or ADHD

or ADHD

Behavioral Distinctions in Children with Reading Disabilities and/or ADHD STEWART PISECCO, PH.D., DAVID B. BAKER, PH.D., PHIL A. SILVA, PH.D., O.B.E.,...

750KB Sizes 0 Downloads 137 Views

Behavioral Distinctions in Children with Reading Disabilities and/or ADHD STEWART PISECCO, PH.D., DAVID B. BAKER, PH.D., PHIL A. SILVA, PH.D., O.B.E., AND MARK BROOKE, B.A.(HoNs.)

ABSTRACT Objective: To investigate behavioral distinctions between children with reading disabilities (RD) and attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Method: A four-group mixed design consisting of children with reading disabilities only (RD only), reading disabilities and ADHD (RD/ADHD), ADHD only, and a comparison group was used. Differences between parent reports, from age 5 to 15 years, and teacher reports, from age 5 to 13 years, were examined on measures of hyperactive and antisocial behaviors. Results: The analyses indicated that, at home, children from the ADHD only and RD/ADHD groups exhibited significantly more hyperactive behaviors than children from the RD only and comparison groups. At school, children from the RD only, ADHD only, and RD/ADHD groups typically exhibited significantly more hyperactive and antisocial behaviors than children from the comparison group. With regard to antisocial behaviors, children from the RD/ADHD group exhibited significantly more antisocial behaviors than children from any other group. Conclusions: The results of the study indicate that children from these groups may exhibit either a “pervasive” or “situational” presentation of behavioral problems, a finding which suggests that in conducting an evaluation of ADHD it is important to obtain both parent and teacher reports of problem behaviors. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.

Psychiatry, 1996, 35(1 1):1477-1484. Key Words: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, reading disability, epidemiology.

One of the most consistent findings in the childhood psychopathology literature is that children with reading disabilities frequently present with additional psychiatric disorders (Hinshaw, 1992; McGee et al., 1986; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1992; Williams et al., 1990; Williams and McGee, 1994). For instance, Williams et al. (1990) examined the relationship between risk factors and behavioral and emotional disorders in 11year-old children and found that reading problems

Accepted April 24, 1996 Dr. Pisecco is Assistant Projssor, University of Houston; Dr. Baker is Associate Proj&sor, University of North Texas: Dr. Silva is Director and Mr. Brooke is Biostatistician, Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit (DMHDRU), Universityof Otago Medical School, New Zealand Dr. Pisecco’s work was compk-ted while on a Fulbright Grant hnded by the New Zealand-United States Educational Founahtion. This research was ah0 supported by grants from the NIMH (MH45070 to Terrie Mofitt and MH49414 to Avshalom Carpi). Dr. Silva and the DMHDRU are supported by the New Zealand Health Research Council. Special thanks to Terrie Mofitt, Avshalom Carpi, and anonymous reviewers for editorial comments. Reprint requests to Dr. Pisecco, University of Houston, College of Education, Department of Educational Psychoingy, Houston, 7X 77204-5071. 0890-8567/96/3511-1477$03.00/001996 by the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

J . A M . ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY,

successfully predicted the coexistence of another psychiatric disorder. Others have reported that the presence of a behavioral disorder is the most common longterm association with reading difficulties (McGee et al., 1988a). In an attempt to distinguish between the type of disruptive disorder and its association with reading difficulties, Anderson et al. (1989) failed to find a relationship between aggressive conduct disorder and poor reading. However, an association between conduct disorder and reading difficulties has been documented when conduct disorder was paired with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (McGee et al., 1988~).This has led researchers to suggest that reading disabilities are most commonly associated with ADHD. This hypothesized association has been well documented (Frick et al., 1991; Hinshaw, 1992), with conservative comorbidity estimates of approximately 23% (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1992). The co-occurrence of ADHD and reading difficulties is so frequent that some have begun to question the nature of this relationship (Hinsaw, 1992; Jorm et al., 1986; McGee and Share, 1988; McGee et al., 1986;

