Accepted Manuscript Caffeine increases food intake while reducing anxiety-related behaviors Patrick Sweeney, Russell Levack, Jared Watters, Zhenping Xu, Yunlei Yang PII:
S0195-6663(16)30102-7
DOI:
10.1016/j.appet.2016.03.013
Reference:
APPET 2922
To appear in:
Appetite
Received Date: 12 January 2016 Revised Date:
8 March 2016
Accepted Date: 9 March 2016
Please cite this article as: Sweeney P., Levack R., Watters J., Xu Z. & Yang Y., Caffeine increases food intake while reducing anxiety-related behaviors, Appetite (2016), doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.03.013. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 2 3
5 6 7
RI PT
4
Caffeine increases food intake while reducing anxietyrelated behaviors
SC
8 9
Patrick Sweeney1, Russell Levack1, Jared Watters1, Zhenping Xu, and Yunlei Yang*
M AN U
10 11 12 13 14
Department of Neuroscience and Physiology, State University of New York Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, NY 13210, USA
1 These authors contributed equally
EP
* Correspondence: Yunlei Yang Institute For Human Performance, NRB#3609, 505 Irving Ave, New York, NY 13210, USA
[email protected] Phone: 315-464-7733 Fax: 315-464-7712
AC C
16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31
TE D
15
32
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ABSTRACT
34
The objective of this study was to determine the effects of different doses of caffeine on
35
appetite and anxiety-related behavior. Additionally, we sought to determine if withdrawal
36
from chronic caffeine administration promotes anxiety. In this study, we utilized rodent
37
open field testing and feeding behavior assays to determine the effects of caffeine on feeding
38
and anxiety-related behavior (n=8 mice; 4-8 weeks old). We also measured 2 hour and 24
39
hour food intake and body-weight during daily administration of caffeine (n=12 mice; 4-8
40
weeks old). To test for caffeine withdrawal induced anxiety, anxiety-related behavior in
41
rodents was quantified following withdrawal from four consecutive days of caffeine
42
administration (n=12 mice; 4-8 weeks old). We find that acute caffeine administration
43
increases food intake in a dose-dependent manner with lower doses of caffeine more
44
significantly increasing food intake than higher doses. Acute caffeine administration also
45
reduced anxiety-related behaviors in mice without significantly altering locomotor activity.
46
However, we did not observe any differences in 24 hour food intake or body weight
47
following chronic caffeine administration and there were no observable differences in
48
anxiety-related behaviors during caffeine withdrawal. In conclusion, we find that caffeine
49
can both increase appetite and decrease anxiety-related behaviors in a dose dependent
50
fashion. Given the complex relationship between appetite and anxiety, the present study
51
provides additional insights into potential caffeine-based pharmacological mechanisms
52
governing appetite and anxiety disorders, such as bulimia nervosa.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
33
53 54 55
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1. Introduction
57
The most broadly consumed behaviorally active substance worldwide is caffeine (Fredholm et
58
al., 1999). Americans consume a daily average of 165 mg of caffeine from beverages alone
59
(Mitchell et al., 2014). Additionally, it has been reported that 85% of Americans will consume a
60
minimum of one caffeinated drink per day (Mitchell et al., 2014). Given that 34.9% of
61
Americans over 20 years of age are considered obese, it is paramount to address the possible
62
effects of caffeine on food intake (Ogden et al., 2014). Interestingly, the effects of caffeine on
63
food intake are controversial. For instance, a study by Retzbach et al. (2014) showed that
64
caffeine increases lever pressing for sucrose, suggesting that caffeine can increase appetitive
65
behavior and feeding. This increase in lever pressing was accompanied by an increase in c-fos
66
expression in the nucleus accumbens, a critical brain region for reward (Berridge &
67
Kringelbach., M.L., 2015) and hedonic control of feeding behavior (O’Conner et al., 2015;
68
Castro & Berridge., 2014). Furthermore, a study by Bonaventura et al. (2012) showed that both
69
high-palatable and low-palatable food intake decreased in rats when A2A adenosine receptors
70
were activated using A2A agonists. Since caffeine exerts its effect by blocking A2A receptors
71
(Ribeiro & Sebastiao, 2010), this finding suggests that caffeine can stimulate food intake.
72
Conversely, Pettenuzzo et al. (2008) found that chronic caffeine administration in rats does not
73
affect the consumption of rat chow, but decreases palatable food intake. Other studies have
74
shown that caffeine has no effect on appetite sensations or energy intake, while at the same time
75
it has been shown that caffeine decreases both consumption of powdered food and body weight
76
in rats (Schubert et al., 2014; Gavrieli et al., 2011; Racotta et al., 1994). A similar effect has been
77
observed in humans where caffeine was shown to intensify the anti-appetitive effects of nicotine
78
(Jessen et al., 2005). In addition to these findings, Gavreli et al. (2013) showed that consumption
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
56
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
79
of caffeinated coffee reduced energy intake in obese individuals. These disparate findings may
80
be explained by the doses of caffeine administered and/or the routes of caffeine administration
81
(injection vs. oral consumption). The neural mechanisms for caffeine's effects on feeding are incompletely understood.
