Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
ScienceDirect Comparative primate neurobiology and the evolution of brain language systems James K Rilling1,2,3,4,5 Human brain specializations supporting language can be identified by comparing human with non-human primate brains. Comparisons with chimpanzees are critical in this endeavor. Human brains are much larger than non-human primate brains, but human language capabilities cannot be entirely explained by brain size. Human brain specializations that potentially support our capacity for language include firstly, wider cortical minicolumns in both Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas compared with great apes; secondly, leftward asymmetries in Broca’s area volume and Wernicke’s area minicolumn width that are not found in great apes; and thirdly, arcuate fasciculus projections beyond Wernicke’s area to a region of expanded association cortex in the middle and inferior temporal cortex involved in processing word meaning. Addresses 1 Department of Anthropology, Emory University, 1557 Dickey Drive, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States 2 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States 3 Center for Behavioral Neuroscience, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States 4 Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States 5 Center for Translational Social Neuroscience, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States Corresponding author: Rilling, James K. (
[email protected])
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14 This review comes from a themed issue on Communication and language Edited by Michael Brainard and Tecumseh Fitch
Available online XXX 0959-4388/$ – see front matter, # 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.04.002
Introduction Language is among the most distinctive attributes of Homo sapiens. Other primate species communicate, but none does so by combining thousands of symbols according to a defined set of rules to generate phrases with a nearly infinite variety of meanings [1,2]. Although chimpanzees raised in human linguistic environments develop limited symbolic abilities, none has eclipsed the language competence of a 2 and 1/2-year old human child [3]. Thus, there must be human brain specializations that support Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14
our capacity for language. As discussed below, comparative studies of primate neurobiology have helped to identify some of these specializations. Comparisons with our closest living relative, the chimpanzee, have been crucial in this endeavor, for we cannot conclude that a trait has uniquely evolved in humans unless we also demonstrate its absence in modern chimpanzees [4].
Brain size The human brain averages 1330 cc in size, a value that far exceeds that for the brain of any other living primate species [5]. Rhesus macaque brains average only 88 cc [6]. Chimpanzee brains average 405 cc and gorilla brains average 500 cc [5]. These numbers suggest the possibility that our unique capacity for language is simply a product of our large brain size. However, evidence from highfunctioning human microcephalics, some of whom have brain volumes within the great ape range, suggests otherwise since their linguistic abilities can eclipse those of chimpanzees. Thus, there are apt to be qualitative differences between human and non-human primate brains that support our capacity for language [7].
Language circuitry In 1970, Geschwind outlined a model of the functional neuroanatomy of human language [8]. This model is a useful starting point for comparative analysis, however it is important to note that it does not include features that we now know to be important, such as the involvement of the thalamus, basal ganglia and cortical areas beyond Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas in language [9,10,11,12]. In this model, Wernicke’s area, the posterior portion of the left superior temporal gyrus, is essential for speech comprehension, and Broca’s area, a region in the left inferior frontal cortex, is responsible for speech production. These two areas are connected by a large white matter fiber tract known as the arcuate fasciculus. Perhaps other primate species lack human language skills because they lack homologues of human Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas [13]. However, when defined based on cytoarchitecture and shared non-linguistic functional properties, such homologues have been identified in non-human primate brains [14]. This leads to the question of whether there might be microstructural differences within these areas between humans and non-human primates.
