Journal Pre-proof Can point-of-sale nutrition information and health warnings encourage reduced preference for sugary drinks?: An experimental study Maree Scully, Belinda Morley, Melanie Wakefield, Helen Dixon PII:
S0195-6663(19)31268-1
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104612
Reference:
APPET 104612
To appear in:
Appetite
Received Date: 2 October 2019 Revised Date:
13 January 2020
Accepted Date: 18 January 2020
Please cite this article as: Scully M., Morley B., Wakefield M. & Dixon H., Can point-of-sale nutrition information and health warnings encourage reduced preference for sugary drinks?: An experimental study, Appetite (2020), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104612. This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2020 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
1
1
Can point-of-sale nutrition information and health warnings
2
encourage reduced preference for sugary drinks?: an experimental
3
study
4
5
Maree Scully1, Belinda Morley1, Melanie Wakefield1,2, Helen Dixon1,2,3*
6 7 8 9 10
1
Centre for Behavioural Research in Cancer, Cancer Council Victoria, Melbourne, Australia.
2
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville,
Australia. 3
School of Psychology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin University, Bentley, Australia.
11 12
13 14
* Corresponding author:
15
Helen Dixon
16
Centre for Behavioural Research in Cancer
17
Cancer Council Victoria
18
615 St Kilda Road
19
Melbourne 3004, Victoria, Australia
20
Phone number: +61 3 9514 6480
21
Email:
[email protected]
22
2
23
Abstract
24
Point-of-sale (POS) interventions that prompt consumers to more critically evaluate sugary
25
drinks could encourage reduced consumption of these drinks and reinforce public health
26
campaign messages. This study tested whether: (i) POS nutrition information and health
27
warnings about sugary drinks promote healthier drink choices and (ii) impacts of prominent
28
POS signs on drink choices vary based on participants’ self-reported prior exposure to a
29
sugary drink public health campaign. In an online experiment, 3,034 Australian adults aged
30
18-59 years who were past-week sugary drink consumers were randomly assigned to one of
31
five POS signage conditions (no signage (control); sugar content of specific beverages;
32
Health Star Rating of specific beverages; generic text health warning about sugary drinks;
33
generic graphic health warning about sugary drinks) and shown their randomly assigned POS
34
sign alone, then alongside a drinks product display and asked to select which drink they
35
would choose to buy. The proportion selecting a sugary drink was significantly lower among
36
participants who viewed either the sugar content (29%), Health Star Rating (33%) or graphic
37
health warning (34%) signs compared to those who saw no sign (43%). These effects held for
38
participants who did not recall previously seeing the campaign; however, for participants
39
with self-reported prior exposure to the campaign, POS signs did not promote significant
40
reductions in sugary drink choices. POS signage has the potential to shift consumers away
41
from choosing sugary drinks and could complement mass media campaigns by reaching
42
people who may not otherwise be exposed to sugary drink public health messages.
43
Keywords: Sugar-sweetened beverages, adults, public health intervention, obesity
44
prevention, point-of-sale
45
Abbreviations: Point-of-sale (POS); front-of-pack (FOP); nutrition information panel (NIP);
46
socio-economic position (SEP); body mass index (BMI)
3
47
Introduction
48
Energy-dense, nutrient-poor sugary drinks (defined in this paper as non-alcoholic water based
49
beverages with added sugar including sugar-sweetened soft drinks, fruit drinks, energy
50
drinks, sports drinks, iced tea and flavoured water, but not milk-based products, 100% fruit
51
juice or non-sugar sweetened drinks) are of little nutritional value, with consumption of these
52
‘empty kilojoules’ increasing people’s risk of obesity and associated health problems.
53
Consumption of sugary drinks is associated with weight gain in both adults and children
54
(Malik, Pan, Willett, & Hu, 2013). Sugary drinks may promote weight gain through
55
stimulating appetite or suppressing satiety, or as a result of individuals not adequately
56
adjusting their overall food intake to compensate for the high amount of energy they consume
57
from these drinks (DiMeglio & Mattes, 2000; St-Onge et al., 2004; Vartanian, Schwartz, &
58
Brownell, 2007). Excess energy intake is important in the aetiology of obesity, with those
59
affected at increased risk of a range of chronic conditions (World Health Organization, 2000),
60
including 13 types of cancer (Lauby-Secretan et al., 2016). Sugary drinks are also
61
independently associated with increased cancer risk (Chazelas et al., 2019; Hodge, Bassett,
62
Milne, English, & Giles, 2018). Furthermore, they contain acid which weakens tooth enamel
63
and can lead to dental decay (Bernabe, Vehkalahti, Sheiham, Aromaa, & Suominen, 2014).
64
Regular consumption of sugary drinks predisposes people to exceeding recommended daily
65
energy intakes for free sugars. The World Health Organization (2015) recommends free
66
sugars should contribute less than 10% of total energy intake, with some evidence that a
67
further reduction to below 5% total energy intake can provide additional health benefits. In
68
Australia, where percentage daily intakes are based on an average adult diet of 8700
69
kilojoules (2080 calories), the upper limit for 10% intake of free sugars is <870 kilojoules
70
(<208 calories). However, many single servings of sugary drinks (e.g. a 600ml bottle of
4
71
Coca-Cola or 500ml can of Red Bull) contain in excess of this amount of energy from sugar,
72
placing regular consumers at higher risk of weight gain and dental caries. According to
73
figures from the 2017-18 National Health Survey, around 40% of Australian adults aged 18
74
to 64 years consume sugary drinks at least once per week, with this rate peaking at 68%
75
among young men (18-24 years) (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2018a). Of particular
76
concern, 10% of 18 to 64 year-old Australians usually consume sugary drinks on a daily basis
77
(Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2018a). Australasia, along with North America, Latin
78
America and Western Europe is among the four regions of the world with the highest sugary
79
drink consumption (Popkin & Hawkes, 2016). There is a pressing global need for public
80
health strategies that help people curb their consumption of sugary drinks.
81
A number of governments have already implemented public health initiatives to reduce
82
sugary drink consumption, such as taxation, limiting availability in schools, restricting
83
marketing to children, public awareness campaigns and positive and negative front-of-pack
84
(FOP) labelling schemes (Hawkes, Jewell, & Allen, 2013; Popkin & Hawkes, 2016; Roberto
85
et al., 2015). In Australia, where this study was conducted, there have been several
86
government and non-government-led obesity prevention campaigns that have recommended
87
reduced sugary drink consumption as well as other simple diet and lifestyle changes to reduce
88
energy intake and increase energy expenditure (Kite et al., 2018; Morley et al., 2019; O'Hara
89
et al., 2016). Public health mass media campaigns have the capacity to produce positive
90
changes or prevent negative changes in health-related behaviours across large populations
91
when they are well designed and of sufficient duration and intensity (Wakefield, Loken, &
92
Hornik, 2010). Following a mass media campaign (LiveLighter®) highlighting the negative
93
health effects of sugary drink consumption and being overweight in Western Australia, a
94
cohort study found a reduction in self-reported sugary drink consumption among heavy
95
consumers in the intervention state (Morley et al., 2019). A controlled cohort study found
5
96
evidence of reduced sugary drink consumption also among adult frequent consumers in
97
Victoria, Australia following implementation of the same campaign in that state (Morley et
98
al., 2018). The present study was conducted following the launch of a new graphic obesity
99
prevention campaign (13 Types of Cancer, herein referred to as 13 Cancers) in Victoria,
100
Australia, highlighting the link between sugary drink consumption, weight gain and increased
101
risk of 13 types of cancer that urged people to avoid sugary drinks. This television-led
102
campaign was complemented by digital, cinema, radio and outdoor advertising, and had a
103
primary target audience of adults aged 25 to 59 years. The present study was conducted
104
during the last three weeks of the five-week campaign, enabling us to assess whether self-
105
reported exposure to this campaign influenced consumers’ beliefs and preferences for sugary
106
drinks.
107
In addition to using mass media as a tool for obesity prevention (World Health Organization,
108
2000), leading public health experts recommend that legislation for consumer-friendly
109
nutrition labelling be part of comprehensive policy action to influence a cultural shift toward
110
healthier preferences (Hawkes et al., 2015). The provision of nutrition and health information
111
at the point-of-sale (POS) has the potential to positively change food and drink purchasing
112
behaviour by increasing awareness and knowledge about products (Liberato, Bailie, &
113
Brimblecombe, 2014). POS interventions have potential to reach consumers further along the
114
path to purchase than mass media campaigns, providing a timely, context-relevant prompt
115
that could complement other education efforts directed at reducing sugary drink consumption.