83
However, caffeine has been shown to increase excitatory synaptic input to agouti-related peptide
84
(AgRP) neurons located in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus (Yang et al., 2011). When
85
activated, AgRP neurons increase feeding behavior by driving consummatory and appetitive
86
behaviors (Aponte et al., 2011; Sternson and Atasoy, 2014). Additionally, caffeine can both
87
increase dopamine levels and activate dopamine neurons in the nucleus accumbens, a brain
88
region critical for motivation and feeding behaviors (Solinas et al., 2002; Retzbach et al., 2014;
89
O’Connor et al, 2015). Taken together, emerging evidence suggests that caffeine increases
90
feeding behavior, at least in part, by increasing the activity of appetite promoting AGRP neurons
91
in the arcuate nucleus and dopamine neurons in the nucleus accumbens. We therefore proposed
92
that acute administration of caffeine would increase food intake, depending on the dose of
93
caffeine administered.
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
82
Beyond caffeine’s effects on feeding, recent reports have implicated caffeine in anxiety-
95
related behaviors. For example, Maximino et al. (2011) showed that caffeine can induce an
96
anxiety-like behavioral response in zebrafish in a dose-dependent manner by blocking A1
97
receptors. However, a recent report showed that caffeine can have anxiolytic effects in rats in the
98
open field test and elevated plus maze (Hughes et al., 2014). Consistently, caffeine
99
administration can exert anxiolytic or anxiogenic effects in rats depending on the anxiety test
100
employed, the rat strain, and the sex of the rat (Hughes and Hancock, 2016). Caffeine has also
101
been reported to have anti-depressive like qualities. For example, a recent study demonstrated
AC C
EP
94
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
that chronic pre-treatment with caffeine prior to social defeat stress in mice can reduce the
103
subsequent development of depressive and anxiogenic like phenotypes (Yin et al., 2015). This
104
effect was prevented when caffeine was co-administered with a selective dopamine D1 receptor
105
antagonist, indicating that caffeine reduces stress induced anxiety and depressive phenotypes via
106
dopaminergic signaling. By contrast, caffeine withdrawal is known to cause a large number of
107
behavioral effects including: irritability, sleepiness, nervousness, restlessness, and anxiety (Dews
108
et al., 2002).
SC
RI PT
102
Due to the known interactions between anxiety-related behaviors and feeding, the current
110
study investigated how both acute and chronic caffeine administration affects feeding and
111
anxiety in AgRP-IRES-Cre transgenic mouse, a common transgenic animal for feeding studies
112
(Maniam and Morris, 2012; Hardaway et al., 2015). Based on the known reciprocal interaction
113
between feeding and anxiety behavior (Maniam and Morris, 2012; Hardaway et al., 2015), we
114
hypothesized that low doses of caffeine would exert an anxiolytic effect and increase feeding
115
while high doses would have an anxiogenic effect and decrease feeding. To elucidate the effects
116
of caffeine on feeding behavior and anxiety, we measured free access food intake and open field
117
exploratory behavior following acute caffeine administration. We also tested for caffeine
118
withdrawal induced anxiety by performing open field behavioral testing following withdrawal
119
from chronic caffeine administration.
TE D
EP
AC C
120
M AN U
109
121
2. Methods
122
All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of
123
State University of New York Upstate Medical University, according to US National Institute of
124
Health’s Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. AgRP-IRES-Cre transgenic mice
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
were obtained from the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME, USA). These mice do not have
126
any known phenotypes when Cre recombinase is not active, as used in this study. AgRP-IRES-
127
Cre mice were used to maintain consistency with potential future studies which will use Cre-loxP
128
recombination strategies to manipulate hypothalamic AgRP neurons to determine their role in
129
caffeine induced feeding behavior. Male and female mice between the ages of 4 and 8 weeks
130
were used for all experiments. Mice were maintained on a 12 hour light/dark cycle (light onset at
131
6:30 am) and housed in ventilated cages with free access to water and standard rodent chow
132
(LabDiet: 5008 Formulab Diet). All behavioral experiments were conducted in the home cage
133
unless otherwise noted.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
125
134
2.1 Feeding Behavior Assays and Caffeine treatment
136
Prior to feeding behavior experiments, mice were single caged and habituated for one week.
