G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
European Psychiatry journal homepage: http://www.europsy-journal.com 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Original article
Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation Q1 T.M.
Lincoln, N. Marin, E.S. Jaya *
Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Institute of Psychology, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
A R T I C L E I N F O
A B S T R A C T
Article history: Received 14 October 2016 Received in revised form 13 December 2016 Accepted 18 December 2016 Available online xxx
Background: The causal role of childhood trauma for psychosis is well established, but the mechanisms that link trauma to psychosis are largely unknown. Since childhood trauma is known to cause difficulties in emotion regulation (ER) and patients with psychosis show impaired ER, we hypothesize that impaired ER explains why people with a background of trauma are prone to psychotic experiences. Methods: The study uses a longitudinal cohort design based on a community sample (N = 562) from Germany, Indonesia, and the United States. Childhood trauma was assessed at baseline. ER and psychotic experiences (defined as positive symptom frequency and related distress) were measured repeatedly at 4-, 8-, and 12-month follow-up. Cross-lagged panel and longitudinal mediation analyses with structural equation modeling were used to test the predictive value of ER on psychotic experiences and its mediating role in the association of childhood trauma and psychotic experiences. Results: The cross-lagged paths from impaired ER to symptom distress (but not frequency) were significant. However, there was also evidence for the reverse causation from symptom frequency and distress to impaired ER. ER partially mediated the significant prospective paths from childhood trauma to symptom distress. Conclusion: The findings demonstrate that ER plays a role in translating childhood trauma into distressing psychotic experiences in later life. Moreover, the findings point to a maintenance mechanism in which difficulties in ER and higher symptom distress exacerbate each other. Thus, ER could be a promising target for interventions aimed at prevention of psychosis.
C 2016 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Schizophrenia and psychosis Symptom distress Social adversity Mediation Structural equation modeling Prospective design
8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
The risk of psychosis is almost threefold for people with a background of childhood trauma (CT) [1]. About 26% to 34% of the people diagnosed with a psychotic disorder have experienced CT [2] that commonly includes sexual abuse [3,4], emotional abuse [5–8] and physical abuse [9]. CT is associated with frequency and severity of positive symptoms [10–12] and with a ten-fold increase in symptom related distress [13]. Moreover, it has been found to predict transition to psychosis in high risk samples [14]. The increasingly clear evidence for a causal role of CT in the development of psychosis has begun to inspire research on the putative mediating mechanisms. Knowing these mechanisms are not only crucial to a comprehensive understanding of how
* Corresponding author at: Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Institute of Psychology, University of Hamburg, Von-Melle-Park 5, 20146 Hamburg, Germany. Tel.: +49 40 42838 8087; fax: +49 40 42838 6170. E-mail address:
[email protected] (E.S. Jaya).
psychosis develops it can also help us to intervene earlier and more effectively. Several studies have found negative affect, such as anxiety and depression, to link different types of trauma to positive symptoms [4,15–17]. It seems intuitive to assume that persistent negative affect might result from difficulties in emotion regulation (ER), which has been defined as the ‘‘processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions, especially their intensive and temporal features, to accomplish one’s goals.’’ [18]. Building on this definition and synthesizing established ER theories [19], Berking and colleagues [20] conceptualized adaptive ER as the ability to consciously process emotions, to support oneself in emotionally distressing situations, to actively modify negative emotions, to accept and tolerate emotions and to confront emotionally distressing situations in order to attain important goals. Developmental and attachment theories point to various mechanisms underlying the development of ER, including
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.12.010 C 2016 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved. 0924-9338/
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 2
T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88
observational learning, modeling and social referencing [21], parenting style [22] and attachment relationships [23]. For instance, parental punishment or neglect of a child’s emotional displays have been linked to maladaptive ER [24]. Prospective studies show that early attachment predicts effective regulation strategies [25] and CT has been found to fundamentally disrupt attachment to the maltreating caregiver [26]. Unsurprisingly, thus, there is a bulk of research showing that CT compromises an individual’s ability to regulate emotions effectively [27,28]. However, the causal inferences of these findings are limited due to the cross-sectional designs used. Psychosis has repeatedly been found related to difficulties in ER [29]. ‘‘Several studies have found psychosis to be associated with difficulties in being aware of [30,31], understanding, tolerating and accepting one’s emotions [32–34], with using less functional strategies, such as reappraising the situation in a functional manner [30,31,35,36] and with more avoidance or suppression of emotions [33,34]. Moreover, difficulties in ER have also been associated with increased frequency of symptoms as well as exacerbated symptom distress in both clinical [29,37]. It is noteworthy that these difficulties are prevalent in people at risk of psychosis, who have found to be characterized by lower emotion awareness [38,39], less use of reappraisal strategies [39,40] and more suppression of emotions [39]. This indicates that difficulties in ER seem to precede the disorder and might contribute to its development. However, only few studies have looked into the temporal relationship between ER and psychosis. A small community based study found maladaptive ER to prospectively predict psychotic symptoms from baseline to a 1-month follow-up assessment [36]. Moreover, ER-skills have been found to predict increases in subjective distress and psychotic symptoms following a stressor in individuals with psychosis [41]. Thus, there is preliminary evidence for a causal role of ER in the development of psychosis, but longitudinal studies are needed to further corroborate the postulated causal direction. To sum up: As CT appears to have a significant influence on ER and difficulties in ER are evidently related to psychosis it seems reasonable to postulate that the ability to effectively regulate emotions could at least partially explain the relationship between CT and psychosis, especially as ER has been shown to mediate the association between CT and other psychopathologies (e.g. eating disorder [42] and depression [43]). Using a longitudinal design with four assessment time-points we hypothesized that (1) CT will significantly predict ER and psychotic experiences (defined as frequency of positive symptoms and related distress), (2) that the inability to regulate emotions at one time-point will predict psychotic experiences at the following time-point, (3) and that the relationship between CT and psychotic experiences will be at least partially mediated by ER.
