Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
Chronic benzylpiperazine (BZP) exposure produces behavioral sensitization and cross-sensitization to methamphetamine (MA) K. Brennan, A. Johnstone, P. Fitzmaurice, R. Lea, S. Schenk ∗ Institute of Environmental Science and Research (ESR), School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, P.O. Box 600, Wellington, New Zealand Received 3 August 2006; received in revised form 12 October 2006; accepted 23 October 2006
Abstract Background: Like other psychostimulant drugs, acute exposure to benzylpiperazine (BZP) increases dopaminergic neurotransmission, producing hyperactivity and stereotypy. The consequences of repeated BZP exposure have not however been investigated. The effects of acute and repeated BZP and methamphetamine (MA) exposure on locomotor activity and stereotypy were measured in order to determine whether there was sensitization and cross-sensitization between these two psychostimulant drugs. Methods: The effects of acute treatment with MA (0.0, 0.5, 1.0 and 2.0 mg/kg, intraperitoneal (IP)) or BZP (0.0, 5.0, 10.0, 20.0 and 40.0 mg/kg, IP) on locomotor activity and stereotypy were determined. Effects of repeated exposure were determined in other groups that received five daily injections of 2.0 mg/kg MA, 20.0 mg/kg BZP or vehicle. Following a 2-day withdrawal period, rats from each treatment group received either a low dose MA (0.5 mg/kg) or BZP (10.0 mg/kg). Results: MA and BZP produced dose-dependent hyperactivity and stereotypy. Repeated MA and BZP resulted in a potentiated locomotor but not stereotypy response. Following the withdrawal period, MA pretreated rats exhibited a sensitized locomotor and stereotypy response to the low dose MA and a conditioned response to saline. BZP pretreated rats also demonstrated a sensitized locomotor response to the low dose of BZP and MA. Conclusions: The present findings indicate that repeated exposure to BZP results in sensitization and cross-sensitization to MA. © 2006 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Methamphetamine; Benzylpiperazine; Hyperactivity; Stereotypy; Sensitization; Cross-sensitization
1. Introduction The use of amphetamine-type stimulants (ATS), mainly methamphetamine (MA), is reaching epidemic proportions (UNODC, 2006). MA users have exhibited psychoses (Murray, 1998; Yui et al., 1999), predisposition towards psychiatric illness (Sekine et al., 2003) and neurocognitive deficits (Volkow et al., 2001; Chang et al., 2002; Monterosso et al., 2005) that might reflect altered neurotransmission or neurotoxicity. Use of this addictive drug has been associated with social and cultural consequences, as well as numerous medical complications (Gibson et al., 2002; McCann and Ricaurte, 2004). It has been suggested that the illicit status and increased awareness of these health risks associated with MA use has led people to seek other alternatives (Wilkins et al., 2006). A stimulant-like compound, benzylpiperazine (BZP), is legal in countries, such as the United Kingdom and New Zealand (NZ) and a large-scale NZ survey ∗
Corresponding author. Tel.: +64 4 4636034; fax: +64 4 4635402. E-mail address:
[email protected] (S. Schenk).
0376-8716/$ – see front matter © 2006 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.10.016
revealed that 44% of regular drug users had opted to consume BZP to avoid the adverse health consequences associated with MA use (Wilkins et al., 2006). BZP is often combined with other piperazine derivatives and sold to consumers as ‘party pills’ or ‘P.E.P. pills’. The pills have been purported to ‘increase energy, vitality and mental capacity’ (Cuddy, 2004; Maurer et al., 2004; Wikstrom et al., 2004). Microdialysis experiments in laboratory rats revealed that BZP exposure increased synaptic overflow of both dopamine (DA) and serotonin (5-HT) (Baumann et al., 2005). Increases in dialysate levels of DA were dose-dependent and correlated with increases in locomotor activity, stereotypical movements and sniffing. In addition, BZP maintained self-administration in primates previously trained to self-administer cocaine and was indistinguishable from dexamphetamine in discrimination studies (Fantegrossi et al., 2005). Pharmacological blockade of DA D1-like receptors attenuated BZP-produced conditioned placepreference in rats (Meririnne et al., 2006). Thus, some of the acute effects of BZP can be attributed to effects on central dopaminergic substrates.
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
A consequence of repeated exposure to psychostimulants is a sensitized behavioral response. Sensitization has been considered a primary stage for drug addiction and some aspects of drug-induced psychoses (Robinson and Becker, 1986; Robinson and Berridge, 1993, 2001; Hyman, 1996). Indeed, repeated, intermittent exposure to MA resulted in sensitized behavioral (Fujiwara et al., 1987; Kitaichi et al., 2003; Kitanaka et al., 2003; Bevins and Peterson, 2004; Fujio et al., 2005; Shuto et al., 2006) and neurochemical (Hamamura et al., 1991; Camp et al., 1994) responses. Pretreatment with MA resulted in cross-sensitization to the behavioral effects of other drugs, including cocaine (Kazahaya et al., 1989; Akimoto et al., 1990; Hirabayashi et al., 1991; Davidson et al., 2005), nicotine (Kuribara, 1999) and a DAT inhibitor (Kawakami et al., 1998) and increased the effects of cocaine on dopaminergic neurotransmission (Kazahaya et al., 1989; Akimoto et al., 1990). Sensitization and crosssensitization have been attributed to neuroadaptations in central dopaminergic substrates (Kazahaya et al., 1989; Akimoto et al., 1990; Hamamura et al., 1991; Suzuki et al., 1997; Shuto et al., 2006). Due to the subjective (Campbell et al., 1973) and neurochemical (Baumann et al., 2005) similarities between BZP and amphetamines, repeated BZP-based party pill ingestion might produce dopaminergic neuroadaptations that sensitize users to other stimulant drugs. BZP could potentially function as a ‘gateway drug’, leading to the use of more harmful illicit drugs. The results from recent drug-use surveys substantiate these claims, as 13.5% of respondents indicated that they had ‘started out using legal party pills, but now mostly use other illegal drugs’ (Wilkins et al., 2006). BZP produces effects on dopaminergic substrates that are comparable to effects produced by other psychostimulant drugs including MA, but the consequences of repeated exposure have not been investigated. The facilitation of dopaminergic neurotransmission (Baumann et al., 2005; Meririnne et al., 2006) and locomotor and stereotypic effects produced following acute BZP administration (Baumann et al., 2005; Fantegrossi et al., 2005; Meririnne et al., 2006) would be expected to produce effects that are comparable to repeated exposure to other direct or indirect DA agonists. Since BZP and MA both exert predominant effects on dopaminergic systems, it was hypothesized that behavioral sensitization and cross-sensitization would occur following chronic BZP exposure. In the present study, the hyperactivity and stereotypic effects produced by BZP following repeated, intermittent administration was compared to effects produced by repeated exposure to MA. Since it was hypothesized that regular intake of BZP-based party pills might produce sensitization to MA, BZP pretreated rats were administered a MA challenge to observe whether cross-sensitization was evident.