Broca’s area Not only is Broca’s area involved in human speech, it also mediates aspects of communication in non-human primates [15]. Mirror neurons are found in macaque area www.sciencedirect.com
Comparative primate neurobiology and language evolution Rilling 11
F5, part of which is homologous to the posterior part of human Broca’s area (area 44). Mirror neurons ostensibly allow monkeys to improve their understanding of actions they observe in others by mapping those observed actions onto their own motor system and simulating the action in their own brain. Mirror neurons are well known for their response to reaching and grasping movements of both self and others, but they also fire when producing or observing communicative mouth movements [16]. Macaque monkeys communicate extensively with orofacial expressions, so mirror neurons are likely vital to macaque communication. There is evidence that mirror neuron regions are also involved in orofacial communication in humans [17], however human Broca’s area is also responsible for the grammatical aspects of speech [18,19]. Perhaps, just as Broca’s area is involved in human speech production, Broca’s area homologue in non-human primates is involved in the production of non-human primate vocalizations. However, this does not seem to be the case, since lesioning the monkey homologue of Broca’s area does not impair vocalization [20,21]. Monkey calls instead depend upon the limbic system and brainstem. This neurological difference reflects differences in the nature of human speech and nonhuman primate vocalizations. In contrast to human speech, monkey calls are largely involuntary expressions of emotional arousal [22], implying that they may not be under voluntary cortical control. On the other hand, there is some evidence that captive chimpanzees will exhibit volitional calls [23], and that Broca’s homologue is involved in their production [24]. This raises the possibility that Broca’s area was recruited for volitional vocal, in addition to orofacial communication, before the divergence of humans and chimpanzees some 5–7 million year ago. Despite these similarities, there are obvious functional differences between human and chimpanzee Broca’s areas, not just in the motor aspects of speech but also in the involvement of human Broca’s area with syntax [11,25]. These functional differences are likely to be supported by underlying anatomical differences. It is now known that Broca’s area has wider cortical minicolumns in humans than in great apes [26], whereas minicolumn width does not differ between humans and great apes in primary somatosensory or motor cortex [27]. Cortical minicolumns are vertical columns consisting of approximately 80–100 neurons with strong interconnections that constitute a coherent functional unit. In sensory cortex, for example, neurons within a given minicolumn share a peripheral receptive field. Cortical minicolumns consist of vertically aligned cells separated by vertically defined cell poor spaces where most connections are made. Minicolumns contrast with macrocolumns that consist of many minicolumns bound together by shortrange horizontal connections [28]. The larger human minicolumns within Broca’s area are thought to facilitate www.sciencedirect.com
greater integration of information by providing more space for connections. Finally, Broca’s area is larger in the left cerebral hemisphere of the human brain and this may be related to the strong tendency for many aspects of language function to be lateralized to the left hemisphere. Importantly, this Broca’s area asymmetry is not present in chimpanzees [29].
Wernicke’s area Whereas production of human and macaque monkey vocalizations depend on different neural substrates, comprehension of species-specific vocalizations seems to depend on similar neural substrates in humans and macaque monkeys [30]. Human speech comprehension depends on the left posterior superior temporal gyrus (i.e. Wernicke’s area) [8]. Similarly, the left superior temporal gyrus is responsible for discriminating species-specific vocalizations, but not other types of auditory stimuli, in Japanese macaques [31]. Single cell electrophysiology similarly implicates the superior temporal gyrus in processing macaque speciesspecific calls. Monkey auditory cortex consists of three longitudinal cytoarchitectonic streams in the superior temporal lobe: a core region, a surrounding belt region, and an adjacent parabelt region [32]. Single cell electrophysiology studies have revealed that unlike core areas that respond best to pure tones, lateral belt areas respond best to complex sounds, including species-specific vocalizations [33]. Further evidence for similarity between humans and macaques in the neural substrates for processing speciesspecific vocalizations comes from a comparative PET neuroimaging study. In a small sample of macaque monkeys, blood flow responses were more pronounced for speciesspecific calls compared with non-biological sounds within cortical area Tpt as well as the dorsal frontal operculum, presumed homologues of Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas, respectively [34]. However, it should be noted that another monkey PET study found that the anterior tip of the left superior temporal gyrus, rather than Wernicke’s area homologue, was specialized for processing speciesspecific calls [35]. The difference could be attributable to the former study measuring blood flow with a temporal resolution of 60 s and the latter measuring glucose metabolism occurring over a period of about 25 min [35]. Like Broca’s area, human Wernicke’s area has functional properties not found in its non-human homologue, such as phonological processing [11]. Are there anatomical differences that support these expanded functional capabilities? A portion of Wernicke’s area known as the planum temporale is leftwardly asymmetric in most humans [36]. However, this same asymmetry is also present in great apes [37,38]. While these findings are consistent with the notion that human brain language systems evolved from pre-existing neural systems present in nonhuman primate ancestors, they also suggest that planum temporale asymmetries are not the neural substrate supporting humanspecific linguistic abilities. Nevertheless, potentially Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14
12 Communication and language
relevant specializations of human Wernicke’s area are apparent at the microstructural level. As with Broca’s area, planum temporale minicolumns are wider in humans compared with chimpanzees, and planum temporale minicolumn width is leftwardly asymmetric (left > right) in humans but not chimpanzees [39]. Against this backdrop of comprehension-related similarity in brain activation patterns, there is also emerging evidence for differences between humans and macaques. A recent comparative fMRI study showed that whereas both humans and macaques activate the lateral sulcus and superior temporal gyrus (STG) when listening to monkey and human vocalizations, only in humans did the STS also respond to intelligible human utterances. Macaque STS did not respond to monkey calls. Notably, the human STS activations spanned nearly the entire length of the sulcus, and the authors conclude that the evolution of language appears to have recruited most of STS in humans [40] (see also [41]).
