Autobiographical Memory

Autobiographical Memory

2.46 Autobiographical Memory M. A. Conway and H. L. Williams, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK ª 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 2.46.1 2.46.2 ...

417KB Sizes 36 Downloads 189 Views

2.46 Autobiographical Memory M. A. Conway and H. L. Williams, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK ª 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

2.46.1 2.46.2 2.46.3 2.46.4 2.46.5 2.46.6 2.46.7 2.46.8 References

Introduction A Brief Biography of Autobiographical Memory Research The Representation of Autobiographical Knowledge in Long-Term Memory Episodic Memory Self-Defining Memories Self-Images The Life Span Distribution of Autobiographical Memories Closing Section: Why Do We Have Autobiographical Memory?

2.46.1 Introduction The term autobiographical memory refers to our memory for specific episodes, episodic memory, and to our conceptual, generic, and schematic knowledge of our lives, autobiographical knowledge. Typically these two types of long-term memory representation are brought together in an act of remembering where they form a specific memory. Consider the following example: My earliest memories relate to a time in my childhood when we were living in Malta. I was about four years old. We lived in the most glorious Italian house on the sea which had a great big flagstone hall and shutters looking out to the sea and a sweeping staircase that led up to the first floor and, I think this is true, but it seems wrong somehow because my parents were very kind to me. I remember having to stand looking at the wall halfway up the stairs because I couldn’t remember the days of the week and I was taught them with reference to the gods, you know, Thor’s day, Woden’s day and so forth, and that I remember very vividly. One lunchtime I was asked to repeat them and I couldn’t remember them and my father told me to go and stand halfway up this great big sweeping staircase and just look at the wall. (Taken from the BBC Radio 4 Memory Survey, July 2006, which collected 11,000 memories from the general public.)

There are various segments of autobiographical knowledge in this memory, e.g., when we lived in Malta, my parents were kind to me, some generic visual imagery, e.g., how various features of the house looked, and some highly specific knowledge of time,

893 893 895 899 901 902 903 906 906

locations, and actions. Autobiographical memories very frequently come to mind as these compilations of different types of knowledge are configured into a memory in a specific act of remembering. As such they clearly illustrate the highly constructive nature of autobiographical remembering. We will return to memory construction in a later section, but now that we have some idea of what is meant by the term autobiographical memory, we might ask about how it has been studied. After all, autobiographical memories are personally important memory representations. They are the content of the self and define who we are, who we have been, and, importantly, who we can yet become. They enable us to have a past, present, and future in which we exist as individuals. They are, therefore, one of our most important bodies of knowledge and because of that would have been, it might be thought, the focus of memory research for many decades.

2.46.2 A Brief Biography of Autobiographical Memory Research Remarkably, however, the study of autobiographical memory has mainly taken place over the last 2 decades, whereas as the formal scientific study of memory itself is at least over a century old, dating, arguably, to the seminal work of Herman Ebbinghaus (1885). Ebbinghaus famously studied memory for relatively meaningless items, such as short lists of constant-vowel-constant (CVC) letter strings. Less well known is that he also studied memory for meaningful materials such as passages of prose, poetry, etc. Ebbinghaus concluded that memory for these latter 893

894 Autobiographical Memory

materials was influenced by too many factors beyond the experimenter’s control and because of this the scientific or experimental study of memory would be more surely advanced using materials that the experimenter had powerful control over, such as CVC strings. Ebbinghaus’s view held sway and the experimental study of memory in the laboratory has generally used to-be-remembered materials generated and controlled by the experimenter. Almost by definition this excludes autobiographical memories, as these are formed outside the laboratory in our everyday lives in response to complicated meaningful experiences – experiences over which the experimenter has no control. Given the dominance of experimental studies of memory, it is perhaps not so surprising that it is only in relatively recent times that autobiographical memory has received any attention at all. According to one view, science moves from the simple to the complex and perhaps it was the case that some understanding of memory, deriving from experimental studies, had to be attained before the field could grapple with the complexities of autobiographical memory and the inevitable role in memory of mysterious entities such as the self, goals, and emotion. There is no doubt some truth in this but, as with all history including personal history, the story is more complicated. So, for instance, at the time Ebbinghaus was writing his field-defining book, another great nineteenth-century scientist, Sir Francis Galton (1883), was reporting his seminal work into memory. One aspect of this research focused on the recall of autobiographical memories. Galton was interested in how many memories we have and developed a technique that 100 years later became known as the cue word technique. In this procedure, Galton revealed to himself, one at a time, words he had previously arranged into an alphabetical list. In response to each word, he noted what thoughts passed through his mind. So when reading abasement, abhorrence, etc. (remember this was Victorian England), he would write out his thoughts. He carried out this procedure for the fairly long list of words on several separate occasions. There were a wide range of findings but one striking outcome was that many of his thoughts were (autobiographical) memories and they often came to mind in the form of visual mental images. Galton was rather disappointed to discover that there was not an endless variety in his thoughts or memories and that he often recalled the same thoughts/ memories on subsequent occasions of testing. He concluded that we probably have far fewer memories

than we imagine we have – about 6500 according to one researcher who tried to recall all her memories (Smith, 1952). An obvious problem with Galton’s method is that once a subject has recalled a memory, then that memory became associated with the cue word and as such was much more likely to be recalled on subsequent occasions. If so, then Galton may well have underestimated the extent of his autobiographical memories. Nonetheless, the cue word method has proved especially useful in more contemporary studies of autobiographical memory and Galton’s original work remains a rewarding read for memory researchers, as does Ebbinghaus’s important book. Another book from this period that remains significant is Theodore Ribot’s (1882) classic case studies of memory distortions and malfunction following brain injury. This work also contains one of the first theories of autobiographical memory and is worth consulting for that alone. Other memory researchers from the late nineteenth century also studied autobiographical memory (see Conway, 1990, 2004, for reviews), and among them Henri and Henri (1896, 1898) conducted the first autobiographical memory survey. However, psychology came to be dominated by behaviorism, at the heart of which was the belief that all psychological theory should be built upon that which was observable. As memories are internal mental states, they cannot be studied by direct observation but can only be inferred by their effects upon behavior, i.e., upon what can be recalled in an experiment where the conditions of learning, retention, and remembering are highly controlled. This approach became known as verbal learning. Indeed, the dominant journal in the area was called the Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior (renamed in the 1980s the Journal of Memory and Language). For many decades, verbal learning dominated memory research and in many respects still does. A lone voice during this period was the British researcher Sir Fredrick Bartlett, whose famous book Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology (1932) is generally credited with having created and maintained a different tradition in memory research. In this tradition, the concept of a schema (some sort of general representation of similar experiences, narrative, and cultural conventions) was central and social interactions and culture played important roles in remembering. Bartlett was, however, largely uninterested in detailed memories of specific experiences – what we now call episodic

