Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461 – 470
Autism: a large unmet medical need and a complex research problem Julia Gerlai, Robert Gerlai* Neuroscience Discovery Research, Eli Lilly and Company, Lilly Corporate Center, Indianapolis, IN 46285, USA Received 4 April 2003; accepted 17 April 2003
Abstract Autism has been becoming the focus of attention as its apparently increasing prevalence is better appreciated. According to some estimates, the frequency of children with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) can be as high as 1 in 150. The diagnosis can be made as early as 2 years of age, and autistic patients often have a normal life span. Thus, in terms of the number of ‘‘patient years,’’ ASD represents a market that is as large as that of the biggest neurological indication, Alzheimer’s disease. Despite the clear unmet medical need, no effective treatment is available. This may be because the mechanism of ASD is not understood. The aim of the present paper is to review recent advances in autism research and to discuss some of the most stressing problems mainly from a preclinical research standpoint. We hope to draw attention to the need to study this devastating disease that places an enormous burden on the society in general and the relatives and caregivers of autistic patients in particular. D 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Autism; Autistic spectrum disorder; Asperger syndrome; Mouse; Rat; Social behavior
1. Introduction Understanding the neurobiology of social behavior has been in the focus of Dr. MacLean’s work [53]. He investigated the specific neural systems involved in social behavior. The questions he asked are highly relevant in the analysis of the mechanism of autism, a human brain disorder that manifests as abnormal social behaviors. Thus, it is our pleasure to contribute to this special issue devoted to honor Dr. MacLean’s research. The last 10 years of the 20th century was proclaimed by U.S. President George Bush to be the decade of the brain. However, the 21st century is starting with a great embarrassment for neuroscientists: one of the biggest brain-related disorders, autism, remains a mystery. Its mechanism is not understood [2] and even diagnostic criteria with which patients may be identified are debated [31,87]. One thing is certain, however, this disease, which in the past was thought to represent a negligible problem affecting only a few unfortunate persons, is now recognized as a major
problem with epidemic proportions [64,84]. Briefly, the need for effective medical intervention is enormous. In this paper, we present a review of some of the advances of autism research and also discuss the most stressing problems. First, we attempt to describe autism and briefly discuss the diagnostic criteria of the disease. Second, we make the argument that the prevalence of autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) puts this disease on par with the biggest neurological disorder, Alzheimer’s disease. Thus, ASD should be a focus of major research efforts. Third, and most importantly, we describe some of the potential mechanisms of ASD that are under investigation and also describe animal models that have been suggested to mimic aspects of ASD. Fourth, we discuss the drug therapies presently employed and point out that they are only palliative. Lastly, we attempt to delineate what we would consider the most important future directions in the investigation of the mechanisms of ASD and in the preclinical research for the development of treatments for ASD.
2. What is autism? Definitions and diagnoses * Corresponding author. 5259 Comanche Trail, Carmel, IN 46033, USA. E-mail address:
[email protected] (R. Gerlai). 0031-9384/$ – see front matter D 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00165-3
Autism is a developmental brain disorder that is characterized by abnormal social behavior, reduced interests in communicating and interacting with others, language dis-
462
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470
orders, repetitive and obsessive behaviors and rituals, and narrowly focused rigid interests (for a review, see Ref. [86]). Furthermore, autistic patients may also often suffer from a sensory overload and thus tend to avoid novel stimuli and situations [25,76]. Two main diagnostic instruments have been developed to identify autistic patients, the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI) [54] and the prelinguistic Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (PL-ADOS) [24]. These instruments, unlike categorical diagnoses, rely on rating scales and diagnostic checklists and offer evaluation of characteristics in three different areas of autistic symptoms: social reciprocity, communication, and repetitive or restricted behaviors. ADI was later revised (ADI-R) by Lord et al. [59] to allow the instrument to better discriminate autistic patients from nonautistic but mentally handicapped persons. ADOS has also been revised (ADOS-G) to make it better fit the tested person’s age and verbal abilities [58]. These diagnostic instruments now should allow clinicians to consistently rate patients for autism. However, despite their availability, they are not always used in research or in the clinic. As information is accumulating on autistic patients, it has been becoming clear that autism is not an obviously demarcated disease but a spectrum of problems with differing combination and severity of symptoms [87]. Although ASD continues to be a frustrating conundrum, since the first description of autism by Kanner [47] and of Asperger syndrome by Hans Asperger (in Ref. [92]), much has been learned about these diseases. In 1981, it was realized that the two diseases described by Kanner and Asperger may in fact be different faces of the same disorder [91]. Nowadays, four main categories are recognized on the spectrum: (1) patients with known medical disorders, e.g., tuberous sclerosis, exhibiting most of the typical signs of autism; (2) classic autism of the Kanner type [1,47]; (3) patients classified as having ‘‘pervasive developmental disorders not otherwise specified’’ (PDD-NOS) who have a milder form of autism or exhibit only subsets of the symptom cluster; and (4) patients diagnosed as having Asperger syndrome [91,92] who have impaired social abilities but possess mainly unaltered and occasionally superior language skills and mental capacity. Given the complicated nature of human social interaction, communication, and language processing and given the potentially large number of molecular mechanisms that subserve these complex behavioral phenomena, it is possible that the above four categories represent different disease mechanisms. Interestingly, however, family studies have revealed that several varieties of the autism spectrum may run, in an inherited manner, in the same family [33]. This finding thus raises the possibility that there may be a (set of) biological core mechanism(s) underlying the autism spectrum. Interaction of these mechanisms with other genetic and/or environmental factors may give rise to an epigenetically modified cluster of changes that manifest as more or less severe symptoms. However, the question of whether
patients with different symptoms represent distinct disease categories or the symptoms actually vary along a continuum is hotly debated [87]. Clearly, we are faced with a catch 22: without the use of consistent diagnostic tools, it is difficult to separate different categories within the autism spectrum [9], and thus it is difficult to study possible underlying mechanisms of the disease. But without knowing the mechanisms, it is difficult to clearly identify the disease categories. This is a significant problem, especially because it is now being recognized that considering all the different forms of ASD, a potentially very large number of people are suffering from this disease.
