Correlation between cognitive deficits and Aβ deposits in transgenic APP+PS1 mice

Correlation between cognitive deficits and Aβ deposits in transgenic APP+PS1 mice

Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386 www.elsevier.com/locate/neuaging Correlation between cognitive deficits and A␤ deposits in transgenic APP⫹PS...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 38 Views

Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

www.elsevier.com/locate/neuaging

Correlation between cognitive deficits and A␤ deposits in transgenic APP⫹PS1 mice Marcia N. Gordona, David L. Kingb, David M. Diamonda,c, Paul T. Jantzena, Kristal V. Boyetta, Caroline E. Hopeb, Jaime M. Hatcherb, Giovanni DiCarloa, W. Paul E. Gottschalla, Dave Morgana,*, Gary W. Arendashb a

Alzheimer’s Research Laboratory, Department of Pharmacology, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33612, USA b Alzheimer’s Research Laboratory, Department of Biology, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33612, USA c Stress and Memory Research Laboratory, Department of Psychology & VA Medical Center, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33612, USA Received 24 April 2000; received in revised form 25 October 2000; accepted 2 November 2000

Abstract Doubly transgenic mAPP⫹mPS1 mice (15–16 months) had impaired cognitive function in a spatial learning and memory task that combined features of a water maze and a radial arm maze. Nontransgenic mice learned a new platform location each day during 4 consecutive acquisition trials, and exhibited memory for this location in a retention trial administered 30 min later. In contrast, transgenic mice were, on average, unable to improve their performance in finding the hidden platform over trials. The cognitive performance of individual mice within the transgenic group were inversely related to the amount of A␤ deposited in the frontal cortex and hippocampus. These findings imply that mAPP⫹mPS1 transgenic mice develop deficits in cognitive ability as A␤ deposits increase. These data argue that radial arm water maze testing of doubly transgenic mice may be a useful behavioral endpoint in evaluating the functional consequences of potential AD therapies, especially those designed to reduce A␤ load. © 2001 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease; Transgenic mice; Learning and memory; Radial arm water maze; Amyloid load; A␤ deposition

1. Introduction Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is characterized by progressive, severe cognitive deficits which include impaired memory for recent events at an early stage of the disease process. Two pathologic characteristics of the disease are A␤ peptide containing, congophilic amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles [49]. Although the extent to which either of these features causes the dementia of Alzheimer’s disease remains unresolved, both have been correlated with the degree of cognitive decline in dementia [11,14,15,18,28,38,46]. One of the first to demonstrate a relationship between amyloid and cognitive function was Blessed et al. [3]. However, this was disputed as primarily arising from an overall group difference between control and demented patients; the correlation disappeared when control cases were excluded [50]. In this study, synapse loss was argued to be the best corre* Corresponding author. Tel.: ⫹1-813-974-3949; fax: ⫹1-813-9742565. E-mail address: [email protected] (D. Morgan).

late of cognitive decline in dements. Roses [47] has mentioned that because the presence of amyloid is required to diagnose AD, and its absence required for “control” cases, it would be impossible for amyloid not to correlate with dementia rating when control cases are included. One of the most careful studies found that both A␤ deposits and neurofibrillary tangles in entorhinal cortex correlate with dementia rating, even when the control cases were excluded [11]. A very recent study [46] found that elevated A␤ content in frontal cortex was predictive of declining cognitive function in a large panel of carefully collected elderly cases. The A␤ elevations preceded neuritic pathology in this patient set. However, even this study did not report correlations between A␤ measurements and dementia rating that excluded nondemented cases. Therefore, the relationship between A␤ pathology and cognitive function in Alzheimer’s disease remains a point of contention (see commentaries following [11]). A small fraction of Alzheimer’s cases results from inheritance of an autosomal dominant mutation. Mutations in at least three genes can account for the disorder in inherited

0197-4580/01/$ – see front matter © 2001 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved. PII: S 0 1 9 7 - 4 5 8 0 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 2 4 9 - 9

378

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

cases [27]. Mice expressing mutant forms of the amyloid precursor protein (mAPP) develop A␤ deposits that resemble those found in AD [24,33]. When mice expressing mAPP are crossed with mice expressing a mutant form of presenilin 1 (mPS1), the rate of deposition is accelerated by a factor of 2 to 4 [5,30,31,43]. Thus far, mice expressing mPS1 transgenes alone have not been found to deposit A␤, although brain concentrations of the long A␤ variant(s) are elevated [6,10,21]. Behaviorally, mAPP⫹mPS1 transgenic mice have been observed to have intact acquisition and retention in the Morris water maze when tested at 6 or 9 months of age, in spite of increasing A␤ load in cortex and hippocampus [1,31]. Age-related increases in brain A␤ deposits have been reported for several transgenic models of AD [24,25, 33,36,49]. In some transgenic lines behavioral effects of the transgenes have been reported [9,32,33,39]. Some of these behavioral deficits appear ontogenically before the histologic deposition of A␤ has started [12,20,32,37,39]. Thus far, a correlation between declines in behavioral performance and the amount of A␤ deposition within individual transgenic mice (excluding controls) has not been reported. The present study examined whether cognitive impairments could be reliably detected in mAPP ⫹ mPS1 transgenic mice, and whether the amount of A␤ pathology in each transgenic mouse was correlated with the extent of its behavioral impairment.