35:11, N O V E M B E R 1996

1477

PISECCO ET AL

Waldie and Spreen, 1993; Williams et al., 1990, Williams and McGee, 1994). For example, McGee and Share (1988) suggested that reading disorders lead to the development of ADHD behaviors. In contrast, DuPaul and Stoner (1994) noted that others have proposed that ADHD leads to the manifestation of reading difficulties. These debates typify the two most common causality hypotheses. The first is the suggestion that disorder A causes disorder B. The second hypothesis is that disorder B causes disorder A. Pennington et al. (1993) put forth an additional hypothesis which suggests that in some cases disorder A could cause symptoms of disorder B to manifest without the full occurrence of disorder B. They refer to this as the phenocopy hypothesis. In an attempt to find support for their hypothesis and to identify cognitive distinctions between these two groups, Pennington et al. (1993) compared and contrasted the performance of various groups (i.e., reading disabilities [RD] only, ADHD only, ADHD and RD, and control groups) on two different domains. Domain A consisted of phonological processing measures and domain B consisted of executive functioning measures. They found that for the phonological processing domain, the RD only and RD/ADHD groups’ performance was impaired compared with the ADHD only and control group. However, on the executive functioning measures the RD only and RD/ADHD groups’ performance was normal. In contrast, the ADHD only group performed worse on the executive measures than did the RD only and RD/ADHD groups. Since the performance of the RD/ADHD group was more consistent with the performance of the RD only group, they concluded that the ADHD symptomatology is likely secondary to the reading disability. Their results support the phenocopy hypothesis, a hypothesis which suggests that reading disorders may cause symptoms of ADHD to manifest without the development of the full syndrome. Pennington and colleagues’ (1993) conclusions are interesting in that they could explain the high comorbidity of ADHD symptoms in the reading disability population while at the same time preserving the validity of the ADHD diagnosis. However, it should be noted that other attempts to identify such distinctions have failed (McGee et al., 1988b). By identifying distinctions between children with reading disabilities and those with ADHD it is possible

1478

to document the independent validity of each diagnostic criterion. While the majority of such attempts have focused on phonological and cognitive differences, efforts to identify other areas of distinction are also important. For instance, if ADHD is to be thought of as an independent disorder, then it would be reasonable to suggest that children with ADHD would present with different behaviors at home and school than would children with reading disabilities. However, if McGee and Share’s suggestion that reading disabilities lead to the development of ADHD-like symptoms is correct, then there would be few, if any, behavioral differences between the two groups. To determine which behavioral characteristics are attributable to a diagnosis of ADHD or to the presence of a reading disability, it would be important to use a four-group mixed design, with the groups consisting of children with reading disabilities only (RD only), children with reading disabilities and ADHD (RD/ADHD), children with ADHD only, and a comparison group. As well as including an index of ADHD symptoms, it would also be important to include an index of antisocial behaviors because children with ADHD and reading difficulties frequently exhibit such behaviors. Research specifically focused on behavioral distinctions between children with reading disabilities only, children with reading disabilities and ADHD, children with ADHD only, and a comparison group is limited. The purpose of this study was to investigate behavioral distinctions, on parent and teacher reports, between four groups of children (RD only, RD/ADHD, ADHD only, and a comparison group) from early childhood to mid-adolescence. O n the basis of a review of the literature, we expected the following results: (1) O n parent reports, children from the RD/ADHD and ADHD only groups will exhibit significantly more ADHD and antisocial behaviors than children from the RD only and comparison groups. (2) O n teacher reports, children from the RD only, RD/ADHD, and ADHD only groups will exhibit elevated levels of ADHD and antisocial behaviors than children from the comparison group. METHOD Subjects Subjects were children involved in the Dunedin (New Zealand) Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study. The cohort’s

J. AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 35:11, NOVEMBER 1996

B E H A V I O R A L DISTl NCTIONS

history has been described by Silva (1990). The study is a longitudinal investigation of the health, development, and behavior of a complete cohort of consecutive births between April 1, 1972, and March 31, 1973, in Dunedin, New Zealand. Perinatal data were obtained at birth. When the children were traced for follow-up at 3 years of age, 1,039 children were deemed eligible for inclusion in the longitudinal study. Since then, follow-ups have been conducted biannually. The sample consisted of subjects predominantly of European ancestry (fewer than 2% were Polynesian) and is therefore comparable with similar white samples from other English-speaking Western cultures. McGee (1985) has compared children who were lost to the study at each age with those remaining by age 11 years and found no systematic differences in social class, IQ, or a variety of behavioral variables.

