Demographic observations (1963–1973) on the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania

Demographic observations (1963–1973) on the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania

G. Teleki E. E. Hunt, Jr. J. H. Pfifferling Demographic Observations (1963-1973) on the Chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania Department of A...

3MB Sizes 12 Downloads 57 Views

G. Teleki E. E. Hunt, Jr. J. H. Pfifferling

Demographic Observations (1963-1973) on the Chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania

Department of Anthropology, 409 C. R. Carpenter Building, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pa. 16802, U.S.A.

This report provides a longitudinal demographic profile for a small study population of chimpanzees living in the Gombe National Park of Tanzania, East Africa. The sizeand composition of this study population, and the trends in mortality, natal&y and migratory patterns it experienced over a span of ten years, are described and analysed within various ethological and ecological frameworks. The validity of the demographic profile is examined in relation to field methodology, as the study population was intensively provisioned with bananas for several years. The results yield some guidelines for managing and conserving wild chimpanzee populations, and special emphasis is placed upon protection from diseases that involve human vectors. The data presented here may also offer some novel insights to the evolution of pongid and hominid populations, so tentative steps are taken to place the Gombe profile into a broader demographic perspective.

Received and accepted 1 April 1976

1. Introduction Longitudinal demographic information on wild non-human primate populations remains sparse even though numerous species have been extensively studied in their natural habitats (see Baldwin & Teleki, 1972, 1973, 1974, in press; Baldwin et al., 1975,1976 and in press for reviews of field studies). The few reports that do include demographic data tend to cover brief observation spans (e.g. Berger, 1972) or to derive from colonies transplanted into artificial settings (e.g. Burton & Sawchuk, 1974; Drickamer, 1974). Only preliminary demographic data are currently availabIe for the African and Asian apes: Pan troglodytes (Nishida, 1968, in press; Lawick-Goodall, 1968, 19756; Kortlandt, no date; Kuijsten, 1972), Pongo pygmaeus (Horr, 1975, in press; Rodman, 1973; Cohen, 1975), Symphalangus syndactylus (Chivers, 1974). These and ofher field studies of living pongids have yielded a wide selection of demographic material, but data on the natality, mortality and migratory patterns of groups or populations cannot be reliably compared because temporal spans and methodological treatments vary greatly. The present report focuses on a continuous IO-year span of demographic events and trends within a small population of long-haired chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) inhabiting the Gombe National Park (4’40’ S, 29”38’ E) of western Tanzania. Our prime concern, in view of the current paucity of published data, is of course the presentation of descriptive field observations. But the relevance of the chimpanzee demographic model to other areas of anthropoIogica1 research will aIso be explored, especially along the following lines. (a) To the paleoanthropologist, demographic studies of living non-human primates may yield models which can then be applied to refine speculations about patterns and processes once exhibited by early hominid populations, such as australopithecines. Although modern human populations have experienced major demographic changes in recent centuries, which are reflected in a steep global growth rate, hominid populations are estimated to have grown only incrementally during the preceding million or more years (e.g. Coale, 1974). The demographic profiles of non-human primate populations, Journal of HumanEvolurion(1976) 5,559-598

560

G. TELEKI

ET AL.

on the other hand, have probably maintained greater stability throughout the Pleistocene, so the patterns exhibited by living monkey and ape populations may provide an accurate mirror for visualizing demographic traits and trends in the early stages of hominid evolution. In addition to serving as a prehistoric template, such demographic models-and especially those derived from African pongids, in view of their biological, behavioral and psychological proximity to hominids-could help resolve some of the dilemmas encountered in paleodemographic research (Mann, 1975; McKinley, 1971). (b) To the primatologist, demographic data provide a key to understanding the structure and organization of social units, and to the adaptive role these have played for various species in an array of habitats. Much attention has recently been given to the ways that primate adaptations are governed by relationships between organizational and ecological variables (Aldrich-Blake, 1970 ; Struhsaker, 1969 ; Gartlan, 1973 ; Clutton-Brock, 1974; Eisenberg, Muckenhirn & Rudran, 1972). Yet no cogent, comprehensive theory of adaptation has emerged from these numerous schemes-perhaps because none rests firmly upon a foundation of demographic profiles. (c) To the cultural or medical anthropologist, demographic studies of non-human primates may, by virtue of the rather simple and elegant models obtainable therefrom, provide insights about the manner in which biological, epidemiological, sociological and other adaptive mechanisms regulate human populations. The progress of recent investigations along these lines (Fox, 1972; Dumond, 1975; Lancaster, in press) has again been hampered by lack of demographic information suitable for interspecific comparison. (d) To the wildlife biologist and game manager, accurate demographic data on living non-human primates are essential for devising suitable policies and programs of conservation. Such information is particularly vital to the task of protecting African and Asian apes, which are today threatened or endangered by human activities in many regions (Harrisson, 197 1; Bermant & Lindburg, 1975). Moreover, breeders of nonhuman primates could, given accurate numerical information, evaluate the success of their custodianship by comparing fertility and survivorship rates in captive and wilderness settings (Teleki & Baldwin, 1975). At present both conservation and reproduction programs suffer from lack of demographic information. 2. The Gombe Setting Research on the Gombe chimpanzees was initiated in 1960, and has been sustained for nearly 15 years. Since 1967, when the Gombe Stream Research Centre was officially founded, the project has developed into a multifaceted, multidisciplinary study of nonhuman primate ethology and ecology. Numerous reports on chimpanzees have been completed by members of the research center, and some reports on other indigenous species are also available (Gombe bibliographic entries are marked with asterisks). Reports describing aspects of the local climate, terrain, flora and fauna include those by Lawick-Goodall (1968), Teleki (1973a), Glutton-Brock (1972) and Wrangham (1975). No detailed, systematic studies of the Gombe ecosystem are as yet available. For the purposes of this report, it should suffice to note that the terrain of this small park (approximate map dimensions : 2.3 x 13.9 kilometers) is rugged, cut into many steep-sided ridges and valleys by a dendritic stream system that flows an horizontal distance of about 1500-3000 metres and drops a vertical distance of about 750 metres

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

between

the escarpment

Tanganyika;

crest of the Great Rift Valley

that the climate is tropical,

rainfall of 75-125

centimeters,

characterized

land, woodland

and the eastern shore of Lake by a lack of frost, a mean annual

and a diurnal temperature

15°C at night to as much as 37°C at midday;

561

DEMOGRAPHY

range of 28°C which rises from

that the Aora comprises a mosaic of grass-

and rain forest whose distribution

is closely related to topography;

and

that the fauna1 community

is rich in primates (prosimian, monkey, ape and human) and Contrary to the 80 km2 estimate given small vertebrates but sparse in large mammals. in previous reports for the size of the park, new calculations, based on Quarter Degree Sheet #92 (published in 1961 by Tanzania’s Geological Survey Division at Dodoma), yield a map area of 32 km2 for the entire park. However, the true surface area may be as much as 20 % larger when the rugged terrain is taken into account. All density measures given in this report are based on the new area estimate.

Some

pertinent

in a later section.

to evaluating

demographic

trends are presented

In the early stages of the field study, chimpanzees in the rugged National

terrain

Park.

and dense vegetation

meteorological

data

were elusive and difficult to observe

characterizing

many

portions

of Gombe

A banana provisioning

system was therefore launched in 1962 to hasten Continued throughthe apes to the presence ofhumanobservers.

the process of habituating

out most of the decade sampled here, artificial feeding reached a peak in frequency sessions) and volume the middle

1973a;

(about 68 kg bananas per session containing

and late 1960s but was then gradually

Wrangham,

bulk provisioning

1974).

Recent

evaluations

suggest that the procedure

reduced

4400 calories)

in the early 1970s (Teleki,

of the consequences modified

(daily during

behavior

of long-term,

large-

patterns, including

the

frequency of aggression and the kinds of mammals taken in predation, and organizational

patterns.

trends during the 1963-1973

The effects of this procedure

as well as nutritional upon demographic events and

study period will be explored in a later section of this report.

3. Demographic The raw census data collected between

Baselines

1 July, 1963 and 30 June, 1973 appear in Table

1.

During this decade, the records included 38 named males (plus 1 unnamed male birth) and 49 named females (plus 2 unnamed female births), as well as 6 cases of fetal death. All census data are tabulated

between

deaths logged for 1964 actually include June, 1964. Birth dates are known

or estimated

consecutive

mid-year

points.

all cases observed between for every individual

For example, 1 July,

the

1963 and 30

in the census records.

For

infants born to named females during the sampled decade, the events could be timed to the day in many cases and to the month for all others. As chimpanzee births occur throughout considered

the annual

cycle,

unobserved

births-i.e.

to have taken place at the mid-year

estimates become increasingly

those prior to 1 July,

1963-are

point of the estimated birth year.

prone to error as age rises, and are probably

These

more accurate

for females than for males because the ages of the former can often be tied to reproductive events. That is, the average age of females at birth of the first infant (1 l-13 years) can be added to the average pregnancy interval (4-6 years) between each known offspring. Flo’s minimum age, for example, woul,d thus be 12 + 5+ 5 + 5 + 5 + 5, or 37 years. Some additional time (3-5 years) could be factored into age estimates for old females on the premise that some fetal wastage and infant mortality occurred, for the

562 Table

G. TELEKI

1964 1965 1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973

Baseline census population

1

Years

ET AL.

Births*

Census

data

(1 July,

Deaths?

23 29 30 24 24 21 21 22 24 16

18 22 32 29 22 22 24 28 27 23

41 51 62 53 46 43 45 50 51 39

2 2 1 1 0 0 1 3 2 0

2 3 4 0 1 2 2 1 1 2

4 5 5 1 1 2 3 4 3 2

0 1 6 1 3 1 1 0 0 2

2 2 0 3 7

Tally 234 Annual x23.4

247

481

12

18

30

15

24.7

48.1

1.2

18

3.0

196380

Fetal deaths:

June,

the stud

Migration Y_ Imnugr.

Emigr.

$6

$3

??

0 1 1 0

2 3 6 4 10 3 1 1 1 2

0 3400+7 0 37 1 0 2 2 0000 10003-3 10101 101 0 0 2 0 0000 0 00

18

33

6

+6

1.5 1.8

3.3

0.6

0.6

2

1973) for

+17

Net

??

0 0

0+10 0 +2 0

0 0

0 0

8

6-14

0.8

4 5 6 3 2

03 +1 +2 0

-8-10

1.7

Total pregnancies§

4 4 3 2

+5

36

0.5

3.6

I.0

* Figures include unnamed births. t Figures include unnamed deaths. 5 Observed fetal deaths are probably below actual occurrence level, especially in the early years. $ Combined list of live births and fetal deaths.

average birth interval may in such cases be reduced by 2-4 years. mum age would then become

40-45

Flo’s estimated maxi-

years.

As such events may not be evenly distributed in the lifespan of all females, minimum age estimates are used in all demographic measures presented pearance

here.

The ages of males are based on more arbitrary

and behaviour,

with opinions

obtained

evaluations

of size, ap-

from several investigators.

Death dates are inherently more difficult to establish, especially for mature individuals, because emigration

and mortality are not always distinguishable

in field conditions.

Thus

the death category here includes only those cases which were directly observed and those for which 1973,

strong circumstantial

19756;

appearances servers.

Teleki,

1973c).

evidence

and all new chimpanzees

Both categories

was available

The migration

remain

who

somewhat

category

(Lawick-Goodall, includes

were previously flexible

because

1968,

1971,

all unaccountable

dis-

unknown

to human

some emigrants

ob-

may have

died and some immigrants may have been familiar to named chimpanzees. Only those individuals who entered into or departed from the main study area for a minimum of 12 months are considered

migrants.

Most named chimpanzees were observed within the central study area (see maps in Teleki, 1973a, 19756) at least once per month, and for many there are daily or weekly attendance records. But few individuals were present for the entire 1963-1973 decade, and there are gaps of as much as 6-12 months in the attendance profiles of some chimpanzees. To cope with these discrepancies, ape-fears were calculated for all named individuals in order to generate life tables, fertility schedules and reproductive rates with For example, female Winkle was thought to have been born in maximum accuracy. 1960 (at the mid-year

point, as she was unknown

then) and was observed

in the study

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

area from 21 August, accordingly

563

DEMOGRAPHY

1968 to the end of the sampled decade

on 30 June,

1973.

She is

defined as contributing

0.8 ape-year for her age at entry (estimated at 8 years), and 1.O ape-year for each of the following 4 years (ages 9, 10, 11, 12). Any individual present for the entire study period

is of course allotted

IO.0 ape-years.

The tally for the

decade yields 237.2 ape-years for males and 246.0 ape-years for females. This technique is designed to cope with two problems specific to data collected field primatologists. beyond

First, some individuals

a home range normally

precludes

the assumption

occupied

temporarily

travel

measure

a prescribed zone. years”-is essential in demographic Even among baboon

alone

by

within or

by a given social unit, and this phenomenon

that anyone not observed by investigators

A standard

membership.

may

of observed

is no longer within

attendance-perhaps

“primate

studies of all species exhibiting

flexible social unit

troops, which are often considered

stable in member-

ship, there are temporary and permanent migrants (Ransom, 1971; Harding, 1973). Second, field data on primate survivorship is less likely to be complete than the mortality data available matologists

to demographers

studying

human

populations.