Autobiographical Memory

memories. Because of this, his work did not reinvigorate the study of autobiographical memory. Instead the reemergence of the study of autobiographical memory after 100 years of silence (Cohen, 1989) started to take place in the 1970s and gathered pace in the 1980s. Figure 1 shows the cumulative frequency of papers, by year since 1970, that have used the phrase autobiographical memory. This admittedly is a crude index of research activity into the topic, but as crude as it is, it nonetheless depicts very strikingly how autobiographical memory research has rapidly increased and developed in the last 35 years. So what happened to end the century of silence? There were, arguably, two main forces that led to renewed interest in this important aspect of memory. The first was the gradual emergence of neuropsychology as a distinct research area and within it the study of malfunctions of human memory following brain damage. One of the striking symptoms of patients with memory impairments caused by brain damage is that they virtually always have disrupted autobiographical memory. In a particularly important paper Crovitz and Schiffman (1974) reintroduced the Galton cue word method as a way of eliciting autobiographical memories in normal populations and later in patients with closed head injuries suffering from various degrees of amnesia, thus simultaneously rediscovering both Galton and Ribot. The second force was the developing interest within cognitive science in how to model and represent stories and memories. An important paper here that demonstrated how autobiographical memory might be studied under

895

laboratory conditions was that of Robinson (1976), who also used the cue word method to investigate differences between memories with different types of affect. Add to this Brown and Kulik’s (1977) original survey of flashbulb memories, a rather timely reminder from Neisser (1978) about the narrowness of memory research in the 1970s and preceding decades, and the highly significant volume edited by Neisser (1982), Memory Observed, which reprinted many of the papers of earlier researchers on autobiographical memory and other then-neglected areas of memory, and a strong impetus was in place to rejuvenate research into autobiographical memory. It is, perhaps, important to note that the renewed interest, reflected in Figure 1, had its roots in a rediscovery of the original work of Galton, Ribot, and others (see too Rapaport, 1950, for an especially interesting review of emotion and memory). It might be noted that the methods used by these early researchers – studying one’s own memory, investigating malfunctions and distortions of memories, and surveying memories – also re-emerged in the contemporary study of autobiographical memory, and it is to the findings of these more recent studies we now turn.

2.46.3 The Representation of Autobiographical Knowledge in Long-Term Memory This section reviews current thinking about the nature of autobiographical knowledge. It is important

200 180 160 140 120 100 80 60 40 20

19 7 19 6 7 19 7 7 19 8 7 19 9 8 19 0 81 19 8 19 2 83 19 8 19 4 8 19 5 8 19 6 8 19 7 88 19 8 19 9 9 19 0 9 19 1 9 19 2 93 19 9 19 4 9 19 5 9 19 6 9 19 7 9 19 8 9 20 9 0 20 0 0 20 1 0 20 2 03 20 0 20 4 0 20 5 06

0

Year

Figure 1 Frequency of articles or research reviews published using the term autobiographical memory in the article title, abstract, or key words from 1970 to 2006. Data obtained from ISI Web of Knowledge, January 2007.

896 Autobiographical Memory

to note that a full review of findings is not undertaken here and instead only main findings and their implications are considered. One current model proposes that autobiographical memories are generated in the self-memory system or SMS (Conway and PleydellPearce, 2000). Very briefly, the SMS is considered to be a virtual memory system consisting of a temporary interaction between control or executive processing systems with a complex multilayered long-term memory knowledge base. Another way to conceive of this is as an interaction between currently active, dynamic, or fluid aspects of the self with more permanent, long-term, or crystallized representations of the self and attributes of the self. The dynamic or executive aspect of the self is termed the working self. The working self consists of a complex hierarchy of currently active goals (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000) through which memories are encoded and retrieved. The working self also contains what Conway et al. (2004) termed the conceptual self, which in turn consists of beliefs, evaluations, and currently active self-images of what the self has been in the past, currently is considered to be, and what it may become in the future. The working self regulates the construction of new memories in the SMS, at both encoding and during retrieval, by controlling access to the autobiographical memory knowledge base. Figure 2 illustrates this relation between the working self and

The working self

Goal hierarchy Conceptual self Self images

Control processes

Autobiographical knowledge base

Autobiographical knowledge

Episodic memories

Figure 2 The relationship between the working self and the autobiographical knowledge base.

the knowledge base. The working self modulates memory by controlling the cues that are used to activate knowledge in the knowledge base. This is achieved by shaping cues so that particular types of information are activated. For example, a person asked to recall a memory of childhood might recall their earliest memory. Thus, elaborating the cue from ‘recall a memory from childhood’ into the cue ‘recall my earliest memory.’ This elaboration may take place several times as a cue is fine-tuned to access the information sought. An idea central to the SMS model is that specific autobiographical memories are formed when stable patterns of activation exist over interconnected representations of autobiographical knowledge and associated episodic memories. Thus, when conceptual and generic knowledge of the attributes of a house one lived in as a child, the relationship one had with one’s parents, and a specific (episodic) memory of a moment in time are all activated together and interlinked, then the rememberer has the experience of remembering and their consciousness is dominated by a specific memory – as in the example we started with. It is these different types of autobiographical knowledge and their organization in long-term memory that we are concerned with next and we return to considering the process of constructing memories in a subsequent section. According to the SMS model, long-term memory contains two distinct types of autobiographical representation: autobiographical knowledge and episodic memories. Autobiographical knowledge is organized in partonomic hierarchical knowledge structures (Conway and Bekerian, 1987; Barsalou, 1988; Conway, 1993, 1996; Lancaster and Barsalou, 1997; Burt et al. 2003) that range from highly abstract and conceptual knowledge (such as that contained in the conceptual self) to conceptual knowledge that is event-specific and experiencenear. Autobiographical memory knowledge structures terminate in episodic memories, the second type of autobiographical representation contained in the autobiographical knowledge base. Figure 3 illustrates how these complex autobiographical memory knowledge structures might be represented in long-term memory. The upper part of Figure 3 focuses on autobiographical knowledge and specifically on the life story, lifetime periods, and general events (Conway, 2005). These divisions of autobiographical knowledge are on a dimension of specificity, and at the most abstract level is a structure termed the life story

Autobiographical Memory

897

Life story Themes Relationship theme

Work theme

The conceptual self

Lifetime periods

Friends with Y: Others Activities Locations Projects Goals

Working at pub X: Others Goals Locations Projects Activities General events Leeds

Supervisor

Clearing tables

Episodic memories

Pay rise

Becoming a waitress

Meeting George

Figure 3 Knowledge structures in autobiographical memory. Adapted from Conway MA (2005) Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53(4): 594–628.

(Pillemer, 1998; Bluck and Habermas, 2001; Bluck, 2003). The life story contains general factual and evaluative knowledge about the individual. It may also contain self-images that divide and separate the self into several different selves. It is represented in more or less coherent sets of themes that characterize, identify, and give meaning to a whole life (Bluck and Habermas, 2000, 2001). Divisions in the life story

may be supported by the way in which different selfimages contain cues that differentially access other knowledge in the autobiographical knowledge base. For example, a self that accesses a particular lifetime period (see Figure 3) will have cues that are channeled by knowledge represented as part of the lifetime period, which in turn can be used to access particular sets of general events that contain cues to

