41
Behavroura/ Processes, 20 (1989) 41-48
Elsevrer COMPARATIVE EFFECTS OF FOOD AND WATER DEPRIVATION ON MOVEMENT PA’ITERNS IN THE PIGEON (COLUMBA LIV’U)
ROBERT W. ALLAN’ and T JAMES MATTHEWS2 i Department Department 10003, USA
of Psychology, Fairlegh Dxkinson Umversdy, Ma&on, New Jersey 07940, USA of Psychology, New York Unrversrty, 6 Washington Place, New York, New York ( Accepted
I August
1989 )
ABSTRACT Allan, R W. and Matthews, T.J , 1989 Comparattve effects of food and water deprtvatton on movement patterns in the ptgeon (Columba Livra). Behave Process. ,20: 4 l-48 Groups of naive pigeons were either food or water deprived and exposed to response-independent ftxed time schedules of reinforcement Reinforcers were always approprtate to the deprtvatton state. After 15 Phase One sesstons under the original deprtvatton state, an addmonal 15 Phase Two sessions were run under the alternate deprrvatton regimen The Phase One results mdrcated that the frequency of movement over time, as measured by floorboard panels, had a characteristic distribution dependmg on deprrvatton state. After state-swatch in Phase Two, both groups exhibited changes in frequency dtstrrbutton over ttme, but the new distributions were dtfferent from those produced m SubJects without any deprivation htstory. There appeared to be sustained effects on movement patterns due to prior exposure to alternate deprtvatron and periodic feeding regimens, results which confirm earlier experimental work. The findings are discussed m hght of research on the effect of prior response-mdependent schedule trammg, the matchmg law, and the effects of contextual condtttonmg. It has frequently been shown that ptgeons ~111engage m stereotyped movement patterns when exposed to response-independent
fixed-time (IT) schedules of reinforcement
(Skmner, 1948;
Staddon& Simmelhag, 1971, Staddon, 1977; Reberg, buns, Mann & Etzenga, 1978, K.tlleen, 1975; Allan & Matthews, 1982). The behavior so produced seems to be a function simply of the deprivanon state m conlunctton with the experimental
environment
(including feeding schedules).
The
chamber is usually limited in size, with a food or water dehvery system on one wall, and with httle else in the enclosure.
In a large experimental chamber, Reberg et al. (1978) observed distmct
behavroral effects when ptgeons were food or water deprived and exposed to the response-independent reinforcer deliveries.
However, after SubJectS were switched to an alternate deprtvatton
regimen, sustained carry-over effects were observed
The carry-over effects mamfested themsel-
ves in a slow drifting of the form and distribution of activity produced by the original motivational state toward those assoctated with the original presentation
of the new deprtvatton state.
Unlike the Reberg et al (1978) study, which also compared food and water-induced the present study employed automated data recording
This meausurement
a more conststent defnution data.
of response classes and allows the recruitment
0376-6357/89/$03
Elsevrer Scrence Publishers BV (Bromedrcal Drvtston)
50
@ 1989
behavtor,
technique provides of a larger body of
42
Figure 1. Schematic diagram of the experimental to measure locomotion frequency and location.
chamber and the segmented floorboard
used
METHOD
Subjects Erght narve, Whrte Carneau pigeons served as subjects. In their home cages, food-deprived and water-deprived
subjects had free access to water and food respectively
Apparatus The experimental
chamber is depicted in figure 1. Its inner drmensrons were 73 cm long, 38
cm wide, and 41 cm high. The front panel was one of two BRS/LVE Prgeon Intelligence
Panels,
the first delivered food reinforcers (mixed grain), the second panel was modified to deliver water (.75 ml). The response key centered on the front panels was left uncovered but was not used The floor of the test chamber was divided into nine panels, each measurmg 16.2 X 12.1 X 0.3 cm (see figure 1). Each corner was supported by a single Honeywell V3L131-D8
rmcroswitch.
Two carriage screws held the panels in place, but m no way restricted their movement against the microswitches
If the prgeon stepped anywhere on the panel, one of the parallel wired mrcros-
watches was acttvated.
