cañal-Bruland R, Mann DL: Time to broaden the scope of research on anticipatory behaviour: A case for the role of probabilistic information. Frontiers in Psychology 2015, 6. 7. Jones CM, Miles TR: Use of advance cues in predicting the flight of a lawn tennis ball. Journal of Human Movement Studies 1978, 4:231-235. 8. Mann DTY, Williams AM, Ward P, Janelle CM: Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 2007, 29:457-478.

**9. Abernethy B, Gill DP, Parks SL, Packer ST: Expertise and the perception of kinematic and situational probability information. Perception 2001, 30:233-252. Occluding squash players’ vision while performing on-court by means of liquid-crystal occlusion spectacles, the authors found that expert players anticipated the outcome of their opponent’s actions above guessing and better than less-skilled players at moments clearly before the opponent had initiated their stroke movements. Findings suggest that experts are able to identify and include (situational) probabilistic information unrelated to the opponent’s kinematics into action outcome anticipation. 10. Cañal-Bruland R, Mooren M, Savelsbergh GJP: Differentiating experts' anticipatory skills in beach volleyball. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 2011, 82:667-674. 11. Loffing F, Hagemann N: Skill differences in visual anticipation of type of throw in teamhandball penalties. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 2014, 15:260-267.

12 12. Müller S, Abernethy B, Farrow D: How do world-class cricket batsmen anticipate a bowler's intention? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2006, 59:2162-2186. 13. Huys R, Cañal-Bruland R, Hagemann N, Beek PJ, Smeeton NJ, Williams AM: Global information pickup underpins anticipation of tennis shot direction. Journal of Motor Behavior 2009, 41:158-170. 14. Williams AM, Huys R, Cañal-Bruland R, Hagemann N: The dynamical information underpinning anticipation skill. Human Movement Science 2009, 28:362-370.

**15. Loffing F, Hagemann N: On-court position influences skilled tennis players' anticipation of shot outcome. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 2014, 36:14-26. Using data from professional matches and a video-based experiment in tennis, the authors demonstrate that an opponent’s on-court position is associated with different shot direction probabilities. Skilled players integrate positional information into anticipation of an opponent’s stroke outcome and players’ reliance on on-court position seems strongest at an early compared to later stage of an opponent’s movement. The study suggests different weighting of contextual and kinematic information during anticipatory processing. 16. Mann DL, Spratford W, Abernethy B: The head tracks and gaze predicts: How the world's best batters hit a ball. PLoS ONE 2013, 8:e58289. 17. Savelsbergh GJP, Williams AM, van der Kamp J, Ward P: Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. Journal of Sports Sciences 2002, 20:279-287. 18. Mann D, Savelsbeergh GJP: Issues in the measurement of anticipation. In Routledge Handbook of Sport Expertise. Edited by Baker J, Farrow D: Routledge; 2015:166-175. Routledge International Handbooks 19. Abernethy B, Russell DG: The relationship between expertise and visual search strategy in a racquet sport. Human Movement Science 1987, 6:283-319. 20. Abernethy B, Russell DG: Expert-novice differences in an applied selective attention task. Journal of Sport Psychology 1987, 9:326-345. 21. Abernethy B: Anticipation in squash: Differences in advance cue utilization between expert and novice players. Journal of Sports Sciences 1990, 8:17-34. 22. Abernethy B, Zawi K: Pickup of essential kinematics underpins expert perception of movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior 2007, 39:353-367.