116
Many countries now have voluntary interpretive nutrition labelling schemes, and some
117
countries have laws requiring certain food and beverages to carry traffic light labels (Ecuador
118
and Sri Lanka) or health warning labels (Chilé and Mexico) (World Cancer Research Fund
119
International, 2016).
6
120
In Australia, FOP food labelling is regulated at a national level, while POS labelling is
121
regulated at a state level. Most packaged foods and beverages are required to display a
122
nutrition information panel (NIP) that usually appears on the back or side of the package in a
123
small font; there is no mandatory national FOP nutrition labelling scheme. However, a
124
voluntary FOP labelling scheme, the Health Star Rating (Commonwealth of Australia, 2016),
125
appears on some packaged products. The Health Star Rating rates the overall nutritional
126
profile of foods and assigns an interpretive rating from 1/2 to 5 stars (with more stars
127
indicating healthier products) to provide consumers with a tool for comparing the healthiness
128
of similar packaged products (MP Consulting, 2019). In terms of POS labelling, most states
129
in Australia now require fast food chains to declare the kilojoule content of standard products
130
on menus (Obesity Evidence Hub, 2019). Some other nutrition labelling schemes have also
131
been proposed, including warning labels for nutrients of concern and pictorial approaches to
132
labelling sugar content (Australian and New Zealand Ministerial Forum on Food Regulation,
133
2019; COAG Health Council, 2019).
134
A recent online choice experiment with young Australian adults found that FOP labels
135
identifying the nutritional quality (as per the Health Star Rating) or sugar content (teaspoons
136
of added sugar) of each drink alternative, or highlighting the health risks of sugary drinks (via
137
a graphic or text warning on these drinks) reduced intended sugary drink purchases, with the
138
strongest effect found for the graphic warning label (Billich et al., 2018). However, this
139
research focused on consumers within a relatively narrow age range and did not assess their
140
perceptions of sugary drinks in response to seeing the FOP labels, which could help us to
141
understand the mechanisms for behaviour change.
142
Other experiments testing effects of various FOP labelling schemes on preferences or
143
purchasing of sugary drinks have been conducted in New Zealand (Bollard, Maubach,
144
Walker, & Ni Mhurchu, 2016), the United Kingdom (Mantzari, Vasiljevic, Turney, Pilling, &
7
145
Marteau, 2018) and the United States (Grummon, Hall, Taillie, & Brewer, 2019; Grummon,
146
Taillie, et al., 2019; Roberto, Wong, Musicus, & Hammond, 2016; VanEpps & Roberto,
147
2016). All these studies were conducted online, except one conducted in a replica
148
convenience store (Grummon, Taillie, et al., 2019). Most of these studies had adult
149
participants, two focused on parents (Mantzari et al., 2018; Roberto et al., 2016), while some
150
involved teens (Bollard et al., 2016; VanEpps & Roberto, 2016). Across these studies, FOP
151
labels were found to influence perceptions of and discourage preference for sugary drinks.
152
However, some labelling schemes performed better than others. Studies that included graphic
153
health warnings among the labelling schemes tested found that these labels outperformed
154
text-only warnings (Billich et al., 2018; Bollard et al., 2016), messages about sugar content
155
(Billich et al., 2018; Mantzari et al., 2018), and the Health Star Rating (Billich et al., 2018)
156
respectively. However, such visually confronting, graphic labelling schemes may be more
157
difficult to implement in practice due to industry opposition (Popova, 2016; Swinburn et al.,
158
2019). Available research indicates that text-based health warnings are still effective in
159
prompting consumers to evaluate sugary drinks more critically, increase intentions to reduce
160
sugary drink consumption and reduce preferences for sugary drinks (Billich et al., 2018;
161
Bollard et al., 2016; Grummon, Hall, et al., 2019; Grummon, Taillie, et al., 2019; Roberto et
162
al., 2016; VanEpps & Roberto, 2016). Thus, text-based warning label schemes are well
163
justified and have been successfully mandated in Chilé and Mexico (World Cancer Research
164
Fund International, 2016), with evaluation results from Chilé indicating the scheme is being
165
well received by parents and children who report it positively influences their food choices
166
(Correa et al., 2019).
167
In Australia, where this research was conducted, progress on FOP food and drink labelling
168
has been historically slow (Swinburn & Wood, 2013), warranting investigation of alternative
169
methods for communicating nutrition information to consumers that may be implemented
8
170
more expediently than mandatory FOP labels. Two alternative channels for reaching
171
consumers with messaging about sugary drinks are POS signs displayed in food retail and
172
service settings, and mass media campaigns. The present study is concerned with both these
173
approaches.
174
Several previous studies have tested the effectiveness of POS signs in influencing consumer’s
175
drink choices. Two US studies tested effects of different POS signs about energy content on
176
teens’ drink purchases in a store-based intervention in low-income predominantly Black
177
neighbourhoods. The first study tested calories, percentage of total recommended daily intake
178
and physical activity equivalent signs respectively, finding that all conditions reduced sugary
179
drink purchases, but the POS sign about a physical activity equivalent (minutes of running)
180
was the most effective (Bleich, Herring, Flagg, & Gary-Webb, 2012). The second study
181
tested four different POS signs about the energy content in sugary drinks (calories; minutes of
182
running; miles of walking; sugar content) finding again that all conditions promoted a
183
reduction in sugary drink purchases (with effects on SSB purchases persisting six weeks after
184
signs were removed), but messages about miles of walking were the most effective (Bleich,
185
Barry, Gary-Webb, & Herring, 2014). Another US study testing effects of several POS signs
186
(text health warnings; graphic health warnings; calories) in hospital cafeterias found that only
187
the graphic health warnings affected purchases, with further testing revealing that graphic
188
health warnings heightened negative affect and prompted consideration of health
189
consequences of consuming sugary drinks (Donnelly, Zatz, Svirsky, & John, 2018). POS
190
signs conveying information about the amount of sugar in sugary drinks have been shown to
191
be effective in reducing selection of these types of drinks in a discrete choice experiment
192
conducted online with Australian adults (Blake, Lancsar, Peeters, & Backholer, 2018). To our
193
knowledge, no previous studies testing reactions to POS signs about sugary drinks have
9
194
compared how health warning signs perform relative to signs about sugar content or the
195
Health Star Rating scheme.
196
Prior to real-world implementation, it is crucial that POS nutrition messaging options
197
undergo careful pre-testing to ensure they are acceptable to the public and effective among
198
varying population groups under simulated conditions. Thus, the primary aim of the present
199
study is to examine the relative efficacy of various POS signs (teaspoons of sugar, Health
200
Star Rating, text-only or graphic health warnings)
201
accurately evaluate the nutritional content of drinks and make healthier drink choices in a
202
hypothetical shopping scenario. A secondary research question is whether impacts of such
203
POS signage on consumer drink choices differ based on self-reported prior exposure to the
204
recent 13 Cancers obesity prevention campaign which urged people to avoid sugary drinks.
205
The campaign was launched in the weeks prior to this study, enabling exploration of
206
complementary effects of a mass media campaign and a simulated POS signage intervention
207
on adults’ perceptions and preferences for sugary drinks.
in encouraging consumers to more
208
209
Method
210
Design and procedure
211
During the final three weeks of a five-week television-led graphic obesity prevention
212
campaign (13 Cancers) produced by the Cancer Council and broadcast only in the Australian
213
state of Victoria, a between-subjects national online experiment was conducted, with
214
participants randomly assigned to one of five POS signage conditions: (A) no signage
215
(control); (B) sugar content of specific beverages; (C) Health Star Rating of specific
216
beverages; (D) generic text health warning about sugary drinks; (E) generic graphic health
10
217
warning about sugary drinks. Screening questions were used to determine study eligibility
218
(i.e. aged 18-59 years, consumed a sugary drink in the past week, not currently living in
219
Western Australia). Eligible participants were exposed to their assigned POS sign alone, and
220
then prominently alongside a display of generic (i.e. unbranded) drink products and asked to
221
select which drink they would choose to buy for themselves. Following the drink choice task,
222
participants answered questions assessing their perceptions of various non-alcoholic drink
223
products and knowledge about sugary drinks, before completing ratings of their cognitive and
224
emotional responses to their assigned POS sign. Finally, all participants were shown the 13
225
Cancers video advertisement on screen to assess prompted recall in order to identify
226
participants who had previously been exposed to this campaign. Ethical approval for the
227
study was obtained from Cancer Council Victoria’s Institutional Research Review Committee
228
(IER 1704).