complete a follow-up survey after 4 (T1), 8 (T2), and 12 months (T3). The follow-up surveys were protected by password to ensure that only participants who completed the baseline survey at T0 received the invitation for further participation. Recruitment was conducted through Crowdflower and other websites (e.g. internet forums and social networking websites). Crowdflower is a crowdsourcing Internet marketplace, similar to Amazon MTurk, on which people complete paid jobs. Participants recruited from Crowdflower received 0.50 US$ for completing the baseline survey analog to the median hourly wage in Amazon MTurk [44]. In order to motivate participants to complete the follow-up surveys, the payment increased with each survey (T1, 0.60 US$; T2, 0.80 US$; T3, 1.00 US$). Participants recruited from other websites were not given compensation for reasons of data security. Participants had to be at least 18 years old and provide written informed consent before entering the study. This study received ethical approval from the ethical commission of the German Psychological Society (DGPs, TL062014_2). There were 2501 completed baseline survey entries of which 151 were excluded due to duplicate entries (n = 98), longstring (i.e. providing the same answer consecutively for 50 items, n = 46 [45]), and inconsistent answers (n = 7). The baseline sample thus consisted of 2350 participants of whom 720 completed the English, 786 the German and 844 the Indonesian version of the survey. Of those participants, 432 completed first follow-up (response rate = 18.4%), 300 completed the second follow-up (response rate = 12.8%), 256 completed the third follow-up (response rate = 10.9%) and 139 completed all follow-ups (response rate = 5.9%). A detailed participant flowchart following the STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) guideline is available in Supplementary Fig. 1. There were 2501 completed baseline survey entries of which 151 were excluded due to duplicate entries (n = 98), longstring (i.e. providing the same answer consecutively for 50 items, n = 46 [45]), and inconsistent answers (n = 7). The baseline sample thus consisted of 2350 participants of whom 720 completed the English, 786 the German and 844 the Indonesian version of the survey. Of those participants, 432 completed first follow-up (response rate = 18.4%), 300 completed the second follow-up (response rate = 12.8%), 256 completed the third follow-up (response rate = 10.9%) and 139 completed all follow-ups (response rate = 5.9%). A detailed participant flowchart following the STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) guideline is available in Supplementary Fig. 1. In total, 562 participants completed at least one follow-up survey, fulfilled the inclusion criteria (i.e. complete entry, no longstring, and ID match) and were included in the analyses.
102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149
1.2. Measures
150
89
1. Method
The back-translation procedure and cultural adaptation of measures was conducted with a native German or Indonesian.
151 152
90
1.1. Participants and procedure
91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101
To enable an interpretation of the findings free of issues inherent to clinical populations (e.g. small samples, reduced variance, medication effects) the data for this study was collected in a community sample that covered the continuum of psychotic experiences including those that would be considered clinically relevant. Moreover, the sample was recruited from three different continents to increase the generalizability of findings beyond the western societies. Participants from Germany, Indonesia, and the United States were requested to complete a 30-minute online survey anonymously (T0). Subsequently, they were invited via e-mail to
1.2.1. Childhood trauma Childhood trauma at T0 was assessed via a brief questionnaire adapted from ‘‘The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study’’ (NEMESIS) [46]. The questionnaire assesses any kind of emotional, physical, psychological or sexual abuse before age 16 years according to the definition presented (e.g. sexual abuse, ‘‘How often were you sexually approached against your will? This means: were you ever been touched sexually by anyone against your will or forced to touch anybody? Were you ever pressured into sexual contact against your will?’’). Frequency of abuse is indicated on a six-point scale ranging from 1 = never to 6 = very often.
153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184
1.2.2. Psychotic experiences Subthreshold and clinically relevant psychotic experiences at T1 through T3 were assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychiatric Experiences (CAPE) [47], a 42-item self-report questionnaire. The CAPE assesses experiences related to positive symptoms, negative symptoms, and depression. The 20-item positive symptom subscale was used for the purpose of this study. It assesses a range of psychotic experiences, such as unusual or persecutory beliefs (e.g. ‘‘Do you ever feel as if you are under the control of some force or power other than yourself?’’, ‘‘Have you ever felt that you were being persecuted in any way?’’) or hallucinatory phenomena (e.g. ‘‘Have your thoughts ever been so vivid that you were worried other people would hear them?’’, ‘‘Do you ever hear voices when you are alone?’’) in the past four weeks on both a frequency scale (ranging from 1 = never to 4 = nearly always) and a distress scale (i.e. ‘‘Please indicate how distressed you are by this experience’’; ranging from 1 = not distressed, 4 = very distressed). Previous studies have demonstrated good convergent and discriminative validity [48] and the positive symptom subscale has good retest reliability (r = 0.63) [49].
185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204
1.2.3. Emotion regulation Functional ER-skills at T1 through T3 were assessed with the Emotion Regulation Skills Questionnaire (ERSQ) [50] derived from the theoretical conceptualization by Berking et al. [20]. The ERSQ is a 27-item self-report measure that assesses the application of ERskills during the previous week on a five-point Likert-type scale (1 = not at all, 5 = almost always). It contains nine scales that correspond to the following nine skills: (a) consciously process emotions/be aware of emotions, (b) identify and label emotions, (c) interpret emotion-related body sensations correctly, (d) understand the prompts of emotions, (e) support oneself in emotionally distressing situations, (f) actively modify negative emotions in order to feel better, (g) accept emotions, (h) be resilient to/tolerate negative emotions, and (i) confront emotionally distressing situations in order to attain important goals. Both the total score and the subscales of the ERSQ show good internal consistencies (Cronbach’s a = 0.90, and 0.68–0.81, respectively) and adequate retest-reliability (rtt = 0.75 and 0.48–0.74, respectively). All scales have demonstrated convergent and discriminate validity, including strong positive correlations with constructs related to ER [50].
205 206 207 208 209
1.2.4. Substance use Use of alcohol, cannabis and other substances was measured with self-report items based on the L section of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview version 1.1 [51] using a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (nearly every day).
210
1.3. Analyses
211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226
All analyses were conducted with structural equation modeling (SEM) using the sem function in lavaan ver. 0.5–20 [52] in R, version 3.2.3. Reported path coefficients are completely standardized. Reported overall total effect, overall direct effect, and overall indirect effect coefficients are unstandardized. The following indices and cut-off criteria were used to assess the fit between hypothesized models and the data: CFI 0.90, SRMR 0.08 [53]; RMSEA 0.08 [54]. Maximum likelihood procedure with robust standard errors and a scaled test statistic, asymptotically equaling Yuan–Bentler test statistic, was used to correct for non-normal distribution. As we found that the frequency of psychotic experiences across measurement points was not missing completely at random (MCAR; x2(16) = 101.73, P < .001), subsequent analyses used full information maximum likelihood (FIML) with a missing at random (MAR) assumption. Variable inter-correlations and missing status are shown in Supplementary Table 1.