205
controlled (21 ◦ C) and humidity-controlled (79%) room. The animals were housed in groups from weaning until 1 day before behavioral testing began. During testing periods, rats were housed singly until the completion of behavioral experiments. Food and water were freely available except during testing. The colony was maintained on a 12 h light:12 h dark cycle with lights on at 07:00 h. The Animal Ethics Committee (AEC) at Victoria University of Wellington New Zealand approved all protocols.
2.2. Apparatus Forward locomotion and stereotypy were assessed using the Activity Monitor Version 5 program (Med Associates Inc., US) consisting of eight sound attenuated chambers. The subject location was tracked using 16 evenly spaced infrared sources and sensors positioned around the periphery of a four-sided chamber (42 cm × 42 cm). The program simultaneously recorded and differentiated between ambulatory and stereotypy counts by dividing the chamber into zones, or ‘boxes’. It was possible to set the ‘box size’ and for these experiments, the size was set to be the approximate dimensions of a rat. When the rat was placed in the chamber, any small movements, such as head weaving, licking or gnawing made within a given box was counted as ‘stereotypy counts’. When the rat travelled distance and crossed the box perimeters into an adjacent box, this was registered as ambulatory counts. A white noise generator masked extraneous auditory disturbance during testing and the room was illuminated with red light. Prior to and after each behavioral test session, the chamber interiors were cleaned and wiped with Virkon ‘S’ disinfectant (Southern Veterinary Supplies, NZ).
2.3. Procedures 2.3.1. Effects of acute exposure to MA or BZP. The effects of acute treatment with MA or BZP on locomotor activity and stereotypy were determined in separate groups of drug-na¨ıve rats (n = 8 (MA) and n = 10 (BZP) group). The rats were first habituated to the locomotor chambers for 15 min after which they were injected with MA (0.0, 0.5, 1.0 and 2.0 mg/kg, IP) or BZP (0.0, 5.0, 10.0, 20.0 or 40.0 mg/kg, IP). Following drug administration activity was recorded at 5 min intervals for 75 min. 2.3.2. Effect of repeated MA exposure. The repeated exposure regimen selected for the present study was modelled after earlier protocols that produced behavioral sensitization (Fujiwara et al., 1987; Fujio et al., 2005). In the present study, groups of drug-na¨ıve rats received IP injection once daily for 5 consecutive days with 2.0 mg/kg MA or vehicle (n = 16 per group) and behavior was recorded as above. On Day 8, following a 2-day withdrawal period, equal numbers (n = 8) from each treatment group received either a 0.5 mg/kg MA challenge dose or vehicle prior to the final behavioral tests. A 0.5 mg/kg dose was selected because it failed to produce significant hyperactivity when administered acutely. 2.3.3. Effect of repeated BZP exposure. Effects of repeated exposure to BZP on the hyperactivity produced by BZP or MA were measured in other groups. The 20.0 mg/kg BZP dose was selected for repeated exposure because it produced hyperactivity that was comparable to levels produced by 2.0 mg/kg MA. Groups of drug-na¨ıve rats received either BZP (20.0 mg/kg) or vehicle during the 5-day pretreatment period (n = 24 per group). On Day 8, following a 2-day withdrawal period, equal numbers (n = 8) from each of the two treatment groups received 10.0 mg/kg BZP or 0.5 mg/kg MA or vehicle. The 10.0 mg/kg dose of BZP was selected because it failed to produce significant hyperactivity when administered acutely.
2. Methods
2.4. Drugs
2.1. Subjects
Methamphetamine hydrochloride (ESR, New Zealand) and benzylpiperazine monohydrochloride (ESR) were dissolved in a 0.9% saline vehicle. Injections were IP and administered in a volume of 1 ml/kg. Drug weights refer to the base.
Subjects were male Sprague–Dawley rats bred in the vivarium of Victoria University. Rats were housed in hanging polycarbonate cages in a temperature-
206
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
Fig. 1. Ambulatory and stereotypy counts produced by ascending acute doses of MA (0.0, 0.5, 1.0 or 2.0 mg/kg, IP). The time course for mean ambulatory (A) or stereotypy (B) counts produced by MA are shown (+S.E.M.). Insets show total ambulatory or stereotypy counts during the 75 min period post-MA injection (+S.E.M.). Significant difference relative to vehicle group, * p < 0.01.
Fig. 2. Ambulatory and stereotypy counts produced by ascending acute doses of BZP (0.0, 5.0, 10.0, 20.0 or 40.0 mg/kg, IP). The time course for mean ambulatory (A) or stereotypy (B) counts produced by BZP are shown (+S.E.M.). Insets show total ambulatory or stereotypy counts during the 75 min period post-BZP injection (+S.E.M.). Significant difference relative to vehicle group, * p < 0.01.
2.5. Statistical analyses Data from the dose effect studies were analysed using ANOVAs as described in Section 3. Univariate analyses followed by Tukey post hoc tests determined whether there were significant differences as a result of pretreatment.
