Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54
Cognitive & Behavioral Assessment
Cognitive impairment and decline in cognitively normal older adults with high amyloid-b: A meta-analysis Q5
Q1
Jenalle E. Bakera,b,*, Yen Ying Lima,c, Robert H. Pietrzakd,e, Jason Hassenstabf,g, Peter J. Snyderh,i, Colin L. Mastersa, Paul Maruffa,b,c a
The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Parkville, Victoria, Australia b CRC for Mental Health, Carlton South, Victoria, Australia c Cogstate Ltd., Melbourne, Victoria, Australia d U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs National Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Clinical Neurosciences Division, VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, CT, USA e Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA f Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA g Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA h Department of Neurology, Warren Alpert School of Medicine, Brown University, Providence, RI, USA i Department of Neurology, Rhode Island Hospital & Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, RI, USA
Abstract
Introduction: This meta-analysis aimed to characterize the nature and magnitude of amyloid (Ab)related cognitive impairment and decline in cognitively normal (CN) older individuals. Method: MEDLINE Ovid was searched from 2012 to June 2016 for studies reporting relationships between cerebrospinal fluid or positron emission tomography (PET) Ab levels and cognitive impairment (cross-sectional) and decline (longitudinal) in CN older adults. Neuropsychological data were classified into domains of episodic memory, executive function, working memory, processing speed, visuospatial function, semantic memory, and global cognition. Type of Ab measure, how Ab burden was analyzed, inclusion of control variables, and clinical criteria used to exclude participants, was considered as moderators. Random-effects models were used for analyses with effect sizes expressed as Cohen’s d. Results: A total of 37 studies met inclusion criteria contributing 30 cross-sectional (N 5 5005) and 14 longitudinal (N 5 2584) samples. Ab-related cognitive impairment was observed for global cognition (d 5 0.32), visuospatial function (d 5 0.25), processing speed (d 5 0.18), episodic memory, and executive function (both d’s 5 0.15), with decline observed for global cognition (d 5 0.30), semantic memory (d 5 0.28), visuospatial function (d 5 0.25), and episodic memory (d 5 0.24). Ab-related impairment was moderated by age, amyloid measure, type of analysis, and inclusion of control variables and decline moderated by amyloid measure, type of analysis, inclusion of control variables, and exclusion criteria used. Discussion: CN older adults with high Ab show a small general cognitive impairment and small to moderate decline in episodic memory, visuospatial function, semantic memory, and global cognition. Ó 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of the Alzheimer’s Association. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Keywords:
Cognition; Amyloid-beta; Impairment; Decline; Meta-analysis; Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease
1. Introduction The authors list no conflicts related to the material presented in this article. *Corresponding author. Tel.: 161 3 9664 1327; Fax: 161 3 9664 1301. E-mail address:
[email protected]
There is now consensus that in cognitively normal (CN) older adults, abnormal levels of amyloid-beta (Ab1) indicates that the pathophysiological process of Alzheimer’s disease
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2016.09.002 2352-8729/Ó 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of the Alzheimer’s Association. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109
2 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13
(AD) has begun, although it may still be up to 20 years before these individuals meet clinical criteria for dementia [1–4]. Neuroimaging and fluid biomarkers allow for in vivo measurement of Ab burden in older individuals using positron emission tomography (PET) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) sampling, respectively [5,6]. Studies using these techniques have shown that Ab burden increases with age, with approximately 10%–20% of CN older adults aged 60–70 years, 20%–30% of those aged 70–80 years, and 30%–40% of those aged 80–90 years being classified as Ab1 [4,7,8]. Despite their CN classification, prospective studies indicate that cognitive decline is faster and progression to a clinical diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or AD more rapid, in those who are Ab1 compared to matched CN adults with low Ab levels (Ab2) [2,3]. Characterizing preclinical AD is therefore important for understanding the pathogenesis of AD. Although PET Ab imaging or CSF sampling identifies reliably the presence of AD pathology in individuals with no overt symptoms, these procedures are expensive, invasive, and must occur in specialized medical centers. Ideally, sensitive and cost-effective clinical measures could be used to identify CN adults who should be referred for these more expensive and invasive testing procedures. Neuropsychological assessment may be useful in this regard, where the presence of a subtle but specific profile of cognitive dysfunction could indicate that Ab1 would be classified on CSF sampling or PET imaging. However, to date, there is no agreement on what constitutes subtle cognitive decline among individuals with Ab1. Evidence in support of a cognitive profile indicative of Ab1 comes from neuropsychological studies that use two types of experimental designs. First are studies that define Ab1-related cognitive impairment on the basis of the comparison of performance on batteries of neuropsychological tests between Ab1 CN older adults and Ab2 CN older adults at a single assessment. Such studies generally report only small and statistically nonsignificant differences in neuropsychological test performance between Ab2 and Ab1 CN older adults [9–13]. Second are studies that define Ab1-related cognitive decline by evaluating changes in performance on neuropsychological test batteries over time between Ab1 and Ab2 CN older adults. Studies using this approach have consistently found evidence of Ab1-related decline on measures of episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, visuospatial function, and language (e.g., [9,11,14–21]). However, although individual studies have identified areas of cognitive impairment and decline associated with Ab1, there is substantial variation between these studies in terms of the sample sizes enrolled, the domains of cognitive function assessed, the specific neuropsychological or cognitive tests used to measure these domains, and the statistical techniques used to compare Ab1 and Ab2 groups. Furthermore, in many studies, conclusions about the effects of Ab1 on cognition have been based only on the presence or absence of statistical significance. Consequently, small but important Ab1-related effects may have been missed when sample
sizes did not provide adequate statistical power to render such differences statistically significant. Meta-analyses of the existing literature on Ab1-related cognitive impairment, and decline could therefore provide an effective method for overcoming the different limitations of individual studies to provide reliable estimates of Ab1-related cognitive impairment and decline in preclinical AD. To date, one meta-analysis evaluated this question and concluded that associations with Ab burden were strongest for episodic memory (e.g., r 5 20.12; Hedden et al., 2013 [16]). Additionally, when combining estimates across studies that measured Ab using CSF, PET, plasma, and histopathologic methods, lower performance in executive function was also related significantly (r 5 0.08) to Ab burden. Post hoc analyses indicated that estimates of Ab1-related cognitive dysfunction were unaffected by the experimental design used, the method of determining Ab levels, or whether demographic or clinical variables were controlled statistically. Although this initial meta-analysis provides a good basis for understanding the effects of Ab on cognition in CN older adults, its conclusions are limited because a large number of studies investigating relationships between Ab1 and neuropsychological test performance have been conducted since its publication. Second, the number of studies using either cross-sectional or longitudinal designs is now sufficient to consider estimates of cognitive impairment and cognitive decline separately. Third, a broader sample of cognitive domains is now available for inclusion in meta-analyses. Finally, samples in studies using longitudinal designs have been followed for longer periods. As such, an updated metaanalysis of this literature is needed to understand the relation between Ab and cognitive impairment and decline in preclinical AD. The aim of this study was therefore to systematically review the literature on the nature and magnitude of Ab1related cognitive impairment and decline in older adults who do not meet clinical criteria for MCI or dementia.
2. Methods 2.1. Study selection 2.1.1. Inclusion/exclusion criteria Inclusion criteria for the meta-analyses were that (a) the study must include a sample of adults with an average age 60 years who did not meet clinical criteria for MCI or dementia and who had undergone assessment with standardized neuropsychological tests; (b) for each participant, Ab levels were determined using PET or CSF sampling; and (c) studies must have provided sufficient information to allow for the computation of effect sizes. Studies were excluded from the meta-analysis if they were one of a series of publications from the same specific cohort where, over time, sample sizes or the length of follow-up had increased. For studies meeting this criterion, data for the meta-analyses were taken from that study which was the
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310
most recent and which had the largest sample. If possible, data for any cognitive domain not reported in the chosen publication were derived from another publication from the same cohort with the next largest sample where relevant data were presented, so as to ensure as many cognitive domains as possible were represented for each cohort. Second, studies were excluded if neuropsychological data or clinical data had been used to classify CN adults “progressors/decliners” or “stable/nondecliners” independent of Ab classification. Finally, for studies that reported results from both PET and CSF sampling, we focused on PET results as indicators of Ab levels to maintain consistency across studies. 2.2. Systematic review methods Supplementary Fig. A summarizes the outcome of the systematic review process. Initially, all studies included in the meta-analysis of Hedden et al. (2013) [16] were screened via title and abstract, to determine relevance to inclusion and exclusion criteria of the current meta-analysis. Fifteen articles were excluded at this stage. A systematic electronic database search was then conducted on Medline Ovid, on the 8th of June 2016, using the subject terms for amyloid, older adults and AD, and cognition that were used in the meta-analysis by Hedden et al. (2013) [16]. The terms are described below: 1. (amyloid) AND (“Pittsburgh Compound B” OR PIB OR florbetapir OR AV-45 OR florbetaben OR flutemetamol OR PET) OR (CSF) 2. (normal OR nondemented OR aging OR older OR Alzheimer’s OR dementia OR “cognitive impairment” OR MCI) 3. (cognitive OR cognition OR memory OR executive OR speed OR visuospatial OR semantic) At this stage, 501 articles were identified to be screened via title and abstract. After removal of duplicates and review articles, 362 articles were screened via title and abstract, with 335 being excluded at this stage (Supplementary Fig. A). After this, full-text screening began of the articles identified from the Hedden et al. analysis and from those identified in the electronic search. Reference lists of articles were also screened for key citations, and newly published articles were screened for relevance. Fifty studies were excluded at this stage (see Supplementary Fig. A for details), leaving 69 studies meeting the full criteria. The largest samples from ongoing observational studies were chosen to represent the data for cognitive domains from that cohort. This process was completed separately for studies using cross-sectional or longitudinal designs. At completion, 38 studies met inclusion/exclusion criteria, consisting of 30 that used crosssectional designs and 14 that used longitudinal designs.