2. Materials and methods 2.1. Animals Ten doubly transgenic PS1M146L ⫹ APPK670N,M671L (3 males and 7 females; PS1 line 5.1, ref 21; APP line Tg2576, ref 33) and 11 nontransgenic litter mates (4 males and 7 females) were tested when 14.5 to 16 months of age; mean 15.5). The Tg2576 mice derive from a C57B6/SJL X C57B6 background, while the PS1–5.1 line derives from a Swiss Webster/B6D2F1⫻ B6D2F1 background, producing progeny with a mixture of these backgrounds. After weaning, animals were genotyped by slot blot analysis of tail DNA and group housed until several weeks prior to behavioral testing, at which time they were individually housed. All animals had free access to water and rodent chow and were maintained on a 12-h light/dark cycle. All behavioral testing was done in the light phase. Prior to water maze testing, a sensorimotor test battery was administered to verify mice were not blind, deaf or neurologically impaired [31,39]. 2.2. Radial arm water maze testing The maze consisted of a 100 cm circular pool with 6 swim arms radiating from a common circular swim area (similar to that described for rats; ref [17]). On each of 9

training days, a submerged escape platform was positioned near the end of a goal arm (goal arm location was moved each day). On each day, there were 4 consecutive acquisition trials of 1 min each (designated T1 through T4). For each acquisition trial, the mouse was placed in a start arm for that trial (which was never the goal arm). The start arm sequence for each day’s acquisition trials was chosen randomly except that no start arm was used for more than one acquisition trial. The number of errors (arm entries that did not result in finding the platform) was recorded. If the animal made an error, it was pulled back into the start arm for that trial to permit another choice to be made. For each testing day, “error reduction” between T1 and T4 was used as an within-day index of learning. Following T4, the animal was returned to its home cage for 30 min, and then given a single retention trial (designated T5), with the start arm being the same as T4 for that day. On a small number of trials (⬍2%), mice did not make any arm entries. When this occurred, these values were excluded from the statistical analyses. All mice used in this study were physically capable of swimming through the maze. 2.3. Histopathology Within 1 week of completing radial arm water maze testing mice were anesthetized with pentobarbital (100 mg/kg i.p.), perfused transcardially with 25 ml normal (0.9%) saline, the brains were rapidly removed, and the left hemispheres were immersion fixed in freshly depolymerized 4% paraformaldehyde (buffered to pH 7.4 in potassium phosphate) for 24 h, then cryoprotected in a series of sucrose solutions. Fixed, cryoprotected hemispheres were frozen and sectioned in the horizontal plane at 25 ␮m using a sliding microtome and stored at 4°C in Dulbecco’s phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) for immunocytochemistry and histology. Immunocytochemistry was performed on floating sections as described in detail previously [26]. The “total A␤” primary antiserum was raised against A␤1– 40 in rabbits and recognizes both A␤1– 42 and A␤1– 40 in ELISA assays, and was blocked by preabsorption with either peptide, or A␤1–16. Antisera selective for A␤ ending at residue 40 and A␤ ending at residue 42 were purchased from Quality Controlled Biochemical (Hopkinton, MA). Selectivity was confirmed by preabsorption experiments, which blocked all punctate staining. Sections were incubated with the primary antibody overnight at 4°C, then incubated in the biotinylated secondary antibody (2 h) followed by streptavidin-peroxidase. Peroxidase reactions consisted of 1.4 mM diaminobenzidine with 0.03% hydrogen peroxide in PBS for exactly 5 min. Nonspecific reaction product formation was negligible as assessed by omitting the primary antibody and/or preincubating antisera with A␤. The low background staining seen in nontransgenic mice confirmed the near absence of nonspecific antibody reactivity. Each assay was balanced with respect to the experimental groups. Congo red histology was carried out using sections

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

mounted on slides and air dried for a minimum of 12 h, then rehydrated for approximately 30 s before staining. Hydrated sections were incubated in an alkaline alcoholic saturated sodium chloride solution (2.5 mM NaOH in 80% reagent alcohol [95% ethanol, 5% isopropanol], freshly prepared) for 20 min, then incubated in 0.2% Congo red in alkaline alcoholic saturated sodium chloride solution (freshly prepared and filtered) for 30 min. Sections were rinsed through three rapid changes of 100% ethanol, cleared through three changes of xylene, then coverslipped with Permount.

379

ANOVA for each 3 day block, with trials and genotype as the independent variables. Where significant ANOVA effects were observed, individual mean differences between transgenic and nontransgenic mice were tested using the Tukey-Kramer test. In transgenic animals only (there was no pathology in nontransgenic mice) linear regression analysis was performed between the A␤/Congo red staining and the T1–T4 or T5 error measurements.