Instruments Reading Ability. At ages 7 , 9, 11, 13, 15, and 18 years, the children completed the Burt Word Reading Test-Revised (New Zealand Council for Research in Education, 1981). The Burt Word Reading Test is a word-recognition reading test normed for New Zealand children. In format it resembles the American Wide Range Achievement Test of reading. The Burt Word Reading Test is considered a very consistent measure, with test-retest reliability coefficients ranging from .95 to .99 and internal consistency coefficients ranging from .96 to .97. Behavior Ratings. When the children were aged 5 years their parents completed form A and when they were aged 7 years their teachers completed form B of the Rutter Child Scales (RCS) (Rutter et al., 1970). The RCS-A consists of 31 items describing three types of problem behaviors: aggressiveness, hyperactivity, and anxiety-worry. The items were coded by the parents, who described the behavior as follows: does not apply (0), applies somewhat (l), or certainly applies (2). The a coefficients are .81 for the aggressiveness scale, .74 for the hyperactivity scale, and .59 for the anxiety-worry scale. The RCS-B consists of 26 items and covers the same areas of behaviors as the parent version and is completed in the same manner. The ci coefficients for the aggressiveness, hyperactivity, and anxiety-worry scales are .83, 3 2 , and .72, respectively. At ages 9 and 11, three items were included which measured additional inattentive and hyperactive behaviors (McGee et al., 1985). When the children were aged 13 years, the teachers completed the RCS-B. However, when the children were aged 13 and 15 years the parents completed the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist (RBPC) (Quay and Peterson, 1987), which contains more extensive and age-appropriate items than the Rutter scales. The RBPC consists of 89 items of which 77 load on four major subscales. Each item is rated: does not apply (0), applies somewhat (l), or certainly applies (2). The major subscales are Conduct Disorder, Socialized Aggression, Inattentive, and Hyperactivity. Each scale consists of 11 to 22 items, and the scales have yielded a coefficients ranging from .82 to .94. For the purposes of this study the Socialized Aggression, Inattentive, and Hyperactivity scales of the RBPC were computed to examine the occurrence of antisocial and ADHD behaviors.

Procedure

All children were categorized on the basis of their reading scores and behavioral ratings when they were 11 years old. RD Only Group. Typically the identification of reading disorders relies on a discrepancy between the child’s reading level and ability, with ability being represented by the child‘s score on an intelligence

J. A M . A C A D . C H I L D A D O L E S C . P S Y C H I A T R Y ,

test (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1992). The basic assumption that drives such an approach is best illustrated by Burt’s (1937) argument that ‘‘ . . . capacity must obviously limit content. It is impossible for a pint jug to hold more than a pint of milk and it is equally impossible for a child’s educational attainment to rise higher than his educable capacity” (p. 477). Share et al. (1989) tested that assumption and found that rates of progress and levels of achievement clearly indicate that I Q does not set a limit on reading progress, even in children with extremely low IQs. They concluded that the use of I Q tests to determine achievement potential in reading appears unjustified. Consequently, a child was considered reading-disabled if he or she obtained a reading score that was at least 1.5 standard deviations below the total ( n = 925) sample’s average reading score. In addition, children in this group did not meet the diagnostic criteria for any type of psychiatric disorder. Since we did not use the standard discrepancy approach to identify reading disability in this study, children in this group will be referred to as having a reading disability rather than a disorder. ADHD Only. Three sources existed for the report of symptoms meeting DSM-III criteria for ADD. These sources included selfreported symptoms as assessed with the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children-Child Version (DISC-C) (Costello et al., 1982) and parent and teacher reports of inattention and hyperactivity from the Rutter scales A and B. Each child was individually interviewed by a child psychiatrist with the DISC-C. An ADHD diagnosis was made if two of the three possible reporters provided independent consensus report of all DSM-Illcriteria, if one reporter met the full criterion and another met some of the criteria, or if one reporter alone provided all symptoms to meet the diagnostic criteria (for a complete description of this method and the results see Anderson et al., 1989). ADHD/RLI Group. Children in this group met the criteria for ADHD as described above and had a reading score that was at least 1.5 standard deviations below the mean of the sample. Comparison Group. Children in this group did not meet the criteria for either a reading disability or ADHD and were free from any other handicapping condition.