3
cognitive domain measured by that test according to reference frameworks from previous meta-analyses of cognition [16,22,23] and standard neuropsychological compendia [24]. The cognitive domains used and the tests classified into each of those domains are summarized in Supplementary Table A. 2.4. Statistical analysis Separate analyses were conducted for studies using crosssectional and longitudinal designs using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis, version 3.3 software (Biostat, NJ). All results are reported using a random-effects model. Effect sizes were calculated using reported statistics such as means and standard deviations (Cohen’s d) and results from analyses such as t tests, correlations, regressions (r), and linear mixedeffects models. If standard error was reported in a study, this was converted to standard deviation before the calculation of effect sizes. For each neuropsychological test or cognitive domain composite score, the sign of effect sizes was adjusted so that negative effect sizes reflected greater Ab1related impairment or decline. For studies that used more than one neuropsychological test to measure the same cognitive domain, effect sizes were averaged. All effects were weighted using inverse variance weighting based on sample size. All effect sizes were transformed into Cohen’s d, and the magnitudes were classified as small, medium, or large according to Cohen (1992) [25]. A more lenient criterion was used to assess statistical significance of heterogeneity (P , .10) due to the lack of statistical power of these tests [26]. Where statistically significant heterogeneity was identified, post hoc subgroup analyses were conducted to seek the source of this heterogeneity [27]. These analyses investigated the extent to which heterogeneity in estimated mean effect sizes arose from variance due to moderators such as: (a) the type of Ab measure (PET or CSF); (b) whether Ab burden was defined as a continuous or categorical measure; (c) whether the study had controlled statistically the effects of demographic or clinical covariates (e.g., age, education, and so forth); and (d) the clinical classification criteria used to exclude participants from CN samples (i.e. the criteria for prodromal AD or MCI used). Where group mean effect sizes were based on 10 or more samples and there was no evidence of heterogeneity, publication bias was assessed by visual inspection of funnel plots and the Egger test, with the significance level set at P , .10 [28,29]. This was done to assess whether the effect sizes of individual studies were distributed symmetrically around the overall mean effect size for the cognitive domain, or whether study size biased the results in a particular direction. 3. Results
2.3. Classification of outcome measures
3.1. Meta-analysis of studies using cross-sectional designs to measure cognitive impairment
For each study, performance data from neuropsychological tests were identified and organized according to the main
Study characteristics and individual effect sizes for studies that used cross-sectional designs are presented in
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377
4
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13
378 (Table 1). Stratifying the studies by participants’ mean age Supplementary Table B. Estimates of effect sizes from cross379 showed that effect sizes for episodic memory and executive sectional studies were based on 30 samples providing 5005 380 function were significant only for studies using samples aged participants with study samples ranging between 23 and 381 75 years and more (P’s 5 .12 and .07 for the younger groups 564 participants. Of these, 2524 were females (50%), 1304 382 respectively), whereas visuospatial function was significant 383 Q2 carried at least one APOEε4 allele (26%), with the average 384 in the younger group only (P 5 .06 for older group). Statisages of the individual samples ranging from 60.5 to 79 years. 385 tically significant heterogeneity was detected for pooled esNineteen studies contributed data for episodic memory 386 timates of effect sizes for episodic memory. No evidence of (n 5 2886), 13 for executive function (n 5 2281), 10 for 387 publication bias was found for the remaining domains. working memory (n 5 1598), 13 for processing speed 388 (n 5 2530), nine for visuospatial function (n 5 1984), 13 389 390 for semantic memory (n 5 2585), and eight for global cogni3.3. Moderator effects on studies of cognitive impairment 391 tion (n 5 1746). 392 Table 2 shows the results of the post hoc analyses for For the majority of studies, Ab levels were determined 393 episodic memory, where significant heterogeneity was deusing PET neuroimaging (87%) with over half (63%) classi394 tected. Group mean effect sizes remained statistically signiffying Ab levels categorically (e.g., positive vs. negative or 395 icant when these accounted for data from studies that had 396 high vs. low). Demographic and clinical variables such as used PET to measure Ab levels, classified Ab as categorical, 397 age, premorbid IQ, sex, education, and APOEε4 were 398 and had controlled statistically for demographic or clinical controlled statistically in 53% of studies. The two main clin399 variables. ical criteria used to exclude participants with MCI/prodro400 mal AD were the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR; [30]) 401 3.4. Meta-analysis of studies using longitudinal designs to 402 scale total score of 0.5 (53%) and the Petersen criteria measure cognitive decline 403 [31]. Four studies [32–35] used alternative criteria, such as 404 the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale ([36]), the Jak and Bondi Study characteristics and individual effect sizes from 405 method [37,38], and a combination of CDR 0.5 and Petersen studies using longitudinal designs included in the analysis 406 criteria. For 77% of studies, the average age of participants 407 are shown in Supplementary Table C. Estimates of effects 408 was under 75 years. from studies using longitudinal designs were based on 14 409 samples providing 2584 participants, with total samples 410 ranging from 38 to 464 CN older adults. The sample 411 3.2. Mean domain specific effect sizes of cognitive included 1295 females (50%), and 743 APOEε4 carriers 412 impairment 413 (29%), with the average age at baseline of samples ranging 414 Mean effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and associated 95% confibetween 60.5 and 78.2 years. The length of follow-up for 415 dence intervals are listed in Table 1. Fig. 1 shows the forest studies ranged between 18 months and 23 years, approxi416 plots for each cognitive domain with individual study effect mate average of 5 years. Nine studies contributed data for 417 sizes and 95% confidence intervals shown as well as the episodic memory (n 5 1781), five for executive function 418 419 mean effect size for each domain. Worse performance in (n 5 1074), three for working memory (n 5 740), six for 420 the presence of Ab1 was evident for all cognitive domains, processing speed (n 5 1568), five for visuospatial function 421 with the magnitude of effects, by convention, small. Ab1(n 5 1243), seven for semantic memory (n 5 1653), and 422 related cognitive impairment was statistically significant seven for global cognition (n 5 1396). 