Connections between Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas Beyond these human specializations within human Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas, there are differences between humans and non-human primates in the white-matter connections that link these two regions. Anterograde and retrograde tracer techniques have been used to describe the connections of Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas homologues in macaque monkeys [42,43,44,45]. Direct connections between the two regions have been
identified, but the pathway is weak [44,45] and its function unknown. In fact, the dominant frontal connection of Wernicke’s homologue (area Tpt) is not with Broca’s area homologue, but rather with dorsal prefrontal cortex [42]. This auditory ‘where’ pathway is involved with localizing sounds in space [46]. On the other hand, the dominant connection of Broca’s area homologue is with the cortex of the middle superior temporal gyrus [43] via a ventral pathway known as the extreme capsule that is involved in auditory object recognition [46]. The tracer methods that have been used to delineate white matter connections in macaque monkeys are invasive, terminal procedures that cannot be used in humans. Instead, human white matter pathways have traditionally been described based on gross dissections of post-mortem brains [47]. However, it is difficult to precisely define cortical terminations of fascicles using these methods. Diffusion tensor imaging provides a non-invasive alternative method that can be applied to humans and nonhuman primates [48–50]. Although DTI has demonstrated connections between Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas (or their homologues) in humans, chimpanzees, and rhesus macaques, the human pathway bears one major difference from that of the non-human species. In both rhesus macaques and chimpanzees, the posterior terminations of the arcuate are focused on the homologue of Wernicke’s area in the posterior superior temporal gyrus. Humans, however, also possess a massive projection of the arcuate into the middle and inferior temporal gyri, ventral to classic Wernicke’s area [51].
Figure 1
• Wider minicolumns • L>R volume
• Wider minicolumns • L>R minicolumns CS
46
9
45
40
PrCS
10 IFS 44 6
IPS 39
22
47
37 STS
21
• Posterior and medial displacement of visual cortex
• Arcuate fasciculus projection into region of expanded temporal association cortex involved in processing word meaning Current Opinion in Neurobiology
Potential neurological specializations supporting human language Broca’s area has wider minicolumns in humans compared with great apes, and the volume of human Broca’s area is leftwardly asymmetric, whereas this is not the case in chimpanzees. Wernicke’s area also has wider minicolumns in humans compared with great apes, and there is a leftward asymmetry in the column width in humans that is absent in chimpanzees or macaque monkeys. Finally, the human arcuate fasciculus pathway includes a massive projection to the cortex ventral to classic Wernicke’s area that is involved in lexical-semantic processing and that has markedly expanded in human evolution, displacing adjacent visual cortex in a posterior and medial direction in the process. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14
www.sciencedirect.com
Comparative primate neurobiology and language evolution Rilling 13
The cortex of the middle and inferior temporal gyri appears to have expanded in human evolution. In the process, it displaced nearby extrastriate visual cortex [52,53]. For example, visual motion area MT lies within the superior temporal sulcus (STS) in chimpanzees and rhesus macaques. However, in humans, it lies considerably posterior to STS, and STS instead appears to contain association cortex [54]. The posterior limit of the temporal-lobe arcuate fasciculus terminations in the human brain coincides very closely with the anterior limit of visual motion area MT, consistent with displacement of MT by the highly expanded arcuate pathway. The region of the middle temporal gyrus that receives arcuate projections has been referred to as an ‘epicenter for lexical-semantic processing’ [55]. Thus, this portion of the arcuate fasciculus may be carrying lexical-sematic information to Broca’s area for further processing. Although some have postulated that the ventral extreme capsule pathway was recruited into the language system during human evolution [44], comparative DTI data suggest that there has been far greater evolutionary expansion of the arcuate fasciculus than there has been of the extreme capsule. These data suggest that the arcuate fasciculus was a more important substrate for human language evolution. Further evidence in support of this claim is the observation that the human arcuate fasciculus is leftwardly asymmetric, in parallel with typical left hemisphere lateralization of language function, whereas the human extreme capsule pathway is not leftwardly asymmetric [56].