3. Market size: is autism worth taking notice? Once a neglected disease, ASD is now regarded an ‘‘epidemic’’ by the media [65] and by the scientific community [84]. The discovery that autism is not an infrequent disorder has sparked considerable interest in scientific and medical communities and pressure from frustrated parents has also invigorated research and its funding. Between the period of 1980 and 2000, the number of publications on autism (registered by MEDLINE) quadrupled and the trend is continuing. In 1997, the NIH started a 5-year US$42 million network of collaborative programs on autism. In 2001, the M.I.N.D. institute started the building of a 13,000 square meter state of the art clinic and research center at UC Davis that is solely devoted to the study of autism. In the same year, a large congressional (United States) caucus was formed for autism. Clearly, both governmental agencies and research communities have started to make significant efforts to address the problems associated with ASD. The question whether the epidemic status of ASD is due to true increase of incidence of the disease or simply its better detection and diagnosis is debated. Nevertheless, according to a most recent report to the legislature on the principal findings from the epidemiology of autism in California, the M.I.N.D. institute has confirmed that the increase of incidence is real and cannot be attributed to changes in diagnostic criteria or misclassification [88]. Autism was estimated to have a frequency of more than 1 in 500 children [31], while more recent studies found its prevalence as high as 1 in 150 (for examples, see Ref. [84]; also see website http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/dd/aic/about/ default.htm). Researchers, private (e.g., Alliance for Autism Research), and government (e.g., National Institutes of Health, USA) agencies have recognized the enormous need. As a result, funding for research has significantly increased. Surprisingly, however, autism is still not among the neurological or neuropsychiatric diseases onto which large pharmaceutical research companies traditionally focus. This is unfortunate as ASD represents a significant unmet medical need with an enormous market size (Fig. 1). Consider the following: ASD may be diagnosed as early as 2 –3 years of age [78]. Some even argue that successful diagnosis may be
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470
463
4. Mechanisms of the disease: a lot of candidates and a lot of unknowns!
Fig. 1. ASD is on par with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the biggest neurological disease market in terms of market size. Market size is expressed as number of patient years. It is the area of the rectangles defined by the x-axis (number of years the patient suffers from the disease, i.e., the length of time for which the patient is expected to take medication) and the y-axis (number of patients suffering from the disease). The inset depicts the size of the area of the rectangles and shows that the number of patient years, and thus the market size, is highly similar between ASD and AD.
made at 8 – 12 months (http://www.nichd.nih.gov/autism/ CPEAupdate.htm). Autistic persons can live a normal life span [14]. The market size can thus be calculated as follows: PY ¼ P Y where PY is the number of ‘‘patient years,’’ P is the number of patients and Y is the number of years for which patients live after diagnosis. Calculating with the conservative prevalence estimate of 1 in 500, there may be approximately 600,000 ASD patients in the USA alone. These persons may live for an average of 76 years. Using the conservative age of 3 years for the time of diagnosis, PY may be calculated as follows. PY = 600,000 73 = 43,800,000, i.e., almost 44 million patient years. To put this number into perspective, let us consider Alzheimer’s disease, a disorder that is considered to represent the largest market by big pharmaceutical research companies. Calculating with P = 9 million (say 15% of people above 65 years of age will have Alzheimer’s disease in the Unites States) and Y = 6 (Alzheimer’s disease patients live, on average, for 6 years after first diagnosis), PY = 54 million for Alzheimer’s disease. The actual numbers may slightly vary. The number of Alzheimer’s disease patients is actually smaller than 9 million but the disease may be diagnosed earlier and patients may live longer than for 6 years after diagnosis. On the other hand, the number of ASD patients can easily be twice or even three times higher than the presently estimated 600 thousand. Thus, it is clear ASD represents an unmet medical need that is comparable in order of magnitude to the largest neurological disease market, that of Alzheimer’s disease. Thus, ASD should be of major interest to pharmaceutical research companies even when the 17-year patent expiration rule is considered.
Despite the concerted efforts, autism has remained an elusive target. The mechanism of the disease is still not understood. Family studies, however, are offering some hope. They have shown that the concordance rate in monozygotic twins is approximately 65%, whereas it is 0% in dizygotic twins [6]. Furthermore, the prevalence of autism is 100 times higher in families in which at least one autism case has been identified compared to the rate in the general population [79]. These quantitative genetic results have been taken as evidence that there may be only a few major genes that underlie this disease, perhaps as few as 4 or 5 but probably not more than 20 [84]. If indeed there are only a few major genes involved, there is hope that once they are identified, the molecular and neurobiological mechanisms of autism may be understood. It is also notable, however, that the concordance rate for monozygotic twins is significantly less than 100%, which implies that, although the disease is highly heritable, the effect of environmental factors is not negligible. Numerous candidate loci or chromosomal regions carrying such loci have been identified (for a recent review of these genes, see Ref. [2], also see Table 1 for summary). Genes on the long arm of chromosome 7, e.g., on the 7q31 region, have been suggested on the basis of linkage studies. Among them, perhaps the most interesting ones are the SPCH1 locus (because it is known to be involved in an autosomal dominant language disorder; Ref. [32]), the CAGH44 gene (whose mutation was also found in a patient with speech and language disorder; Ref. [52]), the 5HT2A locus (because this gene encodes a serotonin receptor protein and the serotonin system has been implicated in autism; Refs. [10,18,62,88]), the Wnt2 gene (because it has been implicated in a genetic mouse model, Dvl1 null mutant, showing signs of social aberration; Ref. [57]), and the reelin locus (reelin protein has been shown to be reduced in autistic patients; Refs. [28,70]). Chromosomal abnormalities involving the 15q11-13 region and a segment of 13q have also been implicated in autism (for a review, see Ref. [2]). Among the numerous candidate genes, perhaps GABAA receptor genes, e.g., b3, may be worth mentioning as the protein products of these genes are known to be involved in inhibitory neurotransmission and can thus influence both developmental and functional plasticity of the brain. In addition to linkage based results, single gene mutations have also been identified in patients with autism. The rare dominant genetic disease, Tuberous Sclerosis Complex (TSC) is due to mutations of two functionally related genes, TSC1 and TSC2 [16,66]. The rate of autism in TSC patients is very high (17 – 68%), but the rate of TSC patients among autistic persons is only 3%, indicating that TSC is not the main underlying mechanism in autism. Nevertheless, similarly to the situation in Alzheimer’s disease (e.g., the
464
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470
Table 1 Examples of molecular mechanisms suspected in ASD Molecular mechanism, gene or chromosomal region
Relevance for ASD
Reference
SPCH1 gene
Autosomal dominant language disorder Mutation detected in speech and language disordered patient Linkage studies and serotonergic hypothesis Linkage studies and animal model showing signs similar to autism Reduced expression in autistic patients Linkage and association studies
[32]
CAGH44 gene
5HT2A gene Wnt2 gene
Reelin gene 15q11-13 and 13q chromosomal regions (potentially numerous genes) GABAA receptor genes TSC1 and TSC2 genes
FMR1 gene ADSL gene ADA allele 2 Oxytocin, vasopressin genes
BDNF gene En2 gene
X chromosome (potentially numerous genes)
Reduced expression in autistic patients Prevalence of ASD is high among persons carrying mutant TSC alleles ASD-like symptoms in fragile X patients Point mutation in family exhibiting autistic features Increased frequency in autistic patients Function in social behavior, blood levels reduced in ASD, animal model with social behavior disturbance Expression elevated in ASD children Association studies, known role in cerebellar development Significantly higher incidence of ASD in males versus females (4:1 ratio). X chromosome appears protective
[52]
[10,18,62,88] [57]
[28] Review in Ref. [2]
[11] [16,66]
[29] [86] [21,71] [37,43,64,93]
[84] [44,63,72]
[25]
discovery of mutations in the amyloid precursor protein gene), a relatively rare genetic alteration can still be a valuable tool with which molecular mechanisms of a disease may be untangled. Fragile X syndrome has also been implicated in autism as some fragile X patients exhibit symptoms compatible with the diagnostic criteria of autism [29]. Fragile X syndrome is due to the expansion of a polymorphic CGG repeat upstream of the coding region of the FMR1 gene, and the expansion blocks the expression of the gene product due to methylation of the FMR1 promoter [90]. As in TSC, fragile X may not represent the core mechanism of the disease, but its analysis may illuminate some possible mechanisms that contribute to autism. It is also important to acknowledge the fact that autism is four to five times more prevalent among males [25].