423 for the domains of global cognition, visuospatial function, Ab analysis was conducted via PET neuroimaging in all 424 425 processing speed, executive function, and episodic memory but one study, with 57% of studies using a categorical 426 427 Table 1 428 Summary of pooled effect sizes and heterogeneity statistics from the meta-analyses 429 430 Cross-sectional design Longitudinal design 431 Cognitive domain n Cohen’s d (95% CI) n Cohen’s d (95% CI) 432 433 y 1781 20.24 (20.44 to 20.03)*y Episodic memory 2886 20.15 (20.27 to 20.03)* 434 Executive function 2281 20.15 (20.26 to 20.05)** 1074 20.04 (20.31 to 0.22) 435 Working memory 1598 20.11 (20.24 to 0.01) 740 20.26 (20.65 to 20.13)y 436 Processing speed 2530 20.18 (20.29 to 20.07)*** 1568 20.18 (20.38 to 0.02)y 437 Visuospatial 1984 20.25 (20.35 to 20.15)*** 1243 20.25 (20.42 to 20.09)** 438 Semantic memory 2585 20.06 (20.18 to 0.06) 1653 20.28 (20.42 to 20.15)*** 439 Global cognition 1746 20.32 (20.47 to 20.17)*** 1396 20.30 (20.48 to 20.11)**y 440 441 NOTE. Cohen’s d represents the standardized difference in means between the amyloid positive and amyloid negative groups, where greater impairment is 442 represented by a negative effect. 443 *P , .05; **P , .01; ***P , .001. y 444 significant heterogeneity P . .05. REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13 Executive function
Episodic memory
Working memory
Amariglio et al. 2012
Amariglio et al. 2012
Doherty et al. 2015
Doherty et al. 2015
Edmonds et al. 2015
Hassenstab et al. 2016a
Hassenstab et al. 2016a Hassenstab et al. 2016b
Hassenstab et al. 2016b
Kantarci et al. 2012
Hollands et al. 2015
Lim et al. 2015
Lim et al. 2015
Lim et al. 2013
Mielke et al. 2016
Loewenstein et al. 2016 Mielke et al. 2016
Nebes et al. 2013
Nebes et al. 2013
Ossenkoppele et al. 2014
Ossenkoppele et al. 2014
Rentz et al. 2010
Rentz et al. 2010
Rolstad et al. 2011
van Harten et al. 2013
Edmonds et al. 2015
Amariglio et al. 2012 Doherty et al. 2015
Hedden et al. 2012
Hassenstab et al. 2016a
2
Edmonds et al. 2015 Hassenstab et al. 2016b
2
Semantic memory Doherty et al. 2015
Aizenstein et al. 2008
Hassenstab et al. 2016a
1
Visuospatial
Processing speed
1
Cohen's d
Cohen's d
Aschenbrenner et al. 2015
0
-1
2
1
0
-1
-2
2
1
0
Cohen's d
-2
Overall
Overall
-1
Hassenstab et al. 2016a Hassenstab et al. 2016b Lim et al. 2013
Lim et al. 2013
Loewenstein et al. 2016 Hassenstab et al. 2016b
Mielke et al. 2016
Mielke et al. 2016
Lim et al. 2013
Loewenstein et al. 2016 Nebes et al. 2013
Ossenkoppele et al. 2014 Papp et al. 2015
Loewenstein et al. 2016
Oh et al. 2016
Petersen et al. 2015
Petersen et al. 2015
Mielke et al. 2016
Rentz et al. 2010
Petersen et al. 2015
Sperling et al. 2013
Rentz et al. 2010 Rolstad et al. 2011 Sperling et al. 2013
Rentz et al. 2010
van Harten et al. 2013
van Harten et al. 2013
Cohen's d
0
-1
2
1
0
Overall -1
2
1
0
-1
-2
Overall
Overall
-2
-2
Aizenstein et al. 2008 Aschenbrenner et al. 2015 Barthel et al. 2011 Doherty et al. 2015 Edmonds et al. 2015 Gidicsin et al. 2015 Lim et al. 2015 Lim et al. 2013 Loewenstein et al. 2016 Mielke et al. 2016 Oh et al. 2016 Ossenkoppele et al. 2014 Petersen et al. 2015 Rentz et al. 2010 Sojkova et al. 2011 Sperling et al. 2013 Stomrud et al. 2010 van Harten et al. 2013 Wang et al. 2015 Overall
-2
Cohen's d
Cohen's d
Global cognition Landau et al. 2012 Mielke et al. 2016 Petersen et al. 2015 Pike et al. 2011 Sperling et al. 2013 Stomrud et al. 2010 van Harten et al. 2013 Xiong et al. 2016
2
1
0
-1
Overall
-2
512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578
5
Cohen's d
Fig. 1. Forest plots from the meta-analysis of studies with cross-sectional designs. Effect sizes are presented as Cohen’s d with 95% confidence intervals. The dotted lines represent no effect of amyloid on cognition. Negative values represent greater impairment in performance in the presence of high Ab. The size of the dots represents study weighting due to sample size.
classification for Ab1. Demographic and clinical variables such as baseline age and test scores, education, sex, IQ, and family history were modeled as covariates in 79% of the studies. Exclusion of participants with prodromal AD or MCI at the baseline assessment was again split between the CDR (57%) and Petersen criteria. Approximately 71% of studies used participants with average ages ,75 years.
results. Statistically significant heterogeneity was evident for the domains of episodic memory, working memory, processing speed, and global cognition (Table 1). Publication bias was unable to be assessed due to the small number of studies contributing to meta-analysis of studies using longitudinal designs [29]. 3.6. Moderator effects on studies of cognitive decline
3.5. Mean domain specific effect sizes of cognitive decline Mean effect sizes and associated 95% confidence intervals are shown in Table 1. Fig. 2 shows the forest plots representing the individual study effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals, as well as the mean effect size for each cognitive domain. Decreased performance in the presence of Ab1 was evident for all cognitive domains with the magnitude of this decline small to moderate and statistically significant for episodic memory, visuospatial function, semantic memory, and global cognition. Stratifying the samples by participants’ mean age did not change these
Results of the moderator analysis of studies with longitudinal designs are listed in Table 2. Owing to the small number of studies available for the longitudinal meta-analysis, the moderator comparisons are limited; in many cases, only one study provided data for a particular subset (e.g., only one study used CSF [39]) and only three studies contributed effect sizes toward the working memory domain [40–42]; hence, post hoc analyses were conducted only where there were sufficient samples. For episodic memory, group mean effect sizes remained statistically significant when these accounted for data from studies that
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645
6 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707 708 709 710 711 712
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13
Table 2 Pooled effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals adjusted for moderator variables Amyloid measure Cognitive domain Cross-sectional Episodic memory Longitudinal Episodic memory Processing speed Global cognition
Type of amyloid analysis
CSF
PET
Continuous
Categorical
20.07 (20.34 to 0.20)
20.17 (20.31 to 20.03)*
20.11 (20.33 to 0.12)
20.16 (20.31 to 20.02)*
— — —
20.27 (20.50 to 20.02)* — 20.31 (20.51 to 20.10)**
20.04 (20.24 to 0.16) 20.02 (20.33 to 0.29) 20.16 (20.37 to 0.05)
20.37 (20.55 to 20.18)***y 20.28 (20.52 to 20.03)* 20.43 (20.67 to 20.19)***
NOTE. Effect sizes presented are Cohen’s d, with associated 95% confidence intervals. Blank spaces represent only one study available for moderator in the domain. Control variables included clinical and demographic variables that were included as covariates in the analyses used to calculate effect sizes. *P , .05; **P , .01; ***P , .001. ygroup difference P , .05.