An assumption

by pri-

that individuals

must be present even when they are not observed for long The ape-year measure periods may artificially inflate various demographic calculations. eliminates such assumptions. Many of the Gombe chimpanzees

grades-infant,

juvenile,

adolescent

and in many cases adulthood and old.

But when physiological

Goodall,

1967, 1968, 1973).

therefore 1975b).

contains Further

and behavioral

discrepancies inconsistencies

among

(We

mature

of 2-3

(e.g. Nishida,

several experienced

interval

1968; Sugiyama,

and if a consensus opinion

field investigators

a note of caution

here about

(Lawick-Goodall, when reports from

1968; Lawick-Goodall,

about the probable

during the course of a study, then the above problems

must introduce

rates (see Lawick-

data on age-class membership

are introduced

are available

(and mixed

the resultant intervals

at variable

years in any given

in class assignment

1975b). If birth dates for many individuals reached

criteria are thus combined

The most recently published

grounds,

into two stages-prime

to assess rates of development,

because individuals

different field sites are compared

individuals

into one of four specific age

physical and/or behavioral

can in turn be readily segmented

with an added measure of intuition) are likely to be inconsistent

can be readily placed and adult-on

ages of older

are largely resolvable.

using growth

captive studies of the same primate species because captivity

can be

rates obtained

can dramatically

from

accelerate

both physical and behavioral development.) But for purposes of demographic analysis, standardized age intervals are essential. We have accordingly chosen to use an interval of 4 years for each of the three lowest age grades listed above, an interval of 14 years for the prime adult grade and an interval of 10+ years for the old adult grade. Thus, infant covers a span of O-4 years, juvenile 5-8 years, adolescent 9-12 years, prime adult 13-26 years? and old adult 27-38 + years. The upper terminus on this scale is close to the estimated age of Flo, the oldest Gombe chimpanzee. As the sample is very small for wild chimpanzees, this upper figure remains flexible. The 12-year marker for achieving adulthood represents an average for males and females. When the two sexes are treated separately, female adulthood is reached at about 11 years, the mean age at first pregnancy, and male adulthood at about 13 years. In order to maintain

consistency

in all analytical

procedures,

all named chimpanzees

have been assigned to age classes on the basis of the number of years elapsed since their

564

G. TELEKI ET AL.

known or estimated dates of birth.

This is in some cases an over-simplification

able data, but will have to serve until field studies covering

of avail-

several generation

spans are

completed. 4. Chimpanzee The chimpanzees

inhabiting

Gombe

National

Population

Units

Park can be viewed in terms of three basic

types of numerical units : a park population, a study population, and a social community. TWO of these units-the park and study populations-are probably somewhat artificial in their construct, the first because park boundaries were drawn long before the ecological and ethological needs of resident chimpanzees were known and the second because long-term

banana provisioning

probably

altered the structure

of natural

units

within

the

park system. The three types of units must therefore be clearly defined in relation to one another and to other units described in the literature before the structural (size, composition) and dynamic (mortality, natality, chimpanzee society are examined in detail. All chimpanzees Some chimpanzees

living

within

occasionally

Gombe

migration)

National

range beyond

characteristics

the park pop_dution.

Park constitute

the park borders,

of Gombe

sometimes

for several

days at a time, but today this unit is probably a near or complete reproductive isolate surrounded on all sides by physical barriers: the rift escarpment and lake shore to the east and west, and human to the east (Teleki, completed,

settlements to the north and south as well as beyond

1973a).*

but it probably

A detailed

census of this park population

consists of about

100-150

chimpanzees

the rift

has not yet been (Lawick-Goodall,

1968). The park population some 20-50

members

These communities productively

contains (Teleki,

at least three social communities, each of which includes 19753),

can be spatially

nor behaviorally

and perhaps

and socially

isolated

because

one or two smaller units as well.

distinguished,

but they are neither re-

some migration

occurs

among

them at

Gombe and elsewhere (Lawick-Goodall, 19756; Nishida & Kawanaka, 1972). Due to the interdisciplinary and international nature of field research on chimpanzees, the literature abounds with synonymous

labels referring to a fundamental

organizational

unit of chimpanzee society that contains some 30-80 members and occupies a given home Thus, range. Such ranges may be discrete or overlapping, stable or seasonally mobile. the natural

“social

called a group a unit group Goodall, “troop”

unit”

(Reynolds (Nishida,

19758).

originally

described

& Reynolds, 1968),

and a social community

In a primatological

level of organization,

by Itani & Suzuki (1967) has also been 1965), a regional population (Sugiyama, 1969))

framework

(Teleki,

1974, 19756;

this chimpanzee

whereas in ethnological

Lawick-

unit is comparable

to

terms it is perhaps most closely The matrifocal unit, which has

allied to a “band” level of human social organization. been structurally and functionally described for several non-human primate societies (Teleki, 1975a), is therefore not, as Lawick-Goodall (1968) once postulated, the only lasting social nexus among wild chimpanzees.

* The eastern range of Pan troglodytes today runs parallel to the western arm of the Great Rift Valley, with several narrow belt that extends from Lake Albert to the latter, the chimpanzee range foilows the to Mt. Lubangagulu (6’35’s) in Tanzania, inland (Kane, 1972).

populations inhabiting a Lake Tanganyika. Along eastern shoreline at least no more than 30-40 km

GOMBE

The Gombe

Stream

Research

larger social communities

within

CHIMPANZEE

Centre

565

DEMOGRAPHY

is located

within

the home range

the park, one that is hereafter

referred

of one of the

to as the “Ka-

kombe

Community” because a valley having that name lies near the center of this home range (see map in Teleki, 19753). B ecause most or all of the Kakombe community presumably became habituated to humans via banana provisioning, and also because most observations between 1963 and 1973 occurred within a small central study zone that covers a map area of about 8 km2, and perhaps as much as 10 km2 surface area, this community is the core of the demographic survey reported here. As will be described in a later section, however, the membership of Kakombe com-

munity was probably augmented by several pseudo-migrants who were attracted to the main study zone by the availability of bananas, and who therefore adopted temporary or permanent

residence

in this area.

This larger demographic

constellation,

which con-

records, constitutes a study pojulation whose If all chimpanzees suspected of having mean size during the decade was 48.1 individuals. tains all named

chimpanzees

pseudo-migrant

status are subtracted

the Kakombe

community

Population opportunity rugged

from the annual

densities cannot The

census records,

the mean

size of

would be about 40 individuals. be precisely

for error is further increased

terrain.

the actual

in the center’s

calculated

from these census figures,

by a problem

in measuring

and the

surface area in such

estimated

map area of the park (32 km2) may be much less than surface area (32 + 6 km2), and a similar discrepancy may exist (8 km2) and the surface area (8 + 2 km2) of the central study zone.

topographic

in the map area

Moreover, estimates of crude and specific densities should reflect differences in patterns of habitat utilization, and preliminary data on ranging habits at Gombe suggest that several km3 of the steep, rocky terrain near the escarpment

crest are not routinely

Given this variability only be roughly 2.2-3.1

in census and area measurements, Starting with 100 chimpanzees estimated.

and the s.d. about 2.94.0;

and the s.d. to 4.3-6.1. community

exhibit

for the entire

with 150 chimpanzees,

utilized.

crude and specific densities can in the park, the cd. is about the c.d. would rise to 3-334.6,

Within the central study zone, the study population

a c.d. range of 4.8-6.0,

park region.

which falls within

and Kakombe

the 2.2-6~1

As most or all of this zone is heavily

range

utilized,

given

the specific

density should be similar to the above figure.

Pojwlaiion size : the annual census The

annual

size of the Gombe

high of 62 members records

include

study population

to a 1973 low of 39 members

96 chimpanzees

: 87 named

fluctuated (Figure

considerably-from

a 1966

1).

census

The

1963-1973

individuals, 9 unnam-ed births and fetal a very high turnover during these years:

deaths. But the study population experienced only 4 females and 9 males (13.5 % of 9’)o were present for the entire decade. births, 33 postnatal plus 6 fetal deaths, and a net gain of 2 immigrants between

With 30 1966 and

1972 (when migration was probably least affected by provisioning), the study population’s composition differed considerably from year to year. Indeed, given these gross internal changes it is remarkable that the population maintained as much numerical and compositional stability as shown in the accompanying graph. In order to estimate the probable size of the Kakombe community,

the average number

566

G. TELEKI

ET AL.

Figure 1. Temporal fluctuations in the Gombe study population (solid line) census data, and in the estimated size of the Kakombe community (broken line), shown in relation to various banana provisioning schedules (A-E).

PROVISIONING

::I, , . , , , , , , . ,964

,965

,966

1907 1968 ,969

1970 197,

1972 ,973

YEARS

of suspected

pseudo-immigrants-i.e.

nanas-can

be subtracted

corrections

those entering

are made only between

was most consistent.

the local home

from the annual census totals.

these trends and provisioning

in Figure

1, these

1966 and 1972, the period when banana provisioning

This provides a crude comparative

within both natural and artificial

range to seek ba-

As depicted

measure of demographic

units,. The coincidental

schedules

(see stages A-E

and/or in Figure

trends

causal links between 1 and Table

9) are

discussed in later sections. Pojudation When

composition : age and sex classes

the mean annual membership

line (48.1 =

of the study population

13.1 ‘A juveniles,

age classes break down as follows: 17.0 ‘A adolescents, 54.7 y0 adults. The adult/subadult

entire study population is 1.21. Sex ratios within the study population differences

among

100.0 females,

is used as a numerical

100 %), the four major

the four age grades.

while the age-specific

infants, 85.3 for juveniles,

base

15.2 % infants, ratio for the

vary little on an annual basis but reflect major The 1963-1973 mean sex ratio is 94.7 males to

sex ratios in the four main classes are:

141.2 for adolescents,

and 94.8 for adults.

breaks down into 83.6 for prime adults and 200.0 for old adults.

65.9 for

The latter class

Thus, while the sexes

seem to be at or near parity within the study population

as a whole, and also within the adult class as a whole, there are marked differences in the remaining classes. For example, males are greatly exceeded by females in the infant and juvenile classes while the reverse is true in the adolescent and old adult classes. These discrepancies suggest that sexspecific factors regulate survivorship within age-classes, all the more so in regard to excess adolescent and old adult males because female births exceeded male births by 50.0 % during the 1963-1973 period (see Table 1). Two population pyramids have been constructed from the baseline census data to further illustrate these features (Figure 2). Pyramid A depicts only percentage membership in various age classes, and shows that a fairly even distribution exists in all but the two youngest classes. Pyramid B adds a sex-class dimension to this age distribution, and thereby shows that females decrease rather evenly during the middle ages (9-24 years) and more sharply at later ages (25-40 years), while males decrease sharply during early

GOMBE

CHIMPANZEE

Figure 2. Percent distribution of age-classes (pyramid A, top) and of age- and sex-classes (pyramid B, bottom) in relation to total population size. Both pyramids reflect small percentages of young individuals (infants and juveniles) in the Gombe study population.

567

DEMOGRAPHY

A

Tzi -

33-36

1

I

29-32 25-28

I

y21-24

z ;;r;-, -

I

13- 16 9-12

1

7 o-4 -

I IO

5

0

15

1 20

PERCENT

ZG -33-36

d

6

2

29-32

25=18 21-24

y

1

z17-20 13-16 9-12 5-8 O-4 -

I I I I f

1

0

5

IO

5

IO

PERCENT

(13-20 years) and more gradually at later ages (2 I-40 years). A preponderance of old adult males is clearly illustrated in pyramid B. There is unfortunately little merit in speculating about the natural causes behind these phenomena because differences

adulthood

in male and female survivorship selective

effects of provisioning

in the 21-32 upon

year brackets may simply be related

the mortality

and migratory

to

patterns of the two

sexes. When compared to standard pyramids for wildlife populations (e.g., Qdum, 1971), the chimpanzee study population seems to reflect neither marked stability nor potential for rapid expansion.

In fact, when the topheavy appearance

of the chimpanzee

pyramids

is viewed in the context of the epidemics recorded at Gombe, the indication is that considerable time is needed to achieve reproductive recovery from such major crises. The slow

maturation

contribute

and

reproduction

to a recovery

chimpanzees,

rates described

in the

following

lag which may span an entire generation,

section

could

which, for the Gombe

is an average of 19.6 years.

be typical for wild chimpanzees,

A topheavy structure may, on the other hand, for the K-group pyramid at the Mahali site also leans in

a topheavy

in pressj.

Although

direction

(Nishida,

there is still no precise scale for gauging

in wild ape populations,

normality

of age-sex distributions

an urgent need exists for field techniques

whereby

the repro-

ductive status of a potentially threatened primate population can be monitored. Data on the composition of the Kasakati (Izawa, 1970) and Mahali (Nishida, in press) study populations supplement those available from Gombe, and the combined figures can be used to develop an “optimal” matrix of age/sex composition panzee unit. These three sites are selected for comparison proximity and ecological similarity.

for an hypothetical chimdue to their geographical

As shown in Table 2, the three study populations exhibit some variation in percentages of immature and mature males and females, but the variation is well within what would

568

G. TELEKI

Table

2

ET AL.