(F(31,345) = 5.84, p < 0.001). Post hoc analyses revealed that the 20.0 and 40.0 mg/kg doses of BZP increased ambulatory and stereotypy counts relative to baseline (p < 0.05). 3.3. Effects of chronic MA exposure
3. Results 3.1. Dose–effect curve for MA-produced activity Fig. 1 shows the time course and dose-dependency of MAproduced hyperactivity (Fig. 1A) and stereotyped behavior (Fig. 1B). Time-by-dose interactions were significant for both ambulatory (F(19,180) = 4.22, p < 0.001) and stereotypy counts (F(24,221) = 4.39, p < 0.001). Post hoc analyses revealed that the 1.0 and 2.0 mg/kg doses of MA produced increased ambulatory and stereotypy counts compared to baseline (p < 0.05). 3.2. Dose–effect curve for BZP-produced activity Fig. 2 shows the time course and dose dependency of BZPproduced hyperactivity (Fig. 2A) and stereotyping behavior (Fig. 2B). Time-by-dose interactions were significant for both ambulatory (F(21,321) = 6.45, p < 0.001) and stereotypy counts
Fig. 3 shows the effects of repeated exposure to MA on ambulatory (Fig. 3A) and stereotypy (Fig. 3B) counts. ANOVAs with repeated measures across days (Day × MA Dose) revealed a significant interaction for ambulatory (F(3,94) = 7.63, p < 0.001), but not stereotypy counts (F(3,83) = 0.43, NS). There were differential effects of chronic treatment on MA (2.0 mg/kg)produced ambulatory and stereotypy counts, as within subject contrasts revealed that only ambulatory counts on Days 2–5 were significantly elevated relative to Day 1 (p < 0.01). The absence of potentiation in stereotypy counts across days (Fig. 3B) might reflect a ‘ceiling effect’. Fig. 4 shows the effects of repeated MA treatment on ambulatory (Fig. 4A) and stereotypy (Fig. 4B) counts produced by an acute dose of MA (0.0 or 0.5 mg/kg) administered following a 2-day withdrawal. As demonstrated in the previous experiment, the control rats showed minimal response to a 0.5 mg/kg dose MA. There was,
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
Fig. 3. Total daily MA (0.0 or 2.0 mg/kg, IP)-produced behavioral counts over the chronic treatment period. The mean total ambulatory (A) or stereotypy counts (B) for the 75 min period post-MA injection is shown for each of the five consecutive testing days (+S.E.M.). Significant difference relative to Day 1 groups, * p < 0.01.
however, a marked increase in ambulation (F(1,14) = 12.10, p < 0.01) and stereotypy (F(1,14) = 9.03, p < 0.01) to 0.5 mg/kg MA dose. MA pretreated rats also exhibited conditioned increases in ambulatory (F(1,14) = 7.51, p < 0.05), but not stereotypy (F(1,14) = 5.97, p > 0.5) counts in response to vehicle injection. Two-way ANOVAs (pretreatment × MA Dose) revealed significant interactions between pretreatment and MA dose for ambulatory (F(1,28) = 10.85, p < 0.01) and stereotypy (F(1,28) = 4.99, p < 0.05) counts for the 75 min post-injection period. There were main effects of pretreatment and MA dose on ambulatory (F(1,28) = 13.29, p < 0.01; F(1,28) = 16.39, p < 0.001) and stereotypy (F(1,28) = 12.63, p < 0.01; F(1,28) = 21.10, p < 0.001) counts. 3.4. Effects of chronic BZP exposure Fig. 5 shows the effects of repeated BZP (20.0 mg/kg) treatment on ambulatory (Fig. 5A) and stereotypy counts (Fig. 5B) during each of the five pretreatment days. Repeated measures ANOVAs (Day × BZP Dose) revealed a significant interaction for ambulatory (F(3,144) = 8.10, p < 0.001), but not stereotypy counts (F(3,135) = 1.827, NS). As with MA pretreatment, within subject contrasts revealed that total BZP (20.0 mg/kg)-produced
207
Fig. 4. Ambulatory and stereotypy counts produced by acute MA (0.0 or 0.5 mg/kg, IP) in MA (2.0 mg/kg) and vehicle pretreated groups. The time course for mean ambulatory (A) or stereotypy (B) counts for both pretreatment groups produced by acute MA challenge on Day 8 following a 2-day withdrawal period are shown (+S.E.M.). Insets show total ambulatory or stereotypy counts during the 75 min period post-MA injection (+S.E.M.). Significant difference relative to vehicle pretreated groups, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
ambulatory counts on Days 3–5 were significantly elevated relative to Day 1 (p < 0.01). Fig. 6 shows the effects of repeated BZP treatment on ambulatory (Fig. 6A) and stereotypy (Fig. 6B) counts produced by an acute dose of BZP (0.0 or 10.0 mg/kg) administered following a 2-day withdrawal. As was observed in the acute studies, the control rats showed minimal response to low dose BZP. There was, however, a marked increase in hyperactivity in BZP pretreated rats compared to controls (F(1,14) = 11.35, p < 0.01), with only moderate increase in stereotypy (F(1,14) = 5.84, p < 0.05). BZP pretreated rats exhibited some conditioned increases in ambulation and stereotypy in response to vehicle injection, but these differences were not significantly different to saline pretreated groups (F(1,14) = 1.20, NS; F(1,14) = 0.40, NS). Two-way ANOVAs (pretreatment × BZP Dose) failed to reveal significant interactions for ambulatory (F(1,28) = 3.57, NS) or stereotypy (F(1,28) = 0.09, NS) counts for the 75 min post-injection period. However, the time course data show that the acute response to BZP occurs in the 30 min postinjection period and does not persist for the entire 75 min test period, as with MA (Fig. 4). Therefore, the ambulatory and
208
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
Fig. 5. Total daily BZP (0.0 or 20.0 mg/kg, IP)-produced behavioral counts over the chronic treatment period. The mean total ambulatory (A) or stereotypy counts (B) for the 75 min period post-BZP injection is shown for each of the five consecutive testing days (+S.E.M.). Significant difference relative to Day 1 groups, * p < 0.01.
stereotypy totals for the 30 min post-injection period were computed and are shown as insets. Two-way ANOVAs revealed significant interactions between pretreatment and BZP dose for ambulatory (F(1,28) = 7.94 p < 0.01), but not stereotypy (F(1,28) = 2.80, NS) counts. The pretreatment interaction for stereotypy counts was not significant due to the relatively high response of the control rats to BZP. There were, however, main effects of pretreatment and BZP dose on both ambulatory (F(1,28) = 12.54, p > 0.01; F(1,28) = 11.66, p < 0.01) and stereotypy (F(1,28) = 5.60, p < 0.05; F(1,28) = 29.03, p < 0.001) counts. Fig. 7 shows the effects of repeated BZP treatment on ambulatory (Fig. 7A) and stereotypy (Fig. 7B) counts produced by an acute dose of MA (0.0 or 0.5 mg/kg) administered following a 2-day withdrawal. As was observed in the acute studies, the control rats showed a lesser response to low dose MA. The response to MA was almost identical to that exhibited following BZP. There was also a marked increase in MA-produced hyperactivity in BZP pretreated rats compared to controls (F(1,14) = 7.57, p < 0.05), with only moderate increases in stereotypy (F(1,14) = 6.00, p < 0.05). BZP pretreated rats exhibited some conditioned increases in ambulation and stereotypy in response to vehicle injection, but these were not signifi-
Fig. 6. Ambulatory and stereotypy counts produced by acute BZP (10.0 mg/kg) or vehicle in BZP (20.0 mg/kg) and vehicle pretreated groups. The time course for mean ambulatory (A) or stereotypy (B) counts for both pretreatment groups produced by acute BZP challenge on Day 8 following a 2-day withdrawal period are shown (+S.E.M.). Insets show total ambulatory or stereotypy counts during the 30 min period post-BZP injection (+S.E.M.). Significant difference relative to vehicle pretreated groups, p < 0.05.