had classified Ab as a categorical measure and had controlled statistically for the effects of demographic and clinical characteristics. For processing speed, effect sizes remained statistically significant when these accounted for data from studies that had classified Ab as a categorical measure. For global cognition, group mean effect sizes remained statistically significant when these accounted for data from studies that had classified Ab levels categorically, and had used the CDR criteria to exclude prodromal AD or MCI at baseline. 4. Discussion The results of this meta-analysis indicate that, in CN older adults, reliable Ab1 related cognitive impairment occurs in the domains of episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, visuospatial function, and global cognition. However, for each of these cognitive domains, impairment was by convention [25], small in magnitude with effect sizes ranging from d 5 0.15 for episodic memory to d 5 0.25 for visuospatial function. Interestingly, the largest effect size observed was for measures of global cognition, computed within studies by combining data across their neuropsychological tests measuring multiple cognitive domains. Nevertheless, even with data from all tests combined in this way, the magnitude of Ab1-related impairment remained only small (d 5 0.32). No statistically significant evidence of impairment was observed for the domains of working or semantic memory and in both cases, the magnitude of impairment was smaller than that observed for the other cognitive domains (d’s , 0.12). As these estimates of cognitive impairment were based on data from 5005 subjects from 30 studies, we believe these effect sizes provide an accurate estimation of the nature and magnitude of cognitive impairment in Ab1 CN older adults. Ab1-related cognitive decline was evident in the domains of episodic memory, semantic memory, visuospatial function, and global cognition, with the magnitude of decline in these cognitive domains ranging from d 5 0.24 for episodic memory to d 5 0.30 for global cognition. Again,
the effect size for decline in global cognitive function was significant and small in magnitude (d 5 0.30). No Ab1related decline was observed for working memory, processing speed, or executive function. Given the greater resources needed to conduct prospective studies, these estimates of cognitive decline in Ab1 CN older adults were based on fewer data points than those for cognitive impairment. However, as data were obtained for 2584 subjects from 14 studies encompassing an average of approximately five years of follow-up that satisfied inclusion/exclusion criteria, therefore these estimates of cognitive decline in Ab1 CN older adults are reliable. When considered together, data from both the cross-sectional and longitudinal meta-analyses suggest that in CN older adults, Ab1 is associated with both subtle cognitive impairment and decline. There was no evidence for a specific profile of cognitive impairment associated with Ab1. In fact, measures of global cognitive function, which typically combined multiple cognitive domains, provided the largest effect sizes. In contrast, analyses of longitudinal studies indicated that Ab1 related cognitive decline manifests predominantly and moderately in episodic and semantic memory. While stratifying the samples by mean age moderated cognitive impairment in episodic memory and executive function, mean age did not influence the estimates of cognitive decline for any domain. These effects will be considered in turn. 4.1. Ab1-related cognitive impairment The results show that Ab1 is associated with moderate but nonspecific levels of cognitive impairment in CN older adults. The pattern of neuropsychological impairments detected suggests that in CN older adults, Ab1 was associated with small impairments across measures of episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, and visuospatial function. It was not associated with impairment in working memory or semantic fluency. This general decrease in attention, executive function, and memory, with relative sparing of verbal fluency is consistent with the observation that the Ab1 related impairment with the greatest magnitude was
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 726 727 728 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 739 740 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13 780 781 782 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793 794 795 796 797 798 799 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826 827 828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 837 838 839 840 841 842 843 844 845 846
Control variables
7
Normal criteria
Yes
No
CDR 5 0
Petersen
20.21 (20.39 to 20.03)*
20.10 (20.25 to 0.06)
20.16 (20.36 to 0.03)
20.15 (20.34 to 0.05)
20.29 (20.53 to 20.05)* 2 20.31 (20.51 to 20.10)**
20.03 (20.53 to 0.48) — —
20.28 (20.59 to 0.03) 20.18 (20.49 to 0.13) 20.38 (20.68 to 20.08)*
20.20 (20.48 to 20.07) 20.20 (20.51 to 0.11) 20.25 (20.57 to 0.07)
for global cognition, a score which in most studies was computed by combining performance across all the neuropsychological tests used in that study. The presence of this subtle and general cognitive impairment in Ab1 CN older adults indicates that even very early in the course of AD, increased deposition of Ab is related to disrupted cognitive function, although this effect is small. Estimates of Ab1 cognitive impairment from this metaanalysis suggest that the neuropsychological tests used to date in studies of preclinical AD are unlikely to be useful for identifying those CN older adults who would have abnormal Ab levels if they underwent PET or CSF assessment. For example, with a Cohen’s d of 0.30 (as observed for global cognition), 88% of the scores for Ab1 and Ab2 individuals would overlap [43]. Thus, there would be a 42% chance of picking an Ab1 individual at random that would have a higher score on the task than a similarly chosen Ab2 individual. Clearly, the sensitivity of the neuropsychological tests analyzed here is insufficient to warrant their use in distinguishing Ab1 CN older adults from Ab2 CN older adults. Zakzanis (2001) [38] suggested that the criteria for a useful clinical marker in neuropsychological disorders would be a Cohen’s d of 3 or above. This corresponds to an overlap of just 13% and ,2% chance of selecting at random an Ab1 individual that would score higher on the task than a similarly chosen Ab2 individual. Therefore, while the results from our meta-analysis confirm that Ab1 does manifest as cognitive impairment early in AD, the magnitude of this impairment is subtle and nonspecific and would therefore be unlikely to be useful clinically. The analyses indicated heterogeneity between studies that provided estimates of episodic impairment in Ab1 CN older adults. Post hoc analyses of this heterogeneity suggested it was due to methodological variation between studies. Indeed, the estimates of impairment in this domain increased when looking at subsets of studies based on moderator variables. The finding that studies using PET had greater memory impairment than those using CSF to measure Ab might be because CSF Ab42 reaches abnormal levels earlier than PiB-PET [45], and therefore,
Ab1 determined from PET occurs in those with more advanced disease. Similarly, the increased impairment in the subset of studies using CDR criteria could reflect the inclusion of individuals who would have otherwise been classified as MCI. Finally, the reduction of variance, or noise, through controlling for extraneous variables or using categorical classifications of Ab could be why an increase in impairment was seen in these subsets of studies. 4.2. Ab1-related cognitive decline The results show that Ab was associated with significant decline in the domains of episodic memory, semantic memory, visuospatial function, and global cognition. Although the magnitude of decline for each domain was small, the effect of Ab extended beyond the domain of episodic memory, and this is important for understanding the nature of cognitive change in early AD. Unexpectedly, effect sizes for Ab1-related decline in episodic memory were smaller than those in visuospatial function and semantic memory. Research from our group and others has continually identified decline in episodic memory as being central to clinical progression in early AD [18,46,47]. Furthermore, dysfunction in episodic memory has been associated with loss of hippocampal volume, in accord with models of the structural brain changes that occur early in AD [2,48–50] and with brain-behavior models of episodic memory [51,52]. The reliable decline observed here for semantic memory and visuospatial function is also consistent with the involvement of medial temporal lobe structures [53]. Consistent with the specificity of Ab1-related cognitive decline to higher cognitive functions dependent on medial temporal lobe structures was the absence of any decline in processing speed. Neuropsychological models suggest that processing speed reflects lower order or more basic cognitive functions that subserve higher processes such as working memory, executive function, and episodic memory [54]. However, this raises an issue of why Ab1 was not associated with decline in executive function, given this is dependent on the integrity of the frontal lobe [54]. One possible explanation
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
847 848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855 856 857 858 859 860 861 862 863 864 865 866 867 868 869 870 871 872 873 874 875 876 877 878 879 880 881 882 883 884 885 886 887 888 889 890 891 892 893 894 895 896 897 898 899 900 901 902 903 904 905 906 907 908 909 910 911 912 913
Episodic memory
Semantic Memory
Cohen's d
Cohen's d
Executive function
Visuospatial
Mielke et al. 2016
Global cognition
Processing speed
Mielke et al. 2016
Mielke et al. 2016
Doraiswamy et al. 2014
Aschenbrenner et al. 2015
Ewers et al. 2012
Doraiswamy et al. 2014
Overall
Cohen's d
-2 .5
Roe et al. 2013
Overall
1. 5
Roe et al. 2013
0. 5
Petersen et al. 2015
-0 .5
Lim et al. 2013
Petersen et al. 2015
-1 .5
Mormino et al. 2014
0. 5
Cohen's d
-1 .5
Cohen's d
1. 5 1. 5
-2 .5
Overall 1. 5
Overall
0. 5
Wirth et al. 2013
-0 .5
Storandt et al. 2009
-1 .5
Roe et al. 2013 Wirth et al. 2013
0. 5
Lim et al. 2013 Petersen et al. 2015
-0 .5
Lim et al. 2013
-1 .5
Mielke et al. 2016
Resnick et al. 2010
-2 .5
1. 5
1. 5
0. 5
-0 .5
-1 .5
-2 .5
Overall
0. 5
Roe et al. 2013 Wirth et al. 2013
-0 .5
Petersen et al. 2015
-1 .5
Lim et al. 2013 Papp et al. 2015
-2 .5
Mielke et al. 2016 Aschenbrenner et al. 2015 Doraiswamy et al. 2014 Ewers et al. 2012 Lim et al. 2013 Petersen et al. 2015 Resnick et al. 2010 Roe et al. 2013 Wirth et al. 2013 Overall
Mielke et al. 2016 Doraiswamy et al. 2014
-2 .5
914 915 916 917 918 919 920 921 922 923 924 925 926 927 928 929 930 931 932 933 934 935 936 937 938 939 940 941 942 943 944 945 946 947 948 949 950 951 952 953 954 955 956 957 958 959 960 961 962 963 964 965 966 967 968 969 970 971 972 973 974 975 976 977 978 979 980
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13
-0 .5
8
Cohen's d
Fig. 2. Forest plots from the meta-analysis of studies with longitudinal designs. Effect sizes are presented as Cohen’s d with 95% confidence intervals. The dotted lines represent no effect of amyloid on cognition. Negative values represent greater decline in performance in the presence of high Ab. The domain of working memory is not included as only one study contributed data for this domain. The size of the dots represents study weighting due to sample size.
for the absence of any decline in executive function is very early AD-related neuronal loss does not involve the prefrontal cortex [55,56]. The lack of correlation between location of Ab deposition and neuronal death has been noted in many pathologic studies (see review by Musiek & Holtzman [57]). In this context, the results of this meta-analysis suggest that early AD-related cognitive declines occur predominantly in domains that rely on the normal function of the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, which are areas that are most sensitive to early neuronal death in AD [58]. Thus, in preclinical AD, despite the widespread deposition of Ab [59], neuronal disruption and cognitive decline remain relatively specific.