3. Results 2.4. Image analysis 3.1. Histopathology Immunocytochemistry or Congo red staining of tissue sections was quantified with an Oncor V150 image analysis system. The software uses hue, saturation and intensity (HSI) to segment objects in the image field. Operationally, thresholds for object segmentation are established using a series of standard slides which have the extremes of intensity for the stain being measured. Thresholds in HSI space are established which accurately identify objects on all standard slides and these segmentation thresholds remain constant throughout the analysis session. Section to section variability in immunostaining is minor owing to rigid fixation and staining protocols and the HSI segmentation parameters accurately identify positive staining on all sections. The operator was unaware of the behavioral score or genotype when measurements were taken. Staining in the frontal cortex and the hippocampus were quantified from 4 horizontal sections from each mouse spaced 600 ␮m apart beginning at 2000 ␮m ventral to bregma. The starting sections from each mouse were matched as closely as possible. The frontal cortex measurement used a 80 ⫻ field with one limit of the field of view resting on the edge of the cortex and the other limit resting the midline in the most anterior position possible. The measurement area was a rectangular video field of 850,000 ␮m2 (0.85 mm2 located within the center of that field of view. The measurement area was primarily of the middle 2/3rd of the cortical mantle (i.e. not including laminae 1 or 6). Hippocampus used the standard boundaries of the fimbria anteriorly, ventricle medially and corpus callosum laterally and posteriorly. Using the standard nomenclature, “A␤ load” is the percent of area in the measurement field occupied by reaction product. Similarly, “Congo red staining” refers to the percent of the area that is stained with Congo red. All values from a given mouse were averaged to represent the single value for that animal in statistical analyses. 2.5. Statistical analysis For analysis of the behavioral data, results were grouped into blocks of 3 days each, representing different stages of procedural knowledge regarding the task. The number of errors for T1, T4 or T5 over those 3 days were averaged for each mouse, and analyzed by 2-way repeated measures

Genotype had a profound influence on the amount of total A␤ deposition (Fig. 1). While the nontransgenic mice had no detectable A␤ immunoreactivity (Fig. 1A), a substantial number of A␤ deposits could be readily discerned in all transgenic mice (Fig. 1B). Although A␤ deposits were restricted to hippocampus and cerebral cortex in young mice [30,31], these deposits were also found in striatum, thalamus and brain stem of 15 months old transgenic mice. In cerebral cortex, there was a general correspondence between birefringent Congo red stained amyloid deposits (Fig. 2D) and A␤ deposits stained in serial sections with antisera for A␤ peptides ending at residue 40 (A␤40; Fig. 2B), residue 42 (A␤42; Fig. 2C) and total A␤ (Fig. 2A). The total A␤ and A␤42 antisera label both the congophilic deposits and a large number of smaller, less intensely stained deposits. The A␤40 antisera stain the congophilic deposits heavily, but are less capable of revealing the small, less intensely stained deposits (Fig. 2). When measured by image analysis, the doubly transgenic mice had total A␤ loads in frontal cortex that averaged 52%, with a range from 28 to 64%. In the hippocampus, the total A␤ load in the transgenic mice averaged 25% (range 10 to 42%). The values for Congo red staining were 20 fold less than the total A␤ load (range 2.0 to 3.3% in frontal cortex). The values for A␤42 load were similar to those for total A␤, while the A␤40 load ranged 5–10%. 3.2. Radial arm water maze performance The radial arm water maze has walls within the water tank which direct the mice to swim into one of six swim paths (arms) in a manner similar to arm entries in the dry radial arm maze (Fig. 3). Each day, mice had the opportunity to learn which arm contained the hidden platform on 4 consecutive swim trials (T1 through T4). The hidden platform was always located in the same arm on each trial within a day and in a different arm across days. The difference between T1 and T4 errors provided a simple means with which to quantify within-day learning for each animal. Within-day retention was tested on T5, which took place 30 min after T4 was completed. Fig. 4 summarizes the performance of both nontrans-

380

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

Fig. 1. Regional distribution of A␤ deposition in 16 month old mice. No A␤ deposition is found in the nontransgenic mice (panel A). In APP⫹PS1 mice (panel B), there is a high density of deposits in hippocampus, cortex and striatum, with smaller numbers of deposits appearing in thalamic and brainstem structures. The cerebellum lacks parenchymal A␤ deposits, although vascular deposits are visible at high magnification. Sections from one hemisphere were collected in the horizontal plane. cbl ⫽ cerebellum, ecx ⫽ entorhinal cortex, hc ⫽ hippocampus, pcx ⫽ pyriform cortex, str ⫽ striatum, thal ⫽ thalamus. Scale bar equals 2 mm.

genic and transgenic mice on this task from the time they were naive (Block 1) until they were well-trained (Block 3). In the first block, during which all mice were first learning

the procedural components of the task, (i.e. that there was an escape platform located at the end of an arm and that the platform location was constant within a day), there were no

Fig. 2. Colocalization of A␤ immunohistochemistry with Congo red staining in frontal cortex. Four adjacent sections were stained with antisera to total A␤ (panel A) A␤40 (panel B), A␤42 (panel C) or Congo red (panel D, viewed with cross-polarization). Four discrete deposits (arrowheads) are found in the same relative location in each panel, suggesting they are the same deposits. Scale bar equals 200 ␮m.