137
Animals were handled daily for the week prior to beginning experiments to habituate them to
138
behavioral assays. All mice were handled by picking them up by their scruff multiple times to
139
mimic handling conditions seen during i.p. injections. Handling was performed to reduce stress
140
responses in response to i.p. injections. Food intake was measured at the indicated time-points by
141
briefly removing the food from the hopper and obtaining its weight. For the acute caffeine intake
142
experiment, 8 mice were used and received the same treatment. Intraperitoneal (i.p.) injections
143
were performed daily at 9:00 am for 8 days. Mice received saline injections (0.9% saline; 200 µl)
144
on days 1-3. Saline injections were performed to obtain baseline food intake measurements prior
145
to caffeine administration. On days 4-8, the mice received an injection of caffeine (Tocris
146
Bioscience, Bristol, UK) every other day with increasing doses (6 mg/kg, 12 mg/kg, and 24
147
mg/kg; dissolved in 200 µl 0.9% saline) and received saline injections on the days where
AC C
EP
TE D
135
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
caffeine was not administered. Caffeine injections were administered every other day to prevent
149
habituation to caffeine treatment. Food was replaced daily with approximately 20 grams of fresh
150
standard chow. Food intake was measured at various time points following injections: 30
151
minutes, 1 hour, 2 hours, and 5 hours. Average food intake and body weight were calculated for
152
each mouse.
RI PT
148
For chronic caffeine administration, 12 mice were used and were separated into two equal
154
groups. Animals were assigned to control (saline injections) or experimental groups (caffeine
155
injections). Injections were administered at 1:45 pm and 2:15 pm for all 10 days. On days 1-2,
156
both of the groups received saline injections at each of the two indicated time-points. Saline
157
injections were administered to obtain a baseline measure of food intake prior to treatment. On
158
days 3-6, the experimental group received a saline injection followed by a caffeine injection (20
159
mg/kg in 200 µl 0.9% saline), while the control received two saline injections. Both groups
160
received two saline injections on days 7-10. Saline was administered on days 7-10 to maintain
161
consistency in injection conditions while testing for behavioral symptoms of caffeine
162
withdrawal. Food was replaced daily with approximately 15 grams of fresh standard chow. Body
163
weight was measured immediately following the first injection each day. Food intake was
164
measured at various time points following the second injection: 30 minutes, 1 hour, 2 hours, and
165
24 hours. Average food intake and body weight were calculated for each mouse.
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
166
SC
153
167
2.2 Open Field Test
168
To measure the potential effects of caffeine on anxiety-related behaviors, we performed an open
169
field exploration task (Fig. 2). In this task mice were allowed to explore a brightly lit open field
170
for 10 minutes. The bright, exposed center of the field is classically considered to be a less
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
anxious environment than the corner of the arena. As such, anxious mice generally enter the
172
center less frequently and spend less time in this region (Hall and Ballenchy, 1932; Calhoon and
173
Tye, 2015). In this task mice were habituated to the behavioral room for 30 minutes prior to
174
beginning the experimental sessions. The open field room contained a brightly lit, 500 mm2
175
arena. The center of the arena was considered to be the middle 250 mm2 of the arena.
176
Exploratory behavior was recorded and analyzed for 10 minutes using ANY-maze software
177
(Stoelting). Between each trial, the arena was cleaned with a 70% ethanol solution. The total
178
distance traveled, average speed, distance traveled in center, time spent in center, number of
179
entries into the center, percent distance traveled in center, distance traveled in periphery, and
180
time spent in periphery were calculated for each mouse. The percent distance traveled in the
181
center area was determined by dividing the distance traveled in the center by the total distance
182
traveled. For the acute caffeine intake experiments, open field testing was run one week after
183
feeding behavior experiments. On the day of open field testing, caffeine (20 mg/kg) or saline was
184
administered via i.p. injection 10 minutes prior to running the open field test. Experiments were
185
performed in a counterbalanced order, where half of the mice received saline on day one and
186
caffeine on day two and the other half received caffeine on day one and saline on day two. Each
187
animal experienced two trials in the open field (caffeine on day 1 & saline on day 2 or saline on
188
day 1 & caffeine on day 2). Average values for saline and caffeine treatments were calculated by
189
averaging day 1 and day 2 values for each treatment condition. For the chronic caffeine intake
190
experiments, open field testing was conducted on three consecutive days following withdrawal
191
from caffeine administration in animals injected with either caffeine or vehicle saline. Relative
192
distance traveled in center was calculated on each day for each mouse as a measure of anxiety
193
from caffeine withdrawal.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
171
194 8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
3. Data Analysis
196
All data were analyzed using GraphPad Prism 6.0 software. For acute feeding and anxiety
197
experiments, average food intake and anxiety measurements were calculated for each mouse for
198
statistical analysis. Feeding behavior experiments were repeated at least twice per mouse for
199
each condition (saline or caffeine) and average food intake was calculated for each mouse for
200
statistical analysis. For acute open field anxiety experiments, each animal experienced two trials
201
in the open field (caffeine on day 1 & saline on day 2 or saline on day 1 & caffeine on day 2).