3
All analyses were conducted with structural equation modeling (SEM) using the sem function in lavaan ver. 0.5–20 [52] in R, version 3.2.3. Reported path coefficients are completely standardized. Reported overall total effect, overall direct effect, and overall indirect effect coefficients are unstandardized. The following indices and cut-off criteria were used to assess the fit between hypothesized models and the data: CFI 0.90, SRMR 0.08 [53]; RMSEA 0.08 [54]. Maximum likelihood procedure with robust standard errors and a scaled test statistic, asymptotically equaling Yuan–Bentler test statistic, was used to correct for non-normal distribution. As we found that the frequency of psychotic experiences across measurement points was not missing completely at random (MCAR; x2(16) = 101.73, P < .001), subsequent analyses used full information maximum likelihood (FIML) with a missing at random (MAR) assumption. Variable inter-correlations and missing status are shown in Supplementary Table 1. Before testing the mediation hypotheses, we computed an autocorrelation model to test the precondition that CT, ER and psychotic experiences are correlated and within-construct autoregressive models to test the precondition that the values of variables at future time points depend at least in part on their respective values at earlier time points. The mediation analysis was based on Baron and Kenny’s [55] principles for mediation and followed suggestions by Cole and Maxwell [56] for mediation testing in longitudinal designs. First, we examined the prospective paths from CT (assessed at T0) to frequency and distress of psychotic experiences at T1–T3 (path c), CT to ER at T1–T3 (path a), and ER (assessed at T1–T3) to frequency and distress of psychotic experiences (assessed at T1–T3, path b) in separate models. Specifically, we examined path b with a crosslagged panel model to take into account various sources of error such as the stability of the variables, cross-sectional associations, prior associations and the possibility of a reverse pathway. According to Cole and Maxwell [56], path c (in this case the path from CT-PE to the final follow-up at T3) needs to be significant in order to conduct the longitudinal mediation analysis and compute the overall total, direct and indirect effect coefficients. These effect coefficients were considered significant if the bias-corrected bootstrap 95% confidence interval (BCa CI) did not include zero [57]. ER was considered a significant mediator if the indirect effect coefficient was significant [58].
227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267
2. Results
268
2.1. Participant characteristics
269
The participants were 24.31 years old on average and 49.3% were female. Moreover, 32.6% (n = 183) reported a life-time mental diagnosis, and 3.7% (n = 21) reported a current diagnosis of psychotic disorder. Slightly more than half (60.1%, n = 338) reported childhood abuse. Specifically, 45.9% (n = 258) of the participants reported to be victims of emotional neglect, 38.3% (n = 215) of psychological abuse, 31.1% (n = 175), and 18.5% (n = 104) of sexual abuse. Detailed participant characteristics are provided in Table 1.
270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278
2.2. Preliminary analyses
279
The autocorrelation model showed that CT, ER, and psychotic experiences (symptom frequency and distress) were significantly inter-correlated in the expected directions at all measurement time points. The within-construct longitudinal models showed that ER and psychotic experiences at a given assessment timepoint predicted the respective variable at the next time-point, which demonstrates sufficient stability across time. Additionally, fit indices of all tested models below were good: all fit indices were
280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
4 Table 1 Participants’ characteristics. Characteristic a
Gender, female (%) Agea Socio-economic statusa Educationa Job positiona Incomea T0 Childhood abusea Emotional neglecta Psychological abusea Physical abusea Sexual abusea T1 Frequency of psychotic experiencesb T1 Distress of psychotic experiencesb T1 Emotion regulationb T2 Frequency of psychotic experiencesc T2 Distress of psychotic experiencesc T2 Emotion regulationc T3 Frequency of psychotic experiencesd T3 Distress of psychotic experiencesd T3 Emotion regulationd a b c d
Mean
SD
Possible range
Study range
227 (49.30%) 35.99 10.30 4.66 2.78 2.86
– 12.77 3.81 1.80 1.93 1.58
– 18–99 3–21 1–7 1–7 1–7
– 18–74 3.00–21.00 1.00–7.00 1.00–7.00 1.00–7.00
1.54 1.62 1.69 1.81 1.45 1.22 3.42 1.46 1.18 3.53 1.40 1.18 3.55
0.50 0.49 0.46 0.39 0.38 0.32 0.70 0.43 0.28 0.76 0.38 0.30 0.75
0–5 0–5 0–5 0–5 1–4 1–4 1–5 1–4 1–4 1–5 1–4 1–4 1–5
0–5 0–5 0–5 0–5 1.00–3.15 1.00–3.00 1.00–5.00 1.00–4.00 1.00–3.00 1.00–11.5 1.00–3.85 1.00–3.75 1.52–5.00
n = 562. n = 432. n = 300. n = 256.
Table 2 Fit indices of the tested models. Model
Yuan–Bentler x2
x CT on symptom frequency (T1) CT on symptom distress (T1) CT on symptom frequency (T2) CT on symptom distress (T2) CT on symptom frequency (T3) CT on symptom distress (T3) CT on ER (T1) CT on ER (T2) CT on ER (T3) ER and symptom frequency ER and symptom distress Longitudinal mediation model of CT, ER, and symptom frequency Longitudinal mediation model of CT, ER, and symptom distress
2
25.76 21.19 21.55 22.94 26.33 21.85 22.97 25.69 16.09 517.93 418.81 611.55 508.69
df
P
13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 124 124 193 192
0.02 0.07 0.06 0.04 0.02 0.06 0.04 0.02 0.24 <0.01 <0.01 <0.01 <0.01
RMSEA [90% CI]
SRMR
CFI
AICa
0.04 0.03 0.03 0.04 0.04 0.04 0.04 0.04 0.02 0.08 0.07 0.06 0.05
0.02 0.04 0.03 0.03 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.08 0.09 0.07 0.08
0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.93 0.95 0.94 0.95
7290 6775 7194 6644 7028 6611 8005 7627 7441 3554 2239 10 262 8930
[0.020–0.063] [0.008–0.054] [0.004–0.057] [0.014–0.057] [0.022–0.063] [0.010–0.056] [0.010–0.060] [0.019–0.063] [0.000–0.046] [0.069–0.082] [0.059–0.072] [0.057–0.068] [0.054–0.066]
Note. CT, childhood trauma; ER, emotion regulation; T0, baseline; T1, month 4; T2, month 8; T3, month 12; RMSEA, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; SRMR, Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; CFI, Comparative Fit Index; AIC, Akaike Information Criterion; 90%CI, 90% confidence interval. a Rounded to the next integer.