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

381

3.3. Correlations between histopathology and radial arm water maze performance Given that there were cognitive deficits found in the transgenic mice, we evaluated whether the extent of A␤ deposition was related to the degree of cognitive impairments in individual mice. In transgenic mice, the Congo red staining in frontal cortex was positively correlated with the number of errors they committed on T5 (r ⫽ 0.74, P ⬍ 0.05; Fig. 5A). A similar correlation was observed with cortical A␤40 load and errors on T5 (r ⫽ 0.83; P ⬍ 0.01; Fig. 5B). Hence, the extent of cortical pathology correlated with the magnitude of the retention trial impairment. Analysis of the learning index (T1 errors–T4 errors) with the histopathology revealed a relationship between total A␤ load in the frontal cortex and hippocampus with learning. There was a significant negative correlation between acquisition and total A␤ load in frontal cortex (r ⫽ ⫺0.74; P ⬍ 0.05; Fig. 6A) and hippocampus (r ⫽ ⫺0.70; P ⬍ 0.05; Fig. 6B). Therefore, greater total A␤ load in these two brain areas correlated with a greater impairment of cognitive function in the transgenic mice.

4. Discussion Fig. 3. Radial arm water maze procedure. The maze consists of a circular pool with 6 aluminum wedges (black inserts in pool) which restrict swimming to 6 arms and a central area. In the example given, the submerged platform is placed in arm 2. The mouse is started in arm 4 for that trial and must find the platform by swimming to the end of arm 2. Entry into arms other than 2 results in the mouse being gently pulled back to arm 4 to begin selection once more. On the next trial for that day, the start arm would be 1, 3, 5 or 6. Mice are given four consecutive trials, and, 30 min later, a fifth retention trial. The start arm for trial 5 is the same as that in trial 4. The next day, the submerged platform is moved to a different arm than the day before.

effects of genotype or trials on the number of errors. In block 2, there was a significant effect of trials (F(2, 28) ⫽ 6.5, P ⬍ 0.01), but not of genotype, indicating that, in general, the animals were improving their performance from Tl to T4 and T5. By the end of training (Block 3), the nontransgenic mice had clearly acquired the task, with most mice making 0 or 1 errors on T5. ANOVA indicated a significant effect of trials (F(2,28) ⫽ 3.5; P ⬍ 0.05), genotype (F(1,29) ⫽ 9.1; P ⬍ 0.005) and a trials by genotype interaction (F(2,26) ⫽ 3.5; P ⬍ 0.05). Tukey-Kramer comparisons indicated that there was a significant difference between the transgenic and nontransgenic mice on T5 (P ⬍ 0.01; asterisk on Fig. 4). Thus, by the third block of training days the control mice were able to learn and remember the within-day location of the hidden platform. The transgenic mice, by contrast, showed no evidence of improvement over trials on either T4 or T5 in the third block of trials.

The clear differences in behavioral performance between two groups compared here indicate the radial arm water maze task is valuable in distinguishing the cognitive performance capacities of transgenic and nontransgenic mice. The feasibility of imposing the radial maze alleyways onto a swim tank was first described by Buresova et al. [7], using a win-shift strategy. Recently, Diamond et al. [17], demonstrated that the win-stay approach (used in the present study) was a very sensitive index of rodent working memory impairment produced by stress. This variant of the water maze paradigm has several advantages over the more widely used reference memory (fixed platform location) version of this task [31,39]. First, the relocation of the platform forces mice to solve the task using working memory. In the traditional water maze task [45], the constant platform location each day simplifies the task and permits the slow acquisition of static information. Second, the ability to use errors rather than escape latency diminishes the impact of swimming performance (ability/speed) on the values used to assess learning. This may prove particularly useful in evaluating older animals, where variations in swim speed may be considerable, independent of memory skills. Third, performing the retention trial on the same day as the repeated acquisition trials permits evaluation of memory functions on a time scale (less than an hour) similar to the registration and recall tests used clinically to diagnose the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease. A final advantage of this task is that it can be performed longitudinally, permitting identification of the age at which each mouse begins to exhibit acquisition and/or retention deficits. The cognitive

382

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

Fig. 4. Impaired working memory in the radial arm water maze by transgenic mice. The number of errors on trial 1 (circles), trial 4 (triangles) and the 30 min retention trial (squares) are presented for the transgenic (solid symbols) and nontransgenic(open symbols) mice. Data are summarized for three day blocks demonstrating the gradual acquisition of the procedural components of the task by the nontransgenic animals. The T1-T4 errors are considered an index of within day learning (lines). Data are mean ⫾ sem. * P ⬍ 0.01 on Trial 5 by Tukey-Kramer analysis. See text for other statistical results.