229
Participants
230
A sample of Australian adults aged 18 to 59 years was recruited from two non-probability
231
(i.e. opt-in) online panels managed by i-Link Research and Lightspeed. Panellists from both
232
panels receive points for completing surveys that can be accumulated and redeemed for
233
rewards such as shopping gift vouchers. After individually accessing the survey at a time of
234
their convenience and completing a series of basic demographic questions, participants were
235
asked “During the past 7 days, on how many days did you drink a can, bottle or glass of a
236
sugary drink such as soft drinks, energy drinks, fruit drinks, sports drinks and cordial? Do not
237
include artificially sweetened drinks. Please note that fruit drinks are those that do not contain
238
100% fruit juice.” Those who indicated ‘0 days’ (i.e. irregular or never consumers) were
239
excluded from participating. Age (18-29, 30-44, 45-59) and gender (male, female) quotas
240
were applied to achieve approximately equal numbers of these groups in each POS signage
241
condition. To help ensure there were sufficient numbers of participants who had seen the 13
11
242
Cancers video advertisement, half of the sample were recruited from Victoria (i.e. the state in
243
which the advertisement aired) and the remaining half were recruited from all other states and
244
territories except Western Australia (due to this state’s higher exposure to public health mass
245
media advertising about sugary drinks (Morley et al., 2019)). Based on power calculations
246
using results from a previous experimental study examining the effect of FOP labels on
247
adult’s drink selections (Billich et al., 2018), as well as campaign awareness figures from the
248
evaluation of the LiveLighter® ‘Sugary Drinks’ advertisement which ran in Victoria in 2016
249
(Morley et al., 2018), it was estimated that a sample size of 3,000 adults (i.e. n=600 per
250
condition) would allow for the detection of small effects in relation to both study aims.
251
Intervention
252
Full-colour digital POS signs representing each condition were designed by a graphic
253
designer, with existing health promotion materials used to inform their layout, imagery and
254
content (see Figure 1). The sugar content sign visibly displayed the number of teaspoons of
255
sugar found in seven popular types of non-alcoholic drinks: soft drinks, 100% fruit juice,
256
flavoured milk, flavoured water, energy drinks, sports drinks and water. The amount of sugar
257
for each drink type was determined by referring to nutritional information for key branded
258
products (whereby 4 grams of sugar was deemed equivalent to one teaspoon), and then
259
asking an Accredited Practicing Dietitian to review them for accuracy. The number of
260
teaspoons of sugar displayed for flavoured milk was subsequently adjusted to remove the
261
amount attributable to naturally occurring lactose (which is also present in plain milk). No
262
adjustment was made for 100% fruit juice, which also contains naturally occurring sugar,
263
given the somewhat mixed evidence regarding its association with chronic health conditions
264
(Auerbach, Dibey, Vallila-Buchman, Kratz, & Krieger, 2018; Chazelas et al., 2019).
12
265
The Health Star Rating sign showed the same seven types of drinks as the sugar content
266
sign, each accompanied by a Health Star Rating providing an overall rating of their
267
nutritional profile (per 100 ml) taking into account both risk (e.g. energy content, saturated
268
fat, sugar, sodium) and positive components (e.g. protein, fibre, calcium, fruit/vegetable
269
content) of the drink. The Health Star Rating is a government-led initiative that rates the
270
nutritional value of packaged foods from 1/2 to 5 stars, with more stars indicating a healthier
271
choice (Commonwealth of Australia, 2016). For each drink type, the rating displayed on the
272
sign was determined by inputting the nutritional information for key branded products into
273
the Health Star Rating online calculator (Australian Department of Health, 2014). Where the
274
rating varied between brands (i.e. ≥1/2 star difference), an Accredited Practicing Dietitian
275
was consulted to determine the most appropriate rating to use.
276
The text health warning sign reinforced one of the key messages that the Cancer Council
277
has communicated through the LiveLighter® ‘Sugary Drinks’ campaign and more recently the
278
13 Cancers campaign, namely that “Drinking sugary drinks can lead to toxic fat around your
279
vital organs”. The graphic health warning sign featured this same message with an image
280
underneath from the 13 Cancers video advertisement graphically depicting the build-up of
281
visceral fat around an internal organ. Neither of the health warning signs singled out
282
particular beverages.
283
Measures
284
The primary outcome was drink choice following exposure to the POS signage intervention
285
which was measured using a hypothetical shopping scenario developed by Billich and
286
colleagues (2018). Participants were asked to imagine they had gone to a convenience store
287
or take-away café or up to a vending machine with the intention of buying a pre-packaged
288
drink (in a bottle, can or carton) to drink immediately themselves. They were shown their
13
289
randomly assigned POS sign alongside a display of 15 drink products and asked to select
290
which drink they would choose, if any, in this situation (see Figure 2). Responses were
291
categorised into sugary drinks (regular soft drink (cola and non-cola), fruit drink (with added
292
sugar), iced tea, sports drink, energy drink, flavoured water), diet drinks (diet soft drink (cola
293
and non-cola), diet energy drink), high (≥4) health star rating drinks (fruit juice (100%
294
juice), plain milk, coconut water, bottled water), flavoured milk and no drink.
295
As secondary outcome measures, participants’ perceptions of how healthy (1 = ‘not healthy at
296
all’ to 7 = ‘very healthy’) they considered each type of drink product and how much sugar (1
297
= ‘no sugar’ to 7 = ‘high sugar content’) they thought each drink contained were assessed.
298
Participants’ ratings of regular soft drink, fruit drink (with added sugar), iced tea, sports
299
drink, energy drink and flavoured water were then averaged to create summary indices of
300
participants’ perceptions of the (i) healthiness and (ii) sugar content of sugary drinks in
301
general. To evaluate their knowledge about sugary drinks, participants were asked to indicate
302
how many teaspoons of sugar an average 600ml bottle of soft drink contains (open-ended
303
question), with responses of 15 to 17 teaspoons classified as correct (i.e. within 10% margin
304
of error of 16 teaspoons). In addition, participants were asked to indicate which, if any,
305
diseases or health effects they associate with drinking sugary drinks from the following pre-
306
coded list of seven options: asthma; cancer; diabetes (type 2); epilepsy; heart disease; tooth
307
decay; weight gain/obesity.
308
To provide potential insight into why particular POS signs may or may not be effective in
309
promoting healthier drink choices, participants were asked to indicate their level of
310
agreement (1 = ‘strongly disagree’ to 7 = ‘strongly agree’) that their assigned POS sign: was
311
easy to understand; was believable; was relevant to me; made me stop and think; is one that I
312
would talk to other people about; taught me something new; was convincing; made a strong
313
argument for why I should drink less sugary drinks; made me feel concerned; was effective;
14
314
and was exaggerated. To gauge emotional responses, participants were asked to indicate, on a
315
scale ranging from 1 = ‘not at all’ to 7 = ‘extremely’, whether reading their assigned POS
316
sign made them feel: confused; surprised; reassured; worried; bored; encouraged; amused;
317
disgusted; guilty; and anxious. The extent to which participants agreed they felt motivated to
318
reduce the amount of sugary drinks they currently drink after reading their assigned POS sign
319
was also assessed.
320
To distinguish those with self-reported prior exposure to the 13 Cancers campaign, all
321
participants were shown the 30-second video advertisement and then asked to indicate
322
whether they had previously seen the advertisement. Participants who responded ‘Yes’ to this
323
question were categorised as having self-reported prior exposure to the 13 Cancers campaign
324
whereas those who responded ‘No’ or ‘Don’t know / can’t say’ were deemed not to recall
325
previously seeing the campaign; any participants who were unable to clearly see or hear the
326
advertisement were excluded (5%; n=155).
327
Data on participants’ gender, age, residential postal code, highest level of educational
328
attainment, parental status and frequency of consuming water and sugary drinks in the past
329
week was collected. Socio-economic position (SEP) was estimated according to the
330
Australian Bureau of Statistics’ Index of Relative Socio-Economic Disadvantage, based on
331
participant’s residential postal code (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2018b). Finally, self-
332
reported height and weight were assessed to enable computation of participants’ body mass
333
index (BMI) [weight (kg)/height (m)2].
334
Statistical analysis
335
Data were analysed using Stata/MP V.14.2 (StataCorp, 2016). For the primary and secondary
336
outcome measures, a combination of logistic (categorical outcomes: type of drink selected;
337
correct number of teaspoons of sugar; awareness of association between drinking sugary
15
338
drinks and each disease/health effect) and linear (continuous outcomes: perceived healthiness
339
and sugar content of sugary drinks) regression analyses with Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise
340
comparisons (to control for type 1 error rates) were conducted to test for differences by POS
341
signage condition on each outcome. Each model was then rerun with an interaction term to
342
assess if the effects of the POS signs were consistent among participants with self-reported
343
prior exposure to the 13 Cancers campaign vs. those who did not recall previously seeing the
344
campaign. Where a significant interaction was found, this is reported in the text. In addition,
345
unplanned exploratory analyses were performed to test the interaction between POS signage
346
condition and the other sample characteristics listed in Table 1 for each type of drink selected
347
(i.e. primary outcomes). To assess whether participants’ cognitive and emotional responses to
348
the POS signs varied according to their assigned condition, one-way analysis of variance with
349
Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons was used.