There were significant prospective paths from CT (T0) to symptom frequency and distress at T3, and thus a significant path c (see Fig. 1a for details). Moreover, there was a significant prospective path from CT to ER at T2, and thus, a significant path a (see Fig. 1b for details). The prospective association between ER and psychotic experiences was tested with a cross lagged panel model (see Fig. 1c). Here we found a significant prospective path from ER at T2 to symptom distress at T3, but no significant prospective paths from ER to symptom frequency. Thus, the path b was only significant for symptom distress.
previously significant path from CT to symptom distress (g = 0.11, P = .12) at T3 was now non-significant. Furthermore, there was a significant path from ER (T2) to symptom distress (T3, b = 0.09, P < .01) indicating a possible mediation of ER in the relationship between CT and distress. Note, however, there was also a significant path from both symptom frequency (b = 0.12, P < .05) and distress (b = 0.16, P < .01) at T1 to ER at T2, indicating that psychotic experiences do not just follow but also precede ER. Since the hypothesized paths from ER to symptom frequency were not significant, the overall total, direct and indirect effect was only computed for symptom distress. The overall total effect of CT on symptom distress at final follow up was significant at 0.0747 (95% BCa CI, 0.0311, 0.1184). This effect consists of a significant overall direct effect of 0.0694 (95% BCa CI, 0.0269, 0.1119) and a significant overall indirect effect at 0.0053 (95% BCa CI, 0.0002, 0.0104). The mediation proportion [59] showed that ER mediated 7.1% of the overall total effect of CT on symptom distress.
306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322
303
2.4. Longitudinal mediation analysis
2.5. Additional exploratory analyses
323
304 305
In the longitudinal mediation analysis model (see Fig. 2), which included all time-points and the putative mediator ER, the
In order to explore whether the associations of interest differ across symptoms, we repeated the analyses using the hallucination
324 325
288 289
met, indicating that the parameters can be interpreted (see Table 2).
290 291
2.3. Prospective association of childhood trauma, emotional regulation, and psychotic experiences
292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360
Fig. 1. Prospective association of childhood trauma, emotional regulation, and psychotic experiences. Note. (distress); [frequencies]; path coefficients are completely standardized; +P < .10, *P < .05, **P < .001. Dotted lines indicate insignificant paths. CT, childhood trauma; ER, emotion regulation; PE, psychotic experiences; T0, baseline; T1, month 4; T2, month 8; T3 = month 12.
5
and paranoia subscale of the CAPE [60] as dependent variables. The results are presented in Supplementary Figs. 2 and 3 for hallucinations and paranoia, respectively. We found childhood trauma to predict both frequency and distress of hallucinations and paranoia over time. Moreover, ER predicted both frequency and distress of paranoia, but not of hallucinations. Consequently, we only conducted the longitudinal mediation analysis on paranoia. The results of this analysis are presented in Supplementary Fig. 4. The overall total effect of CT on paranoia distress at final follow up was significant 0.130 (95% CI, 0.075, 0.186). This effect consists of a significant overall direct effect of 0.116 (95% CI, 0.062, 0.171) and a significant overall indirect effect at 0.014 (95% CI, 0.001, 0.027). The overall total effect of CT on paranoia frequency at final follow up was also significant at 0.109 (95% CI, 0.059, 0.159), which consists of a significant overall direct effect of 0.099 (95% CI, 0.049, 0.149) and a significant overall indirect effect at 0.010 (95% CI, 0.001, 0.020). Thus, ER was a significant partial mediator in the association between CT and paranoia frequency and distress. In order to explore whether the associations of interest differ across symptoms, we repeated the analyses using the hallucination and paranoia subscale of the CAPE [60] as dependent variables. The results are presented in Supplementary Figs. 2 and 3 for hallucinations and paranoia, respectively. We found childhood trauma to predict both frequency and distress of hallucinations and paranoia over time. Moreover, ER predicted both frequency and distress of paranoia, but not of hallucinations. Consequently, we only conducted the longitudinal mediation analysis on paranoia. The results of this analysis are presented in Supplementary Fig. 4. The overall total effect of CT on paranoia distress at final follow up was significant 0.130 (95% CI, 0.075, 0.186). This effect consists of a significant overall direct effect of 0.116 (95% CI, 0.062, 0.171) and a significant overall indirect effect at 0.014 (95% CI, 0.001, 0.027). The overall total effect of CT on paranoia frequency at final follow up was also significant at 0.109 (95% CI, 0.059, 0.159), which consists of a significant overall direct effect of 0.099 (95% CI,
Fig. 2. Longitudinal mediation analysis of childhood trauma, emotional regulation, and psychotic experiences. Note. (distress); [frequencies]; path coefficients are completely standardized; +P < .10, *P < .05, **P < .001. Dotted lines indicate non-significant paths. CT, childhood trauma; ER, emotion regulation; PE, psychotic experiences; T0, baseline; T1, month 4; T2, month 8; T3, month 12.
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 6
T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376
0.049, 0.149) and a significant overall indirect effect at 0.010 (95% CI, 0.001, 0.020). Thus, ER was a significant partial mediator in the association between CT and paranoia frequency and distress. Furthermore, we investigated differences between countries in our variables of interest. We did not find significant mean differences between countries in regard to CT, ER, and positive symptom distress. However, we did find significant differences between countries in positive symptom frequency (F(2,429) = 14.60, P < 0.01), with post hoc t-tests with Bonferroni correction indicating that positive symptom frequency to be significantly higher in participants from Indonesia (M = 1.60) than in those from Germany (M = 1.36) and the United States (M = 1.44). Finally, in order to rule out that ER was merely a proxy for substance abuse, we analyzed correlations between ER and the use of alcohol, cannabis, and other substance, which were all found to be non-significant.