deficits found here in the transgenic mice suggest this task will be useful in screening therapeutic approaches expected to reduce cognitive dysfunction in AD. Our earlier work with mAPP⫹mPS1 mice identified a deficiency in Y maze alternation that appeared prior to the first A␤ deposits [30,31]. Interestingly, at 15–16 months, doubly transgenic mice no longer exhibit deficits in Y maze alternation [1], suggesting this task is unlikely to be useful in screening therapeutic approaches intended for AD. By contrast, doubly transgenic mice are not impaired in radial arm water maze performance at 5–7 or 11.5 months of age [1,44], but are impaired in the working memory aspect of radial arm water maze performance by15.5 months. Thus, age-related cognitive impairment in this task occurs considerably after the appearance of A␤ pathology in the same transgenic model, and only after a significant accumulation of A␤ has occurred, providing the opportunity to determine the relationship between behavior impairment and A␤ pathology. The main outcome of this study was a strong correlation between the extent of A␤ neuropathology in aging transgenic mice and their behavioral impairment in the radial arm water maze. Specifically, fibrillar A␤ deposit area correlated with the number of errors on the retention trial in individual transgenic mice. Whether measured by Congo red staining or by A␤40 immunostaining, the area occupied by compacted, fibrillar amyloid was strongly associated

with this index of memory performance. We emphasize that these correlations did not depend upon the inclusion of control (nontransgenic) mice. These results imply that some transgenic mice (those with the lowest A␤ levels) were demonstrating at least partial learning of the platform location, even though the average performance of the group did not improve over trials. The distribution of A␤40 and A␤42 immunoreactivities reported here was very similar to that found in Alzheimer’s and Down’s syndrome brains [34,35,48]. Alzheimer’s disease researchers have a range of opinions regarding the pathogenicity of fibrillar versus nonfibrillar A␤, including some who argue that A␤ accumulation is epiphenomenal. The results presented here are consistent with the view that fibrillar A␤ can interfere with cognitive function. However they do not rule out the possibility that fibrillar deposits might, alternatively, act as an index of the rate at which A␤ is being produced in that animal, and that all pools of A␤ (soluble, dimeric, oligomeric, intracellular) covary with this readily detectable marker. Possible actions associated with A␤ that might cause such interference are degeneration of synapses [24,51], formation of dystrophic neurites [41,43], induction of inflammatory reactions [2,23], loss of LTP [9] or selective degeneration of memory-critical neurons [8]. The present correlations between memory and histopathology within the transgenic mice were made possible by the broad range of values for both A␤ pathology and be-

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

Fig. 5. Compacted amyloid staining area predicts water maze retention in transgenic mice. The percent of frontal cortex area occupied by Congo red staining was correlated with the number of errors on trial 5 of the radial arm water maze in transgenic mice (panel A). A similar correlation was found between A␤40 staining and the number of errors on trial 5 in transgenic mice (panel B).

havioral performance, permitting detection of correlations in a relatively small group of mice. The association found here is consistent with an earlier study in aged dogs, where a correlation was found between brain A␤ accumulation and acquisition of a visual discrimination task [29]. These results are similarly consistent with the finding that A␤ load correlates with intellectual status in humans, (although, in some studies, other measures of AD pathology; e.g. synapse density, tangle distribution; correlate better; [3,11,13–15,18, 28,38,46,50]). In addition, total A␤ load was correlated with the learning index in transgenic mice. Even though transgenic mice on average did not learn the task, some did improve more than others, as shown in Figs. 5 and 6. This correlation occurred in both the hippocampus and frontal cortex, while the correlation of fibrillar A␤ with trial 5 retention perfor-

383

Fig. 6. Total A␤ load predicts acquisition learning in transgenic mice. The reduction in errors between trial 1 and trial 4 for the transgenic mice correlated inversely with the A␤ load measurement for each mouse in both frontal cortex (panel A) and hippocampus (panel B).

mance was only found in frontal cortex. Typically tasks designed to test working memory are thought to be hippocampus dependent, and one might have predicted a greater role for hippocampal A␤. Importantly, the correlation for hippocampal fibrillar A␤ and trial 5 retention was positive, but not significant statistically. Additionally, water maze tasks appear to involve rapid consolidation, as memory for the hidden platform location becomes stable to electroconvulsive shock between 15 and 30 s post-trial [4]. Perhaps this rapid consolidation indicates a greater role of cortical structures in short-term retention of water maze tasks. The total A␤ load measurement includes both fibrillar (compacted) and presumably nonfibrillar, diffuse A␤ deposits. Given that the amount of Congo red staining is 1/20th of the staining area labeled with antibodies to A␤ (similar to that in human brain, ref [16]), we assume the bulk of the