Although some neuropsychological studies have concluded that decline in executive function does occur in preclinical AD and can also predict clinical progression to MCI and AD [15,60], the neuropsychological tests classified as measuring executive function have varied between studies. For example, some studies include measures of working memory [11,61,62] or tasks of verbal and semantic fluency [13,15] as part of the domain of their executive function composite scores. Previous research suggests that a specific component of executive function, verbal switching, and inhibition is more sensitive to executive dysfunction before dementia than other measures, such as planning [63,64]. In
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
981 982 983 984 985 986 987 988 989 990 991 992 993 994 995 996 997 998 999 1000 1001 1002 1003 1004 1005 1006 1007 1008 1009 1010 1011 1012 1013 1014 1015 1016 1017 1018 1019 1020 1021 1022 1023 1024 1025 1026 1027 1028 1029 1030 1031 1032 1033 1034 1035 1036 1037 1038 1039 1040 1041 1042 1043 1044 1045 1046 1047
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13 1048 1049 1050 1051 1052 1053 1054 1055 1056 1057 1058 1059 1060 1061 1062 1063 1064 1065 1066 1067 1068 1069 1070 1071 1072 1073 1074 1075 1076 1077 1078 1079 1080 1081 1082 1083 1084 1085 1086 1087 1088 1089 1090 1091 1092 1093 1094 1095 1096 1097 1098 1099 1100 1101 1102 1103 1104 1105 1106 1107 1108 1109 1110 1111 1112 1113 1114
accord with previous meta-analyses and standard neuropsychological models, the current study classified tasks of semantic and verbal fluency as representing semantic memory, whereas working memory was classified as a domain separate from executive function. Thus, the definition of executive function used here reflected tests of planning, error monitoring, and inhibition. Therefore, should the data from the present study be considered in the context of a broader definition of executive function, one possible conclusion is that Ab1-related decline in executive function in preclinical AD does not manifest in areas of inhibition, planning, error monitoring, or working memory but rather in semantic and phonemic fluency. This absence of any impairment in semantic and phonemic fluency from the analysis of crosssectional designs reinforces the subtlety of the impairment and shows that repeated assessment is required for it to become evident. The lower than expected estimates of Ab1-related cognitive decline in episodic memory might have been due to the substantial heterogeneity between studies for measures of this domain (Table 1). First, given the centrality of memory dysfunction to early AD, all but one study measured episodic memory (i.e., [65]). This resulted in many more measures of episodic memory being included in the current metaanalysis than measures of other cognitive domains. Thus, there was also the potential for greater variation to be included in estimates of mean effect size. Methodologic differences between studies might also account for some of this heterogeneity. For example, estimates of decline in episodic memory were larger in the subgroups of studies that used PET imaging, categorical Ab levels, statistically controlled for extraneous variables and in those which used the CDR criteria to exclude subjects. As discussed, the reason for a stronger effect under these conditions could be that the samples contain more individuals at later stages of preclinical AD. It is interesting to note that the largest increase in effect size could be attained if including only those studies which used both PET imaging and categorically defined Ab levels (n 5 8; d 5 0.45). While, as mentioned, PiB-PET becomes abnormal much later than CSF Ab42, adding the additional constraint of using a categorical classification for Ab increased the effect above that of just PET alone. 4.3. Comparison with previous meta-analysis The current results extend conclusions about Ab1-related cognitive impairment and decline drawn from a previous meta-analysis [16]. Unlike for the current meta-analysis, Hedden et al. (2013) concluded that the greatest cognitive dysfunction in Ab1 CN older adults occurred in the domain of episodic memory (r 5 0.12). The additional domains of Ab1-related cognitive impairment and decline identified in the present study is likely to have occurred because of the comparatively greater number of participants and studies now available and because the Hedden et al. study did not combine data from studies with cross-sectional and those
9
with longitudinal designs. The inclusion/exclusion criteria for the current meta-analysis were also slightly different to that used previously. For example, the present study did not include data from histopathologic studies as the prevalence of comorbidities in these populations, and the substantial time between clinical assessment and death can reduce the reliability of clinicopathologic correlations [66]. The present study also excluded studies that classified Ab from plasma, given that as yet there are no reliable blood-based biomarkers of CNS Ab [67]. Most importantly, the current results were based on a much larger sample than the previous study. For example, 59% of studies included in the current meta-analysis were published after the period covered by Hedden et al. Furthermore, the length of prospective studies is now greater, enabling more reliable estimates of Ab1-related cognitive decline. Thus, the findings from the present study provide a current and reliable foundation for understanding the nature and magnitude of cognitive impairment and decline in Ab1 CN older adults. 4.4. Limitations and directions for future research Although the estimates of Ab1-related cognitive impairment and decline were based on large sample sizes measuring multiple cognitive domains, there are some important caveats on our conclusions. This study did not consider relationships between tau and cognitive impairment or decline. Given that post mortem studies of AD report that cognitive dysfunction is related more strongly to NFT burden than to amyloid burden [66], it is likely that the addition of levels of tau to meta-analyses will provide greater explanation of variability in cognitive outcomes. Second, as discussed above, there was bias toward the measurement of episodic memory in the studies included. Third, although we identified no evidence of publication bias in studies using cross-sectional designs, the limited number of studies using longitudinal designs meant that publication bias was unable to be assessed. This is indicative of the small number of independent, ongoing observational cohort studies. Furthermore, results from the same cohort are often published in multiple articles, so although there may be a wealth of articles reporting on cognitive decline, only a few can be chosen to represent the cohort. Importantly, 20% of the references that were screened in full were excluded due to not including sufficient information for effect size elicitation, and this lead to exclusion of information from some very important and influential studies in the area. While reporting of means and standard deviations is not always appropriate, the explicit reporting of effect sizes and confidence intervals around these should be standard practice, as basing conclusions solely on the absence or presence of statistical significance is inherently flawed [68]. Fourth, the small sample sizes for some estimates may have reduced the reliability of moderator analyses. Although 60% of studies using PET neuroimaging
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
1115 1116 1117 1118 1119 1120 1121 1122 1123 1124 1125 1126 1127 1128 1129 1130 1131 1132 1133 1134 1135 1136 1137 1138 1139 1140 1141 1142 1143 1144 1145 1146 1147 1148 1149 1150 1151 1152 1153 1154 1155 1156 1157 1158 1159 1160 1161 1162 1163 1164 1165 1166 1167 1168 1169 1170 1171 1172 1173 1174 1175 1176 1177 1178 1179 1180 1181
10 1182 1183 1184 1185 1186 1187 1188 1189 1190 1191 1192 1193 1194 1195 1196 1197 1198 1199 1200 1201 1202 1203 1204 1205 1206 1207 1208 1209 1210 1211 1212 1213 1214 1215 1216 1217 1218 1219 1220 1221 1222 1223 1224 1225 1226 1227 1228 1229 1230 1231 1232 1233 1234 1235 1236 1237 1238 1239 1240 1241 1242 1243 1244 1245 1246 1247 1248
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13
classified Ab burden using a dichotomous outcome (e.g., low/negative or high/positive) in studies using the [11C] PiB-PET tracer, and calculating burden using distribution volume ratios, the criterion score used to define abnormality has ranged between 1.06 [35] and 1.80 [69]. Additionally, the neocortical areas contributing to these estimates of Ab burden varied slightly. Recent recommendations outlined by Dubois et al. (2016) [64] highlighted the large methodological variation between studies with regard to their PET and CSF analyses, stating that differences in thresholds established, protocols and procedures used, and reference and target regions used are possible confounding variables. These caveats notwithstanding, the conclusions here that Ab is associated with small impairments in episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, visuospatial function, and global cognition, and with greater decline in episodic and semantic memory, visuospatial function, and global cognition, are based on a large sample that can be considered to provide a good representation of a preclinical AD population, with an average age of 70 years, approximately equal numbers of males and females, and the number of APOEε4 carriers consistent with population prevalence rates [71]. Although these estimates can be considered representative of what would be seen in a preclinical AD population, it is clear that the effect of Ab on cognition in this early stage is very subtle, and that current neuropsychological measures do not possess the sensitivity to reliably detect Ab1 related cognitive dysfunction. Development or refinement of neuropsychological measures and procedures sensitive to early neurological changes will ultimately provide the sensitivity and specificity that is required to enable the use of cognitive outcomes as clinical markers of disease. This may mean focusing on tools that have characteristics such as the ability for use with increased frequency of testing, sensitivity to other biological markers of disease in addition to amyloid (such as tau or other markers of neurodegeneration) and which provide evidence of reliable change specific to clinical groups such as those with MCI and AD. The estimates generated here do provide a foundation for determination of the extent to which new tests of cognition or behavioral assays can be used to identify Ab in CN adults.
Acknowledgments The authors acknowledge the financial support of the CRC for Mental Health. The Cooperative Research Centre (CRC) programme is an Australian Government Initiative. Y.Y.L is funded by the NHMRC-ARC Dementia Research Development Fellowship.
Supplementary data Supplementary data related to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2016.09.002.