On and off trmes were recorded by a Plessy Micro 1 computer for later
analysrs. A video camera positioned 1 m above the clear acrylic cedmg of the experimental chamber allowed visual monitoring of subjects’ actrvrty
43
Procedure The subjects were divided into two groups of four buds, each run m two experimental
Group Food-Water
(FW; subjects 20,21,22,23)
Phases
were food deprived during the first Phase and
switched to water deprivatron during the second Phase. Group Water-Food
(WF; SubJects 106,
108, 109, 110) were water deprived during Phase One and switched to food deprtvatron during Phase Two. There was a five day recovery period between deprtvatton regimens durmg whrch all subjects had restrrcted daily access to food and water in the home cage Both expertmental Phases were run for fifteen days In the food deprivatton condrtrons, the experimental sessrons were run on consecutive days and conststed of 40 trials with 30 s inter-remforcer mtervals (IRI) During water deprrvatron, however, the bards were run every other day and sessions consisted of 20 trials with 30 s IRIS Thts was necessary because pilot work showed that with more trrals per session, or wrth dally sessions, the pigeons would not drink throughout the session. RESULTS Figures 2 and 3 depict frequencies of floorboard actrvrty (responses per mmute), plotted in 3Each data pomt is an average over all trrals over the last 5 sesstons
s bms over elapsed trial time
of each phase, wtth filled circles depicting actrvrty on panels l-3 (front), and open circles representing movement on panels 4-9 (back). For group FW, during the food-deprrvatton
Phase, three out of four subjects show a front panel
dominance
The exception, subject 22, moved quickly after the reinforcer to panels 4-6 and paced slowly between front and back panels along the srde wall After the reinforcer, the three remaining SubJects immedtately moved to the back of the chamber for a brief period, followed by a sustained, htgher rate of pacing along the front wall until the next food delivery After the shift from food to water deprivatton, the peaked nature of front and back panel actrvtty seemed to disappear
The very high rates of front panel acttvrty for subjects 20 and 21
remained dominant under water deprivation.
Subject 22 continued to pace along the side wall
and subject 23’s response rate dropped to very low levels after swrtching to water deprrvatron. Group WF SubJects in Phase One (given this particular level of water deprrvatron) generally responded at lower rates than drd the birds m Group FW, Phase One (food depravation). In three out of four cases, the dominant response pattern, in the water deprtvatron Phase, was pacing along the side wall at a very low rate. The exception, subJect 106, spent most of the interval moving at a fatrly rapid rate on the back panels
Only subJect 109 produced a peaked distribution
under
water deprivation conditions. When shafted to food deprivation
in Phase Two, all subjects showed an enhancement
of ac-
tivity, front and back. There was also an increase in the peaked nature of the movement distrrbutions with the highest rate of acttvrty occurring m the first half of the IRI. The effect of prror water deprrvatronon
subsequent food activity rates is an enhancement
of relative middle and back panel
Food -----+
Water
120 100 80
20
60 40 20 ‘;Q,
0
C
60
E b
40
21
SL u$
20
E ’
0
$
60
u
22
40 20 0 60
25
40 20 0
0
6
12
16
24
0
6
12
16
24
3
Elapsed trial time
Figure2. Individual average frequencies of panel closures per minute for group FW, plotted over elapsed trial time. The plots on the IetI represent Phase One (food deprived) performances. The plots on the right depict frequencies obtained alter the switch to water deprivation conditions. Bird numbers are indicated at the right of each set of plots.