384

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

total A␤ staining was in the non-Congophilic diffuse form. Thus the correlation between learning deficits and total A␤ load in the transgenic mice could represent interference of nonfibrillar A␤ with neural function. This is consistent with the data that intracranial (i.c.) injections of A␤ peptides can cause amnesia in mice and rats [22,40,42]. It also compliments the recent data from Dodart et al. [19], finding that object recognition deficits in PDAPP mice occurred even on the APO E null background, which fail to deposit fibrillar A␤. In conclusion, these results indicate that deposition of A␤ and formation of congophilic deposits were associated with declining cognitive function in 16 month old mAPP ⫹ mPS1 transgenic mice. The significant relationship between behavioral measures and A␤ pathology in individual mice reinforces the advantages of using the radial arm version of the water maze. Together with our additional work [1,31, 44], the data indicate that behavioral deficits develop over time as the transgenic mice accumulate increasing amounts of A␤. These data support the use of the radial arm water maze to evaluate the functional consequences of potential treatments for AD in transgenic mouse models.

Acknowledgments Supported by AG 15490 (MNG), AG 14633 (DM), AG 12160 (PG), the Benjamin Alzheimer trust (DM), and Merit Review Funding from the Veterans Administration (DMD). We thank Karen Hsiao for providing the APP mice and Karen Duff for providing the PS1 mice.

References [1] Arendash GW, King DL, Gordon MN, Morgan D, Hatcher JM, Hope CE, Diamond DM. Progressive, age-related behavioral impairments in transgenic mice carrying both mutant amyloid precursor protein (APP), and presenilin 1 (PS1) transgenes. Brain Research, 2001;891: 45–53. [2] Benzing WC, Wujek JR, Ward EK, Schaffer D, Ashe KH, Younkin SG, Brunden KR. Evidence for glial-mediated inflammation in APPsw transgenic mice. Neurobiology of Aging 1999;20:581–90. [3] Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M. The association between quantitative measures of dementia, and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br J Psychiatry 1968;114:797– 811. [4] Bohbot V, Otahal P, Liu Z, Nadel L, Bures J. Electroconvulsive shock, and lidocaine reveal rapid consolidation of spatial working memory in the water maze. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996;93: 4016 –9. [5] Borchelt DR, Ratovitski T, Van Lare J, Lee MK, Gonzales V, Jenkins NA, Copeland NG, Price DL, Sisodia SS. Accelerated amyloid deposition in the brains of transgenic mice coexpressing mutant presenilin 1, and amyloid precursor protein. Neuron 1997;19:939 – 45. [6] Borchelt DR, Thinakaran G, Eckman CB, Lee MK, Davenport F, Ratovitsky T, Prada CM, Kim G, Seekins S, Yager D, Slunt HH, Wang R, Seeger M, Levey AI, Gandy SE, Copeland NG, Jenkins NA, Price DL, Younkin SG, Sisodia SS. Familial Alzheimer’s diseaselinked presenilin 1 variants elevate Abeta1– 42/1– 40 ratio in vitro, and in vivo. Neuron 1996;17:1005–13.