Identification of the Groups O n the basis of the above criteria, 43 children had ADHD. Thirty-six of those 43 subjects were male and 7 were female. As a result of the low number of females with ADHD the subsequent analyses were limited to males. Of those 36 males with ADHD, 19 were identified as reading-disabled and 17 as “normal” readers. Of the 19 subjects from the ADHD/RD group, 6 also met the criteria for either a conduct or oppositional defiant disorder. O f the 17 subjects with ADHD only, 8 also met the criteria for either a conduct or oppositional defiant disorder. Of the remaining members of the sample, 57 were reading-disabled and 269 qualified for the comparison group.

RESULTS

All raw data were transformed to Zscores, to make interscale comparisons possible. Therefore, all data presented in the tables are Z scores unless otherwise specified. All group contrasts were done using the Schefle method.

3 5 : 1 1 , N O V E M B E R 1996

1479

PISECCO ET AL.

hyperactive than the RD only or comparison groups. The main effect for time and the interaction effect for time and group were not significant. There was also a significant main effect for group (Wilks' h F[3,310] = 5.45, p < .001) on the index of antisocial behaviors. At ages 5 (F[3,360]= 4.90, p < . O l ) , 7 (F[3,356]= 7 . 4 1 , < ~ .OOl), 9 (F[3,361]= 6.47, p < .001), and 11 (F[3,354]= 5.60, p < .OOl) the RD/ADHD group was described as significantly more antisocial than the comparison or RD only groups. The main effect for time and the interaction effect for time and group were not significant.

Parent Behavioral Ratings

T o examine differences on the parent ratings of ADHD and delinquent behaviors, a 4 X 6 repeatedmeasures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 1. Analyses indicated that there was a significant main effect for group (Wilks' h F[3,294] = 19.57, p < .OOl) on the index of ADHD behaviors. At age 5 (F[3,356] = 9.08, p < .001), the ADHD only and RD/ADHD groups were described as more hyperactive than the RD only and comparison groups. At age 7 (F[3,347]= 7.68, p < .001), children from the RD/ ADHD group were described as significantly more hyperactive than children from either the comparison or RD only groups. At age 9 (F[3,347]= 12.56, p < .001) and 11 (F[3,355]= 1 2 . 7 8 , < ~ . O O l ) , the ADHD only and RD/ADHD only groups were described as significantly more hyperactive than the RD only or comparison groups. At age 13 (F[3,322]= 19.76, p < .001), the ADHD/RD group was described as significantly more hyperactive than the RD only or comparison groups. In addition, the ADHD only and RD only groups were described as significantly more hyperactive than the comparison group. At 15 (F[3,348]= 19.79, p < .001), the ADHD only and RD/ADHD groups were described as significantly more

Teacher Behavioral Ratings

T o examine differences on the teacher ratings of ADHD and antisocial behaviors, a 4 X 5 repeatedmeasures MANOVA was performed. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 2. There was a significant main effect for group (Wilks' h F[3,302]= 55.43, p < .001) on the index of ADHD behaviors. At age 5 (F[3,356]= 20.09, p < .OOl), children from the RD/ADHD group were described as significantly more hyperactive than were children from the RD only, ADHD only, and comparison groups. At age 7 (F[3,351] = 18.39, p < .001), children from the RD/ADHD and ADHD groups were described as significantly more hyperactive than

TABLE 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Univariate Results for Parent Reports of ADHD Symptomatology and Antisocial Behaviors Age (yr)

RD Only

RD/ADHD

ADHD Only

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Comparison Mean SD

0.80 0.89 0.85

0.78 1.06 1.01

0.95 0.99 1.14

0.88 0.25 0.98

1.25 1.01 1.43

-0.06 -0.04 -0.06

0.98 1.oo 0.93

9.08"' 7.68"' 12.56"'

~~

5

ADHD ADHD ADHD

7 9

-0.03 0.01 -0.03

ADHD

11

0.03

0.92

1.00

1.37

0.96

1.27

-0.09

0.91

12.78"'

ADHD

13

0.28

1.10

1.21

1.65

0.74

1.08

-0.20

0.74

19.76''

ADHD

15

0.18

0.97

1.25

1.19

0.94

1.27

-0.15

0.83

19.79''

