Food @alrQ and Prefnmcc Vol. 7, No. 3/4, pp. 177-183, 1996 Copyright 0 1996 Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain. All tights reserved ELSEVIER
PII:SO950-3293(96)00022-5
0950-3293/c% 815.00+0.00
COULDSELECTIONTESTS DETECTTHEFUTURE PERFORMANCEOFDESCRIPTIVEPANELLISTS? I. Lesschaeve*
&S. lssanchoui
INRA Laboratoire de Recherches sur les ArBmes, B.V. 1540, 21034, Dijon Cedex, France (Accepted 2 7 April 1996)
sensory literature, the time dedicated to the training of descriptive panels varies from 10 hours (Stone & Sidel, 1985) to more than 100 hours (Meilgaard et al., 1991;
ABSTRACT
Heisserer & Chambers IV, 1993). To reduce this time, some authors have suggested the use of a generic pre-specified vocabulary for a particular type of product (e.g. beer aroma wheel, Meilgaard et al., 1982; wine aroma wheel, Noble et al., 1984). In that case, some characteristics may be missed if the products do not fit the sensory space ana-
This paper discusses the appropriateness of screening tests in explaining descriptive panellist performances. It is based on a case study aimed at forming a descriptive panel capable of jlavour projling Camembert cheeses. Eighteen subjects were selected using four sensory tasks evaluating smell sensitivities, olfactory knowledge, odour memory and descriptive ability. Three additional tests were proposed during the 45 hour training to evaluate the recognition memoryfor odours, the concentration and the verbal creativity abilities. Panellist performances were determined on repeatability and discrimination abilities, and on the complexi of the individual sensory space. Some signiJicant relationships were observed. Indeed, to be discriminant on flavour attributes, subjects must either have an initial olfactive culture or have a good ability to memorize olfactive stimuli. Copyright 0 I996 Elsevier Science Ltd
lysed for the vocabulary development. Aiming to discard the training altogether, Williams & Langron (1984) proposed a new technique, the Free Choice Profiling (FCP) which enables the profiling of a set of food products with naive subjects using their own vocabulary to describe the sensory characteristics. No vocabulary alignment is thus required. Although sophisticated statistical treatments (Generalized Procrustes Analysis (Gower, 1975) or STATIS (Schlich, 1996)) permit the analysis of such data, the interpretation of the sensory map with the individual attributes is often confusing. In many cases, only the clear differences in taste or texture rather than odour attributes can be unambiguously described. This is perhaps because, in their everyday life, subjects have learnt to associate their gustatory and tactile perceptions with a specific vocabulary (e.g. saltiness and sweetness or hardness), whereas the naming of odours is often more difficult due to the heterogeneity of the vocabulary. The verbalization of odour perception is strongly dependent on the subject’s experience with the odorants (Engen, 1987), and no fixed standards are defined in everyday life.
INTRODUCTION The quantitative descriptive profile of food products or conventional profiling, as defined by Lyon et al. ( 1992), could actually be considered as one of the most complex sensory tasks (Stone & Sidel, 1985). Indeed, panellists have to analyse their perceptions when tasting the product, verbalize them and quantify them relative to the product
Without doubt FCP is a very useful technique to quickly obtain a sensory map with the main differences between products. However, when more precise results are expected, a conventional profiling method remains the more appropriate technique. As mentioned by Ein-
sensory space under study. Besides its complexity, this sensory methodology is known to provide precise and consistent results, providing that the panel has been selected, properly trained and maintained (Einstein, 1991). These two conditions require the investment of a lot of resources, particularly during the training phase which consists primarily of vocabulary development and alignment (Einstein, 1991; Lyon et al., 1992). According to the
stein ( 1991)) the training of descriptive panels should not be too limited because it is likely to lead to an incomplete description of the sensory characteristics of the products. Moreover, Wolters & Allchurch (1994) observed that the longer the training period, the better the product discrimination is and the stronger the agreement among panellists. Chambers & Smith (1993) have showed the superiority of product-specific training over the general sensory experience in improving panel performances.