202
Average values for saline and caffeine treatments were calculating for statistical analysis by
203
averaging day 1 and day 2 values for each treatment condition. For caffeine withdrawal
204
experiments, average food intake or anxiety measurement were obtained for statistical analysis
205
by averaging food intake or anxiety measurements on each individual day for all of the mice in
206
each treatment group (saline vs caffeine withdrawl). 1-way ANOVAs with Tukey’s post hoc test
207
were performed to analyze acute caffeine feeding behavior experiments. Open field data were
208
evaluated with paired t-test. For chronic caffeine feeding experiments, 2-way ANOVAs with
209
Holm-Sidak post hoc testing were performed.
210
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
195
4. Results
212
4.1 Acute caffeine administration markedly increases food intake
213
Both 6 mg/kg and 12 mg/kg doses of caffeine significantly increased food intake after two hours
214
(Fig. 1A; n = 8 mice per group; F (3, 52) = 9.2; p < 0.0001). However, no significant effect of
215
caffeine administration was observed following 24 mg/kg caffeine injections. Five hours
216
following caffeine injections, only 6 mg/kg caffeine significantly increased food intake (Fig. 1B;
217
n = 8 per group; F (3, 52) = 5.8; p = 0.001). Importantly, caffeine administration did not
AC C
211
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
significantly affect animals’ locomotor activities (Fig 2A and B; paired Student’s t-test),
219
although a trend towards increasing locomotion was observed with caffeine administration.
220
Taken together, low doses of caffeine treatment increased food intake with less of an effect
221
observed with progressively higher doses.
RI PT
218
222
4.2 Acute caffeine administration decreases anxiety-related behaviors
224
Acute administration of caffeine (i.p.; 20 mg/kg in 200 µl 0.9 % saline) increased entries into the
225
center (Fig. 2C; paired Student’s t-test; t(7) = 4.09; p = 0.004 ), time in the center (Fig. 2D;
226
paired Student’s t-test; t(7) = 4.84; p = 0.0019), and the distance traveled in the center (Fig. 2E;
227
paired Student’s t-test; t(7) = 4.20; p = 0.004), suggesting that caffeine administration reduces
228
anxiety. In contrast, caffeine administration decreased the time mice spent in the less anxiogenic
229
corner of the field (Fig. 2F; paired Student’s t-test; t(7) = 4.85; p = 0.002). No significant
230
difference was detected for the distance traveled in the corner in response to caffeine
231
administration (Fig. 2G).
M AN U
TE D
232
SC
223
4.3 Chronic caffeine administration does not alter daily food intake or body weight
234
Caffeine administration did not significantly affect 24 hour food intake and body weight (Fig. 3A
235
and B). However, consistent with the trends shown in the Fig. 1, two hour food intake was
236
increased following caffeine administration (Fig. 3C; F (6, 70) = 3.09; p = 0.01). As withdrawal
237
from caffeine use has been associated with anxiety, we performed open field tests on three
238
consecutive days following chronic administration of caffeine or saline (Dews et al., 2002). We
239
did not find any significant changes in anxiety during withdrawal days in animals treated with
AC C
EP
233
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
240
saline vs. caffeine (Fig. 3D; unpaired Student’s t-test), suggesting that withdrawal from once
241
daily caffeine administration is not strongly anxiogenic.
242
5. Discussion
244
In this study, we find that caffeine increases food intake in a dose-dependent manner. For
245
instance, we find that the lower dose of caffeine markedly increased food intake while the extent
246
of the increase was reduced with higher doses of caffeine (Fig. 1).
SC
RI PT
243
The increase in food intake by caffeine can last at least 5 hours, which may be due to the
248
activation of AgRP neurons localized in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus (ARC) since previous
249
work has shown that caffeine increases the synaptic strength at AgRP neurons (Yang et al.,
250
2011). It is well demonstrated that activation of AgRP neurons increases food intake (Aponte et
251
al., 2011; Sternson and Atasoy, 2014). Interestingly, the increase in food intake by activation of
252
AgRP neurons can happen over a delayed period via agouti-related peptide action on
253
melanocortin receptors instead of the fast inhibitory GABA transmitter (Krashes et al., 2013),
254
supporting our results that food intake wasn’t significantly increased until two hours after
255
caffeine injections. Additional mechanisms are possible for caffeine induced increases in food
256
intake, such as caffeine acting on nucleus accumbens neurons to facilitate feeding (Retzbach et
257
al., 2014). Therefore, we postulate that caffeine acts on both AgRP neurons and nucleus
258
accumbens neurons through adenosine receptors to stimulate hunger and increase food intake.