45
Food
Water ------w
80
i
106
td
108
6
12
18
24
'0
6
12
18
24
Elapsed trial time
Figure3. Individual average frequenctes of panel closures per minute for group WF, plotted over elapsed trial time. The plots on the left represent Phase One (water deprived) performances. The plots on the right depict frequencies obtained after the switch to food deprivation conditions. Bird numbers are indicated at the right of each set of plots.
movement,
wtth one case (subject
106) showmg higher mtddle
and back panel rates under food
deprrvatton Overall, food and water deprivation one case, when animals animals were shifted relative
amount
food deprived
condttrons
were shifted
produce
from food to water deprrvatron,
of back panel activity produced subjects.
different
formances
produced
preceded
by treatments
cases (#106-food,
also confirmed
to the back of the chamber
water deprivation
as the rates eventually condrttons
represented observed
sequence
Followmg
Followmg
back panel ac-
Phase Two per-
m Phase Two
of behavior
It is because
during the IRI
food reinforcers
a wa!er reinforcer,
but were less likely to commence
condmons
the
seven out of eight buds
briefly, after which pacing acttvrty was mutated
toward the front wall of the chamber the front of the chamber
#20-water)
that the characterrstrc
differed under food and water depravation
rear of the chamber
In general,
was higher than in
in which the relative rates of front and back panel actrvtty were
and in the same duectton
Video momtormg
In contrast,
when
front panel acttvtty rates as high or higher than back panel
Each of the exceptronal
hrghly disparate
subjects
In seven out of eight buds run under water deprtvatron,
run under food deprtvatton
In all but
actrvtty increased,
acttvtty levels decreased
by water deprived
tivity was as high or hrgher than rates under food deprtvatron movement
levels of acttvtty
from water to food deprrvatton,
animals moved
as the animal moved
subjects also retreated
to the
pacing, and did not usually move toward
of the latter tendency
also resultmg
m less disparate
observatrons
provide,
that overall activity was lower m the relative rates of front and back panel
activity DISCUSSION The present
experimental
tween the movement compartson
patterns
of movement
been changed,
associated
frequencies
and (3) confumatton
have been altered
patterns
by the continued
Although
in the present
regtmen
changes m the experimental
acttvtty (Ktlleen,
regimen
has
et al. (1978),
patterns
of the
phases for both groups FW and WF)
to a prior deprtvation
a change in deprivation
be-
(2) a clear wrthm-group
once the deprtvatton
It seems that the typical adaptive
exposure
dtstmctton
effects noted by Reberg
many sources of the distinct movement
in the level of schedule-induced acttvtty, as observed
carry-over
at some level, throughout
There are, perhaps, water deprivation
elicited
those ehcrted m the first experimental
remam predommant,
of a reliable
with food and water deprrvatron, and patterning
of quahtatrve
at least m the form of locomotton prgeons (presumably
(1) a descrtptton
patterns
These
assoctated
level could be expected
patterns
environment with food and
to produced
a change
1975) a shift in the shape of the drstrtbutron
study, would not be expected
of
to result from a change m deprtva-
tion level alone An alternative duced by pertodtc these reinforcers over patches
approach
recognizes
the consptcuous
food and water presentations Food acqutsttron
of high food denstty
m the natural while water
parallel between
the behavior patterns
and the natural foraging patterns envn-onment
requires
IS assocrated
very httle
locomotor
associated
mwith
with raped pacing behavtor
The
47
schedule-Induced
behavior
tated by species-typical
produced
patterns
effects shown here and elsewhere cement
history in determining
The observation terns of movement to a new deprivation (alternation
(Reberg
of remfor-
behavior IS not
history of an organism
of these effects that is strlkmg
Phase One condutons
effect of the behavioral
1s to confhct with a new, and perhaps
If the pat-
are representative
perseveration
of some
after the switch
more appropriate
This fmdmg also seems to suggest, quite powerfully, from front to back panels and relative frequencies
die-
The carry-over
the importance
effects due to the past experimental
then one potential
was not entirely
mechanisms
of schedule-induced
It IS the relative permanence
regimen,
however,
et al, 1978), demonstrate