[7] Buresova O, Bures J, Oitzl MS, Zahalka A. Radial maze in the water tank: an aversively motivated spatial working memory task. Physiol Behav 1985;34:1003–5. [8] Calhoun ME, Wiederhold KH, Abramowski D, Phinney AL, Probst A, Sturchler-Pierrat C, Staufenbiel M, Sommer B, Jucker M. Neuron loss in APP transgenic mice. [letter] Nature 1998;395:755– 6. [9] Chapman PF, White GL, Jones MW, Cooper-Blacketer D, Marshall VJ, Irizarry M, Younkin L, Good MA, Bliss TV, Hyman BT, Younkin SG, Hsiao KK. Impaired synaptic plasticity, and learning in aged amyloid precursor protein transgenic mice. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:271– 6. [10] Citron M, Westaway D, Xia W, Carlson G, Diehl T, Levesque G, Johnson-Wood K, Lee M, Seubert P, Davis A, Kholodenko D, Motter R, Sherrington R, Perry B, Yao H, Strome R, Lieberburg I, Rommens J, Kim S, Schenk D, Fraser P, St George Hyslop P, Selkoe DJ. Mutant presenilins of Alzheimer’s disease increase production of 42-residue amyloid beta-protein in both transfected cells, and transgenic mice. [see comments] Nat Med 1997;3:67–72. [11] Cummings B, Pike CJ, Shankle R, Cotman CW. Beta-amyloid deposition, and other measures of neuropathology predict cognitive status in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 1996;17:921–33. [12] D’Hooge R, Nagels G, Westland CE, Mucke L, DeDeyn PP. Spatial learning deficit in mice expressing human 751-amino acid betaamyloid precursor protein. NeuroReport 1996;7:2807–11. [13] DeKosky ST, Scheff SW. Synapse loss in frontal cortex biopsies in Alzheimer’s disease: correlation with cognitive severity. Ann Neurol 1990;27:457– 64. [14] Delaere P, Duyckaerts C, Brion JP, Poulain V, Hauw JJ. Tau,paired helical filaments, and amyloid in the neocortex: a moprhometric study of 15 cases with graded intellectual status in aging, and senile dementia of Alzheimer type. Acta Neuropathol 1989;77:645–53. [15] Delaere P, Duyckaerts C, He Y, Piette F, Hauw JJ. Subtypes, and differential laminar distributions of beta A4 deposits in Alzheimer’s disease: relationship with the intellectual status of 26 cases. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1991;81:328 –35. [16] Delaere P, He Y, Fayet G, Duyckaerts C, Hauw JJ. Beta A4 deposits are constant in the brain of the oldest old: an immunocytochemical study of 20 French centenarians. Neurobiol Aging 1993;14:191– 4. [17] Diamond DM, Park CR, Heman KL, Rose GM. Exposing rats to a predator impairs spatial working memory in the radial arm water maze. Hippocampus 1999;9:542–52. [18] Dickson DW, Crystal HA, Bevona C, Honer W, Vincent I, Davies P. Correlations of synaptic, and pathological markers with cognition of the elderly. Neurobiol Aging 1995;16:285–98. [19] Dodart J-C, Mathis C, Bales KR, Paul SM, Ungerer A. Behavioral deficits in APPV717F transgenic mice deficient for the apolipoprotein E gene. NeuroReport 2000;11:603–7. [20] Dodart JC, Meziane H, Mathis C, Bales KR, Paul SM, Ungerer A. Behavioral disturbances in transgenic mice overexpressing the V717F beta-amyloid precursor protein. Behav Neurosci 1999;113:982–90. [21] Duff K, Eckman C, Zehr C, Yu X, Prada CM, Perez-tur J, Hutton M, Buee L, Harigaya Y, Yager D, Morgan D, Gordon MN, Holcomb L, Refolo L, Zenk B, Hardy J, Younkin S. Increased amyloid-beta42(43) in brains of mice expressing mutant presenilin 1. Nature 1996;383:710–3. [22] Flood JF, Morley JE, Roberts E. Amnestic effects in mice of four synthetic peptides homologous to amyloid beta protein from patients with Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1991;88:3363– 6. [23] Frautschy SA, Yang F, Irrizarry M, Hyman B, Saido TC, Hsiao K, Cole GM. Microglial response to amyloid plaques in APPsw transgenic mice. [see comments] Am J Pathol 1998;152:307–17. [24] Games D, Adams D, Alessandrini R, Barbour R, Berthelette P, Blackwell C, Carr T, Clemens J, Donaldson T, Gillespie F, et al. Alzheimer-type neuropathology in transgenic mice overexpressing V717F beta-amyloid precursor protein. [see comments] Nature 1995; 373:523–7. [25] Gordon MN, Gottschall PG, O’Callaghan JP, Hsiao KK, Duff K, Morgan D. Progression of neuropathology in transgenic mice ex-

M.N. Gordon et al. / Neurobiology of Aging 22 (2001) 377–386

[26]

[27] [28]

[29]

[30]

[31]

[32]

[33]

[34]

[35]

[36]

[37] [38]

[39]

pressing both amyloid precursor protein, and presenilin-1 mutations. Neurosci Abstr 1998;24:1502. Gordon MN, Schreier WA, Ou X, Holcomb LA, Morgan DG. Exageratted astrocyte reactivity after nigrostriatal deafferentation in the aged rat. J Comp Neurol 1997;388:106 –19. Hardy J. Amyloid, the presenilins, and Alzheimer’s disease. Trends Neurosci 1997;20:154 – 60. Haroutunian V, Purohit DP, Perl DP, Marin D, Khan K, Lantz M, Davis KL, Mohs RC. Neurofibrillary tangles in nondemented elderly subjects, and mild Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 1999;56:713–78. Head E, Callahan H, Muggenburg BA, Cotman CW, Milgram NW. Visual-discrimination learning ability, and beta-amyloid accumulation in the dog. Neurobiology of Aging 1998;19:415–25. Holcomb L, Gordon MN, McGowan E, Yu X, Benkovic S, Jantzen P, Wright K, Saad I, Mueller R, Morgan D, Sanders S, Zehr C, O’Campo K, Hardy J, Prada CM, Eckman C, Younkin S, Hsiao K, Duff K. Accelerated Alzheimer-type phenotype in transgenic mice carrying both mutant amyloid precursor protein, and presenilin 1 transgenes. Nat Med 1998;4:97–100. Holcomb LA, Gordon MN, Jantzen P, Hsiao K, Duff K, Morgan D. Behavioral changes in transgenic mice expressing both amyloid precursor protein, and presenilin-1 mutations: lack of association with amyloid deposits. Behav Gen 1999;29:177– 85. Hsiao K, Borchelt D, Olson K, Johansdottir R, Kitt C, Yunis W, Xu S, Eckman C, Younkin S, Price D, Iadecola C, Clark HB, Carlson G. Age-related CNS disorder, and early death in transgenic FVB/N mice overexpressing Alzheimer amyloid precursor proteins. Neuron 1995; 15:1203–18. Hsiao K, Chapman P, Nilsen S, Eckman C, Harigaya Y, Younkin S, Yang F, Cole G. Correlative memory deficits, Abeta elevation, and amyloid plaques in transgenic mice. Science 1996;274:99 –102. Iwatsubo T, Mann DM, Odaka A, Suzuki N, Ihara Y. Amyloid beta protein (A beta) deposition: a beta 42(43) precedes A beta 40 in Down syndrome. [see comments] Ann Neurol 1995;37:294 –9. Iwatsubo T, Odaka A, Suzuki N, Mizusawa H, Nukina N, Ihara Y. Visualization of A␤42(43), and A␤40 in senile plaques with endspecific A␤ monoclonals: evidence that an initially deposited species is A␤42(43). Neuron 1994;13:45–53. Johnson-Wood K, Lee M, Motter R, Hu K, Gordon G, Barbour R, Khan K, Gordon M, Tan H, Games D, Lieberburg I, Schenk D, Seubert P, McConlogue L. Amyloid precursor protein processing, and A beta42 deposition in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997;94:1550 –5. Justice A, Motter R. Behavioral characterization of PDAPP transgenic Alzheimer mice. Neurosci Abst 1997;23(2):1637. Kanne SM, Balota DA, Storandt M, McKeel DW, Morris JC. Relating anatomy to function in Alzheimer’s disease: neuropsychological profiles predict regional neuropathology 5 years later. Neurology 1998;50:979 – 85. King DL, Arendash GW, Crawford F, Sterk T, Menendez J, Mullan MJ. Progressive, and gender-dependent cognitive impairment in the