Although the mechanism of this gender bias is not known, it is possible that hormonal differences leading to gender specific brain development and/or the protective effects of having an extra X chromosome in females may be the cause. Two main neurotransmitter systems have also been implicated in autism, the serotonergic system and the glutamatergic system. Increased blood [18] and urine [39] serotonin levels have been detected in autistic patients; and paradoxically, serotonin reuptake inhibitors, which are thought to increase serotonergic tone, have been found to ameliorate some symptoms of autism [41]. Linkage and association studies, however, provided inconsistent results with regard to the role of serotonin receptors or serotonin transporters in autism (for reviews, see Refs. [2,89]). The second neurotransmitter system implicated in autism is the glutamate system [15]. Based on anatomic and imaging studies that indicate alterations in brain regions rich in glutamate-containing neurons in autistic individuals and on symptoms elicited in normal subjects by glutamate antagonists, it has been argued [15] that autism is a ‘‘hypoglutamatergic’’ disorder. To counteract the possibly reduced glutamatergic ‘‘tone’’ in autistic patients, clinical trials are ongoing with AMPAkines, small molecules that potentiate the effect of glutamate on AMPA-R, a ligand gated Ca2 + channel. Although plausible, the above hypotheses were questioned by the result of a recent study that investigated the density and distribution of hippocampal neurotransmitter receptors in autism [11]. The authors used autoradiographic techniques and surprisingly found no alterations in the serotoninergic, cholinergic, and glutamatergic neurotransmitter systems. However, they did detect significantly reduced binding of 3[H]-flunitrazepam and 3[H]muscimol, indicating reduction of benzodiazepine binding sites and GABAA receptor density in the hippocampus of autistic patients. Numerous other mechanisms have also been suggested, but again their validity or importance in autism will need to be ascertained in the future. For example, a point mutation leading to adenylosuccinate lyase (ADSL) deficiency was observed in a single Moroccan family whose members had mental retardation and autistic features [86]. Adenosine deaminase (ADA) allele 2 was found more than twice as frequently in autistic patients as in a control population in Italy [21,71]. ADA activity deficiency is known to lead to severe combined immunodeficiency (SCID). However, the frequency of the ADA2 allele was not increased in autistic patients of Caucasian North American origin (for a review, see Ref. [2]) suggesting that the association found in the Italian studies may be due to random founder effect or genetic drift. Other candidate molecules implicated in autism are oxytocin and vasopression, neuropeptides known to regulate social behavior [43]. For example, blood levels of oxytocin were found reduced in autistic patients [37,64]. Levels of the Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) were also found different (elevated) in children with autism (in Ref. [84]) implying that perhaps analysis of abnormal
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470
gene expression patterns using microarray technology or differential display would be a useful approach in autism research [77]. Finally, weak association between genetic markers of the locus of the engrailed 2 (En2) gene and autism was found [72], an intriguing result because En2 is known to regulate cerebellar development [44,63] and abnormal cerebellar structure has been reported for autistic patients [80]. Although the evidence for genetic factors in ASD is overwhelming, it is clear that the environment also contributes to the disease. As cited previously, the probability of one of a monozygotic twin pair to have autism is 65% if the other one has autism too [6]. This implies that a large proportion of variability (35%) is due to environmental factors. Thus, considerable amount of effort has been devoted to identifying environmental causes or risk factors that may be involved in ASD. Among these are injection of vaccines [22], exposure to toxins and infections [40], immunologic (autoimmune) problems [51], and metabolic problems [46,48]. Many of these factors are hotly debated by both scientists and the public [65], and the final word will have to wait until rigorous controlled studies are conducted. The reader is referred to the above-cited reviews for further discussion of these important topics. It must also be noted that although ASD is highly heritable, this does not imply that environmental intervention may not be employed to ameliorate symptoms of the disease. Genetic influence should not be equated with genetic determinism. Although evidence is mounting suggesting that autism is associated with abnormal development of brain structures leading to miswiring in the brain, given the fact that the brain is highly plastic and is expected to be able to make new synaptic connections and/or alter the strength of such connections throughout the entire life, the role of stimulation, practice, and behavioral training [38,60,83] in the treatment of autistic persons cannot be underestimated. Anatomical and brain imaging studies have revealed numerous structural and functional alterations in the autistic brain. For example, significant neuronal abnormalities were found in the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum and limbic structures and paralimbic cortices [8]. Decreased neuronal size, increased packing density, and decreased complexity of dendritic arborization in the hippocampus and amygdala of autistic patients were also demonstrated [8], suggesting a developmental curtailment of the maturation of the brain. The volume of the amygdala and the hippocampus was found significantly reduced in adolescent boys with autism but without mental retardation compared to age-matched controls, and cerebellar hypoplasia was also reported [19,30,55,73]. Furthermore, a consistent finding has been increased cerebral volume in autistic patients, particularly affecting posterior but not frontal structures [5]. Increased volume of the basal ganglia was also reported in highfunctioning autistic patients as compared to control persons using MRI [81]. Clearly, numerous developmental alterations have been detected but the question of how consistent
465
and typical they are in autism and whether these alterations vary with the severity or particular composition of autistic symptoms will need to be addressed in the future. Finally, functional neuroimaging techniques, including positron emission tomography (PET), single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), and functional MRI (fMRI), have also contributed to the study of pathological brain functions in autism. These neuroimaging approaches have shown an abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autistic patients suggesting that different connections between particular cortical regions could exist in autism [13]. Perhaps the most intriguing among these findings are the results that show how autistic patients process socially relevant information: while normal subjects activated specialized brain areas, including the medial frontal and medial temporal cortices, people with autism relied on general association cortices (for a review, see Ref. [87]). Schultz et al. [82], using fMRI, showed that people with autism or Asperger syndrome activated the same areas of their brain, the temporal gyri, when shown faces or objects; however, normal subjects used their fusiform gyrus specifically only when shown human faces but not when shown objects. In the same vein, Baron-Cohen et al. [7] found that nonautistic adults showed increased activation in the amygdala, an area regarded to be the emotional center of the brain [20], when asked to judge the expression in the eye of a human face, whereas people with autism or Asperger syndrome did not exhibit such activation. These results are intriguing because they are consistent with the frequent observation that autistic people have difficulty understanding someone’s emotions and they treat humans as inanimate objects (for a review, see Ref. [87]).