350
Results
351
Sample characteristics
352
A total of 4,942 panellists accessed the survey over a three-week period (5th – 25th November
353
2018) coinciding with the airing of the 13 Cancers campaign in Victoria. Of these, 1,608
354
were excluded prior to randomisation either due to not meeting the eligibility criteria
355
(n=1,477) or dropping out during screening (n=131). A further 26 panellists did not receive
356
their allocated intervention as condition quotas had been reached. After accounting for
357
incomplete surveys (n=87) and cases removed where data quality was not assured (e.g.
358
survey completed below minimum threshold for acceptable time to complete and/or
359
consistent straight-lined responses to scaled data; n=187), a final sample of 3,034 eligible
360
adults was available for analysis (see Figure 3 for CONSORT flow diagram).
16
361
Table 1 shows the demographic profile of the sample. Compared to the Australian
362
population, the sample was similar in terms of socio-economic status (Australian Bureau of
363
Statistics, 2018b) but skewed towards higher education (Australian Bureau of Statistics,
364
2018c) and lower BMI (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2018a). Overall, 29% (n=839) of
365
those surveyed recalled previously seeing the 13 Cancers campaign advertisement.
366
Sugary drink selection
367
Selection of a sugary drink differed by POS signage condition (χ2(4)=29.88, p<0.001). As
368
Table 2 highlights, compared to the no signage condition, participants exposed to either the
369
sugar content (p<0.001), Health Star Rating (p=0.001) or graphic health warning (p=0.004)
370
sign had a significantly lower prevalence of selecting a sugary drink in the hypothetical
371
shopping scenario. However, the text health warning group did not differ from the control
372
group on this outcome.
373
There was a significant interaction between self-reported prior exposure to the 13 Cancers
374
campaign and POS signage condition (χ2(4)=9.93, p=0.042), which is illustrated in Figure 4.
375
Specifically, participants who reported not previously being exposed to the campaign were
376
less likely to select a sugary drink if they had seen either the sugar content, Health Star
377
Rating or graphic health warning sign (all p<0.001). However, for those who reported
378
previously been exposed to the 13 Cancers campaign, none of the four POS signs tested
379
influenced the likelihood of participants making a sugary drink selection (all p>0.05). It
380
should be noted, though, that the proportion of participants who chose a sugary drink in the
381
no signage control condition was significantly lower among those with self-reported 13
382
Cancers campaign exposure compared to those who did not recall previously seeing the
383
campaign (36% cf. 47%; χ2(1)=6.08, p=0.014).
17
384
There were no significant interactions between POS signage condition and participant’s
385
gender, age group, highest level of education, area-based SEP, geographic location, parental
386
status and BMI category or their past week consumption of sugary drinks and water in
387
exploratory analyses.
388
Non-sugary drink selection
389
Selection of a high health star rating drink differed by POS signage condition (χ2(4)=42.42,
390
p<0.001). Participants were significantly more likely to choose a high health star rating drink
391
if they had been exposed to the Health Star Rating sign compared to if they saw the text
392
(p<0.006) or graphic (p<0.049) health warning sign or no signage (p<0.001). Participants
393
exposed to the sugar content sign also had a significantly higher likelihood of choosing a high
394
health star rating drink than those who saw the text health warning sign (p=0.014) or no
395
signage (p<0.001). The prevalence of selecting a diet drink, flavoured milk or no drink did
396
not vary based on the type of POS signage participants were exposed to (Table 2).
397
No significant interaction between self-reported prior exposure to the 13 Cancers campaign
398
and POS signage condition was found for any of the non-sugary drink selections. Exploratory
399
analyses did, however, reveal a significant condition by gender interaction for choosing a
400
high health star rating drink (χ2(4)=11.44, p=0.022). For female participants, all of the POS
401
signs increased the likelihood of making a high health star rating drink selection compared to
402
the no signage condition (all p<0.001; see Supplementary file 1); whereas for male
403
participants, only the Health Star Rating sign led to an increase in selection of a high health
404
star rating drink relative to the text (p=0.002) and graphic (p=0.030) health warning signs as
405
well as no signage (p=0.023). A significant condition by SEP interaction for choosing
406
flavoured milk was also observed in these exploratory analyses (χ2(8)=15.96, p=0.043);
18
407
however, no significant effects of the POS signs were found when running separate models
408
for each SEP group (see Supplementary file 2).
409
Perceived healthiness and sugar content of sugary drinks
410
Participants’ mean rating of the perceived healthiness of sugary drinks differed by POS
411
signage condition (F(4, 3029)=4.98, p<0.001). Compared to the control condition (Mean
412
(M)=3.18), participants perceived sugary drinks to be less healthy if they had been exposed to
413
either the sugar content (M=2.88; p<0.001) or text health warning (M=2.97; p=0.047) sign.
414
Those who saw the sugar content sign also perceived sugary drinks to be less healthy than
415
participants who saw the Health Star Rating sign (M=3.09; p=0.038). There were no
416
significant effects found for the graphic health warning sign (M=3.00). Exposure to the POS
417
signage did not affect participants’ perceptions of the sugar content of sugary drinks (means
418
ranged from 5.33 for no signage and the Health Star Rating sign to 5.42 for the graphic health
419
warning sign).
420
Knowledge about sugary drinks
421
Knowledge of the number of teaspoons of a sugar in a 600ml bottle of soft drink was
422
associated with POS signage condition (χ2(4)=283.52, p<0.001). Specifically, participants in
423
the sugar content POS signage condition (34%) were significantly more likely to nominate
424
the correct number of teaspoons compared to participants who saw either the Health Star
425
Rating (6%), text (8%) or graphic (8%) health warning signs or no signage (5%; all p<0.001).
426
Across conditions, most participants associated drinking sugary drinks with diabetes (82%),
427
tooth decay (81%) and weight gain/obesity (80%). Just over half (55%) believed there to be a
428
link between heart disease and sugary drink consumption, while one-third (33%) thought that
19
429
cancer was associated with drinking sugary drinks. A minority of participants associated
430
sugary drink consumption with asthma (9%) or epilepsy (6%).
431
Participants’ perceptions of a link between drinking sugary drinks and diabetes (χ2(4)=13.75,
432
p=0.008), heart disease (χ2(4)=9.76, p=0.045) and weight gain/obesity (χ2(4)=10.72, p=0.030)
433
varied by POS signage condition. Those exposed to the text health warning sign (87%) were
434
more likely to believe that drinking sugary drinks is associated with diabetes
435
participants exposed to the sugar content (81%; p=0.049) or graphic health warning signs
436
(81%; p=0.049) or no signage (79%; p=0.004). Participants who saw the text health warning
437
sign were also more likely than those who saw the Health Star Rating sign to associate
438
drinking sugary drinks with heart disease (59% cf. 51%; p=0.048), and more likely than those
439
who saw the graphic health warning sign to associate drinking sugary drinks with weight
440
gain/obesity (84% cf. 77%; p=0.023). A significant interaction between self-reported prior
441
exposure to the 13 Cancers campaign and POS signage condition was found for asthma
442
(χ2(4)=12.98, p=0.011). In exploring this further, the POS signs had no effect among those
443
with self-reported 13 Cancers campaign exposure; however, for those who did not recall
444
previously seeing the campaign, knowledge of a link between asthma and sugary drink
445
consumption was higher after seeing the graphic health warning sign compared to the sugar
446
content sign (9% cf. 4%; p=0.015).
447
Point-of-sale signage ratings
448
As shown in Table 3, the sugar content sign generated stronger cognitive responses than the
449
other conditions on most items except ‘was exaggerated’, which was highest for the graphic
450
health warning sign. The graphic health warning sign tended to elicit stronger negative
451
emotional reactions (i.e. feelings of confusion, worry, disgust, guilt and anxiety) from
452
participants, while the Health Star Rating sign tended to provoke more positive emotional
than
20
453
reactions such as reassurance, encouragement and amusement. The sugar content sign made
454
participants feel more surprised than the other POS signs. Participants who saw the sugar
455
content sign reported stronger motivation to reduce their own sugary drink consumption than
456
those who saw the Health Star Rating sign.