cantly different from saline pretreated groups (F(1,14) = 1.20, NS; F(1,14) = 0.40, NS). To allow direct comparisons of these data with the BZP-produced behavioral responses (insets in Fig. 6), total ambulatory and stereotypy counts are shown 30 min post-MA injection. Two-way ANOVAs revealed significant interactions between pretreatment and MA dose (F(1,28) = 4.51 p < 0.05) for ambulatory counts, but not stereotypy counts (F(1,28) = 2.60, NS). There were main effects of pretreatment and MA dose on ambulatory (F(1,28) = 8.76, p > 0.01; F(1,28) = 19.73, p < 0.01) and stereotypy (F(1,28) = 5.53, p < 0.05; F(1,28) = 24.92, p < 0.001) counts. 4. Discussion MA and BZP produced dose-dependent hyperactivity (Figs.1A and 2A) and stereotypy (Figs.1B and 2B). Repeated exposure to both drugs produced sensitized responses that were more apparent in the hyperactivity (Figs.4A, 6A and 7A) measures and cross-sensitization between BZP and MA was also evident.
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
Fig. 7. Ambulatory and stereotypy counts produced by acute MA (0.5 mg/kg) or vehicle in BZP (20.0 mg/kg) and vehicle pretreated groups. The time course for mean ambulatory (A) or stereotypy (B) counts for both pretreatment groups produced by acute MA challenge on Day 8 following a 2-day withdrawal period are shown (+S.E.M.). Insets show total ambulatory or stereotypy counts during the 30 min period post-MA injection (+S.E.M.). Significant difference relative to vehicle pretreated groups, * p < 0.05.
These acute and sensitized responses might reflect dopaminergic mechanisms. A wealth of data has implicated DA in hyperactivity produced by acute exposure to a number of drugs (Kehne et al., 1996; Le et al., 1997; O’Neill and Shaw, 1999; Schindler and Carmona, 2002; Daniela et al., 2004) and microdialysis studies have demonstrated increased DA overflow that was correlated with drug-produced hyperactivity (Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988; Baumann et al., 2005) and stereotypy (Sharp et al., 1987; Cho et al., 1999; Golembiowska and Zylewska, 2000). Additionally, stimulant-produced hyperactivity (Le et al., 1997; O’Neill et al., 1999; Schindler and Carmona, 2002; Daniela et al., 2004) and stereotypy (Bordi and Meller, 1989; Conti et al., 1997) were attenuated by co-administration of a range of DA antagonists or neurotoxic 6-hydroxydopamine lesions (Roberts et al., 1975; Castall et al., 1977; Koob et al., 1981; Itoh et al., 1984). The acute responses produced by the selected dose range of MA (0.0, 0.5, 1.0 and 2.0 mg/kg) (Fig. 1) and BZP (0.0, 5.0, 10.0, 20.0 and 40.0 mg/kg) (Fig. 2) were similar in magnitude, where the potency of BZP to MA was approximately 10:1. The acute data also indicated that the potency of MA
209
in producing stereotypy was greater than its effects on hyperactivity. In contrast, BZP was equipotent in producing both behavioral outputs. Microdialysis studies have revealed that BZP and MA produced similar effects on monoamine release, with concomitant hyperactivity (Baumann et al., 2002, 2005). The present study conducted a more detailed behavioral analyses by including both hyperactivity and stereotypy, which have been attributed to differential mechanisms (Costall et al., 1975; Roberts et al., 1975; Kelly and Iversen, 1976; Sessions et al., 1980; Swerdlow et al., 1986; Sharp et al., 1987). Stereotypy was attributed to increases in nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurotransmission, whereas hyperactivity was attributed to activation of mesolimbic pathways (Roberts et al., 1975; Kelly and Iversen, 1976; Sessions et al., 1980; Swerdlow et al., 1986; Sharp et al., 1987). These differences between BZP and MA, therefore, indicate that BZP might not be merely a less potent drug than MA, but probably also produces differential neurochemical effects. Consistent with previous studies, the present study showed that MA pretreatment produced sensitization to the locomotor and stereotypy effects of a low dose challenge (Fig. 4) (Fujiwara et al., 1987; Kitaichi et al., 2003; Kitanaka et al., 2003; Bevins and Peterson, 2004; Fujio et al., 2005; Shuto et al., 2006). It has been suggested that the establishment of behavioral sensitization involves several neurochemical mechanisms, as opioid receptors (Chiu et al., 2005), glutamate (Ito et al., 2006) and central gamma amino butyric acid (GABA) (Scheel-Kruger, 1986; Ito et al., 1997; Puopolo et al., 2001; Li et al., 2005) systems were implicated. Additionally, increased plasma and brain MA concentrations evident in sensitized rats following acute MA challenge were attributed to altered pharmacokinetic mechanisms (Kitaichi et al., 2003; Nakagawa et al., 2003). Accumulation of non-metabolized MA in the brain has been attributed to delayed efflux (Nakagawa et al., 2003), decreased mRNA expression of a cation transporter (Kitaichi et al., 2003) and inhibition of hepatic cytochrome P-450 enzymes that metabolise MA (Yamamoto et al., 1988). Although it is likely that these factors do contribute to the expression of behavioral sensitization, total MA administered was 6-times (Kitaichi et al., 2003) and 11-times (Nakagawa et al., 2003) higher than those utilised in the present study. It was also unclear whether these pharmacokinetic changes would generalise to all other stimulant drugs, as sensitization to cocaine was not associated with altered plasma or brain levels (Bonate et al., 1997). The majority of evidence, however, supports the idea that changes in dopaminergic neurotransmission leads to potentiated MA- and other stimulant-produced behavior. Enhanced locomotor responses and stereotypy were associated with potentiated DA dialysate levels in both the striata and nucleus accumbi of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA)-pretreated (Kalivas et al., 1998), amphetamine-pretreated (Giorgi et al., 2005) and MA-pretreated (Hamamura et al., 1991; Camp et al., 1994) animals. These presynaptic effects might be related to the persistent structural modifications in DA output neurons in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex that were observed in sensitised animals (Robinson and Kolb, 1999). Post-synaptic adaptations were also implicated, as D1- and D2-like DA receptor subtypes had a role in both the acquisition and expression of
210
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