377
3. Discussion
378
3.1. Summary of findings and general discussion
379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423
This study tested for a significant association of CT and psychotic experiences, a predictive effect of ER on psychotic experiences and a mediating role of ER in the relationship of CT and psychotic experiences. As hypothesized, and in line with the extensive literature highlighting maltreatment as a major risk factor for later psychosis [9] we found CT to be significantly related to psychotic experiences. Confirming part of our second hypothesis, we found that the ability to regulate emotions significantly and prospectively predicted fluctuations in symptom distress over time. Thus, the fewer ER skills an individual endorsed at a given time-point, the higher the likelihood that this individual would be distressed by psychotic experiences at the next time-point. This finding adds to the preliminary evidence indicating a covariation of at risk-mental states and ER [38–40], a causal influence of ER on psychotic symptoms and distress [36,41], and suggests that the ability to effectively regulate emotions is crucial to arrive at low levels of symptom distress. Given the overwhelming evidence for affective pathways to psychosis [61,62], we were surprised by the absence of a significant path from ER to symptom frequency. Possibly, the mere presence of non-distressing low-threshold delusional beliefs or perceptual phenomenon as assessed in the frequency domain of the CAPE might not have sufficient pathological character to find significant associations with putative risk factors for psychosis. Psychotic symptom distress has been found to be a better discriminator between clinical and non-clinical samples than frequency [63] and to improve the prediction of high risk status relative to frequency of psychotic experiences on their own [64]. Thus, it appears to be a more valid indicator of relevant subclinical symptomatology. However, this assumption warrants further research that includes measures of clinical relevance (e.g. functioning). Moreover, interestingly, our test of the ‘‘reverse causation model’’ revealed that symptom frequency and related distress at one time-point also significantly predicted an impaired ability to regulate emotions at the next time-point. This could be explicable by the fact that psychotic experiences demand attention, leaving less attentional capacity to deal with distressing emotions, an effortful process it itself. Moreover, people who are feeling distressed by psychotic symptoms are dealing with higher levels of negative affect that are more difficult to down-regulate (causing them to report less success in ER) than the milder levels of emotional distress people without disturbing psychotic experiences are faced with. The longitudinal pattern of associations speaks for reciprocal exacerbations of symptom distress and ER difficulties in which people appear to be getting caught up in a vicious
circle of psychotic experiences, negative affect and difficulties in down-regulating negative emotions. This may explain symptom exacerbation over longer time-periods. The significant overall indirect effect indicates that ER partially explains the link between CT and symptom distress. This has not been investigated in regard to psychosis so far and thus makes a novel contribution to the field. However, the mediating effect was only found for symptom distress, not for symptom frequency. Taken together with research showing that ER also mediates between CT and other affective disorders [65] one could speculate that ER may be more relevant to the affective symptomatology associated with psychosis than to the specific psychotic symptomatology as such. Thus, the specificity warrants further research. Moreover, the mediation was partial, explaining 7.1% of the total relationship between trauma and symptom distress. This indicates that other putative mediators should be considered, candidates being negative schema [15,66], social cognition (e.g. impaired theory of mind [67,68]), neurocognitive impairment [69], substance abuse [70], and ongoing social adversities [71]. In addition to the reduced functional ER strategies as assessed in this study (e.g. acceptance, tolerance and clarity of emotions), increased dysfunctional ER strategies (e.g. rumination, suppression, or avoidance), might also contribute to mediating the relationship of CT and symptom distress, in particular because these types of ER have been found to be linked to CT [43,72] and to exacerbated symptom distress in psychosis [29,73,74]. Notably, the focus on functional rather than dysfunctional ER in our study might explain why we did not find a significant correlation between substance use and ER as has been found in other studies [70]. Our additional analyses of the effects for paranoia and hallucinations suggest that although CT was linked to both paranoia and hallucinations the mediating role of ER may be specific to paranoia. However, the insignificant finding for hallucinations needs to be interpreted with caution because the hallucination subscale has only four items. Thus, the question of specificity warrants further research with more suitable measures for this purpose.
424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460
3.2. Limitations
461
The participants were drawn from a pool of people with access to the Internet. These participants tended to be highly educated and have middle to low income. Similar sample characteristics have been reported by other researchers who used crowdsourcing websites for recruitment [75]. Due to the slightly reduced variability of the participants’ demographics, the strength of the observed relationships might have been underestimated. However, the sample was sufficiently heterogeneous in regard to the variables of interest: childhood trauma and psychotic experiences. Moreover, the inclusion of individuals from three countries adds to the representativeness of the sample. Another limitation is that only 23% of the baseline-group participated in the follow-ups and only 5.9% of the initial sample completed all assessments. Although we used FIML procedure in all analyses to take into account the missing data, we cannot rule out that the high attrition rate influenced the results. The correlates of missing data reported in Supplementary Table 1 suggest that participants with more severe symptoms were more likely to miss follow-up assessments. Selective drop-out at the higher end of the symptom spectrum could have resulted in reduced variability and thus in attenuated effect sizes [76]. Another limitation is that only 23% of the baseline-group participated in the follow-ups and only 5.9% of the initial sample completed all assessments. Although we used FIML procedure in all analyses to take into account the missing data, we cannot rule out that the high attrition rate influenced the results. The
462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525
correlates of missing data reported in Supplementary Table 1 suggest that participants with more severe symptoms were more likely to miss follow-up assessments. Selective drop-out at the higher end of the symptom spectrum could have resulted in reduced variability and thus in attenuated effect sizes [76]. Another issue is that the retrospective assessment of CT is prone to recall bias [77]. Similarly, the self-report diagnosis cannot be externally verified. However, the minimal reward for survey termination, the application of data verification procedures and the similarity of prevalence rates of life-time mental diagnosis (32.1%) to representative samples from epidemiological studies (e.g. 29.4% [78]) speak for the validity of the data. Furthermore, the mean CAPE scores for frequency of positive symptoms across all assessment time-points are comparable to those found in a large data set of previously published data [60]. People who reported no mental disorder in our study had a mean score of M = 1.44 (compared to M = 1.41 in other population samples [60]) and those who reported a psychotic disorder had a mean value of M = 1.52 (compared to a mean value of M = 1.73 for participants with a psychotic disorder in other studies [60]). Moreover, our measure of ER assessed what the participant remembered to have done in response to emotions when they arose during the past week rather than assessing ‘‘online ER’’ (e.g. by using experience sampling [79] or online ER in a standardized paradigm [80], which might have led to more variability in ER over time. Nevertheless, we found a sufficient variation in the use of ER strategies (with correlations between time-points of around 0.7, see Fig. 1c), which was similar to the stability found for positive symptoms and can be considered sufficient to investigate whether the inability to regulate emotions at one time-point will predict psychotic experiences at the following time-point as hypothesized. Lastly, the analytical strategy may have overestimated the stability of ER and symptom frequency and distress while underestimating cross-lagged effects [81]. This is partly due to the shared method factor (i.e. psychotic experiences at all timepoints were measured with the exact same items). However, we opted for this more conservative approach keeping the probability of type I error low because the study was not exploratory in nature.