457
Discussion
458
Overall, the results from this experimental study suggest that prominent POS signage has the
459
potential to shift consumers away from choosing sugary drinks, with three of the four types of
460
POS signs tested—sugar content, Health Star Rating and graphic health warning—prompting
461
a significantly lower proportion of adults to select a sugary drink in a hypothetical shopping
462
scenario compared to those who saw no signage. Importantly, this reduction was observed
463
across key demographic factors such as gender, age group, education or weight status,
464
suggesting that POS signage is an equitable method for promoting healthier drink choices
465
among adults.
466
Of all the POS signs tested, the sugar content sign promoted the most favourable cognitive
467
responses. This sign displayed concrete information that was likely to be meaningful to
468
people. In line with its content, this sign appeared to provide consumers with clear
469
information about the amount of sugar in particular drinks, as evidenced by consumers’
470
greater accuracy in estimating how many teaspoons of sugar were in a soft drink following
471
exposure to this sign. Visually displaying the 16 teaspoons of sugar contained in a bottle of
472
soft drink may have better allowed participants to imagine what consuming this amount of
473
sugar on its own would be like rather than if the sign had simply stated the number of
474
teaspoons or the amount of sugar in grams (as appears on the mandated nutrition information
475
panel). Clearly illustrating the different amounts of harmful sugars in various drinks may also
21
476
have enabled consumers to more critically compare products, than the more ‘gain framed’
477
symbolism of health stars, or the singular messaging of the health warnings which did not
478
explicitly compare different drinks to one another.
479
In our study, the graphic health warning signs tended to elicit stronger negative emotional
480
reactions than the other signs, plus perceptions that the sign was exaggerated. However, they
481
were equally effective as the sugar content and Health Star Rating signs in encouraging
482
healthier drink choices compared to the control condition. In an analogous study conducted
483
by Billich and colleagues (2018) that empirically tested the same four types of educational
484
messages in a FOP label format, they found that the graphic health warning label was most
485
effective at reducing intended choice of a sugary drink. The stronger performance of the
486
graphic health warning relative to the other message formats in the Billich et al. (2018) study
487
may be due to their use of a health warning image portraying a visible and more immediate
488
health outcome associated with consuming sugary drinks (i.e. dental caries) compared to the
489
less perceptible, longer-term health outcome of build-up of visceral fat around internal organs
490
which was the health warning image used in the present study to correspond with the 13
491
Cancers campaign. Other studies that found FOP graphic health warnings prompted healthier
492
drink preferences featured dental decay as their health outcome (Bollard et al., 2016;
493
Mantzari et al., 2018). It is also possible that graphic health warnings are more salient for
494
participants when making their drink selection if displayed on or near specific drinks (as in
495
the studies simulating exposure to FOP labels) rather than alongside a display of various
496
drink products (as in the present study to simulate POS signage) where the health warning is
497
not linked to particular types of drink. Future research comparing different types of graphic
498
health warnings (e.g. dental caries vs. internal fat) in FOP label and POS signage formats
499
would help explicate these findings and provide valuable information for governments when
500
considering such policy initiatives.
22
501
The sugar content and Health Star Rating POS signs prompted participants to shift towards
502
selecting high health star rating drinks (i.e. 100% fruit juice, plain milk, coconut water,
503
bottled water). Given these two conditions presented nutritional information about specific
504
drinks and the two health warning conditions did not, we cannot determine the extent to
505
which this finding was driven by the sign topic (i.e. sugar content, health star rating) or the
506
inclusion of information about particular drinks in these two conditions. Our finding that the
507
Health Star Rating condition prompted people to select drinks with higher health star ratings
508
parallels Billich et al’s (2018) finding when testing FOP labels. None of the signs affected
509
participants’ propensity to choose diet drinks, flavoured milk or no drink. With the long-term
510
health impacts of artificially sweetened beverages largely unknown (Borges et al., 2017), it is
511
pleasing that the POS signs did not appear to promote diet drinks as an alternative to sugary
512
drinks. While fruit juice was assigned a health star rating of 5 in our study and was thus
513
considered a high health star rating drink, the recent five year review of this FOP labelling
514
system has recommended changes that would result in fruit juices receiving health star
515
ratings between 2.5 and 4, based on their sugars and energy content (MP Consulting, 2019).
516
This change would mean that fruit juice is no longer treated as equivalent to whole fruit, and
517
would better align the system with the Australian Dietary Guidelines (National Health and
518
Medical Research Council, 2013) and the World Health Organization’s guidelines regarding
519
free sugars (World Health Organization, 2015).
520
Exposure to the text health warning sign was associated with improvements in knowledge
521
specific to the educational message (i.e. awareness of the link between drinking sugary drinks
522
and weight gain/obesity) whereas a similar effect was not found for the graphic health
523
warning sign. Examination of participants’ ratings of the four POS signs point to a couple of
524
possible explanations for this finding. First, the graphic health warning sign was more likely
525
to be perceived as ‘exaggerated’ which may have led to increased derogation of the message
23
526
by participants in this condition. Second, this sign was rated lower than the text-only warning
527
in terms of ease of understanding and higher for provoking feelings of confusion which could
528
have reduced participants’ motivation to critically process the information being presented to
529
them. Nonetheless, we found that the graphic health warning promoted healthier drink
530
choices compared to the control condition, whereas the text-only warning did not impact
531
drink choices. Evidence from other studies that have tested text versus pictorial health
532
warnings on sugary drinks indicates that graphic health warnings are more effective than text-
533
only warnings in promoting stronger negative emotional reactions, consideration of health
534
effects and healthier drink choices (Billich et al., 2018; Bollard et al., 2016; Donnelly et al.,
535
2018; Mantzari et al., 2018). The latter findings are consistent with a meta-analysis on
536
cigarette pack health warnings which found that while graphic warnings tend to elicit more
537
reactance than text-only warnings, they are also consistently found to be more effective (Noar
538
et al., 2016).
539
This is the first study to have assessed the impacts of POS signage on consumer drink choices
540
in the context of a relevant mass media campaign airing concurrently. We found that the POS
541
signs only led to significant reductions in sugary drink choices among those who did not
542
recall previously seeing the 13 Cancers video advertisement. The failure of the POS signs to
543
produce additional effects over and above what exposure to the campaign could have
544
provided is not surprising given the relatively low intensity of the experimental POS signage
545
intervention. It is conceivable that repeated exposure to POS signage could indeed boost the
546
effects of mass media campaigns; although further research is needed to test this proposition.
547
Encouragingly, our results suggest that POS signs could complement and reinforce mass
548
media campaigns by reaching consumers who may not otherwise be exposed to public health
549
messages about sugary drinks. This is a particularly important finding in light of people’s
550
changing media use patterns which demands use of a wider range of media channels to
24
551
achieve sufficient reach to change population health behaviour (Durkin & Wakefield, 2018).
552
While research indicates that FOP labels and POS signs can both be effective in promoting
553
healthier drink choices, in the Australian context we believe that POS signs could be more
554
quickly and readily implemented (either voluntarily in particular retail settings such as
555
cafeterias or through being mandated by state governments) than FOP labels (as this would
556
require changes to national regulations). While all our POS signage formats except the text-
557
only health warning were effective in promoting healthier drink choices, it seems likely that
558
industry and the public would be more accepting of health star ratings or sugar content
559
signage than the more confronting graphic health warnings.
560
Several study limitations should be acknowledged. Our primary outcome was drink choice
561
which we measured using a hypothetical shopping scenario; thus, we are unable to infer what
562
effect the observed reduction in participants choosing sugary drinks in response to the POS
563
signage would have on actual consumption levels. We tested the signs under ideal conditions
564
of exposure, whereas in a real-world retail setting other factors may compete for consumers’
565
attention. As participants were recruited from a non-probability based online panel with age
566
and gender quotas applied, the sample cannot be considered representative of the general
567
Australian population; although obtaining a representative sample was not a major
568
consideration given this was an experimental study whereby participants were randomised to
569
conditions. An important limitation relates to the way we operationalised the POS signage
570
manipulations. While our primary focus was on varying the message topic of the signs, two
571
of the conditions (sugar content and Health Star Rating) also singled out particular drinks to
572
illustrate healthier versus unhealthy drink options, while the two health warning conditions
573
did not. Future research could overcome this limitation by testing POS signs that differ by
574
topic but are equivalent in whether they single out particular drinks. Also, for practical
575
reasons, not all drinks from the choice task featured on the sugar content and Health Star
25
576
Rating POS signs. If testing POS signage options in a real-world setting without the same
577
size constraints as an online survey, a wider array of drink types could be shown. However, it
578
would be important for any expanded signs to first be pre-tested to ensure that consumers’
579
understanding of the message is not diminished by the additional information. Finally, our
580
exploration of how prior campaign exposure might impact reactions to the experimental POS
581
signage manipulation relied on an opportunistic approach, whereby data collection was timed
582
to be coincident with a real-world campaign. The limitation to this approach is that campaign
583
exposure was not randomised but relied on participant’s self-reported recall of the campaign.