Antisocial Antisocial Antisocial Antisocial Antisocial Antisocial

5 7 9 11 13 15

0.07 -0.02 -0.02 -0.13 0.03 -0.10

0.88 0.92 1.08 0.82 1.06 0.87

0.90 1.02 0.90 0.57 0.40 -0.01

0.91 1.14 1.16 1.13 1.07 0.85

0.22 0.14 0.44 0.21 0.16 0.35

0.87 0.83 1.37 1.08 1.04 1.32

-0.03 0.05 -0.18 -0.11 -0.16

1.09 0.95 0.93 0.78 0.72 0.79

4.90" 7.41"' 6.47"' 5.60"' 2.68' 2.06

0.00

Group Contrasts

F ~

~~~

~

2>1&4 2>1&4 2>1&4 3>1&4 2>1&4 3>1&4 4 < 1,3,&2 1<2 2>1&4 3>1&4 2>1&4 2>1&4 2>1&4 2>1&4

Note: ADHD = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; RD = reading disabilities. ' p < .05, nonsignificant Scheffk group contrasts; " p < .01; '"p < .001.

1480

J . AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 3 5 : 1 1 , N O V E M B E R 1996

BEHAVIORAL D I S T I N C T I O N S

TABLE 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and Univariate Results for Teacher Reports of ADHD Symptomatology and Antisocial Behaviors Age (yr)

RD Only Mean SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

F

RD/ADHD

ADHD Only

Comparison

ADHD ADHD

7

0.15 0.36

1.10 1.12

1.58 1.45

1.33 1.63

0.48 0.92

1.06 1.17

-0.11 -0.05

0.88 0.89

20.09' 18.39"

ADHD ADHD

9 11

0.77 0.30

1.15 0.84

1.46 2.01

1.13 1.20

1.35 2.00

1.17 1.37

-0.07 -0.15

0.77 0.8 1

3 1.73* 65.32"

ADHD

13

0.37

1.08

1.26

1.58

0.53

1.15

-0.07

0.89

12.00*

Antisocial

5

0.41

1.42

1.45

2.16

0.24

1.03

-0.11

0.86

15.13"

Antisocial

7

0.22

1.28

1.02

1.69

0.26

1.31

-0.12

0.78

9.89'

Antisocial

9

0.25

1.26

1.45

1.98

0.9 1

1.73

-0.1 1

0.86

17.05"

Antisocial

11

-0.1 1

0.83

1.35

1.79

1.63

1.75

-0.17

0.67

39.53*

Antisocial

13

0.36

1.46

0.92

2.1 1

0.59

1.47

-0.14

0.77

8.80'

5

Note: ADHD * p < .001.

=

attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; RD

=

Group Contrasts 2 > 1,3,&4 2>1&4 3>4 4 < 1,2,&3 4 <1,2,&3 1<2&3 4 <1,2,&3 1<2 4<1&2 2>1&3 4<1&2 2>1&3 2>1&4 3>4 4<2&3 1<2&3 4 < 1,2823

reading disabilities.

were children from the comparison group. In addition, the RD/ADHD group was described as significantly more hyperactive than the RD only group. At age 9 (F[3,3541 = 31.73, p < .OOl), the RD only, RD/ ADHD, and A D H D only groups were described as significantly more hyperactive than the comparison group. At age 11 (F[3,360] = 65.32, p < .OOl), the RD only, RD/ADHD, and A D H D only groups were described as significantly more hyperactive than the comparison group. In addition, the RD/ADHD and A D H D groups were described as significantly more hyperactive than the RD only group. At age 13 (F[3,319] = 12.00, p < .OOl), the RD only, RD/ ADHD, and A D H D only groups were described as significantly more hyperactive than the comparison group. In addition, the RD/ADHD group was described as significantly more hyperactive than the RD only group. Results of the 4 X 5 repeated-measures MANOVA indicated that there was also a main effect for time (Wilks' h F[4,1208] = 10.46, p < .001) and an interaction effect (Wilks' h F[12,1208] = 5.10, p < .001). Further analysis suggests that the A D H D only group became increasingly hyperactive until age 11. The RD/ADHD group was rated as increasingly hyperactive from ages 9 to 11. During that same time span, the RD only group was rated as decreasingly

hyperactive. The RD/ADHD and A D H D only groups' ratings of hyperactivity decreased from age 11 to 13. Analyses of teacher antisocial ratings indicate that there was a significant main effect for group (KVilks' h F[3,312] = 35.35, p < .OOl). At age 5 (F[3,358] = 15.13, p < .001), the RD/ADHD group was rated as significantly more antisocial than the RD only, A D H D only, and comparison groups. In addition, the RD only group was rated as significantly more antisocial than the comparison group. At age 7 (F[3,355] = 9.89, p < .001), both the RD only and RD/ADHD groups were rated as significantly more antisocial than the comparison group. At age 9 (F[3,361] = 17.05, p < .001), The RD/ADHD group was rated as significantly more antisocial than the RD only and comparison groups. The ADHD only group was also rated as significantly more antisocial than the comparison group. At age 11 (F[3,360] = 39.50, p < .OOl), both the RD/ADHD and A D H D only groups were rated as significantly more antisocial than the RD only and comparison groups. At age 13 (F[3,319] = 8.80, p < .OOl), the RD/ADHD, A D H D only, and RD only groups were rated as significantly more antisocial than the comparison group. Results of the 4 X 5 repeated-measures MANOVA also indicate that there was also a main effect for time (Wilks' h F[4,1248] = 3 . 3 0 , ~ < .Ol) and an interaction

J. A M . A C A D . C H I L D A D O L E S C . P S Y C H I A T R Y , 3 5 : 1 1 , N O V E M B E R 1996

1481

PISECCO ET AL.