526
3.3. Therapeutic implications
527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544
The results indicate that targeting ER in victims of childhood trauma and in those at risk of psychosis could be a promising way to interrupt the cascade of negative affect, psychotic experiences and distress. There are several approaches available that can be employed or used to inform novel interventions. For example, third-wave approaches, such as Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) [82] and Emotion Focused Therapy (EFT) [83] aim at enhancing the ability to tolerate negative affective states and to accept the presence of difficult thoughts or feelings [83,84] and have shown promising effects in the treatment of psychosis [82,85]. Moreover, there are programs that attempt to strengthen the focus on ER by training adaptive ER-skills such as acceptance, tolerance, reappraisal and problem solving [20,86] and have been found to be promising in various inpatient populations [20,87]. These types of trainings could potentially be beneficial to reducing transition to psychosis – possibly as an add-on to existing approaches – especially if they are tailored to improve a person’s individual profile of ER skills.
545
4. Conclusion
546 547 548
Researchers have worked toward identifying pathways from CT to psychosis [88] and have put forward the idea of an affective pathway linking trauma to psychosis by heightened emotional
7
distress, anxiety and depression [16,17]. Our findings extend this line of research by pointing to the role of ER as a possible psychological mechanism involved in this process. Although this mechanism is unlikely to be specific to psychosis, the fact that difficulties in ER precede and follow from symptom distress render ER a promising target for interventions aimed at increasing wellbeing and preventing psychotic symptom exacerbation.
549 550 551 552 553 554 555
Disclosure of interest
556
The authors have not supplied their declaration of competing Q2557 interest. 558 Appendix A. Supplementary data
559
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.12. 010.
560 561 562
References
563
[1] Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, Lieverse R, Lataster T, Viechtbauer W, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull 2012;38:661–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs050. [2] Bonoldi I, Simeone E, Rocchetti M, Codjoe L, Rossi G, Gambi F, et al. Prevalence of self-reported childhood abuse in psychosis: a meta-analysis of retrospective studies. Psychiatry Res 2013;210:8–15. [3] Bebbington P, Bhugra D, Brugha T, Singleton N, Farrell M, Jenkins R, et al. Psychosis, victimisation and childhood disadvantage. Br J Psychiatry 2004;185:220–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.185.3.220. [4] Bebbington P, Jonas S, Kuipers E, King M, Cooper C, Brugha T, et al. Childhood sexual abuse and psychosis: data from a cross-sectional national psychiatric survey in England. Br J Psychiatry 2011;199:29–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/ bjp.bp.110.083642. [5] Ackner S, Skeate A, Patterson P, Neal A. Emotional abuse and psychosis: a recent review of the literature. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma 2013;22:1032–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2013.837132. [6] Ashcroft K, Kingdon DG, Chadwick P. Persecutory delusions and childhood emotional abuse in people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Psychosis 2012;4:168–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17522439.2011.619012. [7] Duhig M, Patterson S, Connell M, Foley S, Capra C, Dark F, et al. The prevalence and correlates of childhood trauma in patients with early psychosis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2015;49:651–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0004867415575379. [8] Morrison AP, Petersen T. Trauma, metacognition and predisposition to hallucinations in non-patients. Behav Cogn Psychother 2003;31:235–46. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1352465803003011. [9] Arseneault L, Cannon M, Fisher HL, Polanczyk G, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. Childhood trauma and children’s emerging psychotic symptoms: a genetically sensitive longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2011;168:65–72. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10040567. [10] Bentall RP, Wickham S, Shevlin M, Varese F. Do specific early-life adversities lead to specific symptoms of psychosis? A study from the 2007 the adult psychiatric morbidity survey. Schizophr Bull 2012;sbs049. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1093/schbul/sbs049. [11] Kilcommons AM, Morrison AP, Knight A, Lobban F. Psychotic experiences in people who have been sexually assaulted. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2008;43:602–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-007-0303-z. [12] Sitko K, Bentall RP, Shevlin M, O’Sullivan N, Sellwood W. Associations between specific psychotic symptoms and specific childhood adversities are mediated by attachment styles: an analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Psychiatry Res 2014;217:202–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.019. [13] Bak M, Krabbendam L, Janssen I, Graaf R, Vollebergh W, Os J. Early trauma may increase the risk for psychotic experiences by impacting on emotional response and perception of control. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005;112:360–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00646.x. [14] Thompson AD, Nelson B, Yuen HP, Lin A, Amminger GP, McGorry PD, et al. Sexual trauma increases the risk of developing psychosis in an ultra high-risk ‘‘prodromal’’ population. Schizophr Bull 2014;40:697–706. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1093/schbul/sbt032. [15] Fisher HL, Appiah-Kusi E, Grant C. Anxiety and negative self-schemas mediate the association between childhood maltreatment and paranoia. Psychiatry Res 2012;196:323–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.004. [16] Freeman D, Fowler D. Routes to psychotic symptoms: trauma, anxiety and psychosis-like experiences. Psychiatry Res 2009;169:107–12. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.07.009. [17] Marwaha S, Bebbington P. Mood as a mediator of the link between child sexual abuse and psychosis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2014;1–3. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-014-0966-1.