RESEARCH IN CONTEXT
1. Systematic review: The authors conducted metaanalyses of the published literature examining relationships between amyloid levels and cognition in cognitively normal older adults using online database Medline OVID. Estimates of cognitive impairment from studies using cross-sectional designs and cognitive decline from studies using longitudinal designs were obtained. 2. Interpretation: The findings indicate that subtle yet statistically significant cognitive impairment and cognitive decline occur in cognitively normal older people with high levels of amyloid. Cognitive impairment is general in nature and small in magnitude, whereas moderate cognitive decline manifests primarily in episodic memory, semantic memory, and visuospatial function. 3. Future directions: The estimates of amyloid-related cognitive impairment and cognitive decline developed from the current meta-analyses provide a strong foundation for the design of studies that seek to improve the detection of amyloid in cognitively normal older adults through the development of new approaches to assessment. They also provide a basis for computing statistical power for secondary prevention clinical trials where slowing cognitive decline may determine the effectiveness of experimental drugs designed to reduce amyloid burden.
References [1] Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft S, Fagan AM, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s Association workgroup. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:280–92. [2] Risacher SL, Saykin AJ. Neuroimaging and other biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease: The changing landscape of early detection. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2013;9:621–48. [3] Rowe CC, Bourgeat P, Ellis KA, Brown B, Lim YY, Mulligan R, et al. Predicting Alzheimer disease with b-amyloid imaging: Results from the Australian imaging, biomarkers, and lifestyle study of ageing. Ann Neurol 2013;74:905–13. [4] Jansen WJ, Ossenkoppele R, Knol DL, Tijms BM, Scheltens P, Verhey FR, et al. Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia. JAMA 2015;313:1924. [5] Rowe CC, Ng S, Ackermann U, Gong SJ, Pike K, Savage G, et al. Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging and dementia. Neurology 2007; 68:1718–25. [6] Fagan AM, Mintun MA, Mach RH, Lee SY, Dence CS, Shah AR, et al. Inverse relation between in vivo amyloid imaging load and cerebrospinal fluid Abeta-42 in humans. Ann Neurol 2006;59:512–9. [7] Jack CR, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Rocca WA, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, et al. Age-specific population frequencies of cerebral
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
1249 1250 1251 1252 1253 1254 1255 1256 1257 1258 1259 1260 1261 1262 1263 1264 1265 1266 1267 1268 1269 1270 1271 1272 1273 1274 1275 1276 1277 1278 1279 1280 1281 1282 1283 1284 1285 1286 1287 1288 1289 1290 1291 1292 1293 1294 1295 1296 1297 1298 1299 1300 1301 1302 1303 1304 1305 1306 1307 1308 1309 1310 1311 1312 1313 1314 1315
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13 1316 1317 1318 1319 1320 1321 1322 1323 1324 1325 1326 1327 1328 1329 1330 1331 1332 1333 1334 1335 1336 1337 1338 1339 1340 1341 1342 1343 1344 1345 1346 1347 1348 1349 1350 1351 1352 1353 1354 1355 1356 1357 1358 1359 1360 1361 1362 1363 1364 1365 1366 1367 1368 1369 1370 1371 1372 1373 1374 1375 1376Q3 1377 1378 1379 1380 1381 1382
[8]
[9]
[10]
[11]
[12]
[13]
[14]
[15]
[16]
[17]
[18]
[19]
[20]
[21]
[22]
[23]
[24]
[25] [26] [27]
b-amyloidosis and neurodegeneration among people with normal cognitive function aged 50–89 years: A cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol 2014;4422:997–1005. Ossenkoppele R, Jansen WJ, Rabinovici GD, Knol DL, van der Flier WM, van Berckel BN, et al. Prevalence of amyloid PET positivity in dementia syndromes: A meta-analysis. JAMA 2015;313:1939–49. Wirth M, Oh H, Mormino EC, Markley C, Landau SM, Jagust WJ. The effect of amyloid b on cognitive decline is modulated by neural integrity in cognitively normal elderly. Alzheimers Dement 2013; 9:997–1003. Lim YY, Ellis KA, Harrington K, Kamer A, Pietrzak RH, Bush AI, et al. Cognitive consequences of high Ab amyloid in mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: Implications for early detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 2013;27:322–32. Monsell SE, Mock C, Hassenstab J, Roe CM, Cairns NJ, Morris JC, et al. Neuropsychological changes in asymptomatic persons with Alzheimer disease neuropathology. Neurology 2014;83:434–40. Kantarci K, Lowe V, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, Senjem ML, Ivnik RJ, et al. APOE modifies the association between Ab load and cognition in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology 2012; 78:232–40. Harrington MG, Chiang J, Pogoda JM, Gomez M, Thomas K, Marion SD, et al. Executive function changes before memory in preclinical Alzheimer’s pathology: A prospective, cross-sectional, case control study. PLoS One 2013;8:e79378. Ellis KA, Lim YY, Harrington K, Ames D, Bush AI, Darby D, et al. Decline in cognitive function over 18 months in healthy older adults with high amyloid-b. J Alzheimers Dis 2013;34:861–71. Grober E, Hall CB, Lipton RB, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM, Kawas C. Memory impairment, executive dysfunction, and intellectual decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14:266–78. Hedden T, Oh H, Younger AP, Patel TA. Meta-analysis of amyloidcognition relations in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology 2013;80:1341–8. Huijbers W, Mormino EC, Wigman SE, Ward AM, Vannini P, McLaren DG, et al. Amyloid deposition is linked to aberrant entorhinal activity among cognitively normal older adults. J Neurosci 2014; 34:5200–10. Lim YY, Pietrzak RH, Ellis KA, Jaeger J, Harrington K, Ashwood T, et al. Rapid decline in episodic memory in healthy older adults with high amyloid-b. J Alzheimers Dis 2013;33:675–9. Resnick SM, Sojkova J, Zhou Y, An Y, Ye W, Holt DP, et al. Longitudinal cognitive decline is associated with fibrillar amyloid-beta measured by [11C]PiB. Neurology 2010;74:807–15. Riley KP, Jicha GA, Davis D, Abner EL, Cooper GE, Stiles N, et al. Prediction of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Longitudinal rates of change in cognition. J Alzheimers Dis 2011;25:707–17. Snitz BE, Weissfeld LA, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, Saxton J, Singhabahu DM, et al. Cognitive trajectories associated with b-amyloid deposition in the oldest-old without dementia. Neurology 2013; 80:1378–84. Scott JC, Matt GE, Wrocklage KM, Crnich C, Jordan J, Southwick SM, et al. A quantitative meta-analysis of neurocognitive functioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Bull 2015;141:105–40. Lee RS, Hermens DF, Porter MA, Redoblado-Hodge MA. A metaanalysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode Major Depressive Disorder. J Affect Disord 2012;140:113–24. Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. 3rd ed. Oxford University Press; 2006. Cohen J. A Power Primer. Psychol Bull 1992;112:155–9. Dickersin K, Berlin JA. Meta-analysis: State-of-the-Science. Epidemiol Rev 1992;14:154–76. Ellis PD. The essential guide to effect sizes: Statistical power, metaanalysis, and the interpretation of research results. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2010.
11
[28] Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in metaanalysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997;315:629–34. [29] Sterne JAC, Gavaghan D, Egger M. Publication and related bias in meta-analysis: Power of statistical tests and prevalence in the literature. J Clin Epidemiol 2000;53:1119–29. [30] Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating: Current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993;43:2412–4. [31] Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56:303–9. [32] Edmonds EC, Delano-Wood L, Galasko DR, Salmon DP, Bondi MW. Subtle cognitive decline and biomarker staging in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2015;47:231–42. [33] Oh H, Steffener J, Razlighi QR, Habeck C, Stern Y. b-amyloid deposition is associated with decreased right prefrontal activation during task switching among cognitively normal elderly. J Neurosci 2016; 36:1962–70. [34] Loewenstein DA, Curiel RE, Greig MT, Bauer RM, Rosado M, Bowers D, et al. A novel cognitive stress test for the detection of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Discriminative properties and relation to amyloid load. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016;24:804–13. [35] Sojkova J, Zhou Y, An Y, Kraut MA, Ferrucci L, Wong DF, et al. Longitudinal patterns of b-amyloid deposition in nondemented older adults. Arch Neurol 2011;68:644–9. [36] Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale (DRS). Odessa: FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1988. [37] Bondi MW, Edmonds EC, Jak AJ, Clark LR, Delano-Wood L, McDonald CR, et al. Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment improves diagnostic precision, biomarker associations, and prediction of progression. J Alzheimers Dis 2014;42:275–89. [38] Jak AJ, Bondi MW, Delano-Wood L, Wierenga C, Corey-Bloom J, Salmon DP, et al. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;17:368–75. [39] Ewers M, Insel P, Jagust WJ, Shaw L, Trojanowski JQ, Aisen P, et al. CSF biomarker and PIB-PET-derived beta-amyloid signature predicts metabolic, gray matter, and cognitive changes in nondemented subjects. Cereb Cortex 2012;22:1993–2004. [40] Storandt M, Mintun MA, Head D, Morris JC. Cognitive decline and brain volume loss as signatures of cerebral amyloid-b peptide deposition identified with Pittsburgh compound B: Cognitive decline associated with Ab deposition. Arch Neurol 2009;66:1476–81. [41] Lim YY, Ellis KA, Pietrzak RH, Ames D, Darby D, Harrington K, et al. Stronger effect of amyloid load than APOE genotype on cognitive decline in healthy older adults. Neurology 2012;79:1645–52. [42] Mielke MM, Machulda MM, Hagen CE, Christianson TJ, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Influence of amyloid and APOE on cognitive performance in a late middle-aged cohort. Alzheimers Dement 2016; 12:281–91. [43] Magnusson K. Interpreting Cohen’s d effect size: An interactive visualization. Available at: http://rpsychologist.com/d3/cohend/; 2014. Accessed June 1, 2016. [44] Zakzanis KK. Statistics to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth: Formulae, illustrative numerical examples, and heuristic interpretation of effect size analyses for neuropsychological researchers. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2001;16:653–67. Q4 [45] Xiong C, Jasielec MS, Weng H, Fagan AM, Benzinger TLS, Head D, et al. Longitudinal relationships among biomarkers for Alzheimer disease in the Adult Children Study. Neurology 2016;86:1–8. [46] Pietrzak RH, Lim YY, Ames D, Harrington K, Restrepo C, Martins RN, et al. Trajectories of memory decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Results from the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing. Neurobiol Aging 2015; 36:1231–8. [47] Hassenstab J, Ruvolo D, Jasielec M, Xiong C, Grant E, Morris JC. Absence of practice effects in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 2015;29:940–8.