effect (Wilks’ h F[12,1248] = 4 . 8 1 , ~ < .001). Analysis of the interaction suggests that the A D H D only group was rated as increasingly more antisocial until the age of 11. From ages 1 1 to 13, antisocial ratings for the A D H D only group decreased in severity. Antisocial ratings for the RD only group decreased in severity from ages 9 to 11. However, their antisocial ratings increased from ages 11 to 13. DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to investigate behavioral distinctions, on parent and teacher reports of A D H D and antisocially related behaviors, between four groups of children (RD only, RD/ADHD, A D H D only, and a comparison group) from early childhood to mid-adolescence. This particular design allows for distinctions to be made between which behaviors are characteristic of a diagnosis of A D H D and which are characteristic of reading difficulties. Overall there was general support for the hypothesis that situational and pervasive groups would be identified. However, the groups of children with situational and pervasive problems fluctuated as a function of the specific behavior being monitored. O n parent reports of ADHD-related behaviors, from the ages of 5 to 15, the ratings for the RD/ADHD and A D H D only groups were typically significantly higher than those of the RD only and comparison groups. The only exceptions were at ages 7 and 13. Overall, these results suggest that the A D H D behaviors exhibited, at home, by the RD/ADHD group are primarily attributable to their A D H D diagnosis. For parent antisocial ratings, the aforementioned hypothesis was also supported. The results suggest that the antisocial tendencies of the RD/ADHD group are unlikely to be solely attributable to their reading difficulties or A D H D diagnosis. The results of the analyses of intergroup differences on the teacher behavioral reports are more complex than the results of the analyses for the parent ratings. These results suggest that, for a particular group of children, reading difficulties may lead to the manifestation or exacerbation of ADHD-like symptoms. In addition, it would seem that both reading disabilities and the presence of A D H D behaviors may be associated with the manifestation or exacerbation of antisocial behaviors.

1482

Also noteworthy is the finding that at school both groups exhibited elevated levels of antisocial behaviors at varying times. One possible interpretation would be that both reading problems and A D H D may lead to the development or exacerbation of antisocial behaviors at school but likely in reaction to different stimuli. For example, children with reading difficulties may exhibit more antisocial behaviors as a result of their inability to master academic tasks, while children with A D H D may exhibit antisocial behaviors as a result of their inattentive, impulsive, and hyperactive tendencies. In comparing the parent and teacher reports it becomes apparent that at times the ratings are inconsistent. As Cohen et al. (1990) noted, in the past parentteacher discrepancies were thought to be a function of low rating scale reliability. However, they go on to suggest that parent-teacher discrepancies are more likely a function of the heterogeneity of the population. Others have also suggested that there are likely two groups of children who present with behavioral disorders, a situational group and a pervasive group (Goodman and Stevenson, 1989; Schachar et al., 1981). This suggestion has clear implications for the results of this study. For instance, one group of children exhibited the full A D H D behavioral syndrome. These children would meet a “pervasive” A D H D diagnostic criteria in that they exhibited symptomatology across a variety of situations. The pervasive presentation of symptomatology is an important consideration in making an appropriate diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). This group would include children from the RD/ADHD and A D H D only groups. Pennington et al. (1993) concluded that children from the RD/ A D H D group manifested A D H D symptomatology in reaction to reading problems. Their conclusion implies that these children do not exhibit the full A D H D syndrome and clashes with the results of this study. Analyses of our behavioral data suggest that neither malady is the result of the other, but rather children from the RD/ADHD group may actually be afflicted with both conditions. These children are likely to be representative of a small subtype, who Pennington et al. (1993) referred to as possessing both disorders. That children from this group would meet a pervasive criterion is not surprising because inclusion in the A D H D group required independent consensus from two reporters.

J . A M . A C A D . C H I L D A D O L E S C . P S Y C H I A T R Y , 35:11, N O V E M B E R 1996