564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 8
622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707
T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
[18] Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1994;59:25–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994. tb01276.x. [19] Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol 1998;2:271–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271. [20] Berking M, Wupperman P, Reichardt A, Pejic T, Dippel A, Znoj H. Emotionregulation skills as a treatment target in psychotherapy. Behav Res Ther 2008;46:1230–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.08.005. [21] Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev 2007;16:361–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x. [22] Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL. Parental socialization of emotion. Psychol Inq 1998;9:241–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1. [23] Harder S. Attachment in schizophrenia—implications for research, prevention, and treatment. Schizophr Bull 2014;sbu133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ schbul/sbu133. [24] Waller G, Corstorphine E, Mountford V. The role of emotional abuse in the eating disorders: implications for treatment. Eat Disord 2007;15:317–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10640260701454337. [25] Gilliom M, Shaw DS, Beck JE, Schonberg MA, Lukon JL. Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys: strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. Dev Psychol 2002;38:222–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00121649.38.2.222. [26] Davies PT, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and child adjustment: an emotional security hypothesis. Psychol Bull 1994;116:387–411. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387. [27] Gratz KL, Bornovalova MA, Delany-Brumsey A, Nick B, Lejuez CW. A laboratorybased study of the relationship between childhood abuse and experiential avoidance among inner-city substance users: the role of emotional nonacceptance. Behav Ther 2007;38:256–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.006. [28] Shields A, Cicchetti D. Reactive aggression among maltreated children: the contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. J Clin Child Psychol 1998;27:381–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_2. [29] Khoury B, Lecomte T. Emotion regulation and schizophrenia. Int J Cogn Ther 2012;5:67–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2012.5.1.67. [30] Kimhy D, Vakhrusheva J, Jobson-Ahmed L, Tarrier N, Malaspina D, Gross JJ. Emotion awareness and regulation in individuals with schizophrenia: implications for social functioning. Psychiatry Res 2012;200:193–201. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.029. [31] van der Meer L, van’t Wout M, Aleman A. Emotion regulation strategies in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2009;170:108–13. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.07.010. [32] Lincoln TM, Hartmann M, Ko¨ther U, Moritz S. Do people with psychosis have specific difficulties regulating emotions? Clin Psychol Psychother 2014. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1923. [33] Perry Y, Henry JD, Grisham JR. The habitual use of emotion regulation strategies in schizophrenia. Br J Clin Psychol 2011;50:217–22. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1111/j.2044-8260.2010.02001.x. [34] Perry Y, Henry JD, Nangle MR, Grisham JR. Regulation of negative affect in schizophrenia: the effectiveness of acceptance versus reappraisal and suppression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012;34:497–508. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1080/13803395.2012.661405. [35] Livingstone K, Harper S, Gillanders D. An exploration of emotion regulation in psychosis. Clin Psychol Psychother 2009;16:418–30. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/cpp.635. [36] Westermann S, Boden MT, Gross JJ, Lincoln TM. Maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation prospectively predicts subclinical paranoia. Cogn Ther Res 2013;37:881–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9523-6. [37] Badcock JC, Paulik G, Maybery MT. The role of emotion regulation in auditory hallucinations. Psychiatry Res 2011;185:303–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.psychres.2010.07.011. [38] van Rijn S, Schothorst P, van’t Wout M, Sprong M, Ziermans T, van Engeland H, et al. Affective dysfunctions in adolescents at risk for psychosis: emotion awareness and social functioning. Psychiatry Res 2011;187:100–5. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.10.007. [39] Kimhy D, Gill KE, Brucato G, Vakhrusheva J, Arndt L, Gross JJ, et al. The impact of emotion awareness and regulation on social functioning in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Psychol Med 2016;46:2907–18. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000490. [40] van der Velde J, Opmeer EM, Liemburg EJ, Bruggeman R, Nieboer R, Wunderink L, et al. Lower prefrontal activation during emotion regulation in subjects at ultrahigh risk for psychosis: an fMRI-study. NPJ Schizophr 2015;1:15026. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npjschz.2015.26. [41] Lincoln TM, Hartmann M, Ko¨ther U, Moritz S. Dealing with feeling: specific emotion regulation skills predict responses to stress in psychosis. Psychiatry Res 2015;228:216–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.04.003. [42] Racine SE, Wildes JE. Emotion dysregulation and anorexia nervosa: an exploration of the role of childhood abuse. Int J Eat Disord 2014;48:55–8. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22364. [43] O’Mahen HA, Karl A, Moberly N, Fedock G. The association between childhood maltreatment and emotion regulation: two different mechanisms contributing to depression? J Affect Disord 2015;174:287–95. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.jad.2014.11.028. [44] Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci 2011;6:3–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980.