584
Therefore, an alternative explanation for the findings could be that people who recalled this
585
campaign were already less likely to choose a sugary drink, rather than the campaign causing
586
this, and therefore were unaffected by the POS signs. However, previous population-level
587
evaluations using cohort and controlled cohort designs showing that such campaigns can
588
promote reduced reported sugary drink consumption, adds support to the notion that exposure
589
to the 13 Cancers campaign may have encouraged healthier drink choices, and the POS signs
590
did not add to this impact.
591
Conclusions
592
The findings from this simulation study add to growing evidence that POS signage can
593
positively impact consumer drink choices. We found that POS signs employing sugar
594
content, health star ratings or graphic health warnings in their messaging all promoted
595
healthier drink choices. Of these, the sugar content sign produced the most favourable
596
cognitive responses, whereas the graphic health warnings prompted stronger negative
597
emotions than the other signs. As a next step, we recommend that the effectiveness of such
598
signs be evaluated in actual retail or food service settings to determine their capacity to
26
599
decrease purchasing of sugary drinks and ultimately reduce sugary drink consumption at the
600
population level.
601
602
Acknowledgements
603
The authors thank Hannah Ngaei for designing and producing the four POS signs that were
604
used as experimental stimuli in this research.
605
Author contributions
606
HD designed the study with input from MS, BM and MW. MS managed the data collection.
607
MS and HD analysed and interpreted the data, and drafted the manuscript, with critical input
608
from all other authors. All authors gave final approval for the submitted manuscript.
609
Funding
610
This study was funded by VicHealth and the Victorian Department of Health and Human
611
Services. M.W. was supported by funding from an NHMRC Principal Research Fellowship.
612
The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.
613
Availability of data and materials
614
Data and materials are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
27
615
Ethics approval and consent to participate
616
Cancer Council Victoria’s Institutional Research Review Committee approved all study
617
procedures (reference number IER 1704), and all participants provided informed consent.
618
Declaration of interests
619
All authors are currently employed by an organisation involved in public health research and
620
advocacy.
621
622
28
623
References
624
Auerbach, B. J., Dibey, S., Vallila-Buchman, P., Kratz, M., & Krieger, J. (2018). Review of
625
100% fruit juice and chronic health conditions: Implications for sugar-sweetened
626
beverage policy. Adv Nutr, 9(2), 78-85. https://doi:10.1093/advances/nmx006.
627
Australian and New Zealand Ministerial Forum on Food Regulation. (2019). Australian and
628
New Zealand Ministerial Forum on Food Regulation. Communique - 19 August 2019.
629
Retrieved from
630
https://foodregulation.gov.au/internet/fr/publishing.nsf/content/B3703354A0F92AA8
631
CA25845B000FE61A/$File/Australia%20and%20New%20Zealand%20Ministerial%
632
20Forum%20on%20Food%20Regulation%20Communique%2016%20August%2020
633
19.pdf.
634
Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2018a). 4364.0.55.001 - National Health Survey: First
635
Results, 2017-18. Retrieved from
636
https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/
[email protected]/Lookup/by%20Subject/4364.0.55.001~201
637
7-18~Main%20Features~Key%20Findings~1.
638
Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2018b). 2033.0.55.001 - Census of population and housing:
639
Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), Australia, 2016. Retrieved from
640
http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/
[email protected]/mf/2033.0.55.001.
641 642 643
Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2018c). 6227.0 - Education and work, Australia, May 2018. Retrieved from https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/
[email protected]/mf/6227.0. Australian Department of Health. (2014). Health Star Rating Calculator. Retrieved from
644
http://www.healthstarrating.gov.au/internet/healthstarrating/publishing.nsf/Content/on
645
line-calculator#/step/1.
29
646
Bernabe, E., Vehkalahti, M. M., Sheiham, A., Aromaa, A., & Suominen, A. L. (2014). Sugar-
647
sweetened beverages and dental caries in adults: a 4-year prospective study. J Dent,
648
42(8), 952-958. https://doi:10.1016/j.jdent.2014.04.011.
649
Billich, N., Blake, M. R., Backholer, K., Cobcroft, M., Li, V., & Peeters, A. (2018). The
650
effect of sugar-sweetened beverage front-of-pack labels on drink selection, health
651
knowledge and awareness: An online randomised controlled trial. Appetite, 128, 233-
652
241. https://doi:10.1016/j.appet.2018.05.149.
653
Blake, M. R., Lancsar, E., Peeters, A., & Backholer, K. (2018). The effect of sugar-
654
sweetened beverage price increases and educational messages on beverage purchasing
655
behavior among adults. Appetite, 126, 156-162.
656
https://doi:10.1016/j.appet.2018.03.012.
657
Bleich, S. N., Barry, C. L., Gary-Webb, T. L., & Herring, B. J. (2014). Reducing sugar-
658
sweetened beverage consumption by providing caloric information: how Black
659
adolescents alter their purchases and whether the effects persist. Am J Public Health,
660
104(12), 2417-2424. https://doi:10.2105/AJPH.2014.302150.
661
Bleich, S. N., Herring, B. J., Flagg, D. D., & Gary-Webb, T. L. (2012). Reduction in
662
purchases of sugar-sweetened beverages among low-income Black adolescents after
663
exposure to caloric information. Am J Public Health, 102(2), 329-335.
664
https://doi:10.2105/ajph.2011.300350.
665
Bollard, T., Maubach, N., Walker, N., & Ni Mhurchu, C. (2016). Effects of plain packaging,
666
warning labels, and taxes on young people's predicted sugar-sweetened beverage
667
preferences: an experimental study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act, 13(1), 95.
668
https://doi:10.1186/s12966-016-0421-7.
669 670
Borges, M. C., Louzada, M. L., de Sa, T. H., Laverty, A. A., Parra, D. C., Garzillo, J. M., . . . Millett, C. (2017). Artificially sweetened beverages and the response to the global
30
671
obesity crisis. PLoS Med, 14(1), e1002195.
672
https://doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002195.
673
Chazelas, E., Srour, B., Desmetz, E., Kesse-Guyot, E., Julia, C., Deschamps, V., . . . Touvier,
674
M. (2019). Sugary drink consumption and risk of cancer: results from NutriNet-Sante
675
prospective cohort. BMJ, 366, l2408. https://doi:10.1136/bmj.l2408.
676
COAG Health Council. (2019). Have your say on a national obesity strategy: consultation
677
paper. Retrieved from https://consultations.health.gov.au/population-health-and-
678
sport-division/national-obesity-
679
strategy/supporting_documents/NOSconsultationpaperNovember2019%20a.pdf.
680
Commonwealth of Australia. (2016). About Health Star Ratings. Retrieved from
681
http://healthstarrating.gov.au/internet/healthstarrating/publishing.nsf/Content/About-
682
health-stars.
683
Correa, T., Fierro, C., Reyes, M., Dillman Carpentier, F. R., Taillie, L. S., & Corvalan, C.
684
(2019). "Responses to the Chilean law of food labeling and advertising: exploring
685
knowledge, perceptions and behaviors of mothers of young children". Int J Behav
686
Nutr Phys Act, 16(1), 21. https://doi:10.1186/s12966-019-0781-x.
687
DiMeglio, D. P., & Mattes, R. D. (2000). Liquid versus solid carbohydrate: effects on food
688
intake and body weight. Int J Obes, 24(6), 794-800.
689
https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0801229.
690
Donnelly, G. E., Zatz, L. Y., Svirsky, D., & John, L. K. (2018). The effect of graphic
691
warnings on sugary-drink purchasing. Psychol Sci, 29(8), 1321-1333.
692
https://doi:10.1177/0956797618766361.
693
Durkin, S., & Wakefield, M. (2018). Media use and trends and implications for potential
694
reach of public education and motivation campaigns: application to tobacco control.
695
Retrieved from
31
696
http://www.cancervic.org.au/downloads/cbrc_research_papers/CBRC_RPS_No.49.pd
697
f.
698
Grummon, A. H., Hall, M. G., Taillie, L. S., & Brewer, N. T. (2019). How should sugar-
699
sweetened beverage health warnings be designed? A randomized experiment. Prev
700
Med, 121, 158-166. https://doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.02.010.
701
Grummon, A. H., Taillie, L. S., Golden, S. D., Hall, M. G., Ranney, L. M., & Brewer, N. T.