*Author to whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
[email protected] tE-mail:
[email protected] 177
I. Lesschaeve,
178
S. Issanchou
As the time dedicated ble
without
propose
the
to examine
procedures
to the training
affecting
the
to select
is hardly reduci-
measurement
quality,
appropriateness
the right
persons
of screening to perform
Two
the
profiling task. If the selection tests could detect the future performance
of descriptive
panellists,
train a panel with high abilities available
panellists thou,
in
the
(ASTM,
1992; ISO,
selection
and
Meilgaard
1993). However,
profiling
texture
described
profiling
et al.,
tests and on the profiling
(in 6 minutes)
on these selection and rice
principle: Profiling
if relationships
test (Swets
case study which had as its main purpose the study of the impact of micro-organism associations on the sensory cheese (Molimard,
the abilities
related
description,
because,
1994). The sen-
of the flavour was the main objective
of this study. The selection
to examine
ability
dots. Subjects
have to
index is equal to
of 4 dot groups found on the whole
learned
attributes.
flavour
Panellists
the sensation this
development
to associate
and alignment, perceptions
have
and the associated
information
when
This task is particularly
lary is available order to evaluate subjects,
analysing
a
product
difficult when working
a test was proposed by Lesschaeve
(1995),
out of 103 candidates
by Rabin
& Cain (1984).
Memory
test is also conceived
memory
of the subjects,
were recruited
after passing
by Issanchou
Their olfactory abilities were evaluated
the
et al. (1995).
with four tests out
the seven described in that paper: Familiar
Odour Recog-
and was in fact an adaptation
descriptive
(where only olfactory
memory
of each test are summarized
were scored). in Table
1.
vocabu-
Although
& Issanchou
of the test designed the Grouping
to evaluate
an alternative
Odour
the olfactory
test was investi-
gated with a process which is very close to that which a
nition, Grouping Odour Memory, Ranking and Description oral attributes
to
label and to
contrary to gustatory sensations. In the Olfactory Recognition Memory of
Eighteen subjects ( 1 man, 17 women; mean age = 33 y-0.) described
with
to be able
on olfactory sensations for which no standardized
procedure
The
of 50 lines; each line is made
the vocabulary
panellists
sample.
Screening procedures
parameters
is used to
questionnaire.
retrieve
selection
NL)
to
T.I.B.
of the panellists.
one line. The concentration number
memorize
METHODS
a panellist
The Bourdon
bv, CALISSE,
is composed
appropriate
complex sensory modality and the most difficult to describe.
creativity
mark all the groups of 4 dots. Eight seconds are allowed
During
flavour is the most
in 6 min-
The
requires
intensively.
the concentration
the mean
tests were mainly focused on
3 letters in
but using the same item.
of 25 groups of 3, 4 or 5 black
to the sense of smell and to flavour from experience,
per generated
& Zeitlinger
questionnaire
from a
terms of four
of three sets of pictures
sensory characteristics
filing performances.
data were obtained
1973,
This test is com-
and search of words containing
1 point
estimate
properties of Camembert
decorations
be able to concentrate
The aim of this paper is to determine
& Hemenway, 1994).
of a
index is the sum of the 3 scores.
could be observed between selection tests and flavour pro-
sory characterization
(Hoepfner
This
adaptation
a given order (6 sets of 3 letters are examined
a complex
profile through simple selection tests.