898 Autobiographical Memory

specific episodic memories. It in this way that a memory can be gradually formed or constructed. Lifetime periods contain representations of locations, people, activities, feelings, and goals common to the period they represent. They effectively encapsulate a period in memory and in so doing provide further ways in which access to autobiographical knowledge is channeled, or directed. Lifetime periods have been found to contain evaluative knowledge, negative and positive, of progress in goal attainment (Beike and Landoll, 2000), and lifetime periods may play an important role in the life story. For instance, lifetime periods may provide autobiographical knowledge that can be used to form life story schema and thus support the generation of themes. Lifetime periods may be particularly appropriate for this because of the goalevaluative information they contain. For example, a lifetime period such as ‘when I was at university,’ will consist of representations of people, locations, activities, feelings, and goals common to the period but will also contain some general evaluation of the period, i.e., this was an anxious time for me, living away from home was difficult, I was lonely, I found the work too difficult, etc. (see Cantor and Kihlstrom, 1985). The life story and lifetime periods are part of the conceptual self where they represent a summary account of the self and its history, and where they can be used to initiate and focus searches of the autobiographical knowledge base. General events, on the other hand, are more clearly part of the knowledge base itself and have been found to play important roles in organizing personal knowledge. General events are more strongly event-specific than lifetime periods but not as event-specific as sensory-perceptual episodic memories, which are directly derived from actual experience (Conway, 2001, 2005). General events refer to a variety of autobiographical knowledge structures such as single events, e.g., the day we went to London; repeated events, e.g., work meetings; and extended events, e.g., our holiday in Spain (Barsalou, 1988). General events are organized in several different ways. For example, they can take the form of minihistories structured around detailed and sometimes vivid episodic memories of goal attainment in developing skills, knowledge, and personal relationships (Robinson, 1992). Some general events may be of experiences of particular significance for the self and act as reference points for other associated general events (Singer and Salovey, 1993; Pillemer, 1998). Yet other general events may be grouped together because of their emotional similarity (McAdams et al., 2001), and it is likely that there are yet other

forms of organization at this level which await investigation(see for example, Brown and Schopflocher, 1998). However, the research currently available indicates that organization of autobiographical knowledge at the level of general events is extensive and it appears to virtually always refer to progress in the attainment of highly self-relevant goals. General event knowledge then represents information highly relevant to the goal hierarchy of the working self. In one study of this type of knowledge, Robinson (1992) examined people’s memories for the acquisition of skills, e.g., riding a bicycle, driving a car, and for aspects of personal relationships. These general events were found to be organized around sets of vivid memories relating to goal attainment. Consider two examples from Robinson’s study: Ever agreeable, and eager to do anything that would get me out of the doldrums of inferiority, my father rented a bike and undertook to help me to learn to ride it. I shall always remember those first few glorious seconds when I realized I was riding on my own. . . (Quinn, 1990, cited in Robinson, 1992: 224.) The first time I flew an airplane was one of the best firsts. It marked a sense of accomplishment for myself, and it also started me on the career path I have always wanted to follow. The day was warm and hazy, much as summer days in Louisville are. My nervousness didn’t help the situation, as I perspired profusely. But as we took off from runway 6 the feeling of total euphoria took over, and I was no longer nervous or afraid. We cruised at 2500 feet and I worked on some basic manoeuvres for approximately 45 minutes. We then returned to the airport, where I realized that this will soon be a career. (Robinson, 1992: 226.)

These first-time memories cue other related memories and the whole general event carries powerful self-defining evaluations that persist over long periods of time. Relatively recent experiences, particularly those occurring during the current lifetime period, that give rise to sets of multiply related general events and associated episodic memories must be represented in terms of the currently active goals of the working self that dominate at the time. Burt et al. (2003) investigated this for several extended events, e.g., Christmas shopping. In these studies, events were sorted into groups by participants, and from these groupings currently active themes were identified. Figure 4 shows the organization of a series of episodic memories associated

Autobiographical Memory

Theme PARENTS

Theme HOUSE

899

Theme SPORT

Event

R and I had a good look at some interesting houses in a magazine

Buying a house

We were at home when R told me that the dye that the council had put down the toilet was connected to the council system

23/11/97 18/12/97 R and I put in an offer on a house in Bishopsdale and they accepted

Arrived home and R told me there was a major problem with the sewage system of the house we are buying

30/11/97

16/12/97 R, I, and our mobile mortgage manager talked about our loan application

Had a major bout of depression and could not stop crying, swearing, and yelling at R at home

1/12/97

13/12/97 Memories

R and I met the builder at the new place to get the builder’s consent

R and I received the loan approval at home

4/12/97 5/12/97 Figure 4 Episodic memories associated with the general event of buying a house. From Burt CDB, Kemp S, and Conway MA (2003) Themes, events, and episodes in autobiographical memory. Mem. Cogn. 31: 317–325.

with the general event of buying a house (Burt et al., 2003). The themes shown in Figure 4 are all associated with other memories as well and with lifetime periods in which the themes were present. The findings of Burt et al. (2003) demonstrate that general events typically access groups of episodic memories that connect the general event to unique and specific moments in time. One important property of this organization is that when goals change and new themes and lifetime periods become central to the working self, a record of the past concerns of an older version of the working self exists in the form of general events and the colonies of episodic memories they access. Thus, even if no goal information is explicitly encoded, it can, to at least some extent, be inferred from the groupings of general

events and the associated episodic memories. Indeed, Robinson found that many memories featured goalrelated evaluative knowledge or self-defining memories (Singer and Salovey, 1993) along with more general knowledge and specific episodic memories. General events provide, then, records of complicated and extended goal-related activities. These have powerful implications for the self, especially the conceptual self, and how a person evaluates their self.

2.46.4 Episodic Memory So far we have been concerned with autobiographical knowledge, but specific autobiographical memories

900 Autobiographical Memory Table 1 I II III IV V VI VII VIII

Eight characteristics of episodic memory

They retain summary records of sensory-perceptual-conceptual-affective processing derived from working memory. They are predominantly represented in the form of (visual) images. They represent short time slices, determined by changes in goal processing. They are only retained in a durable form if they become linked to conceptual autobiographical knowledge. Otherwise they are rapidly forgotten. Their main function is to provide a short-term record of progress in current goal processing. They are recollectively experienced when accessed. When included as part of an autobiographical memory construction, they provide specificity. Neuroanatomically they may be represented in brain regions separate from other (conceptual) autobiographical knowledge networks.

consist of autobiographical knowledge and episodic memories. Episodic memories are, however, rather different types of representations. Table 1 lists eight characteristics of episodic memories (from Conway, 2005, Table 4), and each of these is now considered in turn. The first three characteristics of episodic memories (numbered I, II, and III) in Table 1 concern properties of episodic memories. First, the content of episodic memories is highly event-related and consists of detailed records of sensory-perceptual and conceptual-affective processing that was prominent during the original experience. Note that these are summary rather than literal representations, although they may occasionally contain some exact representations of processing that occurred during an experience (see the last paragraph of this section). Second, although they can, and indeed do, contain information from all the sensory modalities, they have been found to be predominantly visual in nature (see Brewer, 1988, for an interesting early study of the content of episodic memories). Finally they represent short time-slices of experience highly related to the moment-by-moment segmentation of experience into events (Williams et al., 2007b; Zacks et al., 2007). Clearly, many episodic memories will be formed every day and simply casting one’s mind back over the events of the day will bring to mind many highly detailed and specific episodic memories of events which occurred earlier in the day (see Williams et al., 2007b). In subsequent days, however, as the retention interval lengthens, many of these episodic memories, which are often of rather low self-relevance, routine events, become inaccessible. Even those that are retained over longer retention intervals are often not as detailed as they were close to the point of their formation. It has been suggested that only those episodic memories that are linked in some way to currently active goals become integrated with