702
(2019). Sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings and purchases: a randomized
703
controlled trial. Am J Prev Med, 57(5), 601-610.
704
https://doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2019.06.019.
705
Hawkes, C., Jewell, J., & Allen, K. (2013). A food policy package for healthy diets and the
706
prevention of obesity and diet-related non-communicable diseases: the
707
NOURISHING framework. Obes Rev, 14 Suppl 2, 159-168.
708
https://doi:10.1111/obr.12098.
709
Hawkes, C., Smith, T. G., Jewell, J., Wardle, J., Hammond, R. A., Friel, S., . . . Kain, J.
710
(2015). Smart food policies for obesity prevention. The Lancet, 385(9985), 2410-
711
2421. https://doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61745-1.
712
Hodge, A. M., Bassett, J. K., Milne, R. L., English, D. R., & Giles, G. G. (2018).
713
Consumption of sugar-sweetened and artificially sweetened soft drinks and risk of
714
obesity-related cancers. Public Health Nutr, 21(9), 1618-1626.
715
https://doi:10.1017/S1368980017002555.
716
Kite, J., Grunseit, A., Bohn-Goldbaum, E., Bellew, B., Carroll, T., & Bauman, A. (2018). A
717
systematic search and review of adult-targeted overweight and obesity prevention
718
mass media campaigns and their evaluation: 2000-2017. J Health Commun, 23(2),
719
207-232. https://doi:10.1080/10810730.2018.1423651.
32
720
Lauby-Secretan, B., Scoccianti, C., Loomis, D., Grosse, Y., Bianchini, F., & Straif, K. for the
721
International Agency for Research on Cancer Handbook Working Group. (2016).
722
Body fatness and cancer--viewpoint of the IARC Working Group. N Engl J Med,
723
375(8), 794-798. https://doi:10.1056/NEJMsr1606602.
724
Liberato, S. C., Bailie, R., & Brimblecombe, J. (2014). Nutrition interventions at point-of-
725
sale to encourage healthier food purchasing: a systematic review. BMC Public Health,
726
14, 919. https://doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-919.
727
Malik, V. S., Pan, A., Willett, W. C., & Hu, F. B. (2013). Sugar-sweetened beverages and
728
weight gain in children and adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Clin
729
Nutr, 98(4), 1084-1102. https://doi:10.3945/ajcn.113.058362.
730
Mantzari, E., Vasiljevic, M., Turney, I., Pilling, M., & Marteau, T. (2018). Impact of warning
731
labels on sugar-sweetened beverages on parental selection: An online experimental
732
study. Prev Med Rep, 12, 259-267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmedr.2018.10.016.
733
Morley, B., Niven, P., Dixon, H., Swanson, M., Szybiak, M., Shilton, T., . . . Wakefield, M.
734
(2019). Association of the LiveLighter mass media campaign with consumption of
735
sugar-sweetened beverages: Cohort study. Health Promot J Aust.
736
https://doi:10.1002/hpja.244.
737
Morley, B. C., Niven, P. H., Dixon, H. G., Swanson, M. G., McAleese, A. B., & Wakefield,
738
M. A. (2018). Controlled cohort evaluation of the LiveLighter mass media
739
campaign’s impact on adults’ reported consumption of sugar–sweetened beverages.
740
BMJ Open, 8, e019574. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2017-019574.
741
MP Consulting. (2019). Health Star Rating system five year review report. Retrieved from
742
http://www.healthstarrating.gov.au/internet/healthstarrating/publishing.nsf/Content/D
743
1562AA78A574853CA2581BD00828751/$File/Health-Star-Rating-System-Five-
744
Year-Review-Report.pdf.
33
745
National Health and Medical Research Council. (2013). Australian Dietary Guidelines.
746
Retrieved from
747
https://www.eatforhealth.gov.au/sites/default/files/content/n55_australian_dietary_gui
748
delines.pdf.
749
Noar, S. M., Hall, M. G., Francis, D. B., Ribisl, K. M., Pepper, J. K., & Brewer, N. T. (2016).
750
Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob
751
Control, 25(3), 341-354. https://doi:10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051978.
752
O'Hara, B. J., Grunseit, A., Phongsavan, P., Bellew, W., Briggs, M., & Bauman, A. E.
753
(2016). Impact of the Swap It, Don't Stop It Australian national mass media campaign
754
on promoting small changes to lifestyle behaviors. J Health Comm, 21(12), 1276-
755
1285. https://doi:10.1080/10810730.2016.1245803.
756
Obesity Evidence Hub. (2019). Kilojoule labelling in fast food outlets. Retrieved from
757
https://obesityevidencehub.org.au/collections/prevention/kilojoule-labelling-in-fast-
758
food-outlets.
759
Popkin, B. M., & Hawkes, C. (2016). Sweetening of the global diet, particularly beverages:
760
patterns, trends, and policy responses. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol, 4(2), 174-186.
761
https://doi:10.1016/S2213-8587(15)00419-2.
762 763 764
Popova, L. (2016). Sugar-sweetened beverage warning labels: lessons learned from the tobacco industry. J Calif Dent Assoc, 44(10), 633-640. Roberto, C. A., Swinburn, B., Hawkes, C., Huang, T. T. K., Costa, S. A., Ashe, M., . . .
765
Brownell, K. D. (2015). Patchy progress on obesity prevention: emerging examples,
766
entrenched barriers, and new thinking. Lancet, 385(9985), 2400-2409.
767
https://doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61744-X.
34
768
Roberto, C. A., Wong, D., Musicus, A., & Hammond, D. (2016). The influence of sugar-
769
sweetened beverage health warning labels on parents' choices. Pediatrics, 137(2), 1-
770
10. https://doi:10.1542/peds.2015-3185.
771
St-Onge, M. P., Rubiano, F., DeNino, W. F., Jones, A., Jr., Greenfield, D., Ferguson, P. W., .
772
. . Heymsfield, S. B. (2004). Added thermogenic and satiety effects of a mixed
773
nutrient vs a sugar-only beverage. Int J Obes, 28(2), 248-253.
774
https://doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0802560.
775 776 777 778 779
StataCorp. (2016). Stata Statistical Software: Release 14.2. College Station, ,TX: StataCorp LP. Swinburn, B., & Wood, A. (2013). Progress on obesity prevention over 20 years in Australia and New Zealand. Obes Rev, 14 Suppl 2, 60-68. https://doi:10.1111/obr.12103. Swinburn, B. A., Kraak, V. I., Allender, S., Atkins, V. J., Baker, P. I., Bogard, J. R., . . .
780
Dietz, W. H. (2019). The global syndemic of obesity, undernutrition, and climate
781
change: The Lancet Commission report. Lancet, 393(10173), 791-846.
782
https://doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(18)32822-8.
783
VanEpps, E. M., & Roberto, C. A. (2016). The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage
784
warnings: a randomized trial of adolescents' choices and beliefs. Am J Prev Med,
785
51(5), 664-672. https://doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2016.07.010.
786
Vartanian, L. R., Schwartz, M. B., & Brownell, K. D. (2007). Effects of soft drink
787
consumption on nutrition and health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J
788
Public Health, 97(4), 667-675. https://doi:10.2105/AJPH.2005.083782.
789
Wakefield, M. A., Loken, B., & Hornik, R. C. (2010). Use of mass media campaigns to
790
change health behaviour. Lancet, 376(9748), 1261-1271. https://doi:10.1016/S0140-
791
6736(10)60809-4.
35
792 793 794 795 796 797
798
World Cancer Research Fund International. (2016). NOURISHING framework. Retrieved from http://www.wcrf.org/int/policy/nourishing-framework. World Health Organization. (2000). Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic. Report of a WHO consultation. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO. World Health Organization. (2015). Guideline: sugars intake for adults and children. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO.
36
Figure captions Figure 1: Point-of-sale signs (clockwise from top left): sugar content; Health Star Rating; graphic health warning; text health warning.
Figure 2: Example of the online drink choice task, for the sugar content point-of-sale signage condition.~ Note: ~ Each participant viewed the point-of-sale sign for their assigned condition alongside a display of drinks they were required to choose from.
Figure 3: CONSORT flow diagram.
Figure 4: Proportion of participants in each point-of-sale signage condition who selected a sugary drink by self-reported prior exposure to the 13 Cancers campaign. Notes: Pairwise differences were assessed with a Bonferroni correction applied. a Significant difference compared to no signage (control) condition at p<0.05.
37
Table 1: Sample characteristics by point-of-sale signage condition.