5. Animal models of autism: limited successes Although research is progressing, the above discussions demonstrate that the molecular and neurobiological mechanisms of ASD are still not clear. Creating an animal model without knowing mechanistic details about the disease is problematic especially if one considers that it is also unclear how ‘‘autism’’ should manifest in a rat or a mouse. Research, however, is usually an iterative process, and it is hoped that parallel preclinical (animal) and clinical (human) studies will strengthen each other and eventually will lead us towards the right answer. Numerous attempts exist to model ASD in animals. Genetic models are as follows (see Table 2 for summary). The Eker rat [49] exhibits phenotypical abnormalities that could be rescued by transgenic overexpression of the TSC2 gene. The TSC2 transgenic mouse was generated in which a mutated TSC2 gene was introduced [68]. Unfortunately, however, no behavioral abnormalities were described in either model [16]. Fragile X syndrome was also modeled in mouse by silencing the FMR1 gene using gene targeting [50]. Although some functional protein may be expressed
466
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470
Table 2 Examples of genetic animal models of ASD Model
Molecular target
Eker rat TSC2 transgenic mouse Fragile X mouse (KO) En2 KO mouse
TSC2 TSC2 FMR1 mutation En2
Vasopressin deficient rat Dvl1 KO mouse Reelin mutant mouse
Vasopressin Dvl1 and Wnt signaling pathway Reelin
Oxytocin KO mouse GS guinea pig
Oxytocin Unknown
Phenotypical alteration
Subtle learning deficits Subtle cerebellar abnormalities, motor learning impairment Reduced social memory and other learning deficits Impaired social behavior and sensorimotor gating Cytoarchitectural abnormalities similar to those of ASD, but severe ataxia Deficits in social behavior Abnormal exploratory behavior, sleep, hypopholiation of vermis, reduced number of cerebellar Purkinje cells
from the mutated gene, these mice exhibited some phenotypical characteristics similar to autistic humans, e.g., macroorchidism and subtle learning deficits [23,50,67,69], as well as abnormal dendritic spines of pyramidal neurons suggesting maturation and pruning deficits [17]. Social behavior has not been tested in these mice. A mouse carrying a null mutation at the En2 locus has been generated [44] and these mice were found to exhibit subtle cerebellar abnormalities at the structural level. Behavioral analysis of these mice was focused on their motor function and a mild motor learning deficit has been described [36]. Learning or social behavior of these mice was not analyzed. Social behavior was found altered, however, in a genetic rat model. Spontaneous vasopressin deficiency has been found in a rat strain, the Brattleboro rats, and these rats were found to exhibit reduced social memory and other learning-related deficits [26]. This rat model may be relevant both mechanistically and because it mimics some phenotypical aspects of autism. As vasopression and oxytocin are known to play roles in social behavior, a most recent mouse model, the oxytocin knockout mouse is particularly noteworthy [93]. In these animals, the gene-encoding oxytocin was silenced with the use of gene targeting. As expected, the knockout mice have been found to exhibit deficits in social behavior. Another intriguing animal model in which abnormal social behavior has been described is the Dvl1 null mutant mouse [57]. These mice were generated by gene targeting and the disrupted gene, Dvl1, lies within the Wnt signaling pathway. Interestingly, Wnt2 is localized to 7q31, one of the chromosomal regions found most strongly associated with autistic
Reference [49] [68] [23,50,67,69] [36,44] [26] [57] [27] [93] [56], also see Ref. [2] for a review
features in the human population. The null mutant mice were found to exhibit impaired social behavior (no cuddling during their sleep) and impaired sensorimotor gating in a prepulse inhibition paradigm [57]. Another noteworthy genetic model is the reelin spontaneous mutant mouse that was discovered more than 50 years ago [27]. Reelin is a signaling glycoprotein that plays a pivotal part in the migration of neuronal cells and in the establishment of neuronal connections during brain development. The reeler mouse exhibits cytoarchitectural abnormalities reminiscent of those seen in autistic patients [72] and the reelin gene maps to the region of 7q implicated in autism. Significant linkage/association has been found between a polymorphic triplet repeat located near the reelin gene and autism [72]. The utility of the reelin mutant for the analysis of phenotypical traits including learning and social interaction, however, is hampered by the fact that these mice exhibit severe ataxia, a reeling gait. Nevertheless, they may be useful for investigating the effects of experimental, e.g., pharmacological or genetic, manipulations that may interact with molecular mechanisms involved in the reelin pathway. The last genetic model to be mentioned is the GS guinea pig. These animals were found to exhibit abnormal exploratory behavior and sleep pattern, and they also possessed a structural abnormality in their brain, hypopholiation of the vermis and reduction of the number of granule and Purkinje cells in their cerebellum [56] that were reminiscent of abnormalities seen in autistic patients. Unfortunately, this strain is not available any more due to reproductive problems of these animals (reviewed in Ref. [2]).