autobiographical knowledge in long-term memory. Episodic memories that become integrated in this way are retained over long retention intervals measured in months, years, decades, and even a lifetime (point IV in Table 1). Relatedly, the SMS model posits that one of the main functions of episodic memories is to provide a record of recent goal-processing episodes. Episodic memories provide a way in which to rapidly and effectively check that goalrelated actions have been executed. They let the rememberer know that they did, for instance, lock the door, post the letter, have a coffee, and so on. If one of these routine events mapped onto an important goal or set of goals, then the episodic memory might become integrated with other knowledge in the autobiographical knowledge base and so become an enduring episodic memory. The study of selfdefining experiences, the experience of trauma, and vivid memories generally provide many examples of how episodic memories become important parts of the autobiographical knowledge base, where they endure for many years (see Pillemer, 1998; Ehlers and Clark, 2000; Singer, 2005). Points VI and VII in Table 1 focus on another important aspect of episodic memories – that they are very highly associated with the experience of remembering. This is often referred to as recollective experience, and this and other forms of memory awareness have been the focus of many contemporary memory studies (see Tulving, 1985; Gardiner and Richardson-Klavehn, 2000, for reviews). Memory awareness in autobiographical remembering appears to be triggered or activated when an episodic memory enters conscious awareness (Conway, 2001, 2005), although it can also occur in other ways (cf. Moulin et al., 2005). Episodic memories, when they enter the construction of an autobiographical memory, cause the experience of remembering and also provided the constructed memory with specificity. As we will

Autobiographical Memory

see, the specificity of the memory is important and is a quality that can be lost when memory malfunctions in, for example, psychological illness. Specificity provides a link to the experience of the world, and episodic memories are experience-near representations and stand in contrast to autobiographical and other conceptual knowledge which is experience-distant. Thus, the experience of remembering and memory specificity are important qualities of episodic memories. Finally, in Table 1 (see VII), it is suggested that episodic memories might be represented in a separate brain region from more autobiographical conceptual knowledge (this is elaborated in Conway, 2005). We will return to this issue in the closing section of this chapter, but we might note here one general and intriguing finding that seems to support it: patients who suffer brain damage which has led to amnesia for much of their preinjury life, and especially amnesia for preinjury episodic memories, have nonetheless been found to retain often extensive autobiographical knowledge (Conway and Fthenaki, 2000).

2.46.5 Self-Defining Memories The autobiographical knowledge base is complex and represents the personal history of an individual in different ways, i.e., as knowledge and as specific memories. Because of this complexity, the knowledge base is highly organized and some parts are more accessible than other parts. Generally, those autobiographical knowledge structures that are strongly associated with current goals and current images of the self are in a more accessible state than knowledge structures that are currently less self-relevant. In this section, we consider how the relation to the self can shape and organize autobiographical memory. One important type of personal knowledge that appears to be highly accessible to the self is that of self-defining memories (SDMs). An SDM is a specific type of autobiographical memory that has the following attributes: affective intensity, vividness, high levels of rehearsal, linkage to similar memories, and connection to an enduring concern or unresolved conflict (Singer and Moffitt, 1991/1992; Singer and Salovey, 1993; Singer, 2005). Self-defining memories can be distinguished from other types of vivid memories. For example, flashbulb memories, as originally defined by Brown and Kulik (1977), are a particularly vivid and affective form of personal event memory (Pillemer, 1998), often about

901

important public events. They have been found to be associated with four interrelated variables: surprise, consequentiality, importance, and emotion (Conway, 1995). Having these qualities does not necessarily indicate, however, that the memory is central to enduring goals of the self, and it is certainly possible to have highly vivid memories of events that are low in self-relevance (Conway et al., 2004). Importantly then, the two distinguishing criteria for self-defining memories that differentiate them from other vivid memories are, first, their linkage to other memories within the individual that share similar personal themes and, second, their relevance to the individual’s enduring concerns or unresolved conflicts. Both of these features – linkage of similar memories and relevance to concerns and conflicts – have been investigated in research into individuals’ motivations and goals. For example, Thorne et al. (1998) looked at young adults’ important relationship memories generated in two interviews over a 6-month period of time. Participants had freedom to describe similar or different relationship episodes in the second interview. Thorne et al. scored the memories for social motives for the memories that varied from time 1 to time 2, as well as the points of emphasis in the twice-told memories. For both unique memories and repeated memories, the authors found ‘‘moderate thematic consistency’’ (Thorne et al., 1998: 258), indicating that these memories, even when varying in content, reflected similar motivational themes and narrative structures. In a related study, Demorest and Alexander (1992) had raters code individuals’ significant personal memories for overarching interpersonal scripts. A month later, these same individuals generated a set of fictional scenarios. Raters coded the themes of these scenarios and found striking overlap in terms of thematic continuity between the original memories and the imaginary stories. These results, along with those of Thorne et al. (1998), suggest that individuals link remembered and imagined experiences through personally significant themes. These themes originate, according to the SMS model, from the goals of the working self, but later can also serve to influence its ongoing goal processing. Further evidence of the relationship of self-defining memories to individuals’ enduring conflicts and concerns comes from the work of Singer and colleagues (Singer, 1990; Moffitt and Singer, 1994; Singer, 2005). These researchers found the affective quality of self-defining memories to be a function of the relevance of the memories to the attainment of a person’s most desired goals. Moreover, this was found

902 Autobiographical Memory

to be the case not only for memories relevant to the attainment of approach goals (desired goals), but also for memories about active efforts to avoid the consequences of undesired outcomes (Moffitt and Singer, 1994). Singer et al. (2002) additionally reported that the more personal growth students attributed to memories that grew out of community service experiences, the more likely these students were to place an overall emphasis on generative goal pursuits in their lives (see also de St. Aubin and McAdams, 1995). Similarly, in examining the relationship of turning-point and other significant personal memories to overall themes of the personality, McAdams (McAdams, 1982; McAdams et al., 1996) has consistently found power-oriented memories to be linked to agentic or individualistic motives, while intimacyoriented memories reflected communal, social, and relationship motives. Jardine (1999) found that women counselors who experienced life transitions during their clinical training associated themes from their self-defining memories with their set of possible selves (Markus and Nurius, 1986). In a series of clinical case studies involving both individual and couples in psychotherapy, Singer found self-defining memories to be linked to critical relationship themes which were expressed in both clients’ intimate relationships and in the transference dynamics of the therapy (Singer and Singer, 1992, 1994; Singer and Salovey, 1996; Singer, 2001; Singer and Blagov, 2004). In addition to their linkage to goals, SDMs also can play directive and mood regulatory functions for the self (Pillemer, 1998, 2003; Bluck, 2003). For example, SDMs have been found to play a role in providing life lessons or integrative meanings that help individuals in optimal adjustment and personal growth. This is what Bluck (2003) termed the directive function of autobiographical memories. Blagov and Singer (2004) demonstrated that individuals with larger numbers of SDMs that contained reflective themes or messages, as reliably coded by three raters (see Singer and Blagov (2000) for an SDM coding manual), displayed optimal levels of self-restraint and emotional expression, as measured by the Weinberger Adjustment Inventory Short Form (Weinberger, 1997, 1998). Thorne et al. (2004) found that, compared to other types of personal memories, individuals were more likely to rely on SDMs involving tension or goal conflict to provide insights and life lessons. SDMs provide information that can guide and direct the individual in everyday life. One specific form of directive function is the regulation of mood. Josephson et al. (1996) found that nondepressed

individuals enlisted positive memories to repair negative moods, while mildly depressed individuals were less likely to recruit positive memories after a negative mood had been induced. Similarly, Moffitt et al. (1994) found that depressed individuals were less likely to recall SDMs when asked to retrieve a positive memory, while they did not differ in memory specificity for negative memories. Williams (1996), though not specifically addressing SDMs, has argued that a lack of memory specificity in depressed and suicidal individuals reflects a cognitive deficit generalized from a learned defense against encoding and retrieving affectively threatening selfrelevant experiences. In summary, the findings from a broad range of studies converge on the view that SDMs are central to goals and conflicts within the individual (see Singer, 2005); they provide important integrative lessons, insights, or directives for the working self (see especially Pillemer, 1998); and they may regulate mood in important ways.