[45] Johnson JA. Ascertaining the validity of individual protocols from Web-based personality inventories. J Res Personal 2005;39:103–29. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.09.009. [46] Janssen I, Krabbendam L, Bak M, Hanssen M, Vollebergh W, de Graaf R, et al. Childhood abuse as a risk factor for psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004;109:38–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0001-690X. 2003.00217.x. [47] Stefanis NC, Hanssen M, Smirnis NK, Avramopoulos DA, Evdokimidis IK, Stefanis CN, et al. Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychol Med 2002;32:347–58. [48] Hanssen M, Peeters F, Krabbendam L, Radstake S, Verdoux H, van Os J. How psychotic are individuals with non-psychotic disorders? Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:149–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-0030622-7. [49] Konings M, Bak M, Hanssen M, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006;114:55– 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00741.x. [50] Berking M, Znoj H. Entwicklung und Validierung eines Fragebogens zur standardisierten Selbsteinscha¨tzung emotionaler Kompetenzen (SEK-27). Z Fu¨r Psychiatr Psychol Psychother 2008;56:141–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/ 1661-4747.56.2.141. [51] Smeets R, Dingemans P. Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). Version 1.1. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1993. [52] Rosseel Y. lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw 2012;48:1–36. [53] Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J 1999;6:1–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. [54] MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods 1996;1:130–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130. [55] Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;51:1173–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00223514.51.6.1173. [56] Cole DA, Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. J Abnorm Psychol 2003;112:558–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558. [57] Cheung GW, Lau RS. Testing mediation and suppression effects of latent variables bootstrapping with structural equation models. Organ Res Methods 2008;11:296–325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428107300343. [58] Mayer A, Thoemmes F, Rose N, Steyer R, West SG. Theory and analysis of total, direct, and indirect causal effects. Multivar Behav Res 2014;49:425–42. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.931797. [59] Ditlevsen S, Christensen U, Lynch J, Damsgaard MT, Keiding N. The mediation proportion: a structural equation approach for estimating the proportion of exposure effect on outcome explained by an intermediate variable. Epidemiol Camb Mass 2005;16:114–20. [60] Schlier B, Jaya ES, Moritz S, Lincoln TM. The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences measures nine clusters of psychosis-like experiences: a validation of the German version of the CAPE. Schizophr Res 2015;169:274–9. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.10.034. [61] Myin-Germeys I, van Os J. Stress-reactivity in psychosis: evidence for an affective pathway to psychosis. Clin Psychol Rev 2007;27:409–24. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.005. [62] Freeman D, Stahl D, McManus S, Meltzer H, Brugha T, Wiles N, et al. Insomnia, worry, anxiety and depression as predictors of the occurrence and persistence of paranoid thinking. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2011;47:1195–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-011-0433-1. [63] Lincoln TM. Relevant dimensions of delusions: continuing the continuum versus category debate. Schizophr Res 2007;93:211–20. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.013. [64] Kline E, Thompson E, Bussell K, Pitts SC, Reeves G, Schiffman J. Psychosis-like experiences and distress among adolescents using mental health services. Schizophr Res 2014;152:498–502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.12. 012. [65] Carvalho Fernando S, Beblo T, Schlosser N, Terfehr K, Otte C, Lo¨we B, et al. The impact of self-reported childhood trauma on emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder and major depression. J Trauma Dissociation 2014;15:384–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2013.863262. [66] Jaya ES, Ascone L, Lincoln TM. Social adversity and psychosis: the mediating role of cognitive vulnerability. Schizophr Bull 2016;sbw104. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1093/schbul/sbw104. [67] Allen P, Freeman D, McGuire P, Garety P, Kuipers E, Fowler D, et al. The prediction of hallucinatory predisposition in non-clinical individuals: examining the contribution of emotion and reasoning. Br J Clin Psychol 2005;44:127–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466504x20044. [68] Garety PA, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington P. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol Med 2001;31:189–95. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003312. [69] Lysaker P, Meyer P, Evans J, Marks K. Neurocognitive and symptom correlates of self-reported childhood sexual abuse in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Ann Clin Psychiatry 2001;13:89–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/ 10401230109148953. [70] Mandavia A, Robinson GGN, Bradley B, Ressler KJ, Powers A. Exposure to childhood abuse and later substance use: indirect effects of emotion
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
708 709 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 726 727 728 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 739 740 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781 782 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793
G Model
EURPSY 3457 1–9 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
794 795 796 797 798 799 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826
[71]
[72]
[73]
[74]
[75]
[76]
[77]
[78]
[79]
dysregulation and exposure to trauma. J Trauma Stress 2016;29:422–9. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.22131. Jaya ES, Lincoln TM. Social adversities and psychotic symptoms: a test of predictions derived from the social defeat hypothesis. Psychiatry Res 2016;245:466–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.09.002. Heleniak C, Jenness JL, Vander Stoep A, McCauley E, McLaughlin KA. Childhood maltreatment exposure and disruptions in emotion regulation: a transdiagnostic pathway to adolescent internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Cogn Ther Res 2015;40:394–415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-0159735-z. Morrison AP, Haddock G, Tarrier N. Intrusive thoughts and auditory hallucinations: a cognitive approach. Behav Cogn Psychother 1995;23:265. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1352465800015873. Morrison AP, Wells A. Relationships between worry, psychotic experiences and emotional distress in patients with schizophrenia spectrum diagnoses and comparisons with anxious and non-patient groups. Behav Res Ther 2007;45:1593–600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.11.010. Shapiro DN, Chandler J, Mueller PA. Using mechanical turk to study clinical populations. Clin Psychol Sci 2013;1:213–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 2167702612469015. Terluin B, de Boer MR, de Vet HCW. Differences in connection strength between mental symptoms might be explained by differences in variance: reanalysis of network data did not confirm staging. PLOS ONE 2016;11:e0155205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0155205. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Woodward LJ. The stability of child abuse reports: a longitudinal study of the reporting behaviour of young adults. Psychol Med 2000;30:529–44. Kessler RC, Demler O, Frank RG, Olfson M, Pincus HA, Walters EE, et al. Prevalence and treatment of mental disorders, 1990 to 2003. N Engl J Med 2005;352:2515–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsa043266. Catterson AD, Eldesouky L, John OP. An experience sampling approach to emotion regulation: situational suppression use and social hierarchy. J Res Personal 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.004.
9
[80] Sheppes G, Meiran N. Better late than never? On the dynamics of online regulation of sadness using distraction and cognitive reappraisal. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2007;33:1518–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616 7207305537. [81] Cole DA, Martin NC, Steiger JH. Empirical and conceptual problems with longitudinal trait-state models: introducing a trait-state-occasion model. Psychol Methods 2005;10:3–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.1.3. [82] Bach P, Hayes SC. The use of acceptance and commitment therapy to prevent the rehospitalization of psychotic patients: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 2002;70:1129–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022006x.70.5.1129. [83] Greenberg LS. Emotion-focused therapy: coaching clients to work through their feelings, vol. xvi. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association; 2002. [84] Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: an experiential approach to behavior change, vol. xvi. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press; 1999. [85] White R, Gumley A, McTaggart J, Rattrie L, McConville D, Cleare S, et al. A feasibility study of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy for emotional dysfunction following psychosis. Behav Res Ther 2011;49:901–7. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.003. [86] Barnow S, Lo¨w C, Dodek A, Stopsack M. Gefu¨hle im Griff – Emotionen intelligent regulieren. PPmP – Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol 2014;64:284– 9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0033-1363683. [87] Berking M, Ebert D, Cuijpers P, Hofmann SG. Emotion regulation skills training enhances the efficacy of inpatient cognitive behavioral therapy for major depressive disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:234–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000348448. [88] Isvoranu A-M, van Borkulo CD, Boyette L-L, Wigman JTW, Vinkers CH, Borsboom D, et al. A network approach to psychosis: pathways between childhood trauma and psychotic symptoms. Schizophr Bull 2016;sbw055. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw055.
827
Please cite this article in press as: Lincoln TM, et al. Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 837 838 839 840 841 842 843 844 845 846 847 848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855 856 857 858 859