Table 3 Examples of nongenetic animal models of ASD Model
Manipulation
Phenotypical alteration
Reference
Bachevalier’s rhesus monkey Rat amygdala model Rat cerebellar model Valproic acid toxicity in rat Borna virus rat model
Bilateral amygdala lesion Amygdala lesions Lesion of vermis Embryonic exposure to valproate Viral infection of newborn rat
Abnormal social behavior (only transient) Abnormal social behavior Hyperactivity, increased perseveration, reduced attention Structural abnormalities in vermis Cerebellar abnormalities, reduced play behavior
[4] [94] [12] [42] [74]
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470
Numerous nongenetic models also exist (for a summary, see Table 3) to mimic autism in animals. Again, their relevance to autism is debated. Given the structural and functional imaging results suggesting the role of amygdala in autism, it was hoped that disruption of this structure in animals will give rise to autism-like symptoms [3]. Bachevalier [4] induced bilateral amygdala lesions in newborn rhesus monkeys and observed abnormal social interactions, changes in body and facial expressions, and development of stereotypical behaviors. However, by the time the monkeys reached adulthood, the abnormalities disappeared diminishing the construct and predictive validity of the model. Amygdala lesions were also generated in rats and these manipulations have led to changes indicating potential alteration in social behaviors particularly in the group lesioned before puberty, i.e., at Day 7 after birth [94]. In order to model cerebellar abnormalities, Bobne´e et al. [12] lesioned midline structures (vermis) of the cerebellum of rats and investigated the behavioral effect of this manipulation. They found that lesioning at the age of postnatal day 10 led to hyperactivity, increased perseveration, and decreased attention. The authors argued that these features are autistic-like and that their model strengthens the argument that cerebellar developmental abnormalities play important roles in autism. Finally, approaches that mimic toxin or infection-induced abnormalities must also be mentioned. In a population study, Stro¨mland et al. [85] observed that exposure to the toxin thalidomide at the time of neural tube formation during early pregnancy (20th – 24th day) led to an incidence of autism of 5 out of 15 cases, an enormous increase compared to the usual incidence (1 in 500) seen in the population. In order to mimic the effects of this toxin, Ingram et al. [42] developed a rat model in which rat embryos were exposed to valproic acid, a teratogene, at prenatal days 11.5– 12.5. The authors observed abnormalities in motor nuclei and in the vermis and argued that the structural changes resembled those seen in autism. In another model, cerebellar abnormalities were achieved by inoculating newborn rats with Borna virus [74]. The infection led to loss of cerebellar neurons and reduced play behavior in the rats, phenotypical alterations that resemble autistic features.
6. Pharmacotherapies for autism: do they help? Briefly, numerous drugs have been tried and some were found to alleviate certain symptoms of autism. However, these drugs represent only palliative treatment as they affect only the secondary symptoms, e.g., aggression and anxiety. The core problems associated with abnormal communication and social behaviors remain unaffected. Prior to appropriate diagnostic criteria for ASD, antipsychotic drugs were widely used in autistic patients. Haloperidol, pimozide, and fluphenazine have been reported to have some ameliorative
467
effects (for a review, see Ref. [41]), but these drugs are currently avoided because of their significant side effects. Clozapine and olanzapine have also been tried with limited or no success (reviewed in Ref. [41]). Risperidone has been investigated the most in recent studies and it has been found to treat moderate to severe behavior problems that are associated with autism [61]. Risperidone is similar to clozapine in that it has a similarly complex pharmacological profile. It is chemically unrelated to any other currently available antipsychotic drugs and is a potent 5HT2A, 5HT7, a1-, a2-adrenergic, and histamine H1 antagonist. It also acts as a weak antagonist at the D2 dopamine receptor. Risperidone treatment (reviewed in Ref. [41]; also see Ref. [61]) has been reported to improve sensorimotor functioning, effectual reactions, reduce repetitive behaviors, irritability and aggression, and improve the overall behavioral symptoms score. Impairments in social interaction and use of language remained unaltered in response to risperidone treatment. Buspirone, a partial agonist at the 5HT1A autoreceptor, has also been reported to reduce aggression, hyperactivity, and repetitive behaviors (for a review, see Ref. [41]). Naltrexone, an opioid antagonist, has been theorized to be beneficial [41] because it may reduce the effect of endogenous opioids and thus lead to reduction of self-absorbed ‘‘introvert’’ attitude. Clinical trials, however, have shown no or only very mild effect of this drug. Based on observations suggesting hyperserotonemia, it may seem reasonable that drugs enhancing synaptic levels of 5HT, such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI’s), would worsen autistic symptoms. However, fluoxetine (trade name Prozac), an SSRI that has been widely prescribed for depression indication, was found to ameliorate symptoms including repetitive, compulsive, and aggressive behaviors. Children on Prozac also tended to sleep better, make better eye contact, and retain more flexibility in their behavior (reviewed in Ref. [41]). Secretin is a newly proposed treatment that has been in the spotlight of the lay public, the popular media, and the scientific community [46]. Secretins belong to a family of enterohormones that are involved in both the function of the gastrointestinal tract and the central nervous system [46]. Secretin receptors have been found in the cerebellum, and a secretin-like polypeptide has been found in the brain. Furthermore, secretin has been reported to lower plasma 5HT levels. Started from a chance observation, secretin has now been the subject of double-blind placebo-controlled studies. But unfortunately, these trials have not shown efficacy compared to placebo (reviewed in Ref. [75]). In summary, despite the large number of drugs tried, presently, drug therapy is only palliative at best.
7. Future directions A large number of investigators, clinicians, and animal researchers are trying to solve the conundrum of autism.