elicited during the respective
sort of foragmg pattern, mg responses
experiment, (deprivational)
the form and distribution
of carry-over
unusual nor unexpected
m the present
tred to motivational
set of forag-
that the locomotion
thereof)
patterns
are related to the depriva-
tion state of the organism It is clear from the present being recorded,
results that, (1) there was modiftcatton
(2) there was development
of new alternating
front and back panels which did not resemble (3) some of the response donunant
patterns
any behavior
of responses
then, provides response
for both groups changed
quantitative
dimension
-- frequency
In light of the present havioral probabilities deprivation
high frequency),
state shift
The present
et al (1978) while utthzing
experiment,
perhaps
descrtpttons
established
responses
dtswork,
a single and simple
and the response
Both response
of potential
patterns
and deprivation
alteration
should be expanded
of present
be-
to include the prior
ekczted (not only emitted)
effects need to be considered
analysis by Pear (1988)
produced
during deprivation
the generalized conditions
matchmg
However,
law was applied
The results of the present
port Pear’s claim that the value of the bias term is affected
changes
(e g ,
and (4) The temporal
under
when examm-
responding
In a recent
delivery
animals,
were ehmmated
of panel closure
history of the orgamsm
mg current
between
seen m Phase One deprived
post-deprivation
of Reberg
by previously
the earlier conditions
movement
support
patterns
of locomotton
seen during Phase One performance
front or back panel pacing with relatively
trrbution
of the movement
patterns
by alterations
instead of simply altering the ongoing schedule,
in deprivatron
we should consider
state
If the matching
the potential
carry-over
law 1s to be applied effects of changing
to stereotypic
study seem to sup-
m the schedule
of food
the present
data resulted from
to response
stereotyples,
deprivatton
condittons
then
and its ef-
fect on the bias parameter Many of the carry-over
effects may be due to discrunmative
toires by the experimental stimuh might be assessed state training phases.
context
(Balsam & Tomie,
Perhaps
behavior
patterns
accompamed
The comparison induced
behavior
the change m deprivation
of motivatronal is strained
mechanisms
by the dlstmctiveness
This putative
of those recorded
and mamtamed
reper-
control by contextual
contexts with the different
more indicative
One for both groups FW and WF would be elicited roundings
1985)
by pairing umque experimental
control over the established
deprivation during Phase
if unique experimental
sur-
regimen and their role m the development of the form of the behavior
of schedule-
associated
with the
48
two motrvatronal
mechanisms
board acttvity, enables of the behavior
recording
Although
observer-coding
schedule-induced methods
compartson
Moreover,
of mdivrdual animals IS strengthened
sible by automated companies
It 1s for this reason that a single, oblectlve
a defensible
of behavior
by the breadth
there 1s some sacrtftce
of behavior,
behavtor
the
based on replicable
behavior
the rehabtlty
establishment
measurement
measure,
floor-
of the characterrzatton
of coverage
that 1s made pos-
m the richness
of detail that ac-
of general
wrll require
prtncrples
mcreasingly
of
systematic
analysis
REFERENCES Allan, R W and Matthews, and water induced vention, Balsam,
Baltimore,
Killeen,
Assoctatrve
Paper presented
and ehctted
factors m the modulatton
at Eastern
Psychologtcal
of food
Assocration
Con-
Maryland
P D. and Tomte,
Hdlsdale,
T J (1982)
stereotyptes.
A (1985)
Context
and Learmng
Lawrence
Erlbaum
Assoctates,
New Jersey.
P. (1975).
Pear, J J (1988)
On the temporal Behavioral
control of behavior.
stereotypy
Psychologzcoi Revrew, 82,89-l
and the generalized
matching
equatron.
15.
Journal of the
Expenmental AnalysLs of Behavror, 50,87-95. Reberg,
D , Inms, N K., Mann, B. and Etzenga,
periodic
response-independent
Skmner, B.F. (1948). Superstttton Staddon,
J.E.R
(1977)
Staddon,
J E.R., and Simmelhag,
tts rmphcattons
behavior
resultmg
from
m the pigeon.
Journal of Expenmental Psychology, 38,168-172.
behavior.
of adaptive
26,507-S 19
In W.K Homg and J E R. Staddon
Appleton-Century-Crofts,
V.L (1971)
for the prmctples
‘Superstttious”
of food or water. AnzmalBehavlour,
Schedule-induced
Handbook of Operant Behavior.
C. (1978)
presentattons
The ‘superstttton’ behavior.
(Eds ),
New York experiment:
Psychologxal
A reexammatton
Revrew, 78,3-43.
of