[40]

[41]

[42]

[43]

[44]

[45] [46]

[47]

[48]

[49]

[50]

[51]

385

APPsw transgenic mouse model for Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Brain Res 1999;103:45–162. MacDonald MP, Overmeir JB, Bandyopadhyay S, Babcock D, Cleary J. Reversal of beta-amyloid induced retention deficit after exposure to training, and state cues. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1996;65:35– 47. Masliah E, Sisk A, Mallory M, Mucke L, Schenk D, Games D. Comparison of neurodegenerative pathology in transgenic mice overexpressing V717F beta-amyloid precursor protein, and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 1996;16:5795– 811. Maurice T, Lockhart BP, Privat A. Amnesia induced in mice by centrally administered beta-amyloid peptides involves cholinergic dysfunction. Brain Res 1996;706:181–93. McGowan E, Sanders S, Iwatsubo T, Takeuchi A, Saido T, Zehr C, Yu X, Uljon S, Wang R, Mann D, Dickson D, Duff K. Amyloid phenotype characterization of transgenic mice overexpressing both mutant amyloid precursor protein, and mutant presenilin 1 transgenes. Neurobiol Dis 1999;6:231– 44. Morgan D, Diamond D, Gottschall P, Ugen KE, Dickey C, Hardy J, Jantzen P, DiCarlo G, Wilcock D, Connor K, Hatcher J, Gordon M, Arendash G. Vaccination with A␤ peptide prevents age-related memory deficits in an animal model of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 2000; 408:982–985. Morris RGM, Garrod P, Rawlins JNP, O’Keefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 1982;297:681–3. Naslund J, Haroutunian V, Mohs R, Davis KL, Davies P, Greengard P, Buxbaum JD. Correlation between elevated levels of amyloid beta-peptide in the brain, and cognitive decline. JAMA 2000 Mar 22–29;283(12):1571–7 2000;283:1571–157. Roses AD. Apolipoprotein E affects the rate of Alzheimer disease expression: beta-amyloid burden is a secondary consequence dependent on APOE genotype, and duration of disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1994;53:429 –37. Savage M, Kawooya J, Pinsker L, Emmons T, Mistretta S, Siman R, Greenberg B. Elevated A␤ levels in Alzheimers disease brain are associated with a selective accumulation of A␤42 in parenchymal amyloid deposits, and both A␤40, and A␤42 in cerebrovascular deposits. Amyloid Int J Exp Clin Invest 1995;2:234 – 40. Sturchler-Pierrat C, Abramowski D, Duke M, Wiederhold K-H, Mistl C, Rothacher S, Ledermann B, Burki K, Frey P, Paganetti PA, Waridel C, Calhoun ME, Jucker M, Probst A, Staufenbiel M, Sommer B. Two amyloid precursor protein transgenic mouse models with Alzheimer disease-like pathology. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997;94: 13287–92. Terry RD, Masliah E, Salmon DP, Butter N, DeTeresa R, Hill R, Hansen LA, Katzman R. Physical basis of cogntive alterations in Alzheimer’s disease: synapse loss is the major correlate of cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 1991;30:572– 80. Wong TP, Debeir T, Duff K, Cuello AC. Reorganization of cholinergic terminals in the cerebral cortex, and hippocampus in transgenic mice carrying mutated presenilin-1, and amyloid precursor protein transgenes. J Neurosci 1999;19:2706 –16.