behavioral sensitization to MA (Hamamura et al., 1991; Yoshida et al., 1995; Suzuki et al., 1997; Shuto et al., 2006). In the present study, sensitization to the behavioral effects of MA was reflected in both hyperactivity and stereotypy (Fig. 4). The effects of repeated BZP treatment were restricted to sensitized hyperactivity (Figs.6A and 7A). It is of interest that MA pretreatment affected hyperactivity to a greater extent, thus both BZP and MA had less of an effect on stereotypy. Similar findings were reported following chronic ‘binge dose’ amphetamine treatment, where there were pronounced increases in locomotion but decreases in stereotypy (Kuczenski and Segal, 1997). Consistent with the idea that stereotypy and locomotor activity were attributable to different underlying mechanisms (Costall et al., 1975; Roberts et al., 1975; Kelly and Iversen, 1976; Sessions et al., 1980; Swerdlow et al., 1986; Sharp et al., 1987), extracellular DA release measured in the caudate putamen and nucleus accumbens during amphetamine treatment showed that the caudate response decreased with successive injections, whereas this was not observed in the nucleus accumbens. These differential neurochemical effects were thought to explain the differential behavioral responses to chronic amphetamine exposure. Chronic BZP and MA exposure might similarly impact mesocorticolimbic systems to a greater extent than nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurotransmission. However, the magnitude and duration of response in both ambulation (Fig. 4A) and stereotypy (Fig. 4B) to MA in MA pretreated rats was substantially greater than the response to BZP (Fig. 6) and MA (Fig. 7) in BZP preexposed groups. This might be explained by some differential mechanisms of action in acute responses and potency differences. MA pretreated rats exhibited conditioned hyperactivity and stereotypy when administered vehicle (Fig. 4), which is consistent with previous studies (Ohmori et al., 1995; Elmer et al., 1996; Itzhak, 1997). In contrast, significant conditioned hyperactivity was not observed in the BZP pretreated rats. These observations might be explained by the fact that the sensitized response following BZP pretreatment was relatively small in magnitude and short in duration compared to that produced by MA. Since chronic stimulant-induced changes in dopaminergic neurotransmission might be responsible for both sensitization and conditioning (Dietze and Kuschinsky, 1994), these behavioral consequences of exposure are possibly related. The conditioned response associated with MA pretreatment is likely to be associated to the sensitization affects of MA that was much more pronounced than for BZP, possibly due to greater effect on DA neural substrates. Evidence indicates that repeated stimulant exposure can produce sensitization in drug-reward systems (Schenk and Partridge, 1997). Indeed, chronic amphetamine, methylphenidate, cocaine or MDMA pretreatment facilitated the acquisition of stimulant self-administration (Woolverton et al., 1984; Horger et al., 1990, 1992; Valadez and Schenk, 1994; Pierre and Vezina, 1997, 1998; Fletcher et al., 2001; Schenk and Izenwasser, 2002) or conditioned place preference (Lett, 1989; Gaiardi et al., 1991; Shippenberg and Heidbreder, 1995; Shippenberg et al., 1996). Additionally, preexposure increased ‘break-point’ for self-administration reinforced on a progressive ratio schedule (Mendrek et al., 1998; Lorrain et
al., 2000). Insofar as the present data reflect sensitization in central DA substrates, they suggest that a similar effect might be occurring as a result of exposure to either MA of BZP. The results from the present study add further evidence to indicate that BZP has a comparable behavioral profile to MA and other drugs of abuse. MA is self-administered (Yokel and Pickens, 1973; Johanson et al., 1976; Shepard et al., 2006), produces behavioral sensitization (Fujiwara et al., 1987; Kitaichi et al., 2003; Kitanaka et al., 2003; Bevins and Peterson, 2004; Fujio et al., 2005; Shuto et al., 2006) and conditioned place preference (Cunningham and Noble, 1992; Suzuki et al., 1992). BZP is also self-administered (Fantegrossi et al., 2005), produces conditioned place preference (Meririnne et al., 2006) and behavioral sensitization (present results). These findings suggest that, although less potent than MA in the ability to produce sensitization, BZP-based ‘party pills’ might produce neuroadaptations that increase susceptibility towards stimulant abuse. Acknowledgements Funding was provided by Institute of Environmental Science and Research (ESR) and the authors greatly acknowledge the technical assistance of Richard Moore. References Akimoto, K., Hamamura, T., Kazahaya, Y., Akiyama, K., Otsuki, S., 1990. Enhanced extracellular dopamine level may be the fundamental neuropharmacological basis of cross-behavioral sensitization between methamphetamine and cocaine—an in vivo dialysis study in freely moving rats. Brain Res. 507, 344–346. Baumann, M.H., Ayestas, M.A., Sharpe, L.G., Lewis, D.B., Rice, K.C., Rothman, R.B., 2002. Persistent antagonism of methamphetamine-induced dopamine release in rats pretreated with GBR12909 decanoate. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 301, 1190–1197. Baumann, M.H., Clark, R.D., Budzynski, A.G., Partilla, J.S., Blough, B.E., Rothman, R.B., 2005. N-substituted piperazines abused by humans mimic the molecular mechanism of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, or ‘Ecstasy’). Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 550–560. Bevins, R.A., Peterson, J.L., 2004. Individual differences in rats’ reactivity to novelty and the unconditioned and conditioned locomotor effects of methamphetamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 79, 65–74. Bonate, P.L., Swann, A., Silverman, P.B., 1997. Behavioral sensitization to cocaine in the absence of altered brain cocaine levels. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 57, 665–669. Bordi, F., Meller, E., 1989. Enhanced behavioral stereotypies elicited by intrastriatal injection D1 and D2 dopamine agonists in intact rats. Brain Res. 504, 276–283. Camp, D.M., Browman, K.E., Robinson, T.E., 1994. The effects of methamphetamine and cocaine on motor behavior and extracellular dopamine in the ventral striatum of Lewis versus Fischer 344 rats. Brain Res. 668, 180– 193. Campbell, H., Cline, W., Evans, M., Lloyd, J., Peck, A.W., 1973. Comparison of the effects of dexamphetamine and 1-benzylpiperazine in former addicts. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 6, 170–176. Castall, B., Marsden, C.D., Naylor, R.J., Pycock, C.J., 1977. Stereotyped behaviour patterns and hyperactivity induced by amphetamine and apomorphine after discrete 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of extrapyramidal and mesolimbic nuclei. Brain Res. 123, 89–111. Chang, L., Ernst, T., Speck, O., Patel, H., DeSilva, M., Leonido-Yee, M., Miller, E.N., 2002. Perfusion MRI and computerized cognitive test abnormalities in abstinent methamphetamine users. Psychiatry Res. 114, 65–79.