Conclusion In summary, the human brain has multiple anatomical specializations that may be relevant to explaining our capacity for language. First, human brains have wider cortical minicolumns in both Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas compared with great apes. Second, human brains exhibit leftward asymmetries in Broca’s area volume and in the width of planum temporale minicolums that are not found in great apes. Third, the projections of the human arcuate fasciculus reach beyond Wernicke’s area to a region of expanded association cortex in the middle and inferior temporal cortex that appears to be involved in processing word meaning (Figure 1).
References and recommended reading Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as: of special interest of outstanding interest
4.
Preuss TM: Who’s afraid of Homo sapiens? J Biomed Discov Collab 2006, 1:17.
5.
Holloway RL, Sherwood CC, Rilling JK, Hof PR: Evolution, of the brain in humans – paleoneurology. In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Edited by Binder MD, Windhorst U, Hirsch MC. Springer-Verlag; 2008.
6.
Herndon JG, Tigges J, Klumpp SA, Anderson DC: Brain weight does not decrease with age in adult rhesus monkeys. Neurobiol Aging 1998, 19:267-272.
7.
Holloway RL: The evolution of the primate brain: some aspects of quantitative relations. Brain Res 1968, 7:121-172.
8. Geschwind N: The organization of language and the brain. Science 1970, 170:940-944. This study summarizes Wernicke’s model of human brain language processing and presents evidence that brain damage associated with multiple types of aphasias are consistent with this model. It also suggested that cerebral asymmetries in Wernicke’s area were related to left hemisphere laterality for language. 9.
Crosson B: Subcortical mechanisms in language: lexicalsemantic mechanisms and the thalamus. Brain Cogn 1999, 40:414-438. This study discusses a series of aphasic cases involving thalamic lesions and concludes that thalamic nuclei are involved in multiple processes which direcetly or indirectly support cortical language functions.
10. Lieberman P: On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language. Yearbook Phys Anthropol 2002, 45:36-62. This study critiques the traditional model of brain language processing that is focused on Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas, presents evidence that the basal ganglia sequence the discrete elements that constitute a complete motor act, syntactic process or thought process. It argues for the importance of cortical–-cortical circuits in walking, talking and comprehending the meaning of sentences. 11. Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Herve PY, Duffau H, Crivello F, Houde O, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N: Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage 2006, 30:1414-1432. 12. Hickok G, Poeppel D: The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007, 8:393-402. 13. Passingham RE: Broca’s area and the origins of human vocal skill. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1981, 292:167-175. 14. Preuss TM: What is it like to be a human? In The Cognitive Neurosciences. Edited by Gazzaniga . MIT Press; 2004:5-22. 15. Corballis MC: The evolution of language. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009, 1156:19-43. 16. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L: Mirror neurons and social cognition. In The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Edited by Dunbar RIM, Barrett L. Oxford University Press; 2007:179-196. 17. Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC, Mazziotta JC, Lenzi GL: Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003, 100:5497-5502. 18. Caramazza A, Zurif EB: Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Brain Lang 1976, 3:572-582. This study demonstrated that Broca’s aphasics exhibit deficits in using syntactic information to comprehend sentences. 19. Hagoort P: On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends Cogn Sci 2005, 9:416-423. 20. Aitken PG: Cortical control of conditioned and spontaneous vocal behavior in rhesus monkeys. Brain Lang 1981, 13: 171-184.