Sensory
test
a French
& Allchurch,
fac-
and alignment.
utes). Each part is scored separately
that it was not
in completing
development
words (in 4 minutes),
to select a
Sauce
panellists.
posed of three parts: search of synonymous
panellists
Rousset-Akrim
of Bolognese
creativity
were eval-
their potential
of panellists is an important
by performing
cited by Wolters
abilities
to estimate
good descriptive
The verbal creativity
verbal
Issan-
between
of the
They concluded
the ability
1991;
procedure
Performances
samples were examined.
impact in detecting
was evaluated
descriptive
relationships
Recently,
a screening
panel.
possible to predict
screen
performances
have not been widely studied. et al. (1995)
to
and one sensory
tor for the vocabulary
Many recommendations
literature
1981;
conven-
non sensory
uated at the end of training
we would expect to
for performing
tional profiling more efficiently. are
Evaluation of complementary abilities
we
The
(Banks,
panellist
is asked. The
is estimated
by
the
olfactory
detectability
recognition index,
d’
1970).
TABLE 1. Summary description of the selection tests Selection test
Ability evaluated
Familiar Odour Recognition Odour Memory Grouping
Olfactory Olfactory
Ranking
Odour intensity discrimination Verbal ability to express sensory perceptions
Description
of food flavour
knowledge memory
10 odorant 15 odorant
Stimuli
Sensory task
solutions solutions
Match an odorant solution with a label Make 5 groups of three odorants, based on persona1 criteria, consistently one week apart Rank the flasks according to the mushroom odour intensity Describe as precisely as possible the olfactory oral sensations
5 solutions of I-octen-3-01 ‘2 Camembert
cheese samples
The Future Performance of Descriptive Panellists
TABLE 2. Olfactory oral sensations used to profile the experimental Camembert cheeses Global Intensity Milk Cream Butter Cancoillotte Melted cheese l
Fermented Cheese
Blue Cheese
We chose both univariate and multivariate statistical indices to estimate the performance of the panel.
Univariate indices
Musty Cardboard Nutty * Plastic
Cowshed Rancid Cabbage Ammoniac Mushroom Dead leaves
179
Smoky
Not used by the panel to discriminate significantly @ < 0.05) between the 16 samples.
l
Experimental design for the sensory measurements
The discrimination power is estimated by DISCanova, which is the number of discriminant attributes used by one panellist. This index is obtained by performing an ANOVA by subject (p < 0.10). The repeatability was estimated by the index Drift-Mood proposed by Schlich (1994) in the procedure GRAPES. Which describes a session effect. The pure error was estimated by the index Unreliability, also proposed in GRAPES.
Multivariate
Training A training
of forty-five hours was planned to develop the descriptive vocabulary and to align it among panellists, as well as to train in scoring on the linear scale.
Sensory measurements The panel evaluated sixteen experimental Camembert cheeses with 37 descriptors describing texture (11 items), taste (3), mouthfeel (1)) olfactory oral sensations (19) and aftertaste (3). For the purpose of this paper, only the nineteen olfactory oral descriptors will be considered (see Table 2). Four cheeses were evaluated per week, twice a week, which enables the control of a session effect. Four weeks were necessary to examine the sixteen experiexperimental information mental cheeses. Additional may be found in Molimard (1994).
Definition of the panellist performance indices A number of papers have been published recently describing simple tools to control panellist performances (e.g. Naes, 1990; Naes et al., 1994; Pritchett-Mangan, 1992; Schlich, 1994). Although the techniques differed (graphical, univariate or multivariate statistical analysis), there is a consensus on the parameters of panellist performance for quantitative descriptive profiling. These are reliability, consistency, discrimination power and complexity of perception of the sensory space.
indices
Three indices were defined after performing a stepwise discriminant analysis by subject (p < 0.10). DISCstep is the number of non-related discriminant attributes given by the variable selection. The discrimination is evaluated with DISCascc which is the average squared canonical correlation; it indicates the power of a panellist to discriminate the 16 samples in the global sensory space. When DISCascc is null, it means that the subject did not perceive any difference between the samples. When it is close to one, it means that the subject was able to discriminate each sample from the others. The complexity of the perception of the sensory space is estimated by the number of discriminant axes used by the panellist (DISCaxis; p < 0.10).