Percentage distribution of individuals, by age and sex, for three chimpanzee population units in western Tanzania, combined to depict an hypothetical population with an optimal composition

Gombe (study population) Males Females Mature Immature Samples :

Kasakati

Mahali

(Z group)

(I( group)

Males

0.27 0.28 0.23 0.22 N = 4% 1 mean over 10 years

B Males

Females

0.20 0.30 0.25 0.25 N = 36 members over 2 years

Average Unit

Females

0.21 0.34 0.09 0.36 N = 26.3 mean over 8 years

Males

Females

0.23 0.20

0.30 0.27

Note: the marker age separating immaturity years in each study population.

be expected

if age and sex distributions

varied randomly

(x2 =

and maturity is about 12

1.61, p = O-90 at 5 degrees

of freedom). Also shown in the table is an average, or optimal, matrix which reflects a preponderance of both immature and mature females. Comparison of these figures with the Gombe

matrix

suggests that the Gombe

study population

is somewhat

deficient

in

both categories. Even if the Gombe study population continuing

percentages

reproduction

additional

measure

to counteract

populations

deviations

managers

could

monitored

and to ensure

if and when necessary, from

need cause no immediate

should be carefully

from

then

regional

determine

long-term

development

become

alarm,

in the coming

it is evident

that this

years in order to evaluate

survival.

Steps could then be taken,

of further

discrepancies.

When

data

available, or local

this tentative model can be refined to Field primatologists and wildlife norms.

the need for protective

action

on a more

pragmatic

basis than is now common. 6. Chimpanzee The analytical disciplines Gombe

techniques

of human

data-small

death dates, etc.-are

Population

Dynamics

applied in this section have been selectively

demography numerical

and wildlife

samples,

in someways

biology.

continuous

borrowed

However,

census records,

imprecise

unique to the field study ofnon-human

tions, and do not always allow for application primatologists

have not yet developed

our approach

is often experimental.

of standard

the analytical Demographic

from the

the nature

demographic

of the

birth

and

primate populatechniques.

As

tool kit needed for this special task, research on non-human primates is

clearly in need of development and refinement, and this is but a first step in that direction. The raw data upon which analyses of mortality, natality and migratory patterns are based

appear

the 1963-1973 Mortality

in Figure

3, which

is a graphic

summary

of all events recorded

during

study period.

patterns

During that decade 33 postnatal deaths were known or estimated to have occurred within the study population, the causes running a gamut from contagious disease to fatal accidents. There were 6 additional cases of fetal death, representingl5.4 ity recorded, but these will be discussed with natality patterns.

% of the total mortal-

569

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE DEMOGRAPHY

Figure 3. Raw data on mortality (top), natality (middle) and migratory (bottom) patterns in the Gombe study population during 1963-1973.

0 0

The

distribution

adolescents, chimpanzees,

48.5 o/oadults). with mortality

the susceptibility wastage.

of postnatal

deaths

but highly variable

of infants

is close to parity

Deaths (ii=331

n

Fetal Deaths

0

Live Births

Emigration

12

(n=6)

Deaths

Postnatal

Immigration

14

54.5 % females)

Fetal

(n=6)

(8 =30)

(n=23) In= 18,

along sex lines (45.5 % males,

along age lines (33.3 % infants, 9.1 % juveniles,

Mortality

in the first four years of life probably to both

No proof has been obtained

9.1 %

is heaviest in the infant and adult classes of Gombe direct

(e.g. disease)

from East African

being exacerbated

and indirect

by

(e.g. orphanage)

studies that carnivore

predation

contributes to chimpanzee mortality (Teleki, 1973a,d), although West African populations are heavily preyed upon by humans (Harrisson, 1971; Sabater Pi & Groves,1972; Gartlan,

1975).

Diseases,

parasites,

causes of nearly all wild chimpanzee

genetic

abnormalities

and injuries

are the likely

deaths.

An epidemic of some paralytic disease (probably poliomyelitis) and two severe bouts of respiratory illness (probably influenza) struck the study population in 1966, 1968 and 1969. Directly or indirectly-that is, via infection or orphanage-these events accounted for most of the 23 postnatal deaths logged over this 4-year span. In fact, some 30 ‘A to 40 % of the mortality recorded at Gombe during the 1963-1973 period can probably be attributed to pathogenic the estimate underscores

causes. Even if some cases have been incorrectly diagnosed, the high susceptibility of this population to contagious diseases.

570

G. TELEKI

Control

of epidemics

conservation,

ET AL.

should thus be a key issue in future programs

at Gombe

and probably

elsewhere

of management

and

as well.

Given this insight, it i.s especially unfortunate that so little is known about the epidemiology of wild chimpanzee populations. Some general information on disease symptoms and internal parasites is provided by Lawick-Goodall (1968,1975), who refers to undiagnosed (one

cases of skin ailments

resembling

(perhaps

goitre),

influenza

and peritonitis. Attempts

Ciliates,

nasal

hookworms

congestion

growths

often

as we11 as confirmed and roundworms

to treat the paralytic

on the face and neck

accompanied

by coughing

cases of pneumonia,

have been recovered

disease

(Lawick-Goodall,

enteritis

from feces. 1968)

and a

as Rhinophycomycosis enteromorphae (Roy & Cameron, 1972 ; Roy Given the duration of the Gombe project, the epidemiological in field conditions.

1974), comm.)

noted

Budongo

sparse.

graphic

Forest.

Behavioral

descriptions

1968,

Lawick-Goodall

1971,

of chimpanzee

1975a-c;

& Hamburg,

deaths are somewhat

Suzuki,

1971a;

1974), but such information

Bygott,

more com-

1972;

Teleki,

is of little use in demo-

analysis of the causes of mortality.

No conclusive traction

Other field projects have fared no better: Kortlandt (pers. cases at Beni, and Suzuki (1971 b) observed an amputee at

7 “paresis”

(Lawick-Goodall,

19736;

evidence

or dissemination

distribution

of crude

that an historical mains somewhat middle

and colds),

mange),

diagnosed

data are extremely

mon

lung

or common

have been made

facial fungus,

(one resembling

years,

of a direct

death

linkage

link between

banana

provisioning

of disease has yet been obtained rates so clearly

cannot

circumstantial.

parallels

at Gombe.

the various

and the conBut the annual

provisioning

schedules

be discounted even though the evidence at present reMortality rates were low in the early years, high in the

in the final years of the decade (Figure 4). The crude death rate ranged from a maximum of 2 17*4/ 1000 in 1969 to a minimum or 19.6/1000 in 1972. Computed from the 33 cases backed by conclusive or strongly circumstantial evidence, the mean mortality rate for the decade was 6%0/l 000. If all losses from the study population

and low again

were considered

as deaths

Figure 4. Temporal distribution of mortality and natality rates per 1000 individuals for the Gombe study population. Fetal wastage is excluded from both samples.

(i.e. no migration),

the crude

250

death

rate would

-

Mortality

--__

Natality

200-

:_

ISO-

L B w 2 az

IOO-

SO-

I 1964

1965

1966

1967

1968

1969

1970

1971

1972

1973

GOMBE

reach

11 l-7/1000

The

individuals.

CHIMPANZEE

true rate, which probably

these two extremes, seems high for such a small population. raised mortality

571

DEMOGRAPHY

is further strengthened

by a concentration

lies somewhere

between

The premise that provisioning of all known prenatal deaths

in the same period that postnatal mortality reached a peak (see Table 6 for a non-parametric runs test of fetal wastage). Abnormally high and frequent aggregations of chimpanzees within a small area, accompanied by increased aggressive limited food resource, may well have raised prenatal and postnatal exceptional

levels between

1967 and 1970.

Various

introduced

and transmitted

quite effectively

in these conditions

(see summary and/or

contagious

competition for a mortality rates to

diseases may have been of high density and stress

of similar cases in other species by Stott, 1969), with imported

bananas

human suppliers perhaps serving as vectors.

Survivorship. Postnatal Gombe

mortality

study population

cases-the published

(Table

can be transformed 3). Despite

into

a current

life table

for the

the structures of a small sample-only

33

computations are enlightening because no survivorship scale has ever been for wild or captive chimpanzee groups from which individuals were not arti-

ficially

culled. Demographic data obtained from captive colonies are neither complete nor reliable (Bourne, 1972; Butler, 1972), and the unpublished demographic report on Kortlandt’s Beni study population (Ku&ten, 1972) attempts to reconstruct chimpanzee

demography Table

3

Age intervals

by mathematically

reshaping human mortality tables and then extrapolating

Current life table based on known and estimated ages at death of 33 Gombe chimpanzees, with both sexes combined. Mean length of life is 13.5 years

Number dead

Apeyears

(years),

xtox+n o-2 1 3AA 5-6 1 7-8 B 9-10 11-12 I c 13-14 15-16 17-18 19-20 D 21-22 23-24 I 25-26

8 3 2 1 S 1 1 1 0 : 2 3 0 2 1 0 1

42.6 34.8 32.4 35.3 40.6 48.4 44.2 39.7 27.4 24.3 26.0 24.8 19.5 12.8 11.0 10.5 5.4 2.0 1.5

Probability of dying in each age interval,

&

Number entering each age interval out of 1000 born, L!

Average years of life remaining to survivors in each age interval %

0.188 0.086 0.062 0.028 0.049 0.041 0.023 0.025 0.036 0.000 0.038 0.081 0.103 0.234 0.000 0.190 0.185 0.000 0.667

1000~0 659.3 550.8 484.7 457.9 414.2 380.9 363.5 345.5 321.1 321.1 297.2 251.0 202.0 118.5 118.5 77.8 51.7 51.7

11.9 15,6 16.5 16.6 15.5 15.0 14.2 12.9 11.5 10.3 8.3 6,9 6.0 5.2 6.1 4.1 3.7 3.1 1.1

Note: to facilitate comparison with other demographic features, the age intervals are also represented in terms of age grades: A, infant; B, juvenile; C, adolescent; D, prime adult; E, old adult.

572

G. TELEKI

to obtain

a fit with wild chimpanzee

ET AL.

population

curves.

Similar

limitations

occur

at

other field sites, such as Mahali, where Nishida (in press) has collected data on natality and infant mortality but has not yet obtained a general picture of postnatal mortality. In contrast ape-years

to the life tables commonly

have

(the d, column) other components

been

used to compute

among

the Gombe

generated

for wildlife and human

the probability

chimpanzees.

in the life table are standard

of dying

The procedures (see, for example,

populations,

for each

age interval

used to generate Deevey,

all

1970).

Abridged life tables for each sex, based on 15 male deaths and 18 female deaths, were then compiled to illustrate differences in male and female life expectancies at various Table

4

Abridged for each 15 males

life table results showiqg average sex, based on kmnwn and estimated and 18 females

Age intervals (years)

O-2 3-s 9-14 15-20 21-26 27-32 33-38

life expectancies ages at death of

Average years of life remaining to survivors in each age interval, e, / Males

\ Females

(years)

(years)

14.3 15.9 16.6 13.3 El.9 6.7 3.0

IO.9 16.2 15.3 13.2 8.4 5.5 3.0

Figure 5. Actual survivorship in the Gombe study population (solid line) during 1963-1973, and estimated survivorship in a hypothetical wildchimpanzee population (dotted line) based partly on Kortlandt’s data from the Beni site. The latter has been adapted from Kuijsten (1972). Both curves are on a logarithmic scale.

20

AGEIN

3”

YEARS

3”

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

ages (Table

4).

The results indicate

573

DEMOGRAPHY

of life

that the expectation

at birth (e,,) is greater (er5). The dif-

for males than for females, but that parity is achieved by early adulthood

ference arises mainly from a less favorable set of death rates for females between infancy The causes remain unidentified, and may involve physiological, ecoand adolescence. logical and/or

behavioral

factors.

When plotted on a logarithmic

scale, from the I, column

in Table

3, the survivorship

curve shows a rapid drop during the initial 4 years of life and a steady decline over the following age intervals (Figure 5). The curve in general exhibits a remarkably even convex shape that is characteristic the end of the life span (Odum, (1972) is also provided

of populations in which most mortality occurs toward 1971). The survivorship curve obtained by Ku&ten

for comparative

purposes. although

what skewed because fragments of several populations The life expectancy that management particularly

and survivorship

rates exhibited

policies should include contingency

among females, when the population

his rates are proba’bly

some-

were combined. by Gombe

chimpanzees

plans for reducing

suggest

infant mortality

is struck by unforeseen calamities

such

as epidemics. Swift recovery by such a small and isolated population is essential if gaps in the age pyramid are to be averted, and may warrant artificial stimulation (e.g. medication) by wildlife managers. Orphan mortality. At Gombe,

chimpanzee

(Clark, in press), and are physically, mothers

infants nurse

psychologically

for

an

for at least that many

tapers of& offspring

years. Even after the nutritional may hinge mainly upon continuing

survival

mother for several additional patterns within the younger behavioral

of

years (Lawick-Goodall,

1975u-c).

5.2 years upon their

basis of dependence association with the

Mortality

and survival

age classes may therefore be strongly affected by social and

factors (relationships

factors (weather,

average

and socially dependent

within matrifocal

units, etc.) as well as various extrinsic

disease, etc.).