468
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470
Each may have a specific angle in their approach and each may view the priorities slightly differently. The list of directions we see as most important also represents our own view and biases and should not be regarded as absolute. Given the heterogeneity of the manifestation and of the mechanisms of ASD, further development of detailed diagnostic criteria and testing/evaluation methods that will allow classification of ASD phenotypes and clustering of symptom sets will help both therapists and basic research scientists [9]. The former may be able to better tailor behavioral therapies according to the particular need of the patient, and the latter may be able to identify molecular and neurobiological mechanisms potentially idiosyncratic to particular subtypes of ASD. Good identification of autistic features in the population may also facilitate drug development, as the need for such drugs in milder forms of ASD will be better appreciated. It is also noteworthy that some of the features of ASD may be observed in other CNS disorders including schizophrenia, nonverbal learning disorders, communication disorders, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorders [25]. Thus, drugs developed for ASD may ameliorate symptoms associated with these diseases and help a larger number of patients than what is estimated only based on the prevalence of ASD, an important consideration for market analysis at pharmaceutical research companies. Given the fact that a large proportion of ASD cases are of heritable type, it should be possible to identify genetic markers that could be tested early. Early diagnostic markers would aid the development and implementation of intervention strategies. Early intervention is considered crucial as the best results have been achieved when the behavioral modification or training is started at the youngest possible age [60]. Linkage and association studies have facilitated the discovery of certain chromosomal regions within which may reside loci of genes involved in the disease, but some of the findings are controversial or not replicable. This may be due to numerous factors, one of them being sampling error. To avoid irreproducibility due to error variation, larger number of subjects will need to be included in future analyses. A large scale and properly randomized linkage or association analysis may also have the potential to identify populations or subgroups in which particular molecular mechanisms underlying ASD differ. Lastly, it is important to realize the limitations inherent in the way scientists are rewarded. Most often, publication of negative results, i.e., results showing no effect or finding no quantitative trait loci, is discouraged in scientific journals. Thus, what one can find in the literature may not contain all pieces of relevant information and the literature may represent a distorted view. To see clearly, it is advisable to publish all results, both positive and negative. Detailed gene expression profiling using reverse transcriptase-based polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR) microarray technology or variants of differential display will
allow identification of genes that are differentially expressed in ASD [77]. These approaches may reveal novel molecular targets irrespective of whether a genetic marker close to the novel gene is known or not. Similar in principle to gene expression analysis is the analysis of expressed proteins, i.e., proteomics. It has also been suggested as a novel technology in the analysis of the molecular mechanisms of ASD [45]. Development of animal models continues to be an important priority for researchers. Presently available animal models have numerous promising features. However, some of the animal models are not yet fully characterized, or they mimic characteristics of ASD only partially. Clearly, generating proper animal models is very difficult but will be facilitated by human genetic studies. It is also noteworthy that although numerous behavioral tests have been used in the analysis of animal models of autism, no test battery has been developed that would specifically and consistently measure the main aspects of autistic-like features in animals. Focusing these batteries on intraspecific social behaviors, including social interaction, communication, and social learning while keeping the ethological characteristics of the species in mind [35], will be a step forward. Development of such a battery will aid the proper characterization of presently available animal models and will also help the generation of future ones. The question of test batteries, the need for standardization, and the problems associated with such batteries have been extensively discussed in recent articles suggesting that the difficulties are not specific for autism research (see review in Ref. [34]). Research has been on-going in all of the above areas and more. Given the strong public pressure and the appreciation of the importance of finding effective treatment by academic and governmental agencies and scientists alike, it is hoped that the first decade of the 21st century will bring a breakthrough in autism research.
Acknowledgements This paper is based on an oral presentation given by RG at the Satellite Symposium ‘‘The Triune Brain, a tribute to Paul MacLearn: the neurobiological relevance of social behavior’’ of the International Behavioral Neuroscience Society in 2002, Capri, Italy. We would like to thank Dr. Kelly G. Lambert, organizer of the symposium, for the opportunity to present. We would also like to thank Leah Helvering (Indianapolis), our friend and colleague, for her help and comments on this paper.
References [1] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). 4th ed. Washington (DC): APA; 1994.
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470 [2] Andres C. Molecular genetics and animal models in autistic disorder. Brain Res Bull 2002;57:109 – 19. [3] Bachevalier J. Medial temporal lobe structures and autism: a review of clinical and experimental findings. Neuropsychologia 1994;32: 627 – 48. [4] Bachevalier J. Brief report: medial temporal lobe and autism: a putative animal model in primates. J Autism Dev Disord 1996;26:217 – 20. [5] Bailey A, Luthert P, Dean A, Harding B, Janota I, Montgomery M, et al. A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain 1998 (May);121 (Pt 5):889 – 905. [6] Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, Bolton P, Simonoff E, Yuzda E, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med 1995;25:63 – 77. [7] Baron-Cohen S, O’Riordan M, Stone V, Jones R, Plaisted K. Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1999 (Oct);29(5):407 – 18. [8] Bauman M, Kemper T. Histo-anatomic observations of the brain in early infantile autism. Neurology 1985;35:866 – 74. [9] Beglinger LJ, Smith TH. A review of subtyping in autism and proposed dimensional classification model. J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31: 411 – 22. [10] Betancur C, Corbex M, Spielewoy C, Philippe A, Laplanche JL, Launay JM, et al. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and hyperserotonemia in autistic disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2002;7(1):67 – 71. [11] Blatt GJ, Fitzgerald CM, Guptill JT, Booker AB, Kemper TL, Bauman ML. Density and distribution of hippocampal neurotransmitter receptors in autism: an autoradiographic study. J Autism Dev Disord 2001 (Dec);31(6):537 – 43. [12] Bobne´e S, Mariette E, Tremblay-Leveau H, Caston J. Effects of early midline cerebellar lesion on cognitive and emotional functions in the rat. Behav Brain Res 2000;112:107 – 17. [13] Boddaert N, Zilbovicius M. Functional neuroimaging and childhood autism. Pediatr Radiol 2002;32:1 – 7. [14] Cade M, Tidwell S. Autism and the school nurse. J Sch Health 2001; 71:96 – 100. [15] Carlsson ML. Hypothesis: is infantile autism a hypoglutamatergic disorder? Relevance of glutamate – serotonin interactions for pharmacotherapy. J Neural Transm 1998;105:525 – 35. [16] Cheadle JP, Reeve MP, Sampson JR, Kwiatkowski DJ. Molecular genetic advances in tuberous sclerosis. Hum Genet 2000;107:97 – 114. [17] Comery TA, Harris JB, Willems PJ, et al. Abnormal dendritic spines in fragile X knockout mice: maturation and pruning deficits. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;94:5401 – 4. [18] Cook EH, Leventhal B. The serotonin system in autism. Curr Opin Pediatr 1996;8:348 – 54. [19] Courchesne E, Yeung-Courchesne R, Press G, Hesselink JR, Jernigan TL. Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism. New Engl J Med 1988 (May 26);318(21):1349 – 54. [20] Davis M. The role of amygdala in conditioned fear. In: Aggleton JP, editor. The amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory and mental dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss; 1992. p. 255 – 306. [21] De Luca D, Lucarelli P, Bottini C, Porfirio C, Curatolo P. Adenosine deaminase polymorphism in autism. J Child Neurol 1999;14:323 – 4. [22] DeStefano F. Vaccines and autism. Pediatr Infect Dis J 2001;20: 887 – 8. [23] D’Hooge R, Nagels G, Franck F, Bakker CE, Reyniers E, Storm K, et al. Mildly impaired water maze performance in male Fmr1 knockout mice. Neuroscience 1997 (Jan);76(2):367 – 76. [24] DiLavore P, Lord C, Rutter M. Pre-linguistic Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (PL-ADOS). J Autism Dev Disord 1995;25: 355 – 79. [25] DSM-IV-TR M. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association; 2000. p. 70 – 6. [26] Engelmann M, Landgraf R. Microdialysis administration of vasopressin into the septum improves social recognition in Brattleboro rats. Physiol Behav 1994;55:145 – 9.