2.46.6 Self-Images Conway et al. (2004) describe what they termed the conceptual self. One important knowledge structure in the conceptual self are self-images. It is proposed that self-images are knowledge structures that summarize complex sets of interlinked autobiographical knowledge and episodic memories that cumulatively support a particular view or version of the self. (Note that selfimages can be permanent stable representations or more transitory, fleeting mental representations.) Conway (2005) proposes that these summary representations may often be experienced as images and hence the term self-images. A question of some interest here is how self-images are related to selective sets of memories. Rathbone et al. (2006; described in Conway, 2005) studied this by having a group of middle-aged participants complete a short questionnaire in which they completed six ‘I am. . .’ statements (Kuhn and McPartland, 1954). An ‘I am. . .’ could be anything, for example, I am bad, I am sociable, I am a banker, I am a mother, etc. Later each person recalled specific autobiographical memories to each of their ‘I am. . .’ statements. The dates of the memories, expressed in age at encoding, and the dates of the emergence of the ‘I am. . .’ statement were then compared; Figure 5 shows the distribution of age at encoding of the memories relative to age of emergence of the ‘I ams. . .’. Figure 5 strikingly shows that age at encoding clusters around the date of emergence of the ‘I ams. . .,’ strongly

Autobiographical Memory

903

14.0

Percentage of memories

12.0 10.0 8.0 6.0 4.0 2.0

40

36

28 32

24

20

16

12

4 8

0

–4

2 –8

6

–1

0

–1

4

–2

8

–2

2

–2

6

–3

–3

–4

0

0.0

Age of memories (in 2-year bins) relative to date of formation of ‘I ams’ Figure 5 Distribution of memories recalled to ‘‘I ams. . .’’ (Rathbone et al., 2006).

suggesting that ‘I ams. . .’ or self-images are grounded in sets of memories of formative experiences. Further work found that the ‘I ams. . .’ could be categorized into two broad classes: roles and traits, e.g., I am a student versus I am charming. However, both types of ‘I ams. . .,’ role and trait, gave rise to the same distribution as that shown for ‘I ams. . .’ overall in Figure 5. Both role and trait ‘I ams. . .’ seem then to be marked in memory by highly accessible specific memories that come first to mind when the ‘I am. . .’ is processed. This may reflect the grounding of these aspects of the conceptual self, self-images, in subsets of memories and knowledge that define and provide the content for that self-image. This differentiation of the self, supported by the organization of autobiographical memory into self-images, might be particularly important in the development of the self – a point we return to after considering the

distribution of memories over the life span and the significance of this for the self.

2.46.7 The Life Span Distribution of Autobiographical Memories Important periods of development of the self are reflected in the life span retrieval curve which is observed when older adults (about 35 years and older) recall autobiographical memories in free recall or in a variety of cued recall conditions (Franklin and Holding, 1977; Fitzgerald and Lawrence, 1984; Rubin et al., 1986, 1998). Memories are plotted in terms of age at encoding of the remembered experiences, and the resulting life span retrieval curve typically takes a form similar to that shown in Figure 6 (this is an

Number of memories

60 50

The reminscence bump

40 30 20

Period of recency

Period of childhood amnesia

10 0 5

10

15 20 25 30 35 40 Age at encoding (in 5-year bins)

45

50

Figure 6 Idealized representation of the life span retrieval curve. From Conway MA (2005) Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53(4): 594–628.

904 Autobiographical Memory

idealized representation derived from many studies and not based on specific data). As Figure 6 shows, the life span retrieval curve consists of three components: the period of childhood amnesia (from birth to approximately 5 years of age), the period of the reminiscence bump (from 10 to 30 years), and the period of recency (from the present declining back to the period of the reminiscence bump). The pattern of the life span retrieval curve is extremely robust and has been observed in many studies – to such an extent that it led Rubin to conclude that it was one of the most reliable phenomena of contemporary memory research (Conway and Rubin, 1993). This reliability is remarkably striking. In a recent study, Conway et al. (2005) sampled groups from five different countries: the United States, the United Kingdom, Bangladesh, Japan, and China. Figure 7 shows the life span retrieval curves for each of these countries. (Note that participants were instructed not to recall events from the previous year to eliminate the recency portion of the curve.) It can be seen from Figure 7 that there were highly similar periods of childhood amnesia and reminiscence bump across countries. This further demonstrates the robustness of the life span retrieval curve and perhaps its universality. If the data for the five countries are collapsed together and an overall life span retrieval curve plotted, then the remarkably consistent distribution shown in the idealized curve of Figure 6 is observed. There are many theoretical explanations of the period of childhood amnesia (see Pillemer and White 1989; Wang, 2003, for reviews), but most flounder on the fact that children below the age of 5 years have

a wide range of specific and detailed autosbiographical memories (Fivush et al., 1996; Bauer, 1997). Explanations that postulate childhood amnesia to be related to general developmental changes in intellect, language, emotion, etc., fail simply because apparently normal autobiographical memories were in fact accessible when the individual was in the period of childhood amnesia. It seems unlikely that an increase in general functioning would make unavailable previously accessible memories. From the SMS perspective, this period is seen as reflecting changes in the working self goal hierarchy, the idea being that the goals of the infant and young child, through which experience is encoded into memory, are so different, so disjunct, from those of the adult that the adult working self is unable to access those memories (see also Howe and Courage, 1997, for a particularly interesting account of childhood amnesia in terms of development of the self). Other accounts emphasize mother/child interactions, the role of language development, and emergence of narrative abilities (Fivush and Nelson, 2004). Socialization and culture must play some role in the development of memory, although it seems that the infant/child capacity to actually have episodic memories may predate these developments (RoveeCollier, 1997). If this is the case, then presumably the effects of socialization, culture, and language are largely on the organization of memory and perhaps on memory content as well, rather than on the processes that mediate the actual formation of episodic memories. For instance, the finding of Conway et al. (2005) that U.S. participants retrieved earlier earliest memories than all other groups might relate to the

Percentage of memories

35

Japan Bangladesh UK China US All

30 25 20 15 10 5 0 5

10

15

20 25 30 35 40 45 50 Age at encoding (in 5-year bins)

55

60

Figure 7 Life span retrieval curves from five countries. From Conway MA (2005) Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53(4): 594–628.