The deaths of G named deaths, are listed in Table

orphans,

all of whom

died within 3.5 years of their mothers’

5. All but one of these (Pepe) died well before the expected

time shown for their respective

ages in the life table.

male whose death was probably

unrelated to being orphaned.)

died within 0.1-2.5

(Pepe was a paraplegic The remaining

adolescent 5 orphans

years of their mothers, and only one (Flint) was older than 4.5 years.

In at least 3 cases (Cindy,

Merlin,

Flint) the symptoms

prior to death included

general

listlessness and lethargy, with severely reduced maintenance activity and social inter-. action. And in the particular case of Flint, who was intensively observed during the 2 5 days after his mother’s death, psychological

distress and depression may well have contributed

to or even caused death, although he also acquired an intestinal infection (Lawick-Goodall, 197%; La-wick-Goodall & Hamburg, 1974), These and other forms of “grieving” behavior reported elsewhere (Teleki, 19736) suggest that wild chimpanzees perceive death among close kin as an emotional events.

issue, and may possess a generalized

concept

of such

Precise prediction of the age at which orphans are likely to survive parental mortality is prohibited by the small size of the available sample. The data merely suggest that a. death/survival transition occurs for most orphans around 5 years of age, but may stretch another 3-4 years in special circumstances. Prior to the 5-year limit, nutritional deficiencies, coupled mortality.

with social and other deprivations,

are the most likely causes of orphan

30 24 24 30 27 26 37

24 26 24 37 27 37 37

December 1964 October 1968 January 1968 December 1964 January 1969 March 1965 December 1972

October 1968 March 1965 January 1968 December 1972

January 1969 December 1972 December 1972

Bessie Vodka Sophie Bessie Oily Marina Flo

Vodka Marina Sophie Flo

Oily Flo Flo

*d = d = -t = ? = ?A = C = ? =

16 26

January 1968 March 1965

F F

F F F

F

M M M

M M

M

M M

M

13.6 16.5 19.5

11.3 11.8 12.6 13.5

4.5 6.3 6.6 7.5 7.6 7.8 8.9

3.2 3.7

14.2 12.1 10.3

4.3 + 0.5 + 0.5 +

16.5 16.5 16.5 16.6 16.6 16.6 16.6

15.0 15.0 15.6 14.2

d + + ? + + d

15.6 15.6

11.9

4.7 + 3.5 d -? 0.5 +

2.5 4.7 4.4 3.5 4.4 8.2 0.1

0.2 d 1.6d

0.1 d

Faben -

Faben Figan -

-

Evered Pepe Faben Figan Fifi -

Pepe Miff Bumble Jomeo Sally -

Sally Sniff -

M 19.5 - --

19.5 16.5

13.6 11.8 19.5 16.5 13.5

7.8 7.5 11.3 12.6

C -

A A A ? A ? ? C A C C -

F F M F -M M M M F ---M M --

M 11.8

? A A

F 12.6 M 6.6 --

Known death, probably related to being orphaned. Known death, probably unrelated to being orphaned. Surviving at end of study period on June 30, 1973. Disappeared, possibly dead. Adoption behavior (described in Lawick-Goodall, 1975c) observed between older sibling and orphan. Contact and interaction, but no overt adoption behavior, observed between older sibling and orphan. Orphan not observed in association with older sibling.

Evered Figan Faben

Jomeo Pepe Sally Fifi

Beattle Sherry Sniff Bumble Gilka Miff Flint

F

Cindy Merlin

1.3

Behavior of older sibling toward orphan?

Circe Marina

F

Sorema

Name of Sex and known or age of suspected older older sibling (years) at age when sibling mother died

Orphan’s expectation of life

24

(years) when mother died

Subsequent survival span (years) of orphaned offspring*

January 1968

spring

spring

Known or estimated age of offspring

Sophie

Off-

Off-

Name of Sex of

Date of mother’s death

Name of mother

Mother’s estimated age at death

Mortality and survival patterns among 17 named offspring who were orphaned (at 1.3-19.5 years of age) when 7 mothers died during the 1963-1973 study period

Table 5

% b t*

!I.+

2

P

UI 2

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

Whether

adoption

of orphans

by older siblings

uncertain.

The final column in Table

commonly

exhibited

575

DEMOGRAPHY

has a positive influence

5 suggests that adoptive

behavior

also remains

tends to be most

toward the youngest

orphans, but sibling adoption does not seem to promote survival below the 5-year age limit. Nor is there any indication that the sex of In terms of wildlife the older sibling regulates adoptive behavior or influences survival. management, it would be advisable to monitor the welfare reproductive females who

of

head matrifocal units, having large matrifocal

and to investigate the potential units within a social community.

survival

benefits

provided

by

Natality patterns During

the 1963-1973

study period,

36 pregnancies

terminating

in 30 live births and 6

The disparity previously fetal deaths were recorded within the study population. for the sex ratio of postnatal deaths among infants (3 males/S females = 0.375) matched

by the disparity

with a low reproductive

present

in births

(12 males/l8

rate, a slow maturation

females = O-667).

rate, and a low incidence

noted is not

Together

of twin pro-

duction (observed occasionally in captivity but not yet in the wild), the above discrepancies in female natality and mortality rates point to a low growth rate for chimpanzee

populations.

The

picture

is somewhat

incomplete,

however,

because

the sex

of stillbirths and miscarriages was not determined. The annual distribution of crude birth rates is a mirror image of the crude death rates described

previously,

and were therefore

as likely

affected

by provisioning

schedules.

Natality rates were high in the early years, low in the middle years, and high again in the final years of the decade (see Figure 4). The crude birth rate ranged from a maximum of 97.6/1000 the entire

in 1964 to a minimum

IO-year period.

the study population Measures

of infant

of 18~8/1000 in 1967, with a mean of 62*0/1000

As the mean mortality

strayed from equilibrium death

rate

(233*3/1000),

and natality

largely as a result of migratory combined

(466*7/1000), infant mortality risk rate (68*3/1000), 1000) can also be obtained from the census records.

for

rates are close to equality,

infant

plus fetal

and fetal mortality

variations. death

risk rate

rate (43.11

These measures are all based on a reduced infancy span of O-l year, rather than on the usual 4-year interval, to match demographic standards established for human populations. A test was undertaken

to determine

year could be assumed for live births, the Gombe

study population.

whether

a constant

fetal deaths,

probability

and total pregnancy

of occurrence terminations

* These data, when fitted to Poisson distributions

per in

(Cooper,

* The fit of an observed set of a events, classified by their frequency of occurrence (f), can be examined by the chi-square method (Bliss, 1953). This procedure is one-tailed, however, in the sense that it merely gives an accurate probability that the observed unbiased variance (se) is greater than the observed mean (a) of the distribution. In a perfect Poisson distribution, s2 = 2. Another approach was therefore adopted, using this equation as a null hypothesis, in order to obtain a two-tailed test of the conformity of each empirical distribution to the expected distributions. The observed mean (2) was taken as an expected variance with infinite degrees of freedom. (This technique increases the conservativeness of the test.) The observed variance (s”) was then considered in terms of its own degrees of freedom (n - 1). The variance ratio (F) was finally computed, using appropriate degrees of freedom in the numerator and denominator, and the two-tailed probability obtained from a table of F (Lindquist, 1953).

576 Table

G. TELEKI 6

Number of events,

f

Types of events

Live births Fetal deaths Total pregnancies

ET

AL.

Annual frequencies of live births, fetal deaths and total pregnancies in relation to expected Poisson distributions, with a non-parametric runs test of annual fetal death frequencies (see census data in Table 1)

Live births obtained b obt

Number of events,

Live births expected, b=a

Fetal deaths obtained, dObt

Fetal Total deaths pregnancies expected, obtained, dem Pobt

Total pregnancies expected, P exn

5 4

6 3

0 0

0

1

1

2 3 3 1 1 0

: 2 2 1 1 1

Obtained Obtained unbiased mean and variance, expected variance,

?I

s2

30 6 36

2.22 0.27 1.60

i 3.00 0.60 3.60

Fmln_l 1.35 2.22 2.25

P (two-tailed) 0.20 0.19 0.19

Non-parametric runs test when a = number of years without fetal deaths, b = number of years with fetal deaths and u = number of runs yields R, = 5 yrs, nl, = 5 yrs and u,,.,,s = 3.

1972), show that the observed or obtained numbers of pregnancies are all close to the expected numbers required by the Poisson hypothesis (Table 6). It should be noted, however, that the empirical variances are consistently smaller than the empirical means. These results suggest that an underlying constant probability of conception seems to occur within the study population on an annual basis. A non-parametric runs test on fetal deaths between 1966 and 1970 shows that such wastage is significant at the 0.05 level (Table 6). As mentioned previously, most fetal deaths were probably associated with the stresses accompanying banana provisioning. Fertility schedules. Male chimpanzees at Gombe reach puberty at 9-10 years of age, but may not contribute significantly to the reproductive pool of the population for another 3-4 years as a result of social peripheralization until full maturity is achieved (LawickGoodall, 19756). Females typically begin to exhibit sexual swellings of the perineum at 9-10 years of age, and then experience an additional 1-2 years of adolescent sterility; these swellings become larger and more regularly paced during this period, and menarche probably occurs at about II-12 years of age (Lawick-Goodall, 19753). First pregnancies are likely to occur between 11 and 14 years of age (Clark, in press).* Most females are * Most of the dates marking developmental stages differ in wild and captive chimpanzees. Rates of physical, behavioral and probably psychological maturation can be 2 or more years slower in the wild (compare this report to Riopelle, 1963). Body sizes and weights are significantly smaller at Gombe (Teleki, 1973c; Lawick-Coodall, 19756).

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

DEMOGRAPHY

577

likely to remain reproductively viable until death, although Flo, the oldest member of the study population, ceased to show swellings 2.8 years prior to her death. No verified cases of menopause have to date been recorded among wild chimpanzees. The mean generation span, or elapsed time between birth of a female and birth of her median offspring, is about 19.6 years for a sample of 10 Gombe females with 3 or more recorded births. In order to establish a standard measure of reproductive performance in the following calculations, the onset of a female’s reproductive span (wr) is here assumed to occur at age 10 years because a gestation period of about 8 lunar months can be subtracted from the minimum age at which Gombe females produce their first offspring. Termination of the reproductive span is assumed to occur at 35 years of age because the oldest Gombe female gave birth to her last infant when she was about 33 years of age. As the ages of initial and final parturition may be highly variable in a small population, however, these marker dates should be viewed as conservative estimates. It is possible that the 25-year average reproductive span used here may in some cases extend another IO-15 years. Among the 49 named females present in the Gombe census data, a sample of 27 females experienced ape-years in the lo-35 years age bracket. The surveillance period for this cohort encompassed a total of 137.8 ape-years. Of these 27 females, 20 exhibited either observed pregnancies or associated progeny during the 1963-1973 study period. Of the remaining 7 females, 6 were at or beyond the lo-year age level for only 6 months to 2 years of reproductive life. Only one female (Gigi) fell within the lo-35 age bracket and did not become pregnant at all during the decade. The reproductive performance of Gombe chimpanzees can be estimated from the above observations (Table 7). Given a potential reproductive span (w$ - wi) of 25 years, the data presented in the table yield the total pregnancy experience (O-235 x 25 years = 5.9 pregnancies), the total fertility (0.188 x 25 years = 4.7 live births), and the total lifetime fetal death experience (5.9 - 4-7 = I.2 fetal deaths) per female in the study population. Over the entire 25-year fertility span, the average probability of a live birth is 0.19 per annum for each female. When only the first 5 and last 5 reproductive years are compared, however, the figures show a 33 % drop in fertility, largely because most pregnancies occurred among those females who were IO-20 years of age. Nonetheless, the number of live births expected (be& is in fact close to the actual distribution observed (bobcj) among Gombe females. A chi-square test, measuring chance divergence of obtained and expected live births when the probability per year is 0.19, shows that the divergence is indeed small (x2 = 1*17,p = 0.9 1 at 4 degrees of freedom). Hence, these chimpanzees show essentially no statistical loss of reproductive capability with advancing age. According to the data presented in Table 7, the net reproductive rate (0.130 x 5 = 0.65 female offspring) is considerably lower than the gross reproductive rate (0.577 x 5 = 2.90 female offspring) in the Gombe study population. This discrepancy results from severe mortality in the young female cohort. The neonatal sex ratio in the small Gombe sample contains a scarcity of males, but the life table indicates that the prereproductive mortality of males is more favorable than that of females. Even so, this compensation is not enough to yield as many male offspring as female offspring a generation later. Coupled with the mortality and migratory patterns described in other sections, the above results allow for a guardedly positive prognosis regarding the future survival of the Gombe study population. But the survival balance may well shift into a negative *

O-228 0.193 0.238 0.128 0.154 z= 0.188

27

Live

z= 0.235

0.285 0.242 0.238 0.256 0.154

Live+ dead

Annual probability of birth

10 8 6 2 1

bexe

Live births expected if probability of birth is 0.2 years,

11*

7 0 3 1 0

Male live births observed

0.363

0.133 o*ooo 0.102 0.128 0.000

fi 0.478 0.404 0.301 0.184 0.071

P.