Statistical analysis To study the relationships between selection and profiling performances, the selected panellists are categorized in two classes for each selection task, according to their respective performance. The score distribution of each test is examined to define the criterion which will split the panel into 2 balanced classes, as far as possible. Class 1 will categorize the good performances and Class 2 the weak ones. This criterion is defined as the median when the distribution was roughly normal and as the breaking point when the normality was rejected. The criteria are reported in Table 3. The basic hypothesis of this work is
TABLE 3. Categorization criteria and performances of the selected panel for each selection and additional tests Selection Test
Class 1 (good scores)
Familiar Odour Recognition Odour Grouping Memory Description test Ranking Recognition Memory Odours (d’) Concentration Verbal creativity
for
= 10 (n= 10) >5 >5 = 10 > 1
(n=9) (n=8) (n= 13) (n=8)
> 5.8 (n=8) > 108.5 (n=9)
Class 2 (weak scores) < 10 <5 55 < 10
(n=8) (n=9) (n= 10) (n=5) (n=lO)
5 5.8 (n= 10) 5 108.5 (n=9)
Possible score Mean score range
Coefficient of Variation (%)
Observed score range
O-10 O-10 0-00 Sl O-7.5
8.7
16.8
5.8 7.2 0.97 1.18
35.8 73.7 4.7 74.9
2-10 O-18 0.9-l -0.1 T-4.03
O-8 o-00
5.93 108.8
9.41 24.9
5.04-6.96 73-164
6-10
180
I. Lesschaeve, S. Issanchou
that high selection scores involve high quantitative descriptive profiling performances. To study the validity of this assumption, a Student l-test was performed between selection test classes for each profiling performance index. When a profiling performance index does not fit the normal distribution, it is also categorized in two classes; a chi-squared test is then performed on the cross-tabulation of selection and profiling performance index classes. All the statistical analysis are conducted using S.A.S. (1988).
RESULTS
AND
DISCUSSION
Selection and additional test performances The additional tests were performed to evaluate supplementary abilities which were not expressed by the other selection tasks. To verify the independence of these two kinds of ability measurement, Spearman correlation coefficients were calculated between additional tests and selection tests. Only three correlations were found to be significant (j.~ ~0.10): Grouping Odour Memory and Concentration tests (r = 0.43, p = 0.07), Ranking and Concentration tests (r = 0.41, p = 0.09), Description and Verbal creativity tests (r = 0.63, p = 0.005). The latter result was expected as we assumed that panellists who had a high verbal creativity score would easily find more words to express their perceptions when tasting food products. The description task is easy to design and to score compared to the Verbal Creativity test; it should be preferred in the future screening tests for descriptive panel. The partial links between grouping odour memory or ranking and concentration abilities could be interpreted by the common need for subjects to concentrate on the task in order to succeed. These correlation coefficients are quite low which suggests only partial TABLE 4. Pearson rank correlation coefficients DlSCanova(a)
DISCstep
1 0.60 2 0.71 ’ 0.87 ’
DISCanova DISCstep DI SCascc DISCaxis Drift-Mood Unreliability
-0%
on profiling
To evaluate the appropriateness of each index to describe the quantitative descriptive profiling performance, Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to estimate the links between these variables and are reported in Table 4. All the discrimination power indices are more or less dependent on each other as they are estimated on the same basis: the use of attributes to describe the sensory space. DISCanova and DISCstep are only partly related since the coefficient is equal to 0.60. This suggests the necessity to take into account the redundancy of attributes to study panellist performances. Indeed, the number of discriminant attributes obtained by the ANOVA can be artificially high if most of the attributes are correlated. This does not occur with the stepwise variable selection, as already mentioned by Rousset-Akrim et al. (1995). However, in the present paper, a high DISCanova led to the use of a large number of discriminant axes to describe the global sensory space. This result indices
DISCascc(c)
DISCaxis’
1 0.73 ’ -0.74 ’ -0.54 s
I ns
I
ns
0.78
0.61 2 -0.66 2 -0.47 3
I: p < 0.001; 2: p < 0.01; 3: p < 0.05; 4: DISCaxis
Profiling performances
performance
1 0.96 ’
3
relationships between these indices. They do not measure exactly the same abilities. Surprisingly, the two tasks evaluating olfactory memory (i.e. Grouping Odour Memory and Recognition Memory for Odours) do not seem to measure the same ability. The mean performances of the selected panel are described in Table 3. The coefficient of variation (CV) obtained for the Familiar Odour Recognition and Ranking test is quite low ( < 20%) which suggests a certain homogeneity of abilities within the panel. The same observation could be made for Concentration ability scores (CV < 10%). However, for the two other selection tests as well as for Recognition Memory for Odours and Verbal creativity, this variation is large (CV > 20%), which reflects the difficulty of forming a perfectly homogeneous panel with the best performance at each task, as was already underlined by Issanchou et al. (1995).