259
Future studies will dissect the neural mechanisms underlying caffeine’s effect on feeding.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
247
260
Since acute administration of caffeine increased feeding, we sought to determine if long
261
term caffeine administration can increase daily food intake and body weight. Surprisingly, daily
262
body weight and food intake over the course of 24 hours were not significantly different between
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
the caffeine and control vehicle saline-treated groups. A possible explanation for this is the short
264
half-life of caffeine as caffeine has been reported to be completely metabolized in mice after 4-5
265
hours (Hartmann and Czok, 1980). Therefore, after 24 hours, caffeine is likely to be no longer
266
active in the brain. Secondly, as caffeine’s effects are short- lived, counter-regulatory neural
267
circuits are likely to be involved in maintaining a stable level of food intake and body weight, a
268
process that has been shown to be exceedingly complex and highly regulated (Williams and
269
Elmquist, 2012). Another explanation for why our results conflict with other groups’ is that the
270
present findings were conducted during the light cycle. It is possible that caffeine treatment has
271
different effects on feeding and anxiety depending on endogenous adenosine levels, which are
272
naturally low when the animal is awake during the dark period and higher during the light
273
period. As adenosine levels are known to naturally fluctuate during the day, and since caffeine is
274
an adenosine antagonist, future work could be conducted to investigate the potential differences
275
of caffeine treatment during various times in the day (Huang et al., 2011).
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
263
The literature regarding the effects of caffeine on anxiety is controversial. Hughes et al.
277
(2014) showed that caffeine reduces stress in rats. Contrary to this, an anxiogenic effect of
278
caffeine has been observed in rats (Bhattakarya et al., 1997). It has been suggested that
279
increasing concentrations of caffeine can cause different effects by acting through different
280
receptors, as higher doses of caffeine increase anxiety in humans and lower doses decrease
281
anxiety (Cunha et al., 2008).
AC C
282
EP
276
Our results show that caffeine administration reduces anxiety levels in mice during open
283
field exploration (Fig. 2C-F). This is consistent with similar testing performed on rats (Hughes et
284
al., 2014). However, the open field test also showed that caffeine withdrawal had no significant
285
effects on various measures of anxiety (Fig. 3D). Given that anxiety is a symptom of caffeine
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
withdrawal, we expected to see an increase in anxiety during caffeine withdrawal (Griffiths and
287
Woodson, 1988). However, since we did not see an increase in anxiety, it is possible that once
288
daily caffeine administration is not sufficient to produce subsequent withdrawal symptoms.
289
Furthermore, previous work showed that consumption of caffeine below doses of 65 mg/kg/day
290
had no effect on the locomotor activity of rats during their withdrawal period (Finn and
291
Holtzman, 1986). Therefore, we postulate that the amount of caffeine injected into our mice may
292
have been too small to produce observable, withdrawal induced anxiety. Furthermore, based on
293
the literature and our current study, caffeine can induce both anxiogenic and anxiolytic effects
294
depending on the dose of caffeine administered.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
286
295
6. Conclusion
297
Overall, our present findings show that caffeine increases appetite in mice. Additionally, our
298
results show that at a dose of 20 mg/kg, caffeine displays an anxiolytic effect. This finding is in
299
accord with the idea that high doses of caffeine exert an anxiogenic effect while low doses exert
300
an anxiolytic effect (Cunha et al., 2008). We also observed that caffeine withdrawal had no effect
301
on the anxiety levels of mice. This is likely due to the dosage of caffeine administered and/or the
302
frequency of caffeine administration. Further testing is needed to determine the precise dose and
303
frequency of administration required to produce caffeine withdrawal symptoms.
EP
AC C
304
TE D
296
Given the high usage of caffeine worldwide, it is important to precisely understand how
305
this compound affects relevant behaviors. Our findings suggest that in addition to caffeine’s well
306
known effects on wakefulness, it may also exert additional effects on appetite and anxiety.
307
Further work is needed to determine the neural correlates and potential targets for caffeine’s
308
wide ranging effects. These targets may reveal additional insights or potential therapeutic
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
309
strategies for the many neurological disorders characterized by abnormalities in sleep, feeding or
310
anxiety.
311
Conflict of interest
313
The authors declare that there is no financial interest related to this work.