Males living at midpoint of each age interval,

0.119

0.064 0.000 0.031 0.024 o*ooo

Pafs

Agespecific fertility for surviving male offspring,

18

5 a 4 0 1



0.577

0.095 0.193 0.136 0.000 0.153

fs

Observed annual probability of live female births,

chimpanzees

Female live births observed

rates of male and female

Observed annual probability of live male births,

and reproductive

0.367 0.299 0.258 0.125 0.011

Pz?

Females living at midpoint of each age interval,

study

0.130

0.035 0.058 0.035 0.000 0*002

Pzf.

Agespecific fertility for surviving female offspring,

in the Gombe

* Note: these figures differ from the 30 births shown in the main census records because the mother of one male infant was not identified and aged in the initial study period.

29*

1376

Totals

6

12 a 7 1 1

3 2 0 1 0

52.7 41.4 29.4 7.8 6.5

Live births,

10 15 20 25 30

Fetal deaths

b obt

Apeyears

Reproductive performances population, 1963-1973

WI= 10 ws = 35

Age of mother,

Table 7

GOMBE

direction

if novel causes

CHIMPANZEE

of mortality-such

be held at a minimum .*

mans-cannot

struck the study population

as diseases The

paralytic

performance

of Gombe

ities is likely to be a long one. to virulent

Birth interuals.

Among

females,

by human vectors,

park boundaries

wild chimpanzees,

from

transient

hu-

epidemics

that

several females of breeding

only briefly

as orphans.

Given

the

the road to recovery from such calam-

If other chimpanzee

diseases transmitted

tions within fixed, narrow

contracted

and respiratory

in 1966, 1968 and 1969 eliminated

age, as well as several young females who survived low reproductive

579

DEMOGRAPHY

populations

are as highly susceptible

the enclosure of small, isolated popula-

may produce the capacity

a major

threat

to compensate

to survival. for high rates of

loss in the lower age classes is related to a shift in birth spacing when dependent offspring die. This mechanism warrants special attention because it carries important implications It has, in fact, been routinely for management of wild populations. ‘%-aise” reproductive rates in captive colonies (Butler, 1972; Bourne, Of the 20 females who produced females experienced various

used to artificially 1972).?

live or dead offspring during the 1963-1973

successive pregnancies.

As the number

period, 11

of successive pregnancies

for

females ranged from 1 to 4 cases, the total sample actually includes 17 paired In 7 cases (41%) the first offspring remained alive when a subsequent one was

events. born, and in 10 cases (59 %) the first died prior to the birth of the second. The average

interval

between

the birth dates of two consecutive

offspring

when each

survives is 5.6 years. According to Clark (in press), Gombe mothers lactate in the absence of perineal swellings for 3-4 years after parturition, then resume their cycles while still lactating

for about 2 more years, and finally wean their offspring at an average

of 5.2 years.

But if this routine

offspring, average

perineal

cycling

sequence

resumes much

of 1.6 years after the first dies.

70 % reproductive compensation (5.6 normal time interval between live births. related

to offspring

sooner and a second infant

survivorship

between Gombe

(Table

mortality

experienced

an

the birth dates of successive

females can therefore

achieve

off-

about

8). to have given birth to their first pro-

period, the mean age at the time of this event was 11.8 years.

As 3 of the 9 recorded cases resulted in prenatal or pregnancies

is born within

is high during the first 2 years

l-7/5*6 = O-696) when offspring die within the The data clearly indicate that offspring spacing

For the 9 females who were known or suspected geny during the 1963-1973

age

by the death of one dependent

As infant mortality

of life, the overall birth interval-measured spring-drops to an average of I.7 years.

is directly

is interrupted

deaths, it is probable

in the early years of the reproductive

risks for the offspring

that first pregnancies, span, involve greater

(x2 = 2.4, p = O-09, at 1 degree of freedom).

* Chimpanzees are especially prized as models in biomedical research, partly because they are susceptible to so many diseases contracted by humans (Harrisson, 1971). There is consequently some risk that contact between humans and wild chimpanzees will follow the pattern of past contact with North American Indian peoples, or present contact with such groups as the Kreen-Akarore of South America. T Despite the extensive medical and nutritional care given to captive chimpanzees, as well as having birth intervals greatly shortened by separation of infants and mothers, the reproductive rate of the Gombe study population compares favorably to breeding rates in captive colonies (Teleki & Baldwin, 1975). Considering the expense of domestic breedina programs for primates (Hobbs, 1975), Their ov&all productivity and effiy ciency should be questioned via comparison with wild populations,

580

G. TELEKI

Table

8

AL.

Birth intervals among successive offspring produced by chimpanzee mothers in the Gombe study population, 1963-1973

Viability of initial offspring when next progeny born

Number of cases, N Range

Dead Alive

10 7

Based

on the fertility

chimpanzees during

ET

a lifespan

however,

schedule

can theoretically

1.0-2.8 3.0-7.6

and birth

be expected

of 40 years.

1.6 5.6

interval

0.6 1.5

data

estimate

the balancing

effect of a reduction

Furthermore,

since old females

achieve

reduced ductive

record

is known

represents

mechanism

exhibited

adaptive

adaptations

6 live offspring,

extreme

this conservative

Given

(Lawick-Goodall,

Birth

or groups

interval

that serve to compensate units,

in the birth

interval

the above

estimate

raised to sexual maturity. female in the Gombe

with 5 surving

of reproduction

by individuals

significance.

beyond

when

their last is further The repro-

census records,

the age of 4 years,

adoption,

compensatory

any compensatory

a chimpanzee

population

has

may be one of many physiological

for a low reproduction

Orphan

serving a similar

and survivorship, within

reduction

at least in those cases observed

social adaptations

habitat.

success among wild chimpanzees.

picture

rate among wild chimpanzees. matrifocal

reproduction,

in a natural

may well die before

1975),

that are successfully

of Flo, who, as the most long-lived

to have produced

probably

major

independence

to about 3-4 offspring

of 6 pregnancies

female are likely to live beyond

dependent progeny

7 and 8), female

of achieved

operate to reduce survivorship

an age of 4 years despite die.

(0.001

a maximum

no more than 5 offspring per reproductive

offspring

6.7

(see Tables

to experience

Th is is not a realistic

because many contingencies

Consequently,

Birth interval in years Mean Standard-----T deviation

rate and a slow maturation

which seems to operate largely within to date,

may in turn be one of many

role.

Migratory patterns Field

investigators

entire chimpanzee the transfer 1970;

of individuals

Nishida

“migration” migratory members

have described

as migration both the seasonal

social communities

from one portion

nomadic

movement

of a home range to another

or small clusters from one community

to another

of and

(e.g., Izawa,

& Kawanaka, 1972 ; Nishida, in press). As used in this report, the term Furthermore, inter-community refers only to the latter phenomenon.

patterns

at Gombe

of one community

seem to include

two patterns:

a temporary form where

members of another for some hours, days, and a more permanent form where various perhaps even weeks before returning home; social ties and eventually full residence are established in a new community, perhaps for a lifetime in some cases. As used here, the term “migration” again refers to only the latter form. An arbitrary

but convenient

meet and join

span of one year of presence

in or absence

from the study

population was used as the minimum duration for assigning migrant status. If all observed departures and arrivals defined in this manner are classified as inter-community migration, then 41 cases of transfer into or out of the study population occurred the 1963-1973 period (see Figure 3). This sample yields an annual immigration

during rate of

GOMBE

2.3 and an emigration

CHIMPANZEE

rate of 1.8 individuals,

581

DEMOGRAPHY

with a net migration

rate of $0.5

chim-

But the validity of these figures is highly suspect because the introduc-

panzees per year.

tion and subsequent reduction

of banana provisioning was probably an integral regulator of migration during certain years. Of the total number of migrants, fully 17 cases (41%) of immigration occurred in 1964 and 1965 while 14 cases (34 %) of emigration occurred in 1973.

Thus, 75 ‘A of the observed

cases occurred

within a mere 30 ‘A of the time en-

compassed by the census records. When the composition of these cohorts is more closely examined, 8 of the 17 “pseudo-immigrants”

the data show that

added at the start of the decade and 8 of the 14 “pseudo-

emigrants” subtracted at the end of the decade were the same individuals. With the exception of 1 female, who was a recent immigrant with only 3.3 ape-years of residence, and of the 5 offspring

born to 3 migrant females who arrived early and then departed

again late in the decade, the final “mass emigration” of 1973 consisted of those who appeared before 1965. It seems noteworthy that staff members of the research center called these 8 individuals

“southern

they were frequently kilometers

chimpanzees”

seen leaving

toward,

to the south of the Kakombe

never seemed to achieve

until the early 197Os, on the grounds

that

arriving from, or travelling in an area some station. Indeed, several of these individuals

full social membership

in the resident community

and often

traveled as a cluster in the main study zone. Coupled

with these qualitative

of the migrants observed another social community. “southern

chimpanzees’

impressions,

If this is corroborated

do not eventually

unit but simply blend into a neighboring

community,

data suggest that many may have belonged

by further field research-i.e.

wherein kinship, consort, coalition

social

intensity of the social bonds that deter-

will be amply demonstrated.

we can at best cautiously

to

if the

establish themselves as an independent

one-the

mine cohesiveness in a natural community now stands, however,

the demographic

at the start and the end of the decade

speculate

As the evidence

that the social nexus within a

and other special relationships

are known

to comprise a matrix of strong bonds, is sufficiently long-lasting and exclusive to withstand years of regular interaction with members of another community. Until 1968 no ‘rcommunity” Ieve 1 o f organization was recognized by observers of the Gombe

chimpanzees.

sistent (Lawick-Goodall,

Only the matrifocal unit had been recognized 1968).

as prevalent and per-

Then, largely due to the flurry of reports produced

the topic of larger social units by Japanese

primatologists

working

on

at other study sites,

Gombe investigators gradually began to view the study po@&on as a social community (Lawick-Goodall, 1973, 1975). Wh en this unit segmented into two unequal portions, while provisioning

was being reduced

fissioning of one Lrcommunity”

during the 197Os, the event was interpreted

into two (Bygott, 1974, in press;

nascent smaller

unit has now been accorded

label “Kahama

community”,

area to the nearby Kahama The ethological

evidence

seem strong, especially

full community

Wrangham,

status, identified

having apparently moved south from the Kakombe ValIey area (Lawick-Goodall, 19753). favoring

this interpretation

when investigators

are looking

of a fissioned community outward,

as the

1975).

This

by the Valley may

so to speak, from the

observation base of the study population; yet the demographic picture presented here suggests that the fissioning interpretation may have been premature. The segmentation of the study population was, in other words, perhaps no more than the departure of a transient group whose members originally and also finally belonged to a neighboring community. Before complete acceptance is given either interpretation, however, the

582

G. TELEKI

available subgroup

ET

evidence should be carefully should be obtained.

In order

to obtain

a more

AL.

reviewed

reasonable

and additional

approximation

data on the “fissioned”

of the normal

scale of inter-

community migration at Gombe, we will now focus on a much smaller sample of transfers into and out of the study population. The equivocal data gathered at the start and the end of the study period will be ignored, and attention given to the 10 remaining cases of permanent migration (possibly 14 cases, if 4 additional dissappearances are counted as transfers) that occurred between mid-1965 and mid-1972. Based on the conservative sample

of 10 cases, the net migration

with an annual

immigration

The discrepancy but, although

(Izawa,

Nishida,

1974;

incomplete

may be due to the attraction

inter-community

societies

rate is calculated

1970;

migration

Sugiyama,

Nishida,

at +0*3

rate of 0.9 and an annual emigration of banana

provisioning

1973;

Nishida

in Kakombe

Valley;

of wild chimpanzee

& Kawanaka,

in press; Pusey, in press), the available

per year

rate of 06 individuals.

appears to be characteristic

1972;

Kawanaka

&

data on rates are probably

and inconclusive.

Yet there are some basic themes that begin to emerge. tion and at least 4 cases-possibly There

chimpanzees

8 cases-of

At Gombe,

emigration

6 cases of immigra-

were logged in seven years.

were no male migrants observed.

scents, 2 adults) 3 adults),

Moreover, of the 6 female immigrants (4 adoleand the possible 8 female emigrants (2 infants, 1 juvenile, 2 adolescents,

7 individuals

were kin-related.

These

data lead to two tentative

generaliza-

tions ; one, that permanent migration among chimpanzee communities is, in contrast to what has been reported for other non-human primate societies (e.g. Rudran, 1973; Packer,

1975),

some migrant

a predominantly chimpanzees

or

even

merely accompany older kin. (Young males become migrants that follow matrifocal units.) have been noted

exclusively

in press).

were initially

and female There

introduced

migrants

were commonly

lone young

are at least 2 cases at Gombe

the tendency bability

When

for avoidance

of permanent

No comparative

(Nishida

two, that offspring who

adults

(11-14

& Kawanaka,

where unnamed

by local males to the study population

several such temporary appearances, estrous cycling probably contributes into new communities.

activity;

could, in the latter situation, sometimes At the Mahali site no adult male transfer

in age) who were often in estrus at the time of transfer Nishida,

female

(36 ‘A of 14 cases) are likely to be dependent

years 1972;

young females

while in estrus, and, after

eventually became permanent immigrants. Hence, to the probability that females transfer permanently

males of different

or conflict

intercommunity

data on frequencies

communities

seems to be higher, transfer

meet, on the other hand, thereby

lowering

the pro-

by males.

of temporary

uersas permanent

migration

have

been reported from any chimpanzee study site. Yet it seems likely that genetic exchange among neighboring communities would occur in both contexts. Indeed, gene flow may be higher in the case of temporary migration because male chimpanzees, who on occasion travel far into the home range of another community at Gombe (Lawick-Goodall, 1968, 1975b), are known to sometimes develop brief consort relationships with sexually receptive females during such sojourns. Paucity of observations on transferring, and on the temporary verms permanent aspects of migration, unfortunately prohibit further speculation about the biological phenomena involved in inter-community migration by wild chimpanzees. detailed theoretical

The biological mechanisms of intercommunity migration are in more need of field investigation if a chimpanzee model of social organization is to achieve significance

for ethnologists

and paleoanthropologists.