1.
Pinker S: The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: Perennial Classics; 2000, .
2.
Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT: The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 2002, 298:1569-1579.
21. Jurgens U, Kirzinger A, von Cramon D: The effects of deepreaching lesions in the cortical face area on phonation. A combined case report and experimental monkey study. Cortex 1982, 18:125-139.
3.
Rumbaugh DM, Washburn DA: Intelligence of Apes and other Rational Beings. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2003, .
22. Deacon TW: What makes the human brain different? Annu Rev Anthropol 1997, 26:337-357.
www.sciencedirect.com
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14
14 Communication and language
23. Hopkins WD, Taglialatela J, Leavens DA: Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim Behav 2007, 73:281-286. 24. Taglialatela JP, Russell JL, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD: Communicative signaling activates ‘Broca’s’ homolog in chimpanzees. Curr Biol 2008, 18:343-348. 25. Sakai KL: Language acquisition and brain development. Science 2005, 310:815-819. 26. Schenker NM, Buxhoeveden DP, Blackmon WL, Amunts K, Zilles K, Semendeferi K: A comparative quantitative analysis of cytoarchitecture and minicolumnar organization in Broca’s area in humans and great apes. J Comp Neurol 2008, 510:117-128. AThis study shows that in contrast to humans, chimpanzees do not have left hemispheric assymetry for any measures of Brodmann’s area 44 and 45 (Broca’s area homologue), implying that the asymmetry emerged in human evolution and may constitute a neurological specialization supporting human language. 27. Semendeferi K, Teffer K, Buxhoeveden DP, Park MS, Bludau S, Amunts K, Travis K, Buckwalter J: Spatial organization of neurons in the frontal pole sets humans apart from great apes. Cereb Cortex 2011, 21:1485-1497. 28. Mountcastle VB: The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain 1997, 120(Pt 4):701-722. 29. Schenker NM, Hopkins WD, Spocter MA, Garrison AR, Stimpson CD, Erwin JM, Hof PR, Sherwood CC: Broca’s area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): probabilistic mapping, asymmetry, and comparison to humans. Cerebral Cortex 2010, 20:730. 30. Wilson B, Petkov CI: Communication and the primate brain: insights from neuroimaging studies in humans, chimpanzees and macaques. Hum Biol 2011, 83:175-189. 31. Heffner HE, Heffner RS: Effect of unilateral and bilateral auditory cortex lesions on the discrimination of vocalizations by Japanese macaques. J Neurophysiol 1986, 56:683-701. 32. Hackett TA, Preuss TM, Kaas JH: Architectonic identification of the core region in auditory cortex of macaques, chimpanzees, and humans. J Comp Neurol 2001, 441:197-222. 33. Rauschecker JP, Tian B, Hauser M: Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. Science 1995, 268:111-114. 34. Gil-da-Costa R, Martin A, Lopes MA, Munoz M, Fritz JB, Braun AR: Species-specific calls activate homologs of Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas in the macaque. Nat Neurosci 2006, 9:1064-1070. This study identified neural systems associated with perceiving speciesspecific vocalizations in rhesus macaques using H215O positron emmission tomography (PET). It found that macaques activate homologues of human Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas. 35. Poremba A, Malloy M, Saunders RC, Carson RE, Herscovitch P, Mishkin M: Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey’s temporal poles. Nature 2004, 427:448-451. 36. Geschwind N, Levitsky W: Human brain: left–right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science 1968, 161:186-187.