Autobiographical Memory

observation that U.S. mothers undertake more memory talk with their children than mothers from other countries. Moreover, Wang and her colleagues (e.g. Wang, 2001) have found powerful cross-cultural differences in the focus and content of memories. Childhood memories from people in cultures with interdependent self-focus (Markus and Kitayama, 1991) such as China tend to be less oriented to the individual, less emotional, and more socially oriented than the childhood memories of people from cultures with independent self-focus, for example, Northern European or North American cultures (see Wang, 2001). Thus, socialization experiences and the selffocus that predominates in a culture may influence the accessibility of earliest memories and their content. The second component of the life span retrieval curve is the period when rememberers were aged 10 to 30 years, known as the reminiscence bump (Rubin et al., 1986). The reminiscence bump is distinguished by an increase in recall of memories relative to the periods that precede and follow it. The reminiscence bump is present not just in the recall of specific autobiographical memories but also emerges in a range of different types of autobiographical knowledge. For example, the reminiscence bump has been observed in the recall of films (Sehulster, 1996), music (cf. Rubin et al., 1998), books (Larsen, 1998), and public events (Schuman et al., 1997; Holmes and Conway, 1999). Memories recalled from the period of the reminiscence bump are more accurate (Rubin et al., 1998), are judged more important than memories from other time periods, and are rated as highly likely to be included in one’s autobiography (Fitzgerald, 1988; Fromholt and Larsen, 1991, 1992; Fitzgerald, 1996; Rubin and Schulkind, 1997). The reminiscence bump is only observed in people over the age of about 35 years and some recent findings suggest that it might only be present, or is much more prominent, in memories of positive experiences (Rubin and Bernsten, 2003). Many of the more obvious explanations of the reminiscence bump have been rejected, e.g., that the memories are of first-time experiences and that is why they are memorable, as in fact it has been found that less than 20% are typically of first-time experiences (Fitzgerald, 1988). Rubin et al. (1998) reviewed a series of potential explanations and argued in favor of an explanation in terms of novelty. According to this view, the period when people are aged 10–30 years, and especially 15–25 years, is distinguished by novel experiences, occurring during a

905

period of rapid change that gives way to a period of stability. It is assumed that memories from the period of rapid change are more distinct than those from the period of stability and this is why they are comparatively more frequently accessed. By this account, a period of rapid change taking place at some other point in the life cycle should also lead to raised accessibility of memories from that period relative to more stable periods, and there is some evidence that this is the case (Conway and Haque, 1999). However, periods of (goal) change and experiences of novelty always involve the self and a related but alternative explanation is that the high accessibility of memories from this period (and other periods as well) may be related to their enduring relation to the self (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). Possibly, many memories from the period of the reminiscence bump are memories of self-defining experiences (see Fitzgerald, 1988) and have a powerful effect in cohering the working self into a particular form. The novelty of reminiscence bump experiences lies in their newness and uniqueness for the self and they may play a crucial role in the final formation of a stable self system and identity formation during late adolescence and early adulthood. The raised accessibility of these memories might then serve processes relating to the coherence of self through time. Thus, the period of the reminiscence bump might be a period in which a sole ‘I am. . .,’ or self-image, develops into multiple ‘I ams. . .,’ e.g., I am a son, I am a student, I am a boyfriend, etc. Also, at this point multiple ‘I will becomes. . .’ may be formed, supported by the differentiation of ‘I ams. . .’ and the final emergence of a complete working self goal hierarchy and conceptual self grounded in autobiographical knowledge and memories (the SMS). Finally it might be noted that older patients with schizophrenia have been found to show an early and disorganized reminiscence bump, with an impairment of conscious recollection associated with memories highly relevant to personal identity (Cuevo-Lombard et al., 2007). This suggests that a developmental failure present in schizophrenia is the consolidation of personal identity in late adolescence/early adulthood. Possibly, one of the features of the abnormal SMS associated with this is a failure or weakening of the grounding of conceptual autobiographical knowledge in episodic memories of formative experiences, further demonstrating the importance of an integrated self with self-images strongly embedded in sets of defining episodic memories.

906 Autobiographical Memory

2.46.8 Closing Section: Why Do We Have Autobiographical Memory? In many respects this may seem a pointless or rhetorical question; after all, if we did not have autobiographical memory there would be little in the way of individuality, personality, culture, society, literature, etc. Much that differentiates humanity from other species would be absent (see Tulving, 1983). At the level of the individual, disruption to or loss of autobiographical memory leads to people who typically cannot function in society. For example, clinically depressed patients often have severely impaired autobiographical memories in which they can no longer generate specific memories, their memories lack detail, they are overly general (Williams, 1996). Such patients cannot operate in the social world and, moreover, have unspecific futures in which they cannot visualize specific plans and goals (Williams et al., 2007a). Similarly, with amnesic patients whose memory disorders arise from organic brain damage, having multiple selfimages in a specific future in which goals and plans originating from memories of the past are realized is no longer possible. Thus, one good reason to have an intact and functioning autobiographical memory is that it allows the individual to have a future in which a continuous self operates. But what does this mean? The future is, of course, a time where new experiences, some anticipated, will take place. But we cannot know we have arrived at the future without a memory – that is, without knowledge of a past. The concept of future makes no sense, conceptually or psychologically, without a past. One way to think about this is to conceive of the future as a place where new goal processing will take place and the past as some sort of record of previous episodes of goal processing. To achieve future goals it is essential to have a record of how one has progressed with the same or related goals in the past. Consider very recent goals. In order to know that one locked the car after parking it this morning, we simply remember that episode. The events of the current day can typically be recalled (on that day) at length and in highly specific detail. Thus, checking on progress with goals, locking the car, making a call, mailing a paper, etc., can be verified. However, within a few days, access to these sorts of detailed memories is lost. No doubt this is useful as retaining a highly detailed record of every action would lead to an overloaded and unworkable memory.

Nonetheless, keeping a detailed record in the short term is highly adaptive and prevents the repetition of actions and the adoption of courses of actions that have a high probability of failing. Conway (2005) argues that episodic memory is the memory system that keeps a record of very recent goal-related activities. It is a system that has evolved highly specific memory representations that facilitate the type of short-term goal processing that can keep goals focused and environmentally relevant. It is suggested that this is a species-wide adaptation and, consequently, episodic memory is common to many species. As such it is probably a phylogenetically older memory system and may be represented in neural networks located toward the middle and posterior of the brain (a temporal-occipital network; see Conway, 2005). In contrast, humans have developed conceptual knowledge that forms complex knowledge structures that endure over long periods of time, even over a lifetime. This, it is suggested, is a more recent evolutionary development and is mediated by neural networks toward the front of the brain: frontotemporal regions. The conceptual memory system supports long-term goal processing, for example, relationships, work projects, etc. Episodic memories that are retained become attached to conceptual knowledge and provide highly specific instances of goal processing related to the more general or generic goals of the conceptual self and self-images. Autobiographical memory then allows us to have both short- and long-term goals and to integrate these in coherent ways that facilitate goal processing in the future.

Acknowledgments Martin Conway was supported by the award of a Professorial Fellowship from the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC), RES-051-27-0127 of the United Kingdom, and Helen Williams by a Research Assistantship, also from the ESRC.