583

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE DEMOGUPHY

7. Ecological Regulation of Chimpanzee Populations Seasonal

variation

panzees

in the availability

is one of many factors

(Wrangham,

1975;

Teleki,

of floral and fauna1 resources

affecting

1976).

their ranging

Fluctuations

habits

utilized

in Gombe

in nutritional

by chim-

National

Park

factors are in turn likely

to affect stamina

and health. Individual body weights, for example, can vary as much as 10 ‘A during the annual cycle of seasons. Moreover, seasonal differences in the techniques of food collection and predation, which entail flexible grouping strategies that are presumably

geared

expenditure, resources

exploitation

are being heavily

probably

provides

exploited,

optimum

while minimizing

animal proteins McGrew,

(Suzuki, 1974),

1971~;

regular

1973; Teleki,

to some extent regulate

aggregation

for the transmission

in the consumption

Nishida,

energy Some

population.

at specific sites within the home range;

the resultant

conditions

It is even possible that seasonal variations 1974;

of key resources

are dispersed while others are clustered

when the latter panzees

to maximize

may in turn affect disease patterns within a chimpanzee

of specific

1973a, 1974;

susceptibility

of chim-

of fatal pathogens. foods, such as,

Hladik

& Viroben

to diseases.

Although nutritional and epidemiological regulators are not yet well documented, we can discuss meteorological regulators with somewhat greater precision. A representative S-year

sample

(1968-1970)

of precipitation

cycle of seasons at the Kakombe Gombe

climate

Wrangham,

Valley

have been reported

and temperature

station,

elsewhere

data, showing the annual appear in Figure 6. Many aspects of the

(Lawick-Goodall,

in the park:

4 months of dry conditions

tions (October-May). by 4 months

of long rains.

The monthly

(June-September)

The wet season includes

with less than a month

Seasonal

difference

distributions

tion during 1963-1973

transitions

about

Comparison

throughout

to the negative fetal wastage

However,

encompassed

season, there is some possibility wastage.

distributed

stable for all seasons, although

7 does not include

(July-December)

If live births

often

in the Gombe

study populaof months has

seasonal trends. The samples 1 because the specific dates of

are unknown.

rainy months may contribute in the field.

intervals,

7, in which the normal chronology

of these raw data suggests that deaths

is relatively

of wet condi-

of short rains followed

occur at fairly constant

of deaths and births recorded

while births are more randomly

Figure

1973a;

from year to year.

appear in Figure

3 cases in each category

observed

Teleki,

and 8 months 4 months

been rearranged (as in the previous figure) to emphasize of deaths and births used here differ from totals in Table

gradient

1968;

1975), so it should suffice to note here that two distinct seasons occur annually

are associated with precipitation the year. The mean temperature the slight temperature

because

most such events were not directly

since all 6 fetal deaths

occurred

in a 6-month

by the dry season and the short-rain that climatic

and fetal

deaths

drop in the

effect of high precipitation.

or other ecological

are added together,

portion

period

of the wet

factors contribute

to fetal

then 21 pregnancy

term-

inations were logged between July and December while only 12 pregancy terminations occurred between January and June. As the 21 pregnancies which terminated between July and December may signify a conception the initial months of the wet season, a relationship tion may lie hidden

behind

the relatively

peak some 8 months between conception

even distribution

earlier, during and precipita-

of live births.

Incomplete

584

G. TELEKI

ET AL.

Figure 6. Meterological data (1968-1970 averages) in Kakombe Valley, Gombe National Park. Note that the maximum monthly precipitation occurs during the short-rain period of the wet season, and that the minimum daily temperatures overlap that same period.

“C 35

30

2.5

20

as they are, these data already point toward new questions. Does copulation frequency exhibit seasonal fluctuations ? Do seasonal variations in grouping tendencies, determined perhaps by cyclic availability of food resources (e.g. fruiting trees, termite migrations), affect sexual behavior and, consequently, conception rates? When additional data become available, more refined questions about ecological regulators will undoubtedly emerge. Births are distributed in a relatively even pattern throughout the year. Deaths, on the other hand, seem to be closely associated with precipitation. Fully 87 % of 30 postnatal deaths occurred during the 8-month span of the wet season. More importantly, 57 % of the postnatal deaths occurred during the 4 months of short rains marking the onset of the main wet season. If the mean monthly rainfall is taken as the independent variable, the Pearson product moment correlation between precipitation and mortality is significantly positive (r = $0.74, j =
GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

Figure 7. Monthly and seasonal distribution of general mortality (top), sex-specifii mortality (middle) and live births (bottom) in the Gombe study population during 1963-1973.

585

DEMOGRAPHY

LongRains ..)....

Dry

Plus6 Prenatal

Deaths

A

S

0

N

D

J

F

M

A

M

June

July

A

S

0

N

D

J

F

M

A

M

June

July

A

S

0

N

0

J

F

M

A

M

June

July

N=30

dependent offspring additionally risk contracting diseases from mothers and/or siblings with whom they have regular contact. When monthly mortality is graphed separately for each sex, a slight tendency toward a male peak between October and January versusa female peak between December and March can be noted. It is possible that males are slightly more prone to die during the short rains while females are slightly more prone to die during the early portion of the long rains. Additional data are needed to determine whether this sex-specific difference is significant. 8. Methodological

Contamination

of

the

Gombe

data

As illustrated in Figure 1, the major fluctuations observed in the 1963-1973 census dataand presumably also in the estimated membership of the Kakombe community-can be chronologically matched to stages in the history of banana provisioning (Teleki, 1973a; Wrangham, 1974). When the demographic components underlying these trends are closely examined, the timing of peaks and slumps in mortality, natality and migratory patterns seem to coincide with feeding schedules (compare Figures 1, 3 and 4). Considered independently, annual variations in each demographic variable could have been

586

G. TELEKI

ET AL.

caused by natural factors-i.e. regulators not related to human activities. It is possible, for example, that the mortality rate rose sharply during 1966-1969 due to an exceptionally heavy influx of contagious diseases which spread from neighboring chimpanzee populations into the Gombe area: or that the drop in natality during the same period was merely part of a normal series of sigmoid oscillations that is perhaps common in wild chimpanzee populations. There is, in sum, no concrete evidence proving that banana provisioning was the direct cause of the major changes observed in the Gombe study populations. When all historical information is collated (Table 9), however, it seems plausible that banana feeding-together with its byproducts of heightened tension, frequent competition, and spatial compression of chimpanzees at the main Kakombe station-either produced or exacerbated several trends in the 1963-1973 census records. The extent to which provisioning affected various trends is difficult to determine in retrospect. Of the various consequences mentioned by Wrangham (1974), aggressive competition and contraction of diseases are most pertinent here. Given that correlation and causation are not always identical, it still seems noteworthy that the run of fetal deaths observed between 1966 and 1970 coincide with the most intense period of provisioning at which time inter- and intra-specific aggression were also high. More aggression around and against pregnant females may well have accelerated the prenatal mortality rate. Bananas are not normally available to the Gombe chimpanzees (except by possible raiding of nearby plantations), but this novel fruit became a major food resource for many regular visitors to the main research station. When massive amounts of bananas which contain only l-2-1.5 g protein per 100 g edible matter (Leung, 1968), are consistently fed to apes that are accustomed to an eclectric omnivorous diet (Teleki, 1974, 19753), negative effects upon natality and mortality, perhaps via lowered resistance to disease, might be expected. For the years 1964-1967, the annual records of poliomyelitis in the human population of Kigoma Region included 18, 143, 31 and 7 cases (D. E. Parmas, Regional Medical Officer, pers. comm. ). In August and September of 1966 there were 8 known cases within 15 km of Gombe National Park. As the epidemic of paralytic disease among the Gombe chimpanzees was recognized in September of the same year, it seems likely that human vectors were involved in the transmission of polio to the chimpanzees. If provisioning affected the Gombe study population along such lines, the magnitude of their demographic responses can be measured by calculating population growth during 1966-1972, a time span when the “pseudo-migration” phenomenon least influenced population stability. Applied to this census period, the growth formula* yields a net 10~sof 17.7 chimpanzees in 7 years, or a per annum loss of 2.5 individuals, from an initial total of 66 named chimpanzees. Given that immigration from regions outside the park boundaries is probably minimal these days, it is evident that the study population could not for long sustain such losses without draining neighboring communities. If the net migration rate of +0.5 chimpanzees per year recorded in 1963-1973 included some individuals who were actually residents of the Kakombe Valley area, and were erroneously “added” as they became habituated to human observers, and the +0*3 net annual migration rate recorded in 1966-1972 is a more realistic measure of transfer, the difference * (P* - P,)ZCIp, = populationgrowth between the first census (PI) and the secondcensus(Pa), with a constant(K) of 100.

GOMBE

Table

CHIMPANZEE

Chronology of provisioning and Gombe Stream Research Centre

9

587

DEMOGRAPHY

demographic

Demographic

Provisioning events

events

at

the

events

Stage

Period

A

August 1962March 1965

Introductory period when many Study population rapidly increases in new chimpanzees are habituated size, but trend is deceptive because most additions are not births but adult via irregular provisioning. “immigrants.” Numerous “southern chimpanzees”attracted to feeding area.

B

March 1965August 1967

Experimental and developmental stage when several distribution techniques are tried, with several increases in the frequency and the volume of banana provisioning.

Study population peaks in 1966, then proceeds to drop rapidly. Mortality rate rises sharply in 1966, due largely to epidemic of paralytic disease. Natality rate drops sharply in 1966 and 1967. Run of fetal deaths begins in 1966.

C

August 1967June 1968

Scheduling and volume of provisioning is repeatedly changed in attempt to cope with increasing difficulty of observing large aggregations of chimpanzees, and also with increasing intraand inter-specific competition for bananas.

Study population continues to shrink. Mortality rate rises sharply in 1968, due partly to an epidemic of respiratory illness. Natality rate bottoms out in 1967 and 1968, with nutritional (e.g. excess bananas) and/or behavioral (e.g. aggressive interaction) factors involved. Run of fetal deaths peaks in 1967.

D

July 196% June 1969

Major compensatory actions taken to reduce stressful situation at feeding station, where 80+ primates (baboons and chimpanzees) gather. often Plans made for new feeding systern.

Study population bottoms out in 1969, following a second epidemic of respiratory disease. Mortality rate peaks in 1968, then drops swiftly in 1969 when feeding is reduced. Natality rate still low, but begins to recover in 1969. Run of fetal deaths continues.

E

June 1969July 1973

Provisioning system completely redesigned to supply few bananas at irregular intervals to lone visitors or small groups.

Study population recovers gradually, due this time mainly to births rather than adult additions. Mortality rate bottoms out in 1971 and 1972, and no epidemics occur. Run of fetal deaths stops in 1970. Mass %migration” of “southern chimpanzees” occurs late in 1972.

between

these rates could be an approximate

or more communities

living near the Kakombe

measure of the drainage

experienced

by one

Community.

The Gombe research setting serves to illustrate how crucial a role can be played by procedures and methodologies applied to long-term study of wild primates. Even though habituation through provisioning may initially seem advantageous and harmless, it can lead to unexpected

dangers for the population

under study.

deemed necessary, we recommend that investigators in order to strike a balance between acceptable

If intensive provisioning is regularly monitor demographic trends and excessive disruption. Moreover,

having applied extensive provisioning, Gombe researchers must now evaluate all results and interpretations in terms of the effects wrought by this technique on a plethora of ethological and ecological factors (e.g., Teleki, 1973a). It remains likely, even then, that some authors will persist to ignore or reject Gombe observations (e.g. Reynolds, 19’75).

588

o.

TELEKI

ET

AL.

9. Tracing matology Modern

anthropologists

Paleodemographic

Trends

favor integration

of demographic

who in principle

Via Pri-

research

primatology, ethnography and paleoanthropology nonetheless acknowledge shortage of suitably complete and comparable data in various areas of research the review by Baker and human partly

primate

because

& Sanders,

1972).

populations

The gap between

studies of extant non-human

seems to have grown particularly

field primatologists

have not provided

That

and apes, should to some extent the Gombe

wide in recent

years,

and accurate

longi-

the detailed

tudinal data needed for drawing comparisons with demographic tions. The Gombe research program, together with other monkeys

studies of human populalong-term

close this gap in the coming

data base is in many

respects

in

a severe (see, e.g.,

severely

limited

field studies of years.

in scope,

possessing

neither the temporal does not completely

nor quantitative depth expected by students of human demography, impair their value. The data we have already presented amply deat one level, the necessity and feasibility of conducting demographic research

monstrate,

on wild non-human speculation-in relevant such

primates.