B E H AVI 0 RAL D I S T I N C T I 0 N S

The presence of a situational group may have significant implications for both clinical practice and research. In this study children from the RD only group exhibited ADHD symptoms only at school. As mentioned, their reading difficulties may have led to the manifestation of ADHD-like symptoms. This finding would be consistent with past suggestions that ADHD symptomatology is often secondary to reading problems (McGee and Share, 1988) and that the presentation of ADHDrelated behaviors may be affected by environmental influences (Barkley, 1991). This has clear clinical implications because a large number of children are typically referred for ADHD evaluations by their school. This finding also confirms the importance of obtaining behavioral ratings from both the child’s parents and his or her teachers and ruling out the presence of a learning problem when reporter discrepancies are noted. For researchers it is important to clarify and validate distinctions between those children who exhibit situational and those who exhibit pervasive ADHD characteristics. Research in the treatment of ADHD may especially benefit from this distinction. Differences between a situational and pervasive presentation may also be valid for the presentation of antisocial behaviors. As was mentioned earlier, the RD only and ADHD only groups exhibited elevated levels of antisocial behaviors at varying times. Such behaviors are likely a result of situational influences, as these children did not have elevated levels of antisocial behaviors at home. As suggested, the development of such behaviors at school may be a reaction to differing demands. In addition, there are those children, with both reading problems and ADHD, who exhibit characteristics of a “pervasive” behavioral disorder. This group of children would likely consist of individuals to whom Moffitt (1993) referred as “life-course persistent delinquents.” As mentioned earlier, their behavior is unlikely solely attributable to reading problems or ADHD. The pervasive nature of their problems would also rule out the solo influence of the school environment. Consequently, additional factors may place these children further at risk for the development of delinquency, such as neurological influences, temperament, family adversity, or other unidentified variables. Before concluding it is important to consider the limitations of this study. The first limitation is that the sample consisted predominantly of subjects of

European ancestry. Consequently, the generalization of these findings to individuals from other ethnic minorities may be limited. Second, as with any longitudinal study, it is important to consider the impact of a cohort effect, which may further limit the generalization of the data. Finally, it is also important to recognize that the parent and teacher rating forms used in this study lack validity indicators. Consequently, it is impossible to know the absolute accuracy with which the respondents completed the forms. This limitation is one that is not unique to this study because the majority of parent and teacher forms do not have validity scales. One reassuring note in regard to this last limitation is that this particular sample has a long history of revealing personal information and in each incidence their confidentiality has never been compromised. In conclusion, the results of this study support the hypothesis that there are behavioral distinctions between children with reading difficulties and those with ADHD. More specifically, we were able to identify a group of children who suffer from pervasive problems related to ADHD and other behavioral problems. In addition, it should be noted that there seem to be situational variables that may lead to the manifestation of symptoms related to an ADHD diagnosis and other disruptive problems. This particular finding supports McGee and Share’s (1988) suggestion that reading problems may lead to the manifestation of ADHD behaviors and Pennington and colleagues’ (1993) phenocopy hypothesis. These results also highlight the need for a comprehensive evaluation that includes both parent and teacher behavioral reports when making a decision about the diagnosis of ADHD. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostir and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-II.3.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Anderson J, Williams S, McGee R, Silva P (1989), Cognitive and social correlates of DSM-III disorders in preadolescent children. ] A m Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 283342-846 Barkley RA (1991), Diagnosis and assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Compr Ment Health Care 1:27-43 Burt C (1937), The Backward Child. London: University of London Press Cohen M, Becker MG, Campbell R (1990), Relationship among four methods of assessment of children with ADHD. 1 Sch Psychol 28: 189-202 Costello A, Edelbrock C, Was R, Kessler M, Klaric SA (1982), Diagnostic Interview Schedule f . r Children (DISC; contract RFP-DB-8 1-0027). Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Mental Health DuPaul GT, Stoner G (1994), ADHD in the Schooh: Assessment and Intervention Strategies. New York: Guilford