is a non normal
variable.
Spearman
STEPDISC
rank correlation
it and the others. a: number
of individual
discriminant
b: number
of individual
non related and discriminant attributes (PROC
c: Average
squared
d: number e: session
canonical
of discriminant effect index
F: pure error index
attributes
correlation
axis (PROC
(ANOVA)
(PROC CANDISC
STEPDISC in S.A.S.)
in S.A.S.)
Drift-Mood(e)
in S.A.S.)
Unreliability(f)
’
coefficient
1 is computed
between
The Future Performance of Descriptive Panellists
in the way of analysing the 16 cheese samples. However, the mean values of these performances permit us to describe aromatic differences between cheeses with about five discriminant and non-related attributes (out of 19) and the complexity of the sensory space perceived by the panel is more than 2 axes.
could be interesting information to the training process. Moreover, the two repeatability indices are also linked which means that, in this set of data, subjects who had a high pure error index also tend to have a high session deviation effect. Although relations between most of the performance indices are observed, only DISCstep and DISCascc could be considered as redundant variables (r = 0.96). This relationship is particularly interesting: it means that each selected attribute contributes to increase, with sharpness, the discrimination power of the panellists in the global sensory space. This is confirmed by the links existing between DISCascc or DISCstep and DISCaxis (the number of discriminant axes). The mean scores of each index are reported in Table 5. Considering the mean scores of Unreliability and DriftMood indices, we observed that they are lower than 1, which reflects good performances. The panel can therefore be considered as a precise tool. However, the oral sensations of the 16 description of olfactory Camembert cheeses with the 19 items remains slightly different among the panellists; indeed, about 7 discriminant attributes are used by subjects, but the CV is quite large (48%). The same remark can be made for DISCstep, DISCascc or DISCaxis. Although the panel was intensively trained, a certain heterogeneity remains TABLE
5.
Relationships between selection and profiling performances Results are reported in Table 6. The prior abilities of the subjects measured with the Ranking test, the Description test, the Verbal Creativity test and the Grouping Odour Memory test do not influence their profiling performances. The score variability of the Ranking test may have been too narrow to point out any effect. The descriptive ability, strongly related to the verbal creativity, is in fact mainly important at the beginning of the training when subjects have to develop an appropriate descriptive vocabulary, and appears minor during profiling measurements. So, the absence of this latter effect on profiling performances is not surprising. However, in spite of the fact that a low coefficient of variation is observed on data, the Familiar Odour Recognition test seems to have a potential predictive
Profiling performance description.