13 23. Cañal-Bruland R, Williams AM: Recognizing and predicting movement effects: Identifying critical movement features. Experimental Psychology 2010, 57:320-326. 24. Piras A, Vickers JN: The effect of fixation transitions on quiet eye duration and performance in the soccer penalty kick: instep versus inside kicks. Cognitive Processing 2011, 12:245-255. 25. Button C, Dicks M, Haines R, Barker R, Davids K: Statistical modelling of gaze behaviour as categorical time series: What you should watch to save soccer penalties. Cognitive Processing 2011, 12:235-244. 26. Dicks M, Button C, Davids K, Chow JY, van der Kamp J: Keeping an eye on noisy movements: On different approaches to perceptual-motor skill research and training. Sports Medicine 2016:1-7. 27. Dicks M, Button C, Davids K: Examination of gaze behaviors under in situ and video simulation task constraints reveals differences in information pickup for perception and action. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics 2010, 72:706-720. 28. Hagemann N, Strauss B, Cañal-Bruland R: Training perceptual skill by orienting visual attention. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 2006, 28:143-158. 29. Savelsbergh GJP, Van Gastel PJ, Van Kampen PM: Anticipation of penalty kicking direction can be improved by directing attention through perceptual learning. International Journal of Sport Psychology 2010, 41:24-41. 30. Abernethy B, Schorer J, Jackson RC, Hagemann N: Perceptual training methods compared: The relative efficacy of different approaches to enhancing sport-specific anticipation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2012, 18:143-153. 31. Klostermann A, Vater C, Kredel R, Hossner E-J: Perceptual training in beach volleyball defence: Different effects of gaze-path cueing on gaze and decision-making. Frontiers in Psychology 2015, 6. 32. Hagemann N: The advantage of being left-handed in interactive sports. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2009, 71:1641-1648. 33. Loffing F, Hagemann N, Schorer J, Baker J: Skilled players’ and novices’ difficulty anticipating left- vs. right-handed opponents’ action intentions varies across different points in time. Human Movement Science 2015, 40:410-421.

14 34. Loffing F, Schorer J, Hagemann N, Baker J: On the advantage of being left-handed in volleyball: Further evidence of the specificity of skilled visual perception. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2012, 74:446-453. 35. Loffing F, Sölter F, Hagemann N, Strauss B: Accuracy of outcome anticipation, but not gaze behavior, differs against left- and right-handed penalties in team-handball goalkeeping. Frontiers in Psychology 2015, 6. 36. Cañal-Bruland R, Schmidt M: Response bias in judging deceptive movements. Acta Psychologica 2009, 130:235-240. 37. Cañal-Bruland R, van der Kamp J, van Kesteren J: An examination of motor and perceptual contributions to the recognition of deception from others' actions. Human Movement Science 2010, 29:94-102. 38. Aglioti SM, Cesari P, Romani M, Urgesi C: Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience 2008, 11:1109-1116.

**39. Mulligan D, Lohse KR, Hodges NJ: An action-incongruent secondary task modulates prediction accuracy in experienced performers: evidence for motor simulation. Psychological Research 2016, 80:496-509. Testing experienced and novice dart players’ ability to predict the outcome of dart throws under varying additional task conditions, the authors demonstrate that only experienced players’ accuracy decreased when they had to simultaneously perform an action-incongruent motor task. Findings are interpreted as evidence of a motor system involvement when experienced players predict action effects. 40. Renden PG, Kerstens S, Oudejans RRD, Cañal-Bruland R: Foul or dive? Motor contributions to judging ambiguous foul situations in football. European Journal of Sport Science 2014, 14:S221-S227. 41. Cañal-Bruland R, Kreinbucher C, Oudejans RRD: Motor expertise influences strike and ball judgements in baseball. International Journal of Sport Psychology 2012, 43:137-152. 42. Alain C, Girardin Y: The use of uncertainty in racquetball competition. Canadian Journal of Applied Sports Sciences 1978, 3:240-243. 43. Alain C, Proteau L: Decision making in sport. In NASPSPA Proceedings, Psychology of motor behavior and sport (1979). Edited by Nadeau CH, Halliwell WR, Newell KM, Roberts GC: Human Kinetics; 1980:465-477.

15

**44. Schläppi-Lienhard O, Hossner E-J: Decision making in beach volleyball defense: Crucial factors derived from interviews with top-level experts. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 2015, 16:60-73. Using focused interviews with elite beach volleyball players (e.g., Olympic or world champions), the authors reveal that players’ decision-making in defensive situations depends on a number of factors related to their opponents and context. Findings stimulate new approaches for research on the interplay between visual information, gaze behaviour and domain-specific knowledge. 45. Loffing F, Sölter F, Hagemann N, Strauss B: On-court position and handedness in visual anticipation of stroke direction in tennis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 2016, 27:195-204. 46. Farrow D, Reid M: The contribution of situational probability information to anticipatory skill. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 2012, 15:368-373. 47. Gray R: Behavior of college baseball players in a virtual batting task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, 28:1131-1148.