RI PT
312
314
Author Contributions
316
Y.Y. conceived the idea. P.S., Y.Y. designed the experiments; P.S., R.L., J.W., Z.X. performed
317
the experiments analyzed the data; P.S., R.L., J.W., and Y.Y. wrote the paper with inputs from
318
all other contributors.
M AN U
SC
315
319
Acknowledgements
321
We thank R. Quinn for mouse husbandry, and thank all the members of Yang laboratory for the
322
critical comments on this manuscript. This research was supported by the State University of
323
New York.
EP
324
TE D
320
References
326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338
Aponte, Y., Atasoy, D., Sternson, S. M. (2011). AGRP neurons are sufficient to orchestrate feeding behavior rapidly and without training. Nature Neuroscience, 14(3), 351-355.
AC C
325
Atasoy, D., Betley, J. N., Su, H. H., Sternson, S. M. (2012). Deconstruction of a neural circuit for hunger. Nature, 488(7410), 172-177. Bhattacharya, S. K., Satyan, K. S., & Chakrabarti, A. (1997). Anxiogenic action of caffeine: an experimental study in rats. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England), 11(3), 219. Beer, B., Chasin, M., Clody, D. E., Vogel, J. R., & Horovitz, Z. P. (1972). Cyclic Adenosine Monophosphate Phosphodiesterase in Brain: Effect on Anxiety. Science, (4033), 428430. 14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Berridge, K., & Kringelbach, M. (2015). Pleasure Systems in the Brain. Neuron, 86(3), 646-664.
RI PT
Bonaventura, M. D., Cifani, C., Lambertucci, C., Volpini, R., Cristalli, G., & Massi, M. (2012). A2A adenosine receptor agonists reduce both high-palatability and low-palatability food intake in female rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 23(5-6), 567-574. Calhoon, G & Tye, K.M., (2015). Resolving the neural circuits of anxiety. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 1394-1404. Castro, D. C., & Berridge, K. C. (2014). Advances in the neurobiological bases for food ‘liking’ versus ‘wanting’. Physiology & Behavior, 136, 22-30.
M AN U
SC
Cunha, R. A., Ferré, S., Vaugeois, J., & Chen, J. (2008). Potential therapeutic interest of adenosine A2A receptors in psychiatric disorders. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 14(15), 1512-1524. Dews, P. B., O'Brien, C. P., & Bergman, J. (2002). Caffeine: behavioral effects of withdrawal and related issues. Food and Chemical Toxicology, 40, 1257-1261. El Yacoubi, M., Ledent, C., Parmentier, M., Costentin, J., & Vaugeois, J. (2005). Reduced appetite for caffeine in adenosine A2A receptor knockout mice. European Journal of Pharmacology, 519(3), 290-291.
TE D
Finn, I. B., & Holtzman, S. G. (1986). Tolerance to caffeine-induced stimulation of locomotor activity in rats. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 238(2), 542546.
EP
Fredholm, B. B., Bättig, K., Holmén, J., Nehlig, A., & Zvartau, E. E. (1999). Actions of caffeine in the brain with special reference to factors that contribute to its widespread use. Pharmacological Reviews, 51(1), 83-133. Gavrieli, A., Karfopoulou, E., Kardatou, E., Spyreli, E., Fragopoulou, E., Mantzoros, C., & Yannakoulia, M. (2013). Effect of different amounts of coffee on dietary intake and appetite of normal weight and overweight/obese individuals. Obesity, 21(6), 1127-1132.
AC C
339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385
Gavrieli A, Yannakoulia M, Fragopoulou E, Margaritopoulos D, Chamberland JP, Kaisari P, Kavouras SA, Mantzoros CS. (2011) Caffeinated coffee does not acutely affect energy intake, appetite, or inflammation but prevents serum cortisol concentrations from falling in healthy men. The Journal of Nutrition 141: 703–707 Griffiths, R. R., & Woodson, P. P. (1988). Caffeine physical dependence: a review of human and laboratory animal studies. Psychopharmacology, 94(4), 437-451. Hall, C. & Ballachey, E.L. (1932). A study of the rat’s behavior in a field. A contribution to method in comparative psychology. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psychol. 6, 1–12
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Hardaway, J. A., N. A. Crowley, C. M. Bulik and T. L. Kash (2015). Integrated circuits and molecular components for stress and feeding: implications for eating disorders. Genes Brain Behav 14(1): 85-97.
RI PT
Hartmann, M., & Czok, G. (1980). Pharmacokinetics of caffeine in mice and its modification by ethanol. Zeitschrift Für Ernährungswissenschaft, 19(3), 215-227. Huang, Z. L., Urade, Y., & Hayaishi, O. (2011). The role of adenosine in the regulation of sleep. Current Topics in Medicinal Chemistry, 11(8), 1047-1057.