Point-of-sale signage condition Total (N=3,034)
No signage (control) (n=605)
Sugar content (n=613)
Health Star Rating (n=608)
Text health warning (n=601)
Graphic health warning (n=607)
%
%
%
%
%
Gender Male
49.5
50.1
49.4
49.2
49.1
49.9
Female
50.3
49.6
50.6
50.8
50.6
50.1
0.1
0.3
0.0
0.0
0.3
0.0
18-29 years
32.6
33.7
32.8
32.4
31.1
33.1
30-44 years
32.9
32.4
32.8
32.9
33.9
32.5
45-59 years
34.5
33.9
34.4
34.7
34.9
34.4
Secondary school or less
29.7
29.6
29.0
29.6
30.8
29.3
TAFE or Trade Certificate or Diploma
28.1
31.1
27.9
24.8
26.3
30.6
University degree
42.2
39.3
43.1
45.6
42.9
40.0
Low (1-33%)
29.1
27.9
29.5
28.5
31.3
28.5
Medium (34-67%)
32.2
32.4
32.8
34.9
27.3
33.4
High (68-100%)
38.7
39.7
37.7
36.7
41.4
38.1
48.0
47.9
48.1
48.2
47.6
47.9
Other Age group
Highest level of education
SEP (area-based)#
Location Victoria
38
Other state/territory
52.0
52.1
51.9
51.8
52.4
52.1
61.3
62.5
62.3
61.3
59.7
60.6
38.7
37.5
37.7
38.7
40.3
39.4
Healthy weight/underweight
46.8
47.4
43.4
44.9
47.2
51.1
Overweight
29.8
29.8
32.0
34.5
25.9
26.9
Obese
23.4
22.8
24.6
20.6
26.9
22.0
<7 cups/week
58.4
57.9
62.2
57.1
57.7
57.2
7+ cups/week
41.6
42.1
37.8
42.9
42.3
42.8
<28 cups/week
36.8
38.0
34.9
37.7
38.4
35.3
28+ cups/week
63.2
62.0
65.1
62.3
61.6
64.7
No/Don’t know/Can’t say
70.9
67.7
70.6
66.7
74.1
75.2
Yes
29.1
32.3
29.4
33.3
25.9
24.8
Parent/carer of child aged <18 No Yes $
BMI category
Sugary drink consumption
Water consumption
Prompted recall of 13 Cancers campaign advertisement^
Note: Percentages may not sum to 100% due to rounding. # Socio-economic position (SEP) was determined according to the Australian Bureau of Statistic’s Index of Relative Socio-Economic Disadvantage ranking for Australia using participant’s residential postcode. This index ranks areas on a continuum of disadvantage (from most disadvantaged to least disadvantaged) taking into consideration characteristics that may enhance or reduce socio-economic conditions of the area. $ Body Mass Index (BMI) information is missing for 675 respondents as they did not self-report their height and/or weight. ^ Prompted campaign recall information is missing for 155 respondents who were unable to see and hear the advertisement clearly.
39
Table 2: Type of drink selected in hypothetical shopping scenario by point-of-sale signage condition.
Point-of-sale signage condition No signage (control) (n=605)
Sugar content (n=613)
Health Star Rating (n=608)
Text health warning (n=601)
Graphic health warning (n=607)
%
%
%
%
%
Sugary drink
43.5
29.4a
32.7a
36.6
33.6a
Diet drink
13.2
12.7
9.0
13.5
13.2
31.1
45.8
46.5
36.8
38.6c
Flavoured milk
6.1
5.5
6.4
5.3
5.8
No drink
6.1
6.5
5.3
7.8
8.9
High (≥4) health star rating drink
a
a
bc
Notes: For each type of drink, pairwise differences were assessed with a Bonferroni correction applied. a Significant difference compared to no signage (control) condition at p<0.05; b Significant difference compared to sugar content sign at p<0.05; c Significant difference compared to Health Star Rating sign at p<0.05.
40
Table 3: Participants’ cognitive and emotional responses to point-of-sale signs.
Sugar content
Point-of-sale signage condition Health Star Rating Text health warning
(n=613) M Cognitive responses Easy to understand Believable Relevant to me Made me stop and think Would talk to others about Taught me something new Convincing Made strong argument Made me feel concerned Effective Exaggerated Emotional responses Confused Surprised Reassured Worried Bored Encouraged Amused Disgusted Guilty Anxious
(n=608) SD
M
Graphic health warning
(n=601)
(n=607)
SD
M
SD
M
SD
6.03d 5.80bcd 5.09cd 5.34bd 5.00bcd 5.19bcd 5.62bcd 5.63b 5.15b 5.55bcd 2.97
1.17 1.30 1.64 1.58 1.69 1.67 1.34 1.57 1.65 1.46 1.75
5.87d 5.54d 5.09cd 4.93 4.50 4.81c 5.24 5.25 4.61 5.27 3.16
1.28 1.37 1.66 1.67 1.77 1.75 1.52 1.66 1.72 1.51 1.83
5.91d 5.48 4.71 5.12 4.59 4.46 5.27 5.45 5.02b 5.15 3.38a
1.25 1.39 1.76 1.67 1.80 1.82 1.46 1.61 1.68 1.50 1.81
5.59 5.32 4.52 5.04 4.48 4.70 5.15 5.39 5.02b 5.11 3.75abc
1.49 1.51 1.78 1.71 1.86 1.85 1.60 1.62 1.72 1.66 1.85
2.36 4.40bcd 3.88cd 4.15b 2.66 4.32cd 2.91d 4.10bc 3.89b 3.35b
1.70 1.87 1.80 1.88 1.76 1.73 1.84 2.03 1.94 1.85
2.58 3.88 4.20acd 3.58 2.97ad 4.46cd 3.05cd 3.31 3.46 2.97
1.79 1.91 1.74 1.89 1.81 1.73 1.85 1.96 1.99 1.85
2.46 3.73 3.37 4.21b 2.70 3.97 2.64 3.52 3.87b 3.60b
1.68 1.86 1.89 1.83 1.76 1.89 1.76 1.92 1.93 1.92
2.78ac 4.08c 3.29 4.41b 2.66 3.85 2.57 4.50abc 3.89b 3.80ab
1.81 1.90 1.90 1.83 1.72 1.84 1.81 1.91 1.89 1.87
41
Motivation To reduce own sugary drink consumption
5.29b
1.67
4.90
1.72
5.04
1.69
5.08
1.73
Notes: Boldfaced figures highlight where a point-of-sale sign produced a stronger response among participants than the other three signs. Pairwise differences were assessed using one-way analysis of variance with Bonferroni correction. a Significantly higher than sugar content sign at p<0.05; b Significantly higher than Health Star Rating sign at p<0.05; c Significantly higher than text health warning sign at p<0.05; d Significantly higher than graphic health warning sign at p<0.05.
Panellists who started the survey (n=4,942)
Excluded (n=1,608) ♦ Outside required age range (n=73) ♦ Reside in Western Australia (n=10) ♦ Did not consume a sugary drink in past 7 days (n=1,394) ♦ Dropped out during screening (n=131)
Randomised (n=3,334)
Allocated to A: No signage (control) (n=673) ♦ Did not receive intervention: quota full (n=4)
Allocated to B: Sugar content (n=668) ♦ Did not receive intervention: quota full (n=5)
Allocated to C: Health Star Rating (n=666) ♦ Did not receive intervention: quota full (n=8)
Allocated to D: Text health warning (n=662) ♦ Did not receive intervention: quota full (n=5)
Allocated to E: Graphic health warning (n=665) ♦ Did not receive intervention: quota full (n=4)
Analysed (n=605) ♦ Excluded from analysis (n=64) - Dropped out (n=24) - Data quality not assured (n=40)
Analysed (n=613) ♦ Excluded from analysis (n=50) - Dropped out (n=19) - Data quality not assured (n=31)
Analysed (n=608) ♦ Excluded from analysis (n=50) - Dropped out (n=14) - Data quality not assured (n=36)
Analysed (n=601) ♦ Excluded from analysis (n=56) - Dropped out (n=13) - Data quality not assured (n=43)
Analysed (n=607) ♦ Excluded from analysis (n=54) - Dropped out (n=17) - Data quality not assured (n=37)
Regular soft drink (cola)
Regular soft drink (non-cola)
Diet soft drink (cola)
Diet soft drink (non-cola)
Fruit juice (100% juice)
Fruit drink (with added sugar)
Iced tea
Plain milk
Flavoured milk
Sports drink
Energy drink
Diet energy drink
Bottled water
Coconut water
Flavoured water
No drink
Ethics approval and consent to participate Cancer Council Victoria’s Institutional Research Review Committee approved all study procedures (reference number IER 1704), and all participants provided informed consent.