We now venture

order to explore the potential

to studies of modern

primatological

research.

models of chimpanzee

and early hominid

The

population

for assessing the plausibility If the broader

human

aim,

then,

structure

implications

step-to

populations

is to determine

and dynamics

and accuracy

theoretical

to take another

a level of open

utility of these data, to see whether

insights

can be gleaned from

whether

demographic

may provide a new framework

of some evolutionary

reconstructions.

of the Gombe results are to be fruitfully exploited

however, it is essential to stress (a) that the information base is small and is therefore prone to erroneous interpretation, and (b) the representativeness of the data must be evaluated.

Neither

completion

of additional

caveat

considered,

for some demographic

site listings in Baldwin tion has been met. The chimpanzee

can at present field research

& Teleki,

gests that the studypopulation

therefore

conclude

chimpanzee

that

population

albeit with caution,

data

in Table

in several basic parameters. and sex distributions-have

data obtained 19?3),

demographic

results are summarized

be satisfactorily in primate

10.

at other

but cannot

doesnotdiffer

The first requires The

second

can

be

sites have been reported

(see

be fully resolved until the first condi-

now available Despite

eliminated.

demography.

for comparison

obvious

radically

limitations,

with the Gombe

this information

sug-

from other chimpanzeepopulations

More detailed comparisons of two specific parameters-age lent further support to this assumption (see Table 2). We

the Gombe structure

to explore

data

are in many

and dynamics, the relevance

respects

representative

and that the Gombe

of non-human

primate

of wild

results can be used, demography

to re-

search in hominid paleodemography. Several authors have recently presented demographic analyses on extinct hominid and extant human populations, and some have also reviewed the interpretive problems encountered McKinley,

in paleodemographic 1971;

Mann,

studies

1975 ; Dumond,

framework for the Gombe results. As shown in Table 11, chimpanzee

(Acsadi 1975).

& NemeskCri, These

life expectancy,

sistently lower than similar parameters in early hominid Qnly the Choukoutien survivorship (O-320) is slightly

1970;

Weiss,

1973;

reports serve as a comparative

survivorship,

and fertility

and modern human lower than Gombe

are con-

populations. survivorship

Z-group

K-group

Band A

Kasakati

Mahali

Filabanga 43

28*

50

80

48*

49

85

< 1.0

1.3-2.6

1.6-2.4

2.4-4.7

29-4.8t

c. 5.0

5.0-7.0

1.5

1.1

0.8

I .4

1.2

1.5-1.8

1.8 ?

0.5

0.5

0.8

06

0.9

0.5

0.6

0.6

0.8

0.9

0.6-0.7

0.7-0.8

13

67”

20

50*

120

9*

23*

Continuous or intermittent* study period (months)

chimpanzees per kma.

0.5-0.8

at Gombe is 3.6-4.6

Mosaic grassland/ woodland forest

Mosaic grassland/ woodland Mosaic grassland/ woodland/forest

Mosaic grassland/ woodland/forest Mosaic grassland/ woodland/forest

Forest edge/plantation

Rain forest (with selective logging)

t Estimated density range for entire park population

Study population M-group

Gombe

Mahali

-

Beni

group

Socionomic sex ratio

Nishida & Kawanaka (1972), Nishida (1968, in press) Izawa & Itani (1966), Izawa (1970) Nishida & Kawanaka ( 1972)) Nishida (1968, in press), Sugiyama (1973) Itani & Suzuki (1967), Suzuki (1969), Kano (1971)

Reynolds & Reynolds (1965), Sugiyama (1968, 1973), Suzuki (1971) Kortlandt (nd., pers. comm.) Kuijsten (1972) This paper

Source

Picnic Site

age ratio

type

Total male/ female sex ratio

Budongo

Adult/ subadult

General habitat

Names of observed units

Sites Known* or Estimated estimated crude size of density units per kma

Comparative summary of basic demographic data obtained from field studies of chimpanzees in eastern Zaire (Beni site), western Uganda (Budongo Forest), and western Tanzania (Gombe N. P., Kasakati Basin, Mahali Mtns)

Table 10

590 Table

G. TELEKI

11

ET AL.

Comparison of selected demographic traits in Gombe panzees, extinct hominids, and extant human societies Location or organizational level

Populations

Pan troglodytes* Australopithecus Homo erectus

Expectation SurvivorBirth Age-specific of life at ship at interval fertility age x years age x years in years rate for female offspring

Gombe National Park South Africa Choukoutien Maghreb-type; Taforalt, Afalou Gatherer-hunters Proto-agriculturalists

Homo sapiens Homo sapiens* Homo sapiens*

cbii-

k,

e1.5

115

11.9 21.4

12.9 12.7 26.9

0.364 0.650 0.3200.463

5.6 5.8 4.0 3.5

0.128 (B,,) -

16.5 19.0

20.0 19.8

0.500 0.500

3.5 2.5

0.213 (&) 0.174 (&,)

* Extant populations.

(0*364),

and this difference

the column

showing

panzees exhibiting ingly anomalous

seems well within

estimated

birth

an interval

sample

has an opposite

error.

In contrast,

with living chimThe seemtwice as long as that of living humans.

about

australopithecine

an expected

intervals

survivorship

trend,

(O-650) will be discussed below.

These demographic measures have been selected for comparison largely because relevant data are available for a temporally and phylogenetically broad range of populations.

Consequently,

as more complete to accumulate,

there remains

demographic comparisons

of data listed in Table

of other

populations

wise be obtained

line diverged,

may extend knowledge

by paleodemographers.

are no more than rough estimates, hominid human The

and modern primate

comparison nonetheless

human

populations

survivorship

is in reasonable

measures

1 I is not that chimpanzees

stock from which the hominid primate

considerable

is of course limited

adults at an age of about

feasible.

The

are supposed to represent

but rather

significance a population

that profiles of living non-human

of demographic processes that cannot otherEven though some figures shown in the table

well.

in Figure

accord with survivorship

likely to survive beyond

will become

But

continue

the Gombe data are remarkably well aligned with early It remains to be seen whether other non-

fit equally

that

interpretation.

ape and human populations

populations.

curves depicted

interesting

room for erroneous

profiles of monkey,

8 also show that the chimpanzee

in the same set of hominid

by the small sample the

13 years,

life cycle

sizes underlying

of Gombe

profile Precise

these curves.

chimpanzees-who

enter old age by about

40 years-does

populations.

It is

are prime

27 years of age, and are not

not seem to differ greatly

from the life cycle of

australopithecines. It is thus tempting to assume that other demographic features shown by Gombe chimpanzees (e.g. mortality and natality patterns) are also in close accord with similar features once exhibited by australopithecine populations. Because the australopithecine sample has a shortage of fossil material in the lower age brackets and includes remains from a large time span rather than a single resident population, it is extremely difficult to conduct paleodemographic reconstructions (Mann, 1975). Such difficulties may account for the anomalies apparent in Table 11 where the australopithecine

sample

seems to have an exceptionally

high survivorship

rate while

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

DEMOGRAPHY

591

Figure 8. Survivorship curves for: A, Australopithecusspp., based on a sampleof 120-155individuals mainlyfrom SouthAfricansites(McKinley, 1971; Mann, 1975); B, Homo erectus, based on a sample of 22 individuals from the Choukoutien site (Acsadi Nemesktri, 1970) ; C, Homo sapiens of the Maghreb-type, based on a sample of about 200 individuals from the Taforalt and Afalou sites (Acsadi & Nemeskeri, 1970); D, Pan trogZodytes,based on a sample of 33 individuals from Gombe National Park (this paper).

AGE

IN YEARS

being similar to Gombe chimpanzees in other respects. The Gombe data may here serve to supplement and refine some of the paleodemographic results obtained by McKinley (1971) and Mann (1975). In both Table 11 and Figure 8 certain australopithecine traits, such as the survivorship probability at age 15 years (0.650) and the entire subadult portion of the survivorship curve (A), seem to be out of phase with both the pongid and hominid data. Only when the raw data are statistically smoothed (Weiss, 1973) does the mean lifetime remaining to australopithecines at 15 years of age (eis = 12.7 years) approach the mean lifetime remaining to chimpanzees at the same age (eis = 12.9 years). Accordingly, it is possible that the mean ages at death proposed for australopithecines (18.0-22.9 years by McKinley and 17-2-22-2 years by Mann, the initial figure for the robust form and the latter for the gracile form) were closer to the mean age at death now exhibited by Gombe chimpanzees (13.5 years). The Gombe data may supplement paleodemographic results in other ways as well. The average interval between births at Gombe is 5.6 years, with a total lifetime productivity of 4.7 or fewer offspring. If the 5.8 year spacing proposed by McKinley (1971: 424) is accurate, then the lifetime fertility of australopithecines may also have been within a range or 4.0-5.0 live births per female. These are of course quite tentative estimates of early hominid survivorship and reproduction. They are, however, based on twu sources of data: living nonhuman primates and fossil hominid remains. If a more comprehensive model of chimpanzee population

G. TELEKI

592

ET AL.

structure and dynamics can eventually be generated from a larger sample of field studies, and if that model would prove to be as compatible with evolutionary trends as the current data seem to be, primatology would add a new dimension to demographic research in ethnology and paleoanthropology. Incomplete as it is now, the model already suggests that a gradual increase of life expectancy and survivorship, along with a gradual decrease in birth spacing, may have jointly contributed to a global expansion of hominid populations since their divergence from common primate stock some millions of years ago. If such evolutionary processes can in time be brought into sharper focus through comparative demographic studies on living monkeys, apes and humans, field primatology will have served to expand our theoretical horizons. A more pragmatic point has perhaps been better demonstrated by this report: namely, that much of the demographic data collected by field primatologists can be arranged, analyzed and presented in accordance with standard procedures used in wildlife biology and human demography. This, in our opinion, is an essential step in producing results that will have comparative value in research focusing on intertaxonomic and interdisciplinary problems. 10. Summary Vital demographic

data on Gombe chimpanzees

The following summary of key numerical data pertains, in the strictest sense, only to the Gombe study population. We believe, however, that this population can provisionally be considered as representative of an average chimpanzee social unit, or community, because its structural components fall within the natural range of size and composition exhibited by unprovisioned populations studied at other sites. 1. Population size and composition (10 year sample) Total size :

mean = 48-l range = 23 = 1.21 = 0.95 = 0.86

Adult/subadult ratio Male/female ratio Turnover rate 2. Mortality and natal@ patterns (10 year sample) Crude death rate Infant death rate Crude birth rate Male/female ratio at birth Male/female ratio for infant deaths 3. Migratory patterns (7 year sample) Annual immigration rate :

Annual emigration rate :

= o-07 = 0.23 = O-06 = O-67 = 0.37

mean standard deviation range mean standard deviation range

= = = = = =

0.86 0.90 2 0.57 1.28 3

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

Net annual migration

= o-29

rate

Adult-male/adult-female Adultlsubadult

593

DEMOGRAPHY

= 0.00

migrant ratio

= 0.43

migrant ratio

4. Reproductivepatterm (10 year sample) Total pregnancies expected in lifetime of an average female Total live births expected Total fetal deaths expected Annual probability Average

= 4.7

birth interval

dies:

mean =

deviation=

1.70 0.60

(in years) when previous offspring lives:

mean= 560 standard deviation= 1.50 = 0.65

rate (female offspring)

Gross reproductive

I.2

= 0.19

of live births per female

birth interval (in years) when previous offspring

Net reproductive

=

in lifetime of an average female

standard Average

= 5.9

in lifetime of an average female

rate (female offspring)

= 2.90

5. Lijk history data (10 year sample) Male life expectancy

(in years) :

Female life expectancy Combined

at birth=

(in years) :

life expectancy

(in years) :

Age (in years) at death: Maximum

15.9

at birth=

10.9

at age 8=

16.2

at birth=

11.9

at age 8=

16.6

mean=

13.5

standard deviation=

12.5

age (in years) at death:

Generation

14*3

at age 8=

span (in years) :

males=

33

*

females=

37

*

mean=

19.6”

standard deviation = 2.12 range= Age (in years) of females at birth of first infant

mean= standard deviation=

Note:

asterisks(*)

mark figures that are probably

of many adult chimpanzees

are only approximately

conservative

8 11*8* 0.34

range= 1 estimates, as the ages

known.

Some hypotheses and conclusions As demographic

investigation

stage of development,

of non-human

the following

primate

populations

selection of comments

is at such a nascent

is offered mainly as a stimulant

to further research. (a) The membership

of a chimpanzee

population

unit may vary from year to year,

or even month to month, but age and sex ratios are likely to hold stable on a long term basis. Based on a sample of 3 discrete populations, stability is estimated to occur when there are about 75 males to 100 females and 90 immature members to 100 mature members. (b) Female births seem to occur somewhat more frequently than male births, but high infant mortality 6

among

females can compensate

to the extent that the sex

594

G. TELEKI

ET AL.

ratio in a given population

unit remains

population

within

unit

dispersed

at or near parity.

a region

can vary

The composition

considerably,

but

of a

a slight

preponderance of females can be expected in an average population sanple. The sex-specific factors involved in differential mortality patterns have not been identified. (c)

Migratorypatternsareprobablyinfluenced on a temporary

byage, sexandkinship.