Test
Mean
Coefficient of variation %
DISCanova’
7.22
48.6
2
DISCstep’ DISCascc3 DISCaxis Drift-Mood’ Unreliability6
4.92 0.22 2.5 0.79 0.84
40.2 31.8 39.2 20.2 9.5
1 0.05 1 0.57 0.69
‘: number
of individual
STEPDISC
in S.A.S.);
(PROC
CANDISC
TABLE
6. Relationships
discriminant 3: Average
in S.A.S.);
attributes squared
(ANOVA);
canonical
(PROC
Maximum score 16 8 0.33 4 1.14 1.02
non related and discriminant
STEPDISC
in S.A.S.);
4: number
attributes
(PROC
of discriminant
axis
5: session effect index; % pure error index
between selection and profiling performances Index DISC
Familiar Odour Recognition Odour Grouping Memory Ranking Description Recognition Memory for Odours Concentration
‘: number
of individual in S.A.S.);
t= 1.94 p = 0.069 ns
ascc3
t=2.08 p = 0.053 ns
DISC
x2.= 3.60 p=o.o59 ns
ns
ns
t =Y.50 p=o.o02
t=:l6 p = 0.0007
axis4
Drift-mood5
ns
ns
ns
ns ns ns
ns ns ns
ns ns ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
3: Average
attributes squared
(ANOVA);
canonical
? number
correlation
of individual
(PROC
5: session effect index; ‘? pure error index
Unreliability’
ns
ns
discriminant
in S.A.S.);
DISC
DISC step*
ns ns ns
Creativity
CANDISC
anova’
t=2.30 p = 0.035 ns
STEPDISC (PROC
Minimum score
*: number of individual
correlation
Test
Verbal
181
non related and discriminant
STEPDISC
in S.A.S.);
4: number
t= -1.87 p=o.o79 ns attributes
(PROC
of discriminant
axis
182
I. Lesschaeve, S. Issanchou
value for panellist performances. Indeed, subjects who seemed to use more attributes to discriminate significantly between the 16 samples (DISCanova, DISCstep) and those who seemed to have a higher discrimination power in the global sensory space (DISCascc) had obtained higher scores on the Familiar Odour Recognition test. Moreover, these subjects tended to describe the aromatic sensory space of the cheeses with more dimensions (DISCaxis). Testing the prior olfactory knowledge or culture seems to be essential when the oral olfactory sensations have to be described and measured by the future panellists. Indeed, when people smell unfamiliar stimuli they have no prior ‘name-odour’ association available and do not have the ability to create it quickly in laboratory conditions (Cain, 1979). Although the sensory modalities were different (texture/oral olfactory perceptions), this result is in accordance with those obtained by RoussetAkrim et al. ( 1995): they found only one significant relationship between the Texture Property Knowledge test and the discrimination power on the rice texture profile (r = 0.42, p < 0.05). These two results demonstrate the low contribution of training procedure to align performance and the preponderance of previous knowledge over the learning during the profiling training. Screening candidates on their previous knowledge of sensory properties may be a clue to reducing the vocabulary alignment period, particularly if it is verified that candidates had a common knowledge. Performances on the Grouping Odour Memory test - which evaluates the olfactory memory of the subjects - did not affect the profiling measurements, contrary to our expectations. This result is actually surprising since a strong effect is observed for the Recognition Memory for Odour test. Indeed, subjects who obtained higher scores at this task are shown to use more nonrelated attributes to discriminate between the samples (DISCstep) and to have a stronger discrimination power in the global sensory space (DISCascc). The observed effect may be explained by the design of the test which is closer to what is asked of a descriptive panellist, i.e. to perceive a sensation and to associate the correct label to it. However, this additional test was not performed at the selection phase, but at the end of training. The sensory experience acquired by the subjects during this time may have influenced their performances. Desor & Beauchamp (1974) showed that training on the testing stimuli could improve the recognition performances; but we previously observed that the sensory experience of subjects do not influence these performances (Lesschaeve & Issanchou, 1995). The Recognition Memory for Odour test seems therefore more interesting to be tested at the screening phase of the panel than the Grouping Odour Memory test. Finally this result underlined the importance of screening the future descriptive panellists on their Recognition Memory for Odours if the role of this ability on the profiling performances is confirmed by further studies.