469
[27] Falconer DS. Two new mutants, Trembler and ‘Reeler’, with neurological actions in the house mouse. J Genet 1951;50:182 – 201. [28] Fatemi SH. Reelin mutations in mouse and man: from reeler mouse to schizophrenia, mood disorders, autism and lissencephaly. Mol Psychiatry 2001;6:129 – 33. [29] Feinstein C, Reiss AL. Autism: the point of view from fragile X studies. J Autism Dev Disord 1998;28:393 – 405. [30] Filipek PA. Neuroimaging in autism: the state of science. J Autism Dev Disord 1996;26:211 – 5. [31] Filipek PA, Accardo PJ, Ashwal MD, Baranek GT, Cook EH, Dawson G, et al. Practice parameter: screening and diagnosis of autism: report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurology Society. Neurology 2000 (Aug 22);55(4):468 – 79. [32] Fisher SE, Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Monaco AP, Pembrey ME. Localisation of a gene implicated in severe speech and language disorder. Nat Genet 1998;18:168 – 70. [33] Folstein S, Santangelo S. Does Asperger syndrome aggregate in families? In: Klin A, Volkmar FR, Sparrow S, editors. Asperger syndrome. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. p. 159 – 71. [34] Gerlai R. Phenomics: fiction or the future? Trends Neurosci 2002;25: 506 – 9. [35] Gerlai R, Clayton NS. Analysing hippocampal function in transgenic mice: an ethological perspective. Trends Neurosci 1999;22:47 – 51. [36] Gerlai R, Millen KJ, Herrup K, Joyner AL, Roder J. Impaired motor learning performance in cerebellar En-2 mutant mice. Behav Neurosci 1996;110:126 – 33. [37] Green L, Fain D, Modahl C, Feinstein C, Waterhouse L, Morris M. Oxytocin and autistic disorder: alterations in peptide forms. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50:609 – 13. [38] Harris S, Handelman J. Preschool education programs for children with autism. Austin (TX): Pro-Ed; 1994. [39] He´rault J, Petit E, Martineau J, Cherpi C, Perrot A, Barthelemy C, et al. Serotonin and autism: biochemical and molecular biology features. Psychiatry Res 1996 (Nov 1);65(1):33 – 43. [40] Hornig M, Lipkin WI. Infectious and immune factors in the pathogenesis of neurodevelopmental disorders: epidemiology, hypotheses, and animal models. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2001;7:200 – 21. [41] Hunsinger DM, Nguyen T, Zebraski SE, Raffa RB. Is there a basis for novel pharmacotherapy of autism? Life Sci 2000;67:1667 – 82. [42] Ingram JL, Peckham SM, Tisdale B, Rodier PM. Prenatal exposure of rats to valproic acid reproduces the cerebellar anomalies associated with autism. Neurotoxicol Teratol 2000;22:319 – 24. [43] Insel TR, O’Brien DJ, Leckman JF. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: is there a connection? Biol Psychiatry 1999;45:145 – 57. [44] Joyner AL, Herrup K, Auerbach BA, Davis CA, Rossant J. Subtle cerebellar phenotype in mice homozygous for a targeted deletion of the En-2 homeobox. Science 1991;251:1239 – 43. [45] Junaid MA, Pullarkat RK. Proteomic approach for the elucidation of biological defects in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31:557 – 60. [46] Kaminska B, Czaja M, Kozielska E, Mazur E, Korzon M. Use of secretin in the treatment of childhood autism. Med Sci Monit 2002;8: 22 – 6. [47] Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv Child 1943; 2:217 – 50. [48] Knivsber AM, Reichelt KL, Nodland M. Reports on dietary intervention in autistic disorders. Nutr Neurosci 2001;4:25 – 37. [49] Kobayashi T, Mitani H, Takahashi R, Hirabayashi M, Ueda M, Tamura H, et al. Transgenic rescue from embryonic lethality and renal carcinogenesis in the Eker rat model by introduction of a wild-type TSC2 gene. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;94:3990 – 3. [50] Kooy RF, D’Hooge R, Reyniers E, Bakker CE, Nagels G, De Boulle K, et al. Transgenic mouse model for the fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet 1996 (Aug 9);64(2):241 – 5. [51] Korvatska E, Van de Water J, Anders TF, Gershwin ME. Genetic and immunologic considerations in autism. Neurobiol Dis 2002;9:107 – 25. [52] Lai CSL, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Levy ER, et al. The SPCH1 region on
470
[53] [54]
[55]
[56]
[57]
[58]
[59]
[60] [61]
[62]
[63]
[64] [65]
[66]
[67] [68]
[69]
[70]
[71]
[72]
J. Gerlai, R. Gerlai / Physiology & Behavior 79 (2003) 461–470 human 7q31: genomic characterization of the critical interval and localization of translocations associated with speech and language disorder. Am J Hum Genet 2000;67:357 – 68. Lambert KG. The life and career of Paul MacLean: a journey toward neurobiological and social harmony. Physiol Behav 2003; 79 [in press]. LeCouter A, Rutter M, Lord C, et al. Autism Diagnostic Interview: a semi-structured interview for parents and caregivers of autistic persons. J Autism Dev Disord 1989;19:363 – 87. Levitt JG, Blanton R, Capetillo-Cunliffe L, Guthrie D, Toga A, McCracken JT. Cerebellar vermis lobules VIII-X in autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1999 (May);23(4):625 – 33. Lev-Ram V, Valsamis M, Masliah E, Levine S, Godfrey HP. A novel non-ataxic guinea pig strain with cerebrocortical and cerebellar abnormalities. Brain Res 1993;606:325 – 31. Lijam N, Paylor R, McDonald MP, Crawley JN, Deng CX, Herrup K, et al. Social interaction and sensorimotor gating abnormalities in mice lacking Dvl1. Cell 1997 (Sep 5);90(5):895 – 905. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000 (Jun);30(3):205 – 23. Lord C, Rutter M, LeCouteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview—revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994;27:501 – 17. Lovaas I. Teaching developmentally disabled children: the me book. Austin (TX): Pro-Ed; 1981. McCracken JT, McGaugh J, Shah B, Cronin P, Hong D, Aman MG, et al. Risperidone in children with autism and serious behavioral problems. New Engl J Med 2002 (Aug 1);347(5):314 – 21. McDougle CJ, Naylor ST, Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR, Heninger GR, Price LH. A double-blind, placebo controlled study of fluvoxamine in adults with autistic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53: 1001 – 8. Millen KJ, Wurst W, Herrup K, Joyner AL. Abnormal embryonic cerebellar development and patterning of postnatal foliation in two mouse Engrailed-2 mutants. Development 1994;120:695 – 706. Modahl C, Green L, Fein D, Morris M. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol Psychiatry 1998;43:270 – 7. Nash MJ. The secrets of autism: the number of children diagnosed with autism and Asperger’s in the US is exploding. Why? Time 2002;159:47 – 56. Nellist M, van Slegtenhorst MA, Goedbloed M, et al. Characterization of the cytosolic tuberin – harmatin complex. Tuberin is a cytosolic chaperone for hamarin. J Biol Chem 1999;274:35647 – 52. Oostra BA, Hoogeeveen AT. Animal model for fragile X syndrome. Ann Med 1997;29:563 – 7. Pasumarthi KB, Nakajima H, Nakajima HO, Jing S, Field LJ. Enhanced cardcardiomyocyte DNA synthesis during myocardial hypertrophy in mice expressing a modified TSC2 transgene. Circ Res 2000;86:1069 – 77. Peier AM, McIlwain KL, Kenneson KL, Warren ST, Paylor R, Nelson DL. (Over)correction of FMR1 deficiency with YAC transgenics: behavioral and physical features. Hum Mol Genet 2000;9:1145 – 59. Persico AM, D’Agruma L, Maiorano N, Totaro A, Militerni R, Bravaccio C, et al. Reelin gene alleles and haplotypes as a factor predisposing to autistic disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2001 (Mar);6(2):150 – 9. Persico AM, Militerni R, Bravaccio C, Schneider C, Melmed R, Trillo S, et al. Adenosine deaminase alleles and autistic disorder: case-control and family-based association studies. Am J Med Genet 2000 (Dec 4);96(6):784 – 90. Petit E, He´rault J, Martineau J, Perrot A, Barthelemy C, Hameury L,
[73]
[74]
[75]
[76]
[77]
[78] [79]
[80] [81]
[82]
[83] [84] [85]
[86]
[87] [88] [89]
[90]
[91]
[92] [93] [94]
et al. Association study with two markers of a human homeogene in infantile autism. Am J Med Genet 1995 (Apr);32(4):269 – 74. Pierce K, Courchesne E. Evidence for a cerebellar role in reduced exploration and stereotyped behavior in autism. Biol Psychiatry 2000;49:655 – 64. Pletnikov MV, Rubin SA, Vasudevan K, Moran TH, Carbone KM. Developmental brain injury associated with abnormal play behavior in neonatally Borna disease virus-infected Lewis rats: a model of autism. Behav Brain Res 1999;100:43 – 50. Posey DJ, McDougle CJ. The pharmacotherapy of target symptoms associated with autistic disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders. Harvard Rev Psychiatr 2000;8:45 – 63. Powers MD. What is autism? In: Powers MD, editor. Children with autism: a parents’ guide. 2nd ed. Bethesda (MD): Woodbine House; 2000. p. 1 – 44. Purcell AE, Jeon O-H, Pevsner J. The abnormal regulation of gene expression in autistic brain tissue. J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31: 545 – 9. Rapin I. The autistic spectrum disorders. N Engl J Med 2002;347: 302 – 3. Rutter M, Silberg J, O’Connor T, Simonoff E. Genetics and child psychiatry: II. Empirical research findings. J Child Psych 1999;40: 19 – 55. Saitoh O, Courchese E. Magnetic resonance imaging study of the brain in autism. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998;52:219 – 22. Sears LL, Vest C, Mohamed S, Bailey J, Ranson BJ, Piven J. An MRI study of the basal ganglia in autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1999 (May);23(4):613 – 24. Schultz R, Gauthier I, Klin A, Fulbright RK, Anderson AW, Volkmar F, et al. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000 (Apr);57(4):331 – 40. Schopler E. Parent survival manual. New York: Plenum; 1995. Stokstad E. New hints into the biological basis of autism. Science 2001;294:34 – 7. Stro¨mland K, Nordin V, Miller M, Akerstrom B, Gillberg C. Autism in thalidomide embryopathy: a population study. Dev Med Child Neurol 1994;36:351 – 6. Stone RL, Aimi J, Barshop B, Jaeken J, Van den Berghe G, Zalkin H, et al. A mutation in adenylosuccinate lyase associated with mental retardation and autistic features. Nat Genet 1992;1:59 – 63. Tager-Flusberg H, Joseph R, Folstein S. Current directions in research on autism. Ment Retard Dev Disabl Res Rev 2001;7:21 – 9. The M.I.N.D. Institute Newsletter, Winter 2002/03;3. Tordjman S, Gutknecht L, Carlier M, Spitz E, Antoine C, Slama F, et al. Role of the serotonin transporter gene in the behavioral expression of autism. Mol Psychiatry 2001 (Jul);6(4):434 – 9. Warren ST, Sherman SL. The fragile X syndrome. In: Sciver CR, Beaudet AL, Sly WS, Valle D, editors. The metabolic and molecular basis of inherited disease. 8th ed. New York (NY): McGraw-Hill; 2000. Wing L. Syndromes of autism and atypical development. In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR, editors. Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. 2nd ed. New York (NY): Wiley; 1997. p. 148 – 70. Wing L. Asperger’s syndrome: a clinical account. Psychol Med 1981; 11:115 – 29. Winslow JT, Insel TR. The social deficits of the oxytocin knockout mouse. Neuropeptides 2002;36:2221 – 9. Wolterink G, Daenen LEWPM, Dubbeldam S, Gerrits MA, van Rijn R, Kruse CG, et al. Early amygdala damage in the rat as a model for neurodevelopmental psychopathological disorders. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2001 (Feb);11(1):51 – 9.