References Barsalou LW (1988) The content and organization of autobiographical memories. In: Neisser U and Winograd E (eds.) Remembering Reconsidered: Ecological and Traditional Approaches to the Study of Memory, pp. 193–243. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bartlett FC (1932) Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Autobiographical Memory Bauer P (1997) Development of memory in early childhood. In: Cowan N (ed.) The Development of Memory in Childhood, pp. 83–112. Sussex: Psychology Press. Beike DR and Landoll SL (2000) Striving for a consistent life story: Cognitive reactions to autobiographical memories. Soc. Cogn. 18: 292–318. Blagov PS and Singer JA (2004) Four dimensions of selfdefining memories (specificity, meaning, content, and affect) and their relationships to self-restraint, distress, and repressive defensiveness. J. Pers. 72: 481–511. Bluck S (2003) Autobiographical memory: Exploring its functions in everyday life. Memory 11: 113–123. Bluck S and Habermas T (2000) The life story schema. Motiv. Emot. 24: 121–147. Bluck S and Habermas T (2001) Extending the study of autobiographical memory: Thinking back about life across the life span. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5: 135–147. Brewer WF (1988) Memory for randomly sampled autobiographical events. In: Neisser U and Winograd E (eds.) Remembering Reconsidered: Ecological and Traditional Approaches to the Study of Memory, pp. 21–90. New York: Cambridge University Press. Brown NR and Schopflocher D (1998) Event clusters: An organization of personal events in autobiographical memory. Psychol. Sci. 9: 470–475. Brown R and Kulik J (1977) Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5: 73–99. Burt CDB, Kemp S, and Conway MA (2003) Themes, events, and episodes in autobiographical memory. Mem. Cogn. 31: 317–325. Cantor N and Kihlstrom JF (1985) Social intelligence: The cognitive basis of personality. In: Shaver P (ed.) Self, Situations, and Social Behavior. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, pp. 15–34. Beverly Hills: Sage. Cohen G (1989) Memory in the Real World, 1st edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Conway MA (1990) Autobiographical Memory: An Introduction. Buckingham: Open University Press. Conway MA (1993) The structure of memory. In Conway MA and Morris PE (eds.) International Library of Critical Writings in Psychology, Vol. 2: Memory Structure, pp. 21–27. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing. Conway MA (1996) Autobiographical memories and autobiographical knowledge. In: Rubin DC (ed.) Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory, pp. 67–93. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Conway MA (2001) Sensory perceptual episodic memory and its context: Autobiographical memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 356: 1297–1306. Conway MA (2004) Autobiographical memory and the self. In: Byrne JH, Eichenbaum H, Roediger H III, and Thompson RF (eds.) Learning and Memory, 2nd edn. Farmingham Hills, MI: Macmillan Reference. Conway MA (2005) Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53(4): 594–628. Conway MA and Bekerian DA (1987) Organization in autobiographical memory. Mem. Cogn. 15 119–132. Conway MA and Fthenaki A (2000) Disruption and loss of autobiographical memory. In: Cermak LS (ed.) Handbook of Neuropsychology: Memory and Its Disorders, 2nd edn., pp. 281–312. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Conway MA and Haque S (1999) Overshadowing the reminiscence bump: Memories of a struggle for independence. J. Adult Dev. 6: 35–44. Conway MA and Pleydell-Pearce CW (2000) The construction of autobiographical memories in the self memory system. Psychol. Rev. 107: 261–288. Conway MA and Rubin DC (1993) The structure of autobiographical memory. In: Collins AE, Gathercole SE,

907

Conway MA, and Morris PEM (eds.) Theories of Memory, pp. 103–137. Hove, Sussex: Lawrence Erlbaum. Conway MA, Singer JA, and Tagini A (2004) The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Soc. Cogn. 22: 495–537. Conway MA, Wang Q, Hanyu K, and Haque S (2005) A crosscultural investigation of autobiographical memory: On the universality and cultural variation of the reminiscence bump. J. Cross-Cultur. Psychol. 36: 739–749. Crovitz HF and Schiffman H (1974) Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 4: 517–518. Cuevo-Lombard C, Jovenin N, Hedelin G, Rizzo-Peter L, Conway MA, and Danion J-M (2007) Autobiographical memory of adolescence and early adulthood events: An investigation in schizophrenia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13: 335–343. Demorest AP and Alexander IE (1992) Affective scripts as organizers of personal experience. J. Pers. 60: 645–663. De St. Aubin E and McAdams DP (1995) The relations of generative concern and generative action to personality traits, satisfaction/happiness with life, and ego development. J. Adult Dev. 2: 99–112. Ebbinghaus H (1885/1964) Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology, Ruger HA and Bussenius CE (trans.). New York: Dover Publications. Ehlers A and Clark DM (2000) A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 38: 319–345. Fitzgerald JM (1988) Vivid memories and the reminiscence phenomenon: The role of a self narrative. Hum. Dev. 31: 261–273. Fitzgerald JM (1996) Intersecting, meanings of reminiscence in adult development and aging. In: Rubin DC (ed.) Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory, pp. 360–383. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Fitzgerald JM and Lawrence R (1984) Autobiographical memory across the lifespan. J. Gerontol. 39: 692–698. Fivush R and Nelson K (2004) Culture and language in the emergence of autobiographical memory. Psychol. Sci. 15: 573–577. Fivush R, Hayden C, and Reese E (1996) Remembering, recounting, and reminiscing: The development of memory in a social context. In: Rubin D (ed.) Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory, pp. 341–359. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Franklin HC and Holding DH (1977) Personal memories at different ages. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 29: 527–532. Fromholt P and Larsen SF (1991) Autobiographical memory in normal aging and primary degenerative dementia (dementia of the Alzheimer type). J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci. 46: 85–91. Fromholt P and Larsen SF (1992) Autobiographical memory and life-history narratives in aging and dementia (Alzheimer type). In: Conway MA, Rubin DC, Spinnler H, and Wagenaar WA (eds.) Theoretical Perspectives on Autobiographical Memory, pp. 413–426. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Galton F (1883) Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development, 1st edn. London: Macmillan and Co. Gardiner JM and Richardson-Klavehn A (2000) Remembering and knowing. In: Tulving E and Craik FIM (eds.) Handbook of Memory, pp. 229–244. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Henri V and Henri C (1896) Enqueˆte sur les premiers souvenirs de l’enfance. Anne´e Psychol. 3: 184–198. Henri V and Henri C (1898) Earliest recollections. Pop. Sci. Month. 53: 108–115. Holmes A and Conway MA (1999) Generation identity and the reminiscence bump: Memory for public and private events. J. Adult Dev. 6: 21–34.