The most discriminating panellists performed better at the Familiar Odour Recognition test or at the Recognition Memory for Odour test. However, the high performances observed on these two screening tests are not related (x * = 2.2, p = 0.13); this suggests the need to focus the selection on both directions. Finally, an interesting relationship is observed between the Unreliability index and the Concentration Ability test. Although this level of significance is only 8%, it seems that panellists who have difficulty in concentrating tend to be more unreliable than the others. If this phenomenon is confirmed on other data, it means that a non-sensory test could estimate the tendency of a candidate to give inconsistent measurements in spite of intensive training on the methodology and products under study. Wolters & Allchurch (1994) concluded for their data that “apparently, more training did not lead to greater reliability” and explained it by the fact that “the more a panel tries to find small differences between samples, the more attributes are liable to show a replicate effect, just because it is more difficult to describe the differences”. Our data suggested that panellist reliability may be determined not only by training but might also depend on the personal ability of subjects to concentrate. Further research is needed to confirm these preliminary findings.
CONCLUSIONS Relationships between selection and profiling performances were investigated among the selected panellists. Although a low coefficient of variation was observed on the Familiar Odour Recognition test scores (16%), this test was shown to be a potential screening evaluation tool for selecting discriminating panellists. Moreover, the olfactory memory estimated through the Recognition Memory for Odour test has a strong influence on the multidimensional discrimination power of the panellists. Finally, panellist reliability may be detected by a nonsensory test, evaluating the concentration ability. These results were obtained on eighteen subjects who passed the screening procedure; these data have to be considered as preliminary, although significant relationships have been observed. The real selection effect could only be studied by comparing the profiling performances of selected panellists and nonselected panellists. Such work is currently being undertaken by the authors.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The authors would like to thank C. J. Wolters for the fruitful discussions on the screening and training of descriptive panellists and assistance in adapting the
The Future Performance
Verbal creativity test and the Bourdon T.I.B. test to the French language. This collaboration was supported by a FLAIR-SENS project. Pascal Schlich is warmly thanked for his enthusiastic interest for sensory data analysis and for his contribution to provide useful and original statistical tools to better interpret our data. Catherine Juffard and Pierre Lavier are thanked for their high quality technical assistance.
Lesschaeve,
I. & Issanchou,
formance:
memory
familiarity
M. A., Hasdell,
183 per-
and label
15. T. A. & Lawson,
K.
Guidelines for Sensol;r Analysis in Food Product Development
and Quality Control. Chapman Meilgaard,
M.,
Evaluation
Civille,
Meilgaard,
M.
Reference
C.,
CRC
Reid,
standards
London.
& Carr,
B. T.
(1991).
Sensory
Press, Boca Raton.
D. S. & Wyborski,
for
beer
flavour
K.
A.
(1982).
terminology
system.
40, 119-128.
ASBC Journal, P.
& Hall,
G. V.
Techniques.
Molimard,
(1994).
Etude
de la cooperation
candidum et Penicillium camemberti: sur les qualite’s organoleptiques
REFERENCES
Olfactory
Chemical Senses, 20, 114-l
D. H., Francombe,
(1992).
S. (1995).
effects of sensory experience,
use consistency. Lyon,
of Descriptive Panellists
These de Doctorat
entre Geotrichum
impact sur le pro@ aromatique et
dun fromuge
de ‘Sciences
de type camembert.
de 1’Alimentation’.
Universite
de Bourgogne. Naes, ASTM
(1981).
Guidelines
sory panel Testing Banks,
members.
Materials,
W.
P.
E-18
STP
758.
Philadelphia,
(1970).
Signal
and training
American
of sen-
Society
for
W.
S.
(1979).
identification. Chambers
To
know with
Theory
and
Human
experience
E. on
& Smith,
the nose:
keys to odour
E. A.
performance
(1993).
of trained
Effects
of testing
sensory
panellists.
Desor, J. A. & Beauchamp, to transmit
olfactory
G. K. (1974).
information.
The human
Perception
capacity
&? Psychophysics,
A.
hybridization.
(1991).