This study applied functional MRI to humans and macaque monkeys listening to identical stimuli in order to compare the cortical networks involved in processing vocalizations. Human superior temporal sulcus (STS) was responsive to human speech, but macaque STS was not responsive to macaque species-specific vocalizations. 41. Petkov CI, Kayser C, Steudel T, Whittingstall K, Augath M, Logothetis NK: A voice region in the monkey brain. Nat Neurosci 2008, 11:367-374. 42. Petrides M, Pandya DN: Association fiber pathways to the frontal cortex from the superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 1988, 273:52-66. 43. Petrides M, Pandya DN: Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human and the macaque ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and corticocortical connection patterns in the monkey. Eur J Neurosci 2002, 16:291-310. 44. Petrides M, Pandya DN: Distinct parietal and temporal pathways to the homologues of Broca’s area in the monkey. PLoS Biol 2009, 7:e1000170. This study used tracer injections and autoradiography to demonstrate a dorsal connection between the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and Broca’s area homologue in macaque monkeys. The authors refer to this minor connection as a simple arcuate fasciculus. 45. Deacon TW: Cortical connections of the inferior arcuate sulcus cortex in the macaque brain. Brain Res 1992, 573:8-26. 46. Romanski LM, Tian B, Fritz J, Mishkin M, Goldman-Rakic PS, Rauschecker JP: Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 1999, 2:1131-1136. This study combined microelectrode recording and anatomical tract tracing to demonstrate the existance of dorsal and ventral auditory streams in macque monkeys, involved with spatial and non-spatial aspects of auditory processing, respectively. 47. Dejerine J: Anatomie des Centres Nerveux. Paris: Rueff et Cie; 1895, . 48. Mori S, Van Zijl PC: Fiber tracking: principles and strategies—a technical review. NMR Biomed 2002, 15:468-480. 49. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN, Wang R, Dai G, D’Arceuil HE, de Crespigny AJ, Wedeen VJ: Association fibre pathways of the brain: parallel observations from diffusion spectrum imaging and autoradiography. Brain 2007, 130:630-653. 50. Catani M, Howard RJ, Pajevic S, Jones DK: Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. Neuroimage 2002, 17:77-94. 51. Rilling JK, Glasser MF, Preuss TM, Ma X, Zhao T, Hu X, Behrens TE: The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nat Neurosci 2008, 11:426-428. This study used diffusion weighted imaging and tractography to show that the arcuate facsiculus pathway connects with Wernicke’s area or its homologue in humans, chimpanzees and rhesus macaques. However, the human arcuate fasciculus has a much greater projection into the middle and inferior temporal gyri, ventral to classsic Wericke’s area.
37. Gannon PJ, Holloway RL, Broadfield DC, Braun AR: Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: humanlike pattern of Wernicke’s brain language area homolog. Science 1998, 279:220-222.
52. Orban GA, Van Essen D, Vanduffel W: Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans. Trends Cogn Sci 2004, 8:315-324. Reviews comparative fMRI studies of visual system pathways in macaque monkeys and humans to conclude that human early and mid-level visual areas are located more posterior and medially than their corresponding macaque counterparts.
38. Hopkins WD, Marino L, Rilling JK, MacGregor LA: Planum temporale asymmetries in great apes as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Neuroreport 1998, 9:2913-2918.
53. Ungerleider LG, Courtney SM, Haxby JV: A neural system for human visual working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1998, 95:883-890.
39. Buxhoeveden DP, Switala AE, Litaker M, Roy E, Casanova MF: Lateralization of minicolumns in human planum temporale is absent in nonhuman primate cortex. Brain Behav Evol 2001, 57:349-358. It showed that humans, but not rhesus macaques or chimpanzees, have wider cell columns and more neuropil space in the left compared with the right hemisphere within the planum temporale, a part of Wernicke’s area.
54. Glasser MF, Goyal MS, Preuss TM, Raichle ME, Van Essen DC: Trends and properties of human cerebral cortex: correlations with cortical myelin content. Neuroimage 2013. 55. Turken AU, Dronkers NF: The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Front Syst Neurosci 2011, 5:1.
40. Joly O, Pallier C, Ramus F, Pressnitzer D, Vanduffel W, Orban GA: Processing of vocalizations in humans and monkeys: a comparative fMRI study. Neuroimage 2012, 62:1376-1389.
56. Rilling JK, Glasser MF, Jbabdi S, Andersson J, Preuss TM: Continuity, divergence, and the evolution of brain language pathways. Front Evol Neurosci 2011, 3:11.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14
www.sciencedirect.com