908 Autobiographical Memory Howe ML and Courage ML (1997) The emergence and early development of autobiographical memory. Psychol. Rev. 104: 499–523. Jardine KF (1999) Transitions of women counsellors-in-training: Self-defining memories, narratives, and possible selves. Diss. Abst. Int. A Hum. Soc. Sci. 59(11-A): 4068. Josephson B, Singer JA, and Salovey P (1996) Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cogn. Emo. 10: 437–444. Kuhn MH and McPartland TS (1954) An empirical investigation of self attitudes. Am. Sociol. Rev. 19: 68–76. Lancaster JS and Barsalou LW (1997) Multiple organisations of events in memory. Memory 5: 569–599. Larsen SF (1998) What is it like to remember? On phenomenal qualities of memory. In: Thompson CP (ed.) Autobiographical Memory: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives, pp. 163–190. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Markus HR and Kitayama S (1991) Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98: 224–253. Markus H and Nurius P (1986) Possible selves. Am. Psychol. 41: 954–969. McAdams DP (1982) Experiences of intimacy and power: Relationships between social motives and autobiographical memory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 42: 292–302. McAdams DP, Hoffman BJ, Mansfield ED, and Day R (1996) Themes of agency and communion in significant autobiographical scenes. J. Pers. 64: 229–378. McAdams DP, Reynolds J, Lewis M, Patten AH, and Bowman PJ (2001) When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27: 474–485. Moffitt KH and Singer JA (1994) Continuity in the life story: Selfdefining memories, affect, and approach/avoidance personal strivings. J. Pers. 62: 21–43. Moffitt KH, Singer JA, Nelligan DW, Carlson MA, and Vyse SA (1994) Depression and memory narrative type. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103: 581–583. Moulin CJA, Conway MA, Thompson RG, James N, and Jones RW (2005) Disordered memory awareness: Recollective confabulation in two cases of persistent de´ja` ve´cu. Neuropsychologia 43: 1362–1378. Neisser U (1978) Memory: What are the important questions? In: Gruneberg MM, Morris PE, and Sykes RN (eds.) Practical Aspects of Memory. London: Academic Press. Neisser U (ed.) (1982) Memory Observed: Remembering in Natural Contexts, San Francisco: Freeman. Pillemer DB (1998) Momentous Events, Vivid Memories. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Pillemer DB (2003) Directive functions of autobiographical memory: The guiding power of the specific episode. Memory 11: 193–202. Pillemer DB and White SH (1989) Childhood events recalled by children and adults. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 21: 297–340. Quinn DK (1990) Freedom on wheels. The Christian Science Monitor. April 19, p. 17. Cited in Robinson JA (1992) First experience memories: Contexts and function in personal histories. In: Conway MA, Rubin DC, Spinnler H, and Wagenaar W (eds.) Theoretical Perspectives on Autobiographical Memory, pp. 223–239. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Rapaport D (1950/1971) Emotions and Memory. New York: International Universities Press. Rathbone CJ, Moulin CJA, and Conway MA (2006) Autobiographical memory and identity: Self-centred memories. Presented at the Fourth International Conference on Memory (ICOM-4), Sydney, Australia, July.

Ribot T (1882) Diseases of Memory: An Essay in the Positive Psychology, Smith WH (trans.). New York: D. Appleton. Robinson JA (1976) Sampling autobiographical memory. Cogn. Psychol. 8: 578–595. Robinson JA (1992) First experience memories: Contexts and function in personal histories. In: Conway MA, Rubin DC, Spinnler H, and Wagenaar W (eds.) Theoretical Perspectives on Autobiographical Memory, pp. 223–239. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Rovee-Collier C (1997) Dissociations in infant memory: Rethinking the development of implicit and explicit memory. Psychol. Rev. 104: 467–498. Rubin DC and Berntsen D (2003) Life scripts help to maintain autobiographical memories of highly positive, but not highly negative, events. Mem. Cogn. 31: 1–14. Rubin DC and Schulkind MD (1997) The distribution of autobiographical memories across the lifespan. Mem. Cogn. 25: 859–866. Rubin DC, Wetzler SE, and Nebes RD (1986) Autobiographical memory across the life span. In: Rubin DC (ed.) Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rubin DC, Rahhal TA, and Poon LW (1998) Things learned in early adulthood are remembered best. Mem. Cogn. 26: 3–19. Schuman H, Belli RF, and Bischoping K (1997) The generational basis of historical knowledge. In: Jodelet D, Pennebaker J, and Paez D (eds.), Political Events and Collective Memories, pp. 47–78. London: Routledge. Sehulster JR (1996) In my era: Evidence for the perception of a special period in the past. Memory 4: 145–158. Singer JA (1990) Affective responses to autobiographical memories and their relationship to long-term goals. J. Pers. 58: 535–563. Singer JA (2001) Living in the amber cloud: A life story analysis of a heroin addict. In: McAdams DP, Josselson R, and Lieblich A (eds.) Turns in the Road: Narrative Studies of Lives in Transition, pp. 253–277. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Singer JA (2005) Memories That Matter: Using Self-Defining Memories to Understand and Change Your Life. Oakland: New Harbinger. Singer JA and Blagov PS (2000) Classification system and scoring manual for self-defining autobiographical memories. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Applied Research on Memory and Cognition, Miami Beach, FL, June. Singer JA and Blagov PS (2004) Self-defining memories, narrative identity, and psychotherapy: A conceptual model, empirical investigation, and case report. In: Angus LE and McLeod J (eds.) Handbook of Narrative and Psychotherapy: Practice, Theory and Research, pp. 229–246. Thousand Oaks CA: Sage. Singer JA and Moffitt KH (1991/1992) An experimental investigation of specificity and generality in memory narratives. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 11: 233–257. Singer JA and Salovey AP (1993) The Remembered Self. New York: The Free Press. Singer JA and Salovey AP (1996) Motivated memory: Selfdefining memories, goals, and affect regulation. In: Martin L and Tesser A (eds.) Striving and Feeling: Interactions Among Goals, Affect, and Self-Regulation, pp. 229–250. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Singer JA and Singer JL (1992) Transference in psychotherapy and daily life: Implications of current memory and social cognition research. In: Barron JW and Eagle MN (eds.) Interface of Psychoanalysis and Psychology, pp. 516–538. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Singer JA and Singer JL (1994) Social-cognitive and narrative perspectives on transference. In: Masling JM and Bornstein

Autobiographical Memory RF (eds.) Empirical Perspectives on Object Relations Theory. Empirical Studies of Psychoanalytic Theories, vol. 5, pp. 157–193. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Singer JA, King LA, Green MC, and Barr SC (2002) Personal identity and civic responsibility: ‘‘Rising to the occasion’’ narratives and generativity in community action interns. J. Soc. Issues 58: 535–556. Smith ME (1952) Childhood memories compared with those of adult life. J. Genet. Psychol. 80: 151–182. Thorne A, Cutting L, and Skaw D (1998) Young adults’ relationship memories and the life story: Examples or essential landmarks. Narr. Inq. 8: 237–268. Thorne A, McLean KC, and Lawrence AM (2004) When remembering is not enough: Reflecting on self-defining memories in late adolescence. J. Pers. 72: 513–542. Tulving E (1983) Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Tulving E (1985) Memory and consciousness. Can. Psychol. 26: 1–12. Wang Q (2001) Cultural effects on adults’ earliest childhood recollection and self-description: Implications for the relation between memory and the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81: 220–233.

909

Wang Q (2003) Infantile amnesia reconsidered: A cross-cultural analysis. Memory 11: 65–80. Weinberger DA (1997) Distress and self-restraint as measures of adjustment across the life span: Confirmatory factor analyses in clinical and non-clinical samples. Psychol. Assess. 9: 132–135. Weinberger DA (1998) Defenses, personality structure, and development: Integrating psychodynamic theory into a typological approach to personality. J. Pers. 66: 1061–1080. Williams JMG, Barnhofer T, Crane C, et al. (2007a) Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol. Bull. 133(1): 122–148. Williams HL, Conway MA, and Baddeley AD (2007b) The boundaries of episodic memories. In: Shipley TF and Zacks JM (eds.) Understanding Events: How Humans See, Represent, and Act on Events. New York: Oxford University Press. Williams JMG (1996) Depression and the specificity of autobiographical memory. In: Rubin DC (ed.) Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory, pp. 244–267. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Zacks JM, Speer NK, Swallow KM, Braver TS, and Reynolds JR (2007) Event perception: A mind/brain perspective. Psychol. Bull. 133: 273–293.