Descriptive
techniques
and
In Sensory science theory and applications
ed. H. T. Lawless
& B. P. Klein,
Marcel
Dekker,
their
in foods,
New York,
pp. 3 17-338. Engen,
T.
(1987).
Remembering
odours
and
their
J.
C.
Psychometrica, Heisserer,
(1975).
Journal
of Sensory Studies, 8, 121-132.
Monitor, ISO.
Hollywood,
(1993).
Procrustes
IV, E. C. (1993).
sensory
attributes
R. & Hemenway,
J.
of aged
(1973).
California,
natural
degustateurs. Issanchou, individual products. bert
cheese
349368.
cheese.
standardized
Rabin,
Basic
et le con-
Paris.
et la formation
I. & Koster,
to perform statements
descriptive
P. (1994).
differences
Using
cumulative
in sensory
R. A., Masuda,
& Stern, system
P. M.
of wine
P. A.
sensory panellists.
B. M.,
(1984).
aroma
profiling.
Pecore,
Progress
S. D.,
towards
terminology.
a
American
Performance
and
Psychology:
assessment
of
of Sensory Studies, 7, 2299252.
W. S. (1984).
identifiability
Experimental
(1992).
journal
M. D. & Cain,
Odor
encoding
Learning,
recognition:
consistency.
famil-
of
Journal
Memory and Cognition, 2, 3 16-
325. Rousset-Akrim,
S.,
C. (1995).
Research
for graphical
Martin,
J.-F.,
Pilandon,
of selective profiling.
C. & Touraille,
tests for discerning
an effi-
of Sensory Studies, 10,
Journal
application
analysis
of food
to a Camem-
Journal of Sensory Studies, 10,
a method
representations
P. (1996).
mises about E. Risvik. H.
Defining
product
Cary, NC.
and a SAS@
program
of assessors performances.
journal
and validating
distances
Elsevier
Science, J.
L.
Amsterdam, (1985).
compro-
correlations.
In
pp. 259-306.
Sensory Evaluation
Practices.
Press, London.
A. A. & Langron,
profiling
assessor
and attribute
Ananlysis of Data in Sensory Science, ed. T. Naes &
& Sidel,
Academic Williams,
Screening
User’s Guide. SAS Institute,
GRAPES:
of Sensory Studies, 9, 157-169.
Stone,
d’un jury de
E. P. (1995).
descriptive
and
panel.
P. (1994).
Multivariate
Annales de Gembloux, 98, 85-99. ability
2,
of Enology and Viticulture, 35, 107-109.
Journal
Schlich,
U.S.A.
La selection
S., Lesschaeve,
0.
S.A.S.( 1988). SAS / STAT
‘Test of Creative Potential.
Guide gene’ral pour la selection, l’entrainement
S. (1992).
J.
Analysis.
Determination
trdle des sujets. Partie 1: sujets qua&ties. AFNOR, Issanchou,
between
of Sensory Studies, 9, 87-99.
A. C., Arnold,
Schlich,
of the Hoepfner,
Noble,
individual
cient assessor in texture
40, 35-5 1. flavour
differences
Quality and Preference,
217-237.
Generalized
D. M. & Chambers
Food
D. & Baardseth,
to detect
names.
American Scientist, 75, 497-503. Gower,
ranks
iarity,
M.
individual
profiling.
187-199.
Pritchett-Mangan,
16, 55 l-556. Einstein,
Handling
in sensory
Schmidt,
of Sensory Studies, 8, 155-166.
journal
(1990).
Journal
Science, 203, 467-470.
IV,
T.
assessors
Naes, T., Hirst,
USA.
Detection
Psychological Bulletin, 74, 81-99.
Memory. Cain,
for the selection
for the evaluation
S. P. (1984).
The use of free-choice
of commercial
ports.
journal
of
Science Food and Agriculture, 35, 558-568. Wolters,
C. J.
procedure
& Allchurch,
E. M.
on the performance
Quality and Preference, 5, 203-2 14.
(1994).
Effect
of descriptive
of training panels.
Food