J . A M . A C A D . C H I L D A D O L E S C . P S Y C H I A T R Y , 35:11, N O V E M B E R 1996

1483

P I S E C C O E T AL. Frick PJ, Kamphaus RW, Lahey BB et al. (19911, Academic underachievement and the disruptive behavior disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol 59:289-294 Goodman R, Stevenson J (1989), A twin study of hyperactivity: I. An examination of hyperactivity scores and categories derived from Rutter teacher and parent questionnaires. J Child PsycholPsychiatry 30:671-689 Hinshaw SP (1992), Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychol Bull 11 1: 127-1 55 Jorm AF, Share DL, Matthews R, Maclean R (1986), Behaviour problems in specific reading retarded and general reading backward children: a longitudinal study. J Psychol Psychiatry 27:33-43 McGee R (1985), Response Rates at Phase XI of the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study. Report from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Otago Medical School, Dunedin, New Zealand McGee R, Share DL (1988), Attention deficit disorder-hyperactivity and academic failure: which comes first and what should be treated? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 27:318-325 McGee R, Share D, Mofitt TE, Williams S, Silva PA (1988a), Reading disability, behaviour problems and juvenile delinquency. In: Individual Differences in Childrenand Adolescents: International Research Perspectives, Saklofske D, Eysenck S, eds. London: Hodder and Stoughton, pp 158-172 McGee R, Williams S, Bradshaw J, Chapel JL, Robins A, Silva PA (1985), The Rutter scale for completion by teachers: factor structure and relationship with cognitive abilities and family adversity for a sample of New Zealand children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 26:727-739 McGee R, Williams S, Mofitt TE, Anderson J (1988b), A comparison of 13-year-old boys with attention deficit and/or reading disorder on neuro-psychological measures. J Abnorm Child Psychol 17:37-53 McGee R, Williams S, Share DL, Anderson J, Silva PA (1986), The relationship between specific reading retardation, general reading backwardness and behavioural problems in a large sample of Dunedin

1484

boys: a longitudinal study from five to eleven years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 27:597-610 McGee R, Williams S, Silva PA (1988c), Slow starters and long-term backward readers: a replication and extension. Br J Educ Psychol 58:330-337 Mofitt TE (1993), “Life-course-persistent” and “adolescent-limited” antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev 100:679-70 1 New Zealand Council for Research in Education (1981), The Burt Word Reading Test, 1981 New Zealand Revision. Wellington: Lithoprint Pennington BF, Groisser D, Welsh MC (1993), Contrasting cognitive deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder versus reading disability. Dev Psychol 29:511-523 Quay HC, Peterson DR (1987), Manualfor the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami Rutter M, Tizard J, Whitmore K (1970), Education, Health, and Behavior. London: Longman Schachar R, Rutter M, Smith A (1981), The characteristics of situationally and pervasively hyperactive children: implications for syndrome definition. J Child Psychol Psychiany 4:375-392 Semrud-Clikeman M, Biederman J, Sprich-Buckrninister S, Lehaman BK, Faraone SV, Norman D (1992), Comorbidity between ADDH and learning disability: a review and report in a clinically referred sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiaty 31:439-448 Share DL, McGee R, Silva PA (1989), I Q and reading progress: a test of the capacity notion of IQ. J A m Acad ChildAdolescPsychiatry 28:97-100 Silva PA (1990), The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study: a fifteen year longitudinal study. Paediatr Perimt Epidzmiol 4:96-127 Waldie K, Spreen 0 (1993), The relationship between learning disabilities and persisting delinquency. J Learn Disaabil26:417-423 Williams S, Anderson J, McGee R, Silva PA (1990), Risk factors for behavioral and emotional disorder in preadolescent children. J A m Acad Child Adoksc Psychiatry 29:413-419 Williams S , McGee R (1994), Reading attainment and juvenile delinquency. J Child Psychol Psychiany 35:441-459

J . AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 35:11, NOVEMBER 1996