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
1383 1384 1385 1386 1387 1388 1389 1390 1391 1392 1393 1394 1395 1396 1397 1398 1399 1400 1401 1402 1403 1404 1405 1406 1407 1408 1409 1410 1411 1412 1413 1414 1415 1416 1417 1418 1419 1420 1421 1422 1423 1424 1425 1426 1427 1428 1429 1430 1431 1432 1433 1434 1435 1436 1437 1438 1439 1440 1441 1442 1443 1444 1445 1446 1447 1448 1449
12 1450 1451 1452 1453 1454 1455 1456 1457 1458 1459 1460 1461 1462 1463 1464 1465 1466 1467 1468 1469 1470 1471 1472 1473 1474 1475 1476 1477 1478 1479 1480 1481 1482 1483 1484 1485 1486 1487 1488 1489 1490 1491 1492 1493 1494 1495 1496 1497 1498 1499 1500 1501 1502 1503 1504 1505 1506 1507 1508 1509 1510 1511 1512 1513 1514 1515 1516
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13
[48] Braak H, Braak E, Bohl J. Staging of Alzheimer-related cortical destruction. Eur Neurol 1993;33:403–8. [49] Gu Y, Razlighi QR, Zahodne LB, Janicki SC, Ichise M, Manly JJ, et al. Brain amyloid deposition and longitudinal cognitive decline in nondemented older subjects: Results from a multi-ethnic population. PLoS One 2015;10:e0123743. [50] Chetelat G, Villemagne VL, Pike KE, Ellis KA, Bourgeat P, Jones G, et al. Independent contribution of temporal b-amyloid deposition to memory decline in the pre-dementia phase of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2011;134:798–807. [51] Wolk DA, Dickerson BC. Fractionating verbal episodic memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage 2011;54:1530–9. [52] Squire LR, Zola SM. Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1996; 93:13515–22. [53] Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, Addis DR, Westmacott R, Grady C, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: A unified account based on multiple trace theory. J Anat 2005;207:35–66. [54] Walsh KW, Darby D. Neuropsychology: A clinical approach. London: Churchill Livingstone; 1999. [55] Chetelat G, Villemagne VL, Villain N, Jones G, Ellis KA, Ames D, et al. Accelerated cortical atrophy in cognitively normal elderly with high b-amyloid deposition. Neurology 2012;78:477–84. [56] Jack CR, Lowe VJ, Senjem ML, Weigand SD, Kemp BJ, Shiung MM, et al. 11C PiB and structural MRI provide complementary information in imaging of Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain 2008;131:665–80. [57] Musiek ES, Holtzman DM. Three dimensions of the amyloid hypothesis: Time, space and “wingmen”. Nat Neurosci 2015;18:800–6. [58] Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991;82:239–59. [59] Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Devous MD, Rieck JR, Hebrank AC, Diaz-Arrastia R, et al. b-Amyloid burden in healthy aging. Regional distribution and cognitive consequences. Neurology 2012;78:387–95. [60] Ewers M, Brendel M, Rizk-Jackson A, Rominger A, Bartenstein P, Schuff N, et al. Reduced FDG-PET brain metabolism and executive function predict clinical progression in elderly healthy subjects. Neuroimage Clin 2014;4:45–52. [61] Lim YY, Maruff P, Pietrzak RH, Ellis KA, Darby D, Ames D, et al. Ab and cognitive change: Examining the preclinical and prodromal stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2014;10:743–751.e1. [62] Lim YY, Pietrzak RH, Bourgeat P, Ames D, Ellis KA, Rembach A, et al. Relationships between performance on the Cogstate Brief Battery, neurodegeneration, and Ab accumulation in cognitively normal older adults and adults with MCI. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2015; 30:49–58. [63] Clark LR, Schiehser DM, Weissberger GH, Salmon DP, Delis DC, Bondi MW. Specific measures of executive function predict cognitive decline in older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012;18:118–27. [64] Aschenbrenner AJ, Balota DA, Tse CS, Fagan AM, Holtzman DM, Benzinger TLS, et al. Alzheimer disease biomarkers, attentional control, and semantic memory retrieval: Synergistic and mediational effects of biomarkers on a sensitive cognitive measure in nondemented older adults. Neuropsychology 2015;29:368–81. [65] Rolstad S, Berg AI, Bjerke M, Blennow K, Johansson B, Zetterberg H, et al. Amyloid-b42 is associated with cognitive impairment in healthy elderly and subjective cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis 2011; 26:135–42. [66] Nelson PT, Alafuzoff I, Bigio EH, Bouras C, Braak H, Cairns NJ, et al. Correlation of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes with cognitive status: A review of the literature. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2012; 71:362–81. [67] Asih PR, Chatterjee P, Verdile G, Gupta VB, Trengove RD, Martins RN. Clearing the amyloid in Alzheimer’s: Progress towards earlier diagnosis and effective treatments - An update for clinicians. Neurodegener Dis Manag 2014;4:363–78.
[68] Cumming G. The new statistics: Why and how. Psychol Sci 2013; 25:1–23. [69] Aizenstein HJ, Nebes RD, Saxton JA, Price JC, Mathis CA, Tsopelas ND, et al. Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Arch Neurol 2008; 65:1509–17. [70] Dubois B, Hampel H, Feldman HH, Scheltens P, Aisen P, Andrieu S, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimers Dement 2016;12:292–323. [71] Liu CC, Kanekiyo T, Xu H, Bu G. Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: Risk, mechanisms and therapy. Nat Rev Neurol 2013; 9:106–18.