SC
Hughes, R. N., & Hancock, N. J. (2016). Strain-dependent effects of acute caffeine on anxiety related behavior in PVG/c, Long–Evans and Wistar rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 140, 51-61.
M AN U
Hughes, R. N., Hancock, N. J., Henwood, G. A., & Rapley, S. A. (2014). Research report: Evidence for anxiolytic effects of acute caffeine on anxiety-related behavior in male and female rats tested with and without bright light. Behavioural Brain Research, 271, 7-15. Jessen, A., Buemann, B., Toubro, S., Skovgaard, I. M., & Astrup, A. (2005). The appetitesuppressant effect of nicotine is enhanced by caffeine. Diabetes, Obesity & Metabolism, 7(4), 327-333.
TE D
Krashes MJ, Shah BP, Koda S, Lowell BB (2013). Rapid versus delayed stimulation of feeding by the endogenously released AgRP neuron mediators, GABA, NPY and AgRP. Cell metabolism. 18(4), 588-595. Maniam, J. and M. J. Morris (2012). The link between stress and feeding behaviour. Neuropharmacology. 63(1), 97-110.
EP
Maximino, C., Lima, M. G., Olivera, K. R. M., Picanço-Diniz, D.,L.W., & Herculano, A. M. (2011). Adenosine A1, but not A2, receptor blockade increases anxiety and arousal in Zebrafish. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology, 109(3), 203-207. Mitchell, D. C., Knight, C. A., Hockenberry, J., Teplansky, R., & Hartman, T. J. (2014). Beverage caffeine intakes in the U.S. Food and Chemical Toxicology, 63, 136-142.
AC C
386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432
O’Connor, E. C., Kremer, Y., Lefort, S., Harada, M., Pascoli, V., Rohner, C., & Lüscher, C. (2015). Accumbal D1R Neurons Projecting to Lateral Hypothalamus Authorize Feeding. Neuron, 88, 553-564. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. (2014). Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the united states, 2011-2012. Jama, 311(8), 806-814. Pettenuzzo, L. F., Noschang, C., von Pozzer Toigo, E., Fachin, A., Vendite, D., & Dalmaz, C. (2008). Effects of chronic administration of caffeine and stress on feeding behavior of rats. Physiology & Behavior,95(3), 295-301
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Quinlan, P., Lane, J., & Aspinall, L. (1997). Effects of hot tea, coffee and water ingestion on physiological responses and mood: the role of caffeine, water and beverage type. Psychopharmacology, 134(2), 164-173.
RI PT
Racotta, I. S., Leblanc, J., & Richard, D. (1994). The effect of caffeine on food intake in rats: involvement of corticotropin-releasing factor and the sympatho-adrenal system. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 48(4), 887-892. Retzbach, E. P., Dholakia, P. H., & Duncan-Vaidya, E. (2014). The effect of daily caffeine exposure on lever-pressing for sucrose and c-fos expression in the nucleus accumbens in the rat. Physiology & Behavior, 135, 1-6.
SC
Ribeiro, J.A., & Sebastiao, A.M. (2010). Caffeine and adenosine. J Alzhermers Dis. 20 Suppl 1, S3-15.
M AN U
Rice, A. M., Fain, J. N., & Rivkees, S. A. (2000). A1 adenosine receptor activation increases adipocyte leptin secretion. Endocrinology, 141(4), 1442-1445. Schubert, M. M., Grant, G., Horner, K., King, N., Leveritt, M., Sabapathy, S., Desbrow, B. (2014). Coffee for morning hunger pangs. An examination of coffee and caffeine on appetite, gastric emptying, and energy intake. Appetite, 83, 317-326.
TE D
Solinas, M., Ferre, S., You, Z. B., Karcz-Kubicha, M., Popoli, P., & Goldberg, S. R. (2002). Caffeine induces dopamine and glutamate release in the shell of the nucleus accumbens. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 22(15), 6321-6324. Sternson, S.M., Atasoy, D. (2014). Agouti-related protein neuron circuits that regulate appetite. Neuroendrocrinology, 2-3(100), 95-102.
EP
Williams, K. W. and J. K. Elmquist. (2012). From neuroanatomy to behavior: central integration of peripheral signals regulating feeding behavior. Nat Neurosci 15(10), 1350-1355. Yang, Y., Atasoy, D., Su, H., & Sternson, S. (2011). Hunger States Switch a Flip-Flop Memory Circuit via a Synaptic AMPK-Dependent Positive Feedback Loop. Cell, 146(6), 9921003.
AC C
433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472
Yin, Y., Zhang, C., Wang, J., Hou, J., Yang, X., & Qin, J. (2015). Chronic caffeine treatment enhances the resilience to social defeat stress in mice. Food & Function, 6(2), 479-491.