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213 Chiu, C.T., Ma, T., Ho, I.K., 2005. Attenuation of methamphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization in mice by systemic administration of naltrexone. Brain Res. Bull. 67, 100–109. Cho, A.K., Melega, W.P., Kuczenski, R., Segal, D.S., Schmitz, D.A., 1999. Caudate-putamen dopamine and stereotypy response profiles after intravenous and subcutaneous amphetamine. Synapse 31, 125–133. Conti, L.H., Segal, D.S., Kuczenski, R., 1997. Maintenance of amphetamineinduced stereotypy and locomotion requires ongoing dopamine receptor activation. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 130, 183–188. Costall, B., Naylor, R.J., Neumeyer, J.L., 1975. Differences in the nature of the stereotyped behaviour induced by aporphine derivatives in the rat and in their actions in extrapyramidal and mesolimbic brain areas. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 31, 1–16. Cuddy, M.L., 2004. Common drugs of abuse—Part II. J. Pract. Nurs. 54 (5–8), 25–31, quiz 17–29. Cunningham, C.L., Noble, D., 1992. Methamphetamine-induced conditioned place preference or aversion depending on dose and presence of drug. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 654, 431–433. Daniela, E., Brennan, K., Gittings, D., Hely, L., Schenk, S., 2004. Effect of SCH 23390 on (+/−)-3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine hyperactivity and self-administration in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 77, 745–750. Davidson, C., Lee, T.H., Ellinwood, E.H., 2005. Acute and chronic continuous methamphetamine have different long-term behavioral and neurochemical consequences. Neurochem. Int. 46, 189–203. Di Chiara, G., Imperato, A., 1988. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 85, 5274–5278. Dietze, S., Kuschinsky, K., 1994. Effect of conditioning with d-amphetamine on the extracellular concentration of dopamine and its metabolites in the striatum of behaving rats. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 350, 22–27. Elmer, G.I., Gorelick, D.A., Goldberg, S.R., Rothman, R.B., 1996. Acute sensitivity vs. context-specific sensitization to cocaine as a function of genotype. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 53, 623–628. Fantegrossi, W.E., Winger, G., Woods, J.H., Woolverton, W.L., Coop, A., 2005. Reinforcing and discriminative stimulus effects of 1-benzylpiperazine and trifluoromethylphenylpiperazine in rhesus monkeys. Drug Alcohol Depend. 77, 161–168. Fletcher, P.J., Robinson, S.R., Slippoy, D.L., 2001. Pre-exposure to (+/−)3,4methylenedioxy-methamphetamine (MDMA) facilitates acquisition of intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, 195–203. Fujio, M., Nakagawa, T., Suzuki, Y., Satoh, M., Kaneko, S., 2005. Facilitative effect of a glutamate transporter inhibitor (2S,3S)-3-{3-[4(trifluoromethyl)benzoylamino]benzyloxy}aspartate on the expression of methamphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization in rats. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 99, 415–418. Fujiwara, Y., Kazahaya, Y., Nakashima, M., Sato, M., Otsuki, S., 1987. Behavioral sensitization to methamphetamine in the rat: an ontogenic study. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 91, 316–319. Gaiardi, M., Bartoletti, M., Bacchi, A., Gubellini, C., Costa, M., Babbini, M., 1991. Role of repeated exposure to morphine in determining its affective properties: place and taste conditioning studies in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 103, 183–186. Gibson, D.R., Leamon, M.H., Flynn, N., 2002. Epidemiology and public health Consequences of methamphetamine use in California’s Central Valley. J. Psychoactive Drugs 34, 313–319. Giorgi, O., Piras, G., Lecca, D., Corda, M.G., 2005. Differential activation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core and shell after acute or repeated amphetamine injections: a comparative study in the Roman highand low-avoidance rat lines. Neuroscience 135, 987–998. Golembiowska, K., Zylewska, A., 2000. Effect of adenosine kinase, adenosine deaminase and transport inhibitors on striatal dopamine and stereotypy after methamphetamine administration. Neuropharmacology 39, 2124– 2132. Hamamura, T., Akiyama, K., Akimoto, K., Kashihara, K., Okumura, K., Ujike, H., Otsuki, S., 1991. Co-administration of either a selective D1 or D2 dopamine antagonist with methamphetamine prevents methamphetamine-
211
induced behavioral sensitization and neurochemical change, studied by in vivo intracerebral dialysis. Brain Res. 546, 40–46. Hirabayashi, M., Okada, S., Tadokoro, S., 1991. Comparison of sensitization to ambulation-increasing effects of cocaine and methamphetamine after repeated administration in mice. J. Pharm. Pharmacol. 43, 827–830. Horger, B.A., Shelton, K., Schenk, S., 1990. Preexposure sensitizes rats to the rewarding effects of cocaine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 37, 707–711. Horger, B.A., Giles, M.K., Schenk, S., 1992. Preexposure to amphetamine and nicotine predisposes rats to self-administer a low dose of cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 107, 271–276. Hyman, S.E., 1996. Addiction to cocaine and amphetamine. Neuron 16, 901–904. Ito, K., Abekawa, T., Koyama, T., 2006. Valproate blocks high-dose methamphetamine-induced behavioral cross-sensitization to locomotioninducing effect of dizocilpine (MK-801), but not methamphetamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 186, 525–533. Ito, K., Ohmori, T., Abekawa, T., Koyama, T., 1997. Clonazepam prevents the development of sensitization to methamphetamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 58, 875–879. Itoh, K., Fukumori, R., Suzuki, Y., 1984. Effect of methamphetamine on the locomotor activity in the 6OHDA dorsal hippocampus lesioned rat. Life Sci. 34, 827–833. Itzhak, Y., 1997. Modulation of cocaine- and methamphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization by inhibition of brain nitric oxide synthase. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 282, 521–527. Johanson, C.E., Balster, R.L., Bonese, K., 1976. Self-administration of psychomotor stimulant drugs: the effects of unlimited access. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 4, 45–51. Kalivas, P.W., Duffy, P., White, S.R., 1998. MDMA elicits behavioral and neurochemical sensitization in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 18, 469– 479. Kawakami, Y., Suemaru, K., Araki, H., Kawasaki, H., Gomita, Y., Tanizaki, Y., 1998. Repeated mazindol and methamphetamine administration produces cross-sensitization to stereotyped behavior induced by these agents in rats. Acta Med. Okayama 52, 169–171. Kazahaya, Y., Akimoto, K., Otsuki, S., 1989. Subchronic methamphetamine treatment enhances methamphetamine- or cocaine-induced dopamine efflux in vivo. Biol. Psychiatry 25, 903–912. Kehne, J.H., Ketteler, H.J., McCloskey, T.C., Sullivan, C.K., Dudley, M.W., Schmidt, C.J., 1996. Effects of the selective 5-HT2A receptor antagonist MDL 100,907 on MDMA-induced locomotor stimulation in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 15, 116–124. Kelly, P.H., Iversen, S.D., 1976. Selective 6OHDA-induced destruction of mesolimbic dopamine neurons: abolition of psychostimulant-induced locomotor activity in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 40, 45–56. Kitaichi, K., Morishita, Y., Doi, Y., Ueyama, J., Matsushima, M., Zhao, Y.L., Takagi, K., Hasegawa, T., 2003. Increased plasma concentration and brain penetration of methamphetamine in behaviorally sensitized rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 464, 39–48. Kitanaka, N., Kitanaka, J., Takemura, M., 2003. Behavioral sensitization and alteration in monoamine metabolism in mice after single versus repeated methamphetamine administration. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 474, 63–70. Koob, G.F., Stinus, L., Le Moal, M., 1981. Hyperactivity and hypoactivity produced by lesions to the mesolimbic dopamine system. Behav. Brain Res. 3, 341–359. Kuczenski, R., Segal, D.S., 1997. An escalating dose/multiple high-dose binge pattern of amphetamine administration results in differential changes in the extracellular dopamine response profiles in caudate-putamen and nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 17, 4441–4447. Kuribara, H., 1999. Does nicotine modify the psychotoxic effect of methamphetamine? Assessment in terms of locomotor sensitization in mice. J. Toxicol. Sci. 24, 55–62. Le, A.D., Tomkins, D., Higgins, G., Quan, B., Sellers, E.M., 1997. Effects of 5-HT3, D1 and D2 receptor antagonists on ethanol- and cocaine-induced locomotion. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 57, 325–332. Lett, B.T., 1989. Repeated exposures intensify rather than diminish the rewarding effects of amphetamine, morphine, and cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 98, 357–362.
212
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213
Li, J.X., Han, R., Deng, Y.P., Chen, S.Q., Liang, J.H., 2005. Different effects of valproate on methamphetamine- and cocaine-induced behavioral sensitization in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 161, 125–132. Lorrain, D.S., Arnold, G.M., Vezina, P., 2000. Previous exposure to amphetamine increases incentive to obtain the drug: long-lasting effects revealed by the progressive ratio schedule. Behav. Brain Res. 107, 9–19. Maurer, H.H., Kraemer, T., Springer, D., Staack, R.F., 2004. Chemistry, pharmacology, toxicology, and hepatic metabolism of designer drugs of the amphetamine (ecstasy), piperazine, and pyrrolidinophenone types: a synopsis. Ther. Drug Monit. 26, 127–131. McCann, U.D., Ricaurte, G.A., 2004. Amphetamine neurotoxicity: accomplishments and remaining challenges. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 821– 826. Mendrek, A., Blaha, C.D., Phillips, A.G., 1998. Pre-exposure of rats to amphetamine sensitizes self-administration of this drug under a progressive ratio schedule. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 135, 416–422. Meririnne, E., Kajos, M., Kankaanpaa, A., Seppala, T., 2006. Rewarding properties of 1-benzylpiperazine, a new drug of abuse, in rats. Basic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 98, 346–350. Monterosso, J.R., Aron, A.R., Cordova, X., Xu, J., London, E.D., 2005. Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 79, 273–277. Murray, J.B., 1998. Psychophysiological aspects of amphetamine– methamphetamine abuse. J. Psychol. 132, 227–237. Nakagawa, N., Hishinuma, T., Nakamura, H., Yamazaki, T., Tsukamoto, H., Hiratsuka, M., Ido, T., Mizugaki, M., Terasaki, T., Goto, J., 2003. Brain and heart specific alteration of methamphetamine (MAP) distribution in MAPsensitized rat. Biol. Pharm. Bull. 26, 506–509. Ohmori, T., Abekawa, T., Koyama, T., 1995. Environment modifies the expression of behavioral sensitization produced by methamphetamine: behavioral and neurochemical studies. Behav. Pharmacol. 6, 133–142. O’Neill, M.F., Shaw, G., 1999. Comparison of dopamine receptor antagonists on hyperlocomotion induced by cocaine, amphetamine, MK-801 and the dopamine D1 agonist C-APB in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 145, 237–250. O’Neill, M.F., Heron-Maxwell, C.L., Shaw, G., 1999. 5-HT2 receptor antagonism reduces hyperactivity induced by amphetamine, cocaine, and MK-801 but not D1 agonist C-APB. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 63, 237–243. Pierre, P.J., Vezina, P., 1997. Predisposition to self-administer amphetamine: the contribution of response to novelty and prior exposure to the drug. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 129, 277–284. Pierre, P.J., Vezina, P., 1998. D1 dopamine receptor blockade prevents the facilitation of amphetamine self-administration induced by prior exposure to the drug. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 138, 159–166. Puopolo, M., Hochstetler, S.E., Gustincich, S., Wightman, R.M., Raviola, E., 2001. Extrasynaptic release of dopamine in a retinal neuron: activity dependence and transmitter modulation. Neuron 30, 211–225. Roberts, D.C., Zis, A.P., Fibiger, H.C., 1975. Ascending catecholamine pathways and amphetamine-induced locomotor activity: importance of dopamine and apparent non-involvement of norepinephrine. Brain Res. 93, 441– 454. Robinson, T.E., Becker, J.B., 1986. Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced by chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal models of amphetamine psychosis. Brain Res. 396, 157–198. Robinson, T.E., Berridge, K.C., 1993. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247–291. Robinson, T.E., Kolb, B., 1999. Alterations in the morphology of dendrites and dendritic spines in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex following repeated treatment with amphetamine or cocaine. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1598–1604. Robinson, T.E., Berridge, K.C., 2001. Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction 96, 103–114. Scheel-Kruger, J., 1986. Dopamine-GABA interactions: evidence that GABA transmits, modulates and mediates dopaminergic functions in the basal ganglia and the limbic system. Acta Neurol. Scand. Suppl. 107, 1–54. Schenk, S., Partridge, B., 1997. Sensitization and tolerance in psychostimulant self-administration. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 57, 543–550.