References included in the meta-analyses [1] Van Harten AC, Smits LL, Teunissen CE, Visser PJ, Koene T, Blankenstein MA, et al. Preclinical AD predicts decline in memory and executive functions in subjective complaints. Neurology 2013; 81:1409–16. [2] Edmonds EC, Delano-Wood L, Galasko DR, Salmon DP, Bondi MW. Subtle cognitive decline and biomarker staging in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2015;47:231–42. [3] Landau SM, Mintun MA, Joshi AD, Koeppe RA, Petersen RC, Aisen PS, et al. Amyloid deposition, hypometabolism, and longitudinal cognitive decline. Ann Neurol 2012;72:578–86. [4] Hollands S, Lim YY, Buckley R, Pietrzak RH, Snyder PJ, Ames D, et al. Amyloid-b related memory decline is not associated with subjective or informant rated cognitive impairment in healthy adults. J Alzheimers Dis 2015;43:677–86. [5] Pike KE, Ellis KA, Villemagne VL, Good N, Chetelat G, Ames D, et al. Cognition and beta-amyloid in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Data from the AIBL study. Neuropsychologia 2011;49:2384–90. [6] Lim YY, Maruff P, Pietrzak RH, Ames D, Ellis KA, Harrington K, et al. Effect of amyloid on memory and non-memory decline from preclinical to clinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2013;137:221–31. [7] Sperling RA, Johnson KA, Doraiswamy PM, Reiman EM, Fleisher AS, Sabbagh M, et al. Amyloid deposition detected with florbetapir F 18 (18 F-AV-45) is related to lower episodic memory performance in clinically normal older individuals. Neurobiol Aging 2013;34:822–31. [8] Ossenkoppele R, Madison C, Oh H, Wirth M, Van Berckel BN, Jagust WJ. Is verbal episodic memory in elderly with amyloid deposits preserved through altered neuronal function? Cereb Cortex 2014; 24:2210–8. [9] Sojkova J, Zhou Y, An Y, Kraut MA, Ferrucci L, Wong DF, et al. Longitudinal patterns of b-amyloid deposition in nondemented older adults. Arch Neurol 2011;68:644–9. [10] Lim YY, Maruff P, Schindler R, Ott BR, Salloway S, Yoo DC, et al. Disruption of cholinergic neurotransmission exacerbates Ab-related cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2015;36:2709–15. [11] Stomrud E, Hansson O, Zetterberg H, Blennow K, Minthon L, Londos E. Correlation of longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers with cognitive decline in healthy older adults. Arch Neurol 2010; 67:217–23. [12] Oh H, Steffener J, Razlighi QR, Habeck C, Stern Y. b-amyloid deposition is associated with decreased right prefrontal activation during task switching among cognitively normal elderly. J Neurosci 2016; 36:1962–70. [13] Rolstad S, Berg AI, Bjerke M, Blennow K, Johansson B, Zetterberg H, et al. Amyloid-b42 is associated with cognitive impairment in healthy elderly and subjective cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis 2011; 26:135–42. [14] Gidicsin CM, Maye JE, Locascio JJ, Pepin LC, Philiossaint M, Becker JA, et al. Cognitive activity relates to cognitive performance but not to Alzheimer disease biomarkers. Neurology 2015;85:48–55.
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
1517 1518 1519 1520 1521 1522 1523 1524 1525 1526 1527 1528 1529 1530 1531 1532 1533 1534 1535 1536 1537 1538 1539 1540 1541 1542 1543 1544 1545 1546 1547 1548 1549 1550 1551 1552 1553 1554 1555 1556 1557 1558 1559 1560 1561 1562 1563 1564 1565 1566 1567 1568 1569 1570 1571 1572 1573 1574 1575 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580 1581 1582 1583
J.E. Baker et al. / Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring - (2016) 1-13 1584 1585 1586 1587 1588 1589 1590 1591 1592 1593 1594 1595 1596 1597 1598 1599 1600 1601 1602 1603 1604 1605 1606 1607 1608 1609 1610 1611 1612 1613 1614 1615 1616 1617 1618 1619 1620 1621 1622 1623 1624 1625 1626 1627 1628 1629 1630 1631 1632 1633 1634 1635 1636 1637 1638 1639 1640 1641 1642 1643 1644 1645 1646 1647 1648 1649 1650
[15] Papp KV, Mormino EC, Amariglio RE, Munro C, Dagley A, Schultz AP, et al. Biomarker validation of a decline in semantic processing in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 2016;30:624–30. [16] Amariglio RE, Becker JA, Carmasin J, Wadsworth LP, Lorius N, Sullivan C, et al. Subjective cognitive complaints and amyloid burden in cognitively normal older individuals. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50:2880–6. [17] Hedden T, Mormino EC, Amariglio RE, Younger AP, Schultz AP, Becker JA, et al. Cognitive profile of amyloid burden and white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal older adults. J Neurosci 2012; 32:16233–42. [18] Petersen RC, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Rocca WA, Roberts RO, Mielke MM, et al. Association of elevated amyloid levels with cognition and biomarkers in cognitively normal people from the community. JAMA Neurol 2015;73:85–92. [19] Kantarci K, Lowe V, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, Senjem ML, Ivnik RJ, et al. APOE modifies the association between Ab load and cognition in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology 2012; 78:232–40. [20] Mielke MM, Machulda MM, Hagen CE, Christianson TJ, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Influence of amyloid and APOE on cognitive performance in a late middle-aged cohort. Alzheimers Dement 2016; 12:281–91. [21] Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA, Moran EK, Eng E, Buckner RL, et al. Cognition, reserve, and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Ann Neurol 2010;67:353–64. [22] Loewenstein DA, Curiel RE, Greig MT, Bauer RM, Rosado M, Bowers D, et al. A novel cognitive stress test for the detection of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Discriminative properties and relation to amyloid load. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016;24:804–13. [23] Barthel H, Gertz HJ, Dresel S, Peters O, Bartenstein P, Buerger K, et al. Cerebral amyloid-b PET with florbetaben (18F) in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and healthy controls: A multicentre phase 2 diagnostic study. Lancet Neurol 2011;10:424–35. [24] Nebes RD, Snitz BE, Cohen AD, Aizenstein HJ, Saxton JA, Halligan EM, et al. Cognitive aging in persons with minimal amyloid-beta and white matter hyperintensities. Neuropsychologia 2013;51:2202–9. [25] Aizenstein HJ, Nebes RD, Saxton JA, Price JC, Mathis CA, Tsopelas ND, et al. Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Arch Neurol 2008;65:1509–17. [26] Doherty BM, Schultz SA, Oh JM, Koscik RL, Dowling NM, Barnhart TE, et al. Amyloid burden, cortical thickness, and cognitive function in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2015;1:160–9.
13
[27] Aschenbrenner AJ, Balota DA, Fagan AM, Duchek JM, Benzinger TLS, Morris JC. Alzheimer disease cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers moderate baseline differences and predict longitudinal change in attentional control and episodic memory composites in the Adult Children Study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2015;21:573–83. [28] Xiong C, Jasielec MS, Weng H, Fagan AM, Benzinger TLS, Head D, et al. Longitudinal relationships among biomarkers for Alzheimer disease in the Adult Children Study. Neurology 2016;86:1–8. [29] Hassenstab J, Chasse R, Grabow P, Benzinger TLS, Fagan AM, Xiong C, et al. Certified normal: Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers and normative estimates of cognitive functioning. Neurobiol Aging 2016;43:23–33. [30] Wang L, Benzinger TL, Hassenstab J, Blazey T, Owen C, Fagan AM, et al. Spatially distinct atrophy is linked to b-amyloid and tau in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2015;84:1254–60. [31] Ewers M, Insel P, Jagust WJ, Shaw L, Trojanowski JQ, Aisen P, et al. CSF biomarker and PIB-PET-derived beta-amyloid signature predicts metabolic, gray matter, and cognitive changes in nondemented subjects. Cereb Cortex 2012;22:1993–2004. [32] Lim YY, Ellis KA, Pietrzak RH, Ames D, Darby D, Harrington K, et al. Stronger effect of amyloid load than APOE genotype on cognitive decline in healthy older adults. Neurology 2012;79:1645–52. [33] Doraiswamy P, Sperling RA, Johnson K, Reiman EM, Wong TZ, Sabbagh MN, et al. Florbetapir F 18 amyloid PET and 36-month cognitive decline: A prospective multicenter study. Mol Psychiatry 2014; 19:1044–51. [34] Wirth M, Oh H, Mormino EC, Markley C, Landau SM, Jagust WJ. The effect of amyloid b on cognitive decline is modulated by neural integrity in cognitively normal elderly. Alzheimers Dement 2013; 9:997–1003. [35] Resnick SM, Sojkova J, Zhou Y, An Y, Ye W, Holt DP, et al. Longitudinal cognitive decline is associated with fibrillar amyloid-beta measured by [11C]PiB. Neurology 2010;74:807–15. [36] Mormino E, Betensky RA, Hedden T, Schultz AP, Amariglio RE, Rentz DM, et al. Synergistic Effect of b-Amyloid and Neurodegeneration on Cognitive Decline in Clinically Normal Individuals. JAMA Neurol 2014;71:1379. [37] Roe CM, Fagan AM, Grant EA, Hassenstab J, Dreyfus DM, Sutphen CL, et al. Amyloid imaging and CSF biomarkers in predicting cognitive impairment up to 7.5 years later. Neurology 2013; 80:1784–91. [38] Storandt M, Mintun MA, Head D, Morris JC. Cognitive decline and brain volume loss as signatures of cerebral amyloid-b peptide deposition identified with Pittsburgh compound B: Cognitive decline associated with Ab deposition. Arch Neurol 2009;66:1476–81.
REV 5.4.0 DTD DADM122_proof 18 October 2016 7:47 am ce
1651 1652 1653 1654 1655 1656 1657 1658 1659 1660 1661 1662 1663 1664 1665 1666 1667 1668 1669 1670 1671 1672 1673 1674 1675 1676 1677 1678 1679 1680 1681 1682 1683 1684 1685 1686 1687 1688 1689 1690 1691 1692 1693 1694 1695 1696 1697 1698 1699 1700 1701 1702 1703 1704