*48. Mann DL, Schaefers T, Cañal-Bruland R: Action preferences and the anticipation of action outcomes. Acta Psychologica 2014, 152:1-9. Using a video-based experiment on handball goalkeeping, the authors demonstrate that knowledge of an opponent’s action preference may benefit or harm goalkeepers’ predictions, depending on whether an opponent actually acts according to her preference or not. Findings indicate that contextual information (e.g., action preference) may also turn out detrimental to performance. 49. Navia JA, van der Kamp J, Ruiz LM: On the use of situation and body information in goalkeeper actions during a soccer penalty kick. International Journal of Sport Psychology 2013, 44:234-251. 50. Cañal-Bruland R, Filius MA, Oudejans RRD: Sitting on a fastball. Journal of Motor Behavior 2015, 47:267-270. 51. McRobert AP, Ward P, Eccles DW, Williams AM: The effect of manipulating contextspecific information on perceptual–cognitive processes during a simulated anticipation task. British Journal of Psychology 2011, 102:519-534. 52. Milazzo N, Farrow D, Ruffault A, Fournier JF: Do karate fighters use situational probability information to improve decision-making performance during on-mat tasks? Journal of Sports Sciences 2016, 34:1547-1556.

16 53. Loffing F, Stern R, Hagemann N: Pattern-induced expectation bias in visual anticipation of action outcomes. Acta Psychologica 2015, 161:45-53. 54. Gray R: The moneyball problem: What is the best way to present situational statistics to an athlete? Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 2015 International Annual Meeting 2015. 55. Williams AM: Perceiving the intentions of others: How do skilled performers make anticipation judgments? In Progress in Brain Research. Edited by Raab M, Johnson JG, Heekeren HR: Elsevier; 2009:73-83. vol 174, Mind and motion: The bidirectional link between thought and action.] 56. Körding KP: Decision theory: What "should" the nervous system do? Science 2007, 318:606-610. 57. Roca A, Ford PR, McRobert AP, Williams AM: Perceptual-cognitive skills and their interaction as a function of task constraints in soccer. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 2013, 35:144-155.

*58. Cocks AJ, Jackson RC, Bishop DT, Williams AM: Anxiety, anticipation and contextual information: A test of attentional control theory. Cognition and Emotion 2015:1-12. Testing skilled and less-skilled tennis players’ ability to anticipate an opponent’s stroke outcome in tennis under high and low anxiety conditions, results tentatively indicate that availability of contextual information may be less helpful to players under high as opposed to low anxiety conditions. The authors speculate that an anxiety-induced shift in players’ attentional control may have contributed to this effect. 59. Triolet C, Benguigui N, Le Runigo C, Williams AM: Quantifying the nature of anticipation in professional tennis. Journal of Sports Sciences 2013, 31:820-830. 60. Mann DL, Abernethy B, Farrow D: Action specificity increases anticipatory performance and the expert advantage in natural interceptive tasks. Acta Psychologica 2010, 135:17-23. 61. Pluijms JP, Cañal-Bruland R, Kats S, Savelsbergh GJP: Translating key methodological issues into technological advancements when running in-situ experiments in sports: An example from sailing. International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching 2013, 8:89-103. 62. Müller S, McLaren M, Appleby B, Rosalie SM: Does expert perceptual anticipation transfer to a dissimilar domain? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2015, 41:631-638.

17 63. Müller S, Brenton J, Dempsey A, Harbaugh A, Reid C: Individual differences in highly skilled visual perceptual-motor striking skill. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2015, 77:1726-1736. 64. Dicks M, Davids K, Button C: Individual differences in the visual control of intercepting a penalty kick in association football. Human Movement Science 2010, 29:401-411.

18 Figure Caption Figure 1. Examples of experimental paradigms used in research on anticipation in sports: (A) temporal occlusion (e.g., a handball penalty is occluded at three different time points), (B) spatial occlusion (e.g., selected body areas of a penalty-taker or the ball are removed/presented in isolation) and (C) point-light display.