473 474
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
n.s.
A
**
S a lin e
***
6 m g /k g C a ffe in e 1 2 m g /k g C a ffe in e
RI PT
0.4
SC
0.2
2 4 m g /k g C a ffe in e
M AN U
2 hr food intake (g)
0.6
0.0
n.s.
n.s.
0.8
0.4
EP
***
S a lin e 6 m g /k g C a ffe in e 1 2 m g /k g C a ffe in e 2 4 m g /k g C a ffe in e
AC C
5 hr food intake (g)
1.2
TE D
B
0.0
Sweeney et al., Figure 1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Figure 1: Acute caffeine administration facilitates feeding. Feeding behavior assays indicating that administration of caffeine increases feeding at both two and five hours following i.p. injections. Injections of caffeine (6 mg/kg-24 mg/kg; dissolved in 0.9% saline) or saline were administered and subsequent levels of food intake were calculated at ascending time-points, as shown. (A) Caffeine shows a dose-dependent effect on appetite with 6 mg/kg exerting the most significant orexigenic effects on feeding after two hours. Higher doses were less effective at increasing feeding although 12 mg/kg also significantly increased food intake. (B) Five hours post injection, only 6 mg/kg administration of caffeine significantly increased food intake. n=8 mice per group for all figures. Data represents mean +/- SEM. ** p<.01; ***p<0.001; ns (not significant).
20
Time in center (s)
**
80 60
40
SC
**
TE D
40
60
Caffeine
M AN U
80
20
20
Saline
Caffeine
**
400
200
0
Saline
EP
0
G
Distance travelled in corner (m)
Entries to center (#)
Saline
Caffeine
D
600 Time in corner (s)
0.05
0
100
F
0.1
0
0
n.s.
Caffeine
Saline
Caffeine
E
Distance travelled in center (m)
40
0.15
RI PT
Average speed (m/s)
60
Saline
C
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
B
n.s.
80
AC C
Total distance travelled (m)
A
80
** 60 40 20 0
Saline
Caffeine
n.s.
80 60 40 20 0
Saline
Caffeine
Sweeney et al., Figure 2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Figure 2: Acute caffeine administration decreases anxiety related behaviors. (A and B) Open field behavioral analysis indicates that caffeine administration (20 mg/kg) does not significantly affect total distance traveled (A) or average speed (B) in the open field. (C-G) Open field behavioral experiments showing that acute administration of caffeine (20 mg/kg) increases entries to the center (C), time in the center (D), and distance traveled in the center (E) of the open field. (F) Time in the corner of the open field is lower in mice injected with caffeine compared to mice injected with vehicle saline. (G) No significant differences were detected between saline and caffeine conditions in distance traveled in the corner of the open field. Caffeine or saline was administered 10 minutes prior to open field behavioral experiments. All experiments were repeated twice on consecutive days in counterbalanced order. n=8 mice per group; paired Student’s t-test; Data represents mean +/- SEM. **p<0.01; ns (not significant).
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Saline 20 mg/kg Caffeine Withdrawal
20 Body weight (g)
n.s. 4
4
5
6 7 Experimental Day
8
C 0.4
***
** 0.3
AC C
0.2
EP
*
TE D
3
0.1
0.0 3
4
5
6
Experimental Day
7
10
M AN U
2
2 hr food intake (g)
15
n.s.
SC
3
3
4
5
6 9 8 7 Experimental Day
10
D
Relative distance travelled in center (%)
24 hr food intake (g)
5
Saline 20 mg/kg Caffeine Withdrawal
B
RI PT
A
20
No withdrawal (Saline) Withdrawal (Caffeine)
15 10
5 0 7
9
8 Experimental Day
Sweeney et al., Figure 3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Figure 3: Chronic caffeine administration does not affect food intake or body weight. (A and B) Average 24 hour food intake and body weight during a long term feeding paradigm. Mice were administered i.p. injections of saline on days 1 and 2, either caffeine (20 mg/kg) or saline on days 3 to 6, and saline on days 7 through 10. No significant changes in daily food intake (A) or body weight (B) were detected during caffeine administration or during caffeine withdrawal (2-way ANOVA). (C) Daily food intake two hours following injections of saline or caffeine. Daily i.p. injections of caffeine increased feeding two hours following caffeine administration. (D) Open field behavioral assays during caffeine withdrawal. Open field experiments were conducted on three consecutive days during withdrawal from caffeine administration in animals previously injected with caffeine or vehicle saline. No significant differences in anxiety behavior were detected between caffeine and saline treated animals in percent distance traveled in the center of the open field on any of the caffeine withdrawal days. Data represents mean +/- SEM. n= 6 mice per group *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; ns (not significant).