Schenk, S., Izenwasser, S., 2002. Pretreatment with methylphenidate sensitizes rats to the reinforcing effects of cocaine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 72, 651–657. Schindler, C.W., Carmona, G.N., 2002. Effects of dopamine agonists and antagonists on locomotor activity in male and female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 72, 857–863. Sekine, Y., Minabe, Y., Ouchi, Y., Takei, N., Iyo, M., Nakamura, K., Suzuki, K., Tsukada, H., Okada, H., Yoshikawa, E., Futatsubashi, M., Mori, N., 2003. Association of dopamine transporter loss in the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices with methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1699–1701. Sessions, G.R., Meyerhoff, J.L., Kant, G.J., Koob, G.F., 1980. Effects of lesions of the ventral medial tegmentum on locomotor activity, biogenic amines and responses to amphetamine in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 12, 603– 608. Sharp, T., Zetterstrom, T., Ljungberg, T., Ungerstedt, U., 1987. A direct comparison of amphetamine-induced behaviours and regional brain dopamine release in the rat using intracerebral dialysis. Brain Res. 401, 322–330. Shepard, J.D., Chuang, D.T., Shaham, Y., Morales, M., 2006. Effect of methamphetamine self-administration on tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine transporter levels in mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine pathways of the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 185, 505–513. Shippenberg, T.S., Heidbreder, C., 1995. Sensitization to the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine: pharmacological and temporal characteristics. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 273, 808–815. Shippenberg, T.S., Heidbreder, C., Lefevour, A., 1996. Sensitization to the conditioned rewarding effects of morphine: pharmacology and temporal characteristics. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 299, 33–39. Shuto, T., Kuroiwa, M., Hamamura, M., Yabuuchi, K., Shimazoe, T., Watanabe, S., Nishi, A., Yamamoto, T., 2006. Reversal of methamphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization by repeated administration of a dopamine D(1) receptor agonist. Neuropharmacology 50, 991–997. Suzuki, H., Shishido, T., Watanabe, Y., Abe, H., Shiragata, M., Honda, K., Horikoshi, R., Niwa, S., 1997. Changes of behavior and monoamine metabolites in the rat brain after repeated methamphetamine administration: effects of duration of repeated administration. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 21, 359–369. Suzuki, T., Shiozaki, Y., Masukawa, Y., Misawa, M., 1992. 5-HT3 receptor antagonists block cocaine- and methamphetamine-induced place preference. Yakubutsu Seishin Kodo 12, 33–38. Swerdlow, N.R., Vaccarino, F.J., Amalric, M., Koob, G.F., 1986. The neural substrates for the motor-activating properties of psychostimulants: a review of recent findings. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 25, 233–248. UNODC, 2006. United Nations Office of Drugs and Crime: World Drug Report. http://wwwunodcorg/unodc/world drug reporthtml (accessed 19/09/06). Valadez, A., Schenk, S., 1994. Persistence of the ability of amphetamine preexposure to facilitate acquisition of cocaine self-administration. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 47, 203–205. Volkow, N.D., Chang, L., Wang, G.J., Fowler, J.S., Leonido-Yee, M., Franceschi, D., Sedler, M.J., Gatley, S.J., Hitzemann, R., Ding, Y.S., Logan, J., Wong, C., Miller, E.N., 2001. Association of dopamine transporter reduction with psychomotor impairment in methamphetamine abusers. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 377–382. Wikstrom, M., Holmgren, P., Ahlner, J., 2004. A2 (N-benzylpiperazine) a new drug of abuse in Sweden. J. Anal. Toxicol. 28, 67–70. Wilkins, C., Girling, M., Sweetsur, P., Huckle, T., Huakau, J., 2006. National Household Survey. Legal Party Pill Use in New Zealand: Prevalence of Use, Availability Health Harms and ‘gateway effects’ of Benzylpiperazine (BZP) and Trifluorophenylmethylpiperazine (TFMPP)., http://www. shore.ac.nz/projects/Legal%20party%20pills%20in%20New%20Zealand break (accessed 19/09/06). Woolverton, W.L., Goldberg, L.I., Ginos, J.Z., 1984. Intravenous selfadministration of dopamine receptor agonists by rhesus monkeys. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 230, 678–683. Yamamoto, T., Takano, R., Egashira, T., Yamanaka, Y., 1988. Reversible inhibition of aromatic hydroxylation of methamphetamine in rat liver microsomal preparations pretreated with methamphetamine. Biochem. Pharmacol. 37, 1433–1437.
K. Brennan et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 88 (2007) 204–213 Yokel, R.A., Pickens, R., 1973. Self-administration of optical isomers of amphetamine and methylamphetamine by rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 187, 27–33. Yoshida, H., Ohno, M., Watanabe, S., 1995. Roles of dopamine D1 receptors in striatal fos protein induction associated with methamphetamine behavioral sensitization in rats. Brain Res. Bull. 38, 393–397.
213
Yui, K., Ishiguro, T., Goto, K., Ikemoto, S., Kamata, Y., 1999. Spontaneous recurrence of methampetamine psychosis: increased sensitivity to stress associated with noradrenergic hyperactivity and dopaminergic change. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 249, 103–111.