Adultmalesmay,

basis, migrate as or even more often than adult females,

are less likely to permanently

transfer into other communities.

but males

Subadult

members

of matrifocal units seem to account for a high proportion of the more persistent form of inter-community migration. The dual roles played by “temporary” versus “persistent” migration in genetic exchange among communities require further

study.

(d) Mortality patterns.

patterns Illness

are more and

susceptible

death

season, when precipitation year,

although

dation where. (f)

The

upon

being strongly

survivorship

curve

as in many shared (g) When

human

account

influenced

of the wet and con-

factors.

negligible,

approaches

a markedly

convex

that most mortality This

of deaths per

The effect of pre-

at Gombe

shape

and else-

after the initial

4

occurs toward the end of the life span,

may be one of several

demographic

patterns

by the higher primates. multiple

likely to produce

factors more

are considered, than

3-4

commercial

exploitation

does not exhibit

average

chimpanzee

that will achieve

female

maturity.

to survive prime adulthood.

of wild populations

(h) With a low reproductive adoption of some orphans), and long birth intervals

the

offspring

no more than 2 offspring can be expected

tion

for a high percentage

by climatic

is probably

groups.

are natality

reach daily minima,

mortality

years of life, and indicates

than months

Live births occur throughout the that conception is seasonally influenced.

there is some possibility

chimpanzee

vagaries

the early

the wet season.

Diseases and to some extent accidents year, the former

during

is high and temperatures

tinue at a high rate throughout (e)

to seasonal

are common

is not Perhaps

Accordingly,

must be strictly regulated.

rate (notwithstanding rate, a high infant mortality a slow maturation rate, a limited fertility span in females

between potential

surviving

offspring,

for rapid

population

a wild

chimpanzee

growth.

A long

popularecovery

period can be expected after a population has been struck by calamitous events. Management and conservation programs should therefore focus on isolating populations (i)

from such events,

especially

and on protecting the lives of subadult If the age and sex ratios characteristic

contagious

diseases carried

and adult females. of wild chimpanzee

populations

by humans, maximize

reproductive potential, then similar ratios may be advantageous in captive breeding programs as well. Emulation of natural community size may also be advantageous. Other wild chimpanzee demographic traits should be examined for the purpose

(j)

of designing biologically efficient breeding programs, and demographic, as well as ethological and ecological, factors should be incorporated into such programs. As some demographic traits exhibited by wild chimpanzees seem to be in reasonable accord with human demographic patterns, especially at the early hominid level, the detailed population models that can be constructed by field primatologists may aid in reconstructing hominoid evolutionary processes.

GOMBE CHIMPANZEE

We are indebted

to the Gombe

and the Republic

of Tanzania

during

two years

Stream Research for providing

of field research

Centre,

financial

in Gombe

595

DEMOGRAPHY

the National

and logistical

National

Park;

Geographic

Society

support to G. Teleki

and to Jane

Goodall,

Director of GSRC, for granting unlimited access to the center’s data files during a visit to the station by J. H. Pfifferling. We are also grateful to the many field investigators who contributed

to those records

over more than a decade

of research

on chimpanzees;

and in particular vital

to David Bygott, Richard Wrangham and Patrick McGinnis for providing The advice and comments of Alan Mann, John supplementary information.

Pfeiffer,

Toshisada

Nishida,

Adriaan

Kortlandt,

and Paul Leslie, who read preliminary

Special thanks are due Lori Baldwin, drafts of the typescript, are also greatly appreciated. whose patient encouragement and assistance were invaluable. References

Gombe bibliographic

entries are marked with asterisks.

Acsadi, Gy. & Nemeakeri, J. (1970). History of Human La3 Span and Mortality. Budapest: Akademiai Kiad6 Aldrich-Blake, F. I’. G. (1970). Problems of social structure in forest monkeys. In (J. H. Crook, ed.) Social Behaviour of Birds and Mammals. London: Academic Press. Baker, P. T. & Sanders, W. T. (1972). Demographic studies in anthropology. Annual Reviews in Anthropo1opy 1, 151-178. Baldwin, L. A., Kavanagh, M. & Teleki, G. (1975). Field research on langur and proboscis monkeys: an historical, geographical, and bibliographical listing. Primates 16, 35 l-363. Baldwin, L. A. Patteron, T. L. & Teleki, G. (in press). Field research on callitrichid and cebid monkeys: an historical, geographical, and bibliographical listing. Primates 18. Baldwin, L. A. & Teleki, G. (1972). Field research on baboons, drills, and geladas: an historical, geographical, and bibliographical listing. Primates 13, 427-432. Baldwin, L. A. & Teleki, G. (1973). Field research on chimpanzees and gorillas: an historical, geographical, and bibliographical listing. Primates 14, 315-330. Baldwin, L. A. & Teleki, G. (1974). Field research on gibbons, siamangs, and orang-utans: an historical, geographical, and bibliographical listing. Primates 15, 365-376. Baldwin, L. A. & Teleki, G. (in press). Field research on tree shrews and prosimians: an historical, geographicai, and bibliographical listing. Primates 18. Baldwin, L. A., Teleki, G. & Kavanagh, M. (1976). Field research on colobus, guenon, mangabey, and patas monkeys: an historical, geographical, and bibliographical listing. Primates 17, 233-251. Berger, M. E. (1972). Population structure of olive baboons (Papio anubis (J. P. Fischer)) in the Laikipia District of Kenya, East African Wild&+ Journal 10, 159-164. Bermant, G. & Lindburg, D. S. eds. (1975). Primate Utilization and Conservation. New York: Wiley Interscience. Bliss, C. I. (1953). Fitting the negative binomial distribution to biological data. Biometrics 9, 176-200. Bourne, G. H. (1972). Breeding chimpanzees and other apes. In (W. I. B. Beveridge, ed.), Breeding Primates. Basel: S. Karger. Burton, F. D. & Sawchuk, L. A. (1974). Demography of Macaca sylvanus of Gibraltar. Primates 15, 271-278. Butler, T. M. (1972). Reproductive history of the Holloman chimpanzee colony. Journal of Medical Primatolopy 1, 51-57. “Bygott, D. (1972). Cannibalism among wild chimpanzees. Nature 238,410~11. *Bygott, D. (1974). Agonistic behaviour and social relationships among adult male chimpanzees. unpublished Dissertation. University of Cambridge, England. “Bygott, D. (in press). Agnoistic behavior and dominance among wild chimpanzees. In (J. Goodall & D. A. Hamburg, eds) The Great Apes. (Perspectives in Human Evolution, vol. IV.) Menlo Park: W.A. Benjamin. Chivers, 6). J. (1974). The siamang in Malaya: a field study of a primate in a tropical rain forest. Contributions to Primatology 4, l-335. *Clark, C. B. (in press). A preliminary report on weaning among chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In (F. G. Poirer & S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, eds) Primate Socialization, vol. 2. Chicago: Aldine. *Glutton-Brock, T. H. (1972). Feeding and ranging behaviour of the red colobus monkey. Unpublished Dissertation. University of Cambridge, England. Glutton-Brock, T. H. (1974). Primate social organisation and ecology. Nature 250, 539-542. Coale, A. J. (1974). The history of human population. In (S. H. Katz, ed.) Biological Anthropology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.

596

G. TELEKI

ET AL.

Cohen, J. E. (1975). The size and demographic composition of social groups of wild orang-utans. Anime[ Behaviour 23, 543-550. Cooper, R. B. (1972). Introduction to Queing Theory, New York: Macmillan. Deevey, E. S. ,Jr. (1970). Life tables for natural populations of animals. In (W. E. Hazen, ed.) Readings in Population and Community Ecology. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders. Drickamer, L. C. (1974). A ten-year summary of reproductive data for free-ranging Macaca mulatta.F&z primatologica 21, 61-80. Dumond, D. E. (1975). The limitations of human population: a natural history. Science 187, 713-721. Eisenberg, J. F., Muckenhirn, N. A. & Rudran, R. (1972). The relation between ecology and social structure in primates. Science 176,863-874. Fox, R. (1972). Alliance and constraint: sexual selection and the evolution of human kinship systems. In (B. Campbell, ed.) Sexual Selectionand the Descent of Man. Chicago: Aldine. Gartlan, J. S. (1973). Influences of phylogeny and ecology on variations in the group organization of primates. In (E. W. Menzel, ed.) Precultural Primate Behavior. Basel: S. Karger. Gartlan, J. S. (1975). The African coastal rain forest and its primates-threatened resources. In (G. Bermant & D. G. Lindburg, eds) Primate Utilization and Conservation. New York: Wiley Interscience. Harding, R. S. 0. (1973). Range utilization by a troop of olive baboons (Papio anubis). Unpublished Dissertation. University of California, Berkeley. Harrisson, B. (1971). Conservationof Nonhuman Primates in 1970. Basel: S. Karger. Hladik, C. M. & Viroben, G. (1974). L’alimentation proteique du chimpanzee dans son environment forestier naturel. C. R. Academic Science, Paris, Serie-D 279, 1475-1478. Hobbs, K. R. (1975). The feasibility of supplying relatively large numbers of primates for research. In (G. Bermant & D. G. Lindburg, eds) Primate Utilization and Conservation. New York: Wiley Interscience. Horr, D. A. (1975). The Borneo orang-utan: population structure and dynamics in relationship to ecology and reproductive strategy. Primate Behavior 4, 307-323. Horr, D. A. (in press). Orang-utan social structure: a computer simulation. In (G. E. Friedlander & J. Friedlander, eds) The Measures of Man. Cambridge: Peabody Museum Press. Itani, J. & Suzuki, A. (1967). The social unit of chimpanzees. Primates 8, 355-381. Izawa, K. (1970). Unit groups of chimpanzees and their nomad&m in the savanna woodland. Primates 11, l-45. Izawa, K. & Itani, J. (1966). Chimpanzees in Kasakati Basin, Tanganyika. Kyoto University African Studies 1, 73-156. Kano, T. (1971). The chimpanzees of Filabanga, western Tanzania. Primates 12, 229-246. Kano, T. (1972). Distribution and adaptation of the chimpanzee on the eastern shore of Lake Tanganyika. Kyoto University African Studies 7, 37-129. Kawanaka, K. & Nishida, T. (1975). Recent advances in the study of inter-unit-group relationships and social structure of wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains, In (S. Kondo, M. Kawai, A. Ehara & S. Kawamura, eds) Proceedings from the Symposiaof the Ffth Congress of the International Primatological Society, Tokyo: Japan Science Press. Kortlandt, A. (no date). Computation of the longevity and mortality from the age composition of chimpanzees in the wild. Unpublished manuscript. University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Kuijsten, A. C. (1972). Een model voor de leeftijdsopbouw van het wild levende chimpanses. Unpublished manuscript. Univeristy of Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Lancaster, J. B. (in press). Sex roles in primate societies and the evolution of the division of labor in man. In (M. S. Teitelbaum, ed) Social and Biological Bases of Sex Di&rences. *Lawick-Goodall, J. van (1965). Chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream Reserve. In (I. Devore, ed) Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. *Lawick-Goodall, J. van (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monograph 1, 161-311. *Lawick-Goodall, J. van (1971). In the Shadow of Man. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. *Lawick-Goodall, J. van (1975a). Cultural elements in a chimpanzee community. In (E. W. Mensel, ed.) Precultural Primate Behavior. Basel: S. Karger. *Lawick-Goodall, J. van (19756). The behaviour of the chimpanzee. In (G. Kurth & I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, eds) Hominisation and Behavior. Stuttgart: Gustav Fischer Verlag. *L.awick-Goodall, J. van (1975c). The chimpanzee. In (V. Goodall, ed.) The Questfor Man. New York: Praeger. “Lawick-Goodall, J. van & Hamburg, D. A. (1974). Gombe East, Gombe West. Stanford Magazine 2, 66-70. *Leung, W. T. W. (1968). Food composition table for use in Africa. Manual published by U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Bethesda, Md. *Lindquist, E. F. (1953). Design and Analysis of experiments in Psychology and Education. Boston: HoughtonMifflin. Mann, A. E. (1975). Paleodemographic aspects of the South African australopithecines. University of Pennsylvania Publications in Anthropology 1, 1-17 1.

GOMBE CHIMPANZEEDEMOGRAPHY

597

*McGrew, W. C. (1974). Tool use by wild chimpanzees in feeding- upon driver ants. .lournal of Human _ Euoluti~n3, 5011508.. McKinley, K. R. (1971). Survivorship in gracile and robust australopithecines: a demographic comparison and a proposed birth model. American Journal of Physical AnthropoloQ 34,417-426. Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mtns. Primates 9, 167-224. Nishida; T. (1973). The ant-gathering behavior by-the use of tools among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. .lburnal of Human Evolution 2, 357-370. Nishida, T. (in-press). Social structure of chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. In (J. Goodall & D. A. Hamburg, eds) The Great Apes. (Perspectives in Human Evolution, vol. IV.) Menlo Park: W. A. Benjamin. Nishida, T. & Kawanaka, K. (1972). Inter-unit-group relationships among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Kyoto University African Studies 7, 13 l-169. Odum, E. P. (1971). Fundamentals of_Ecalogy. Philadelphia: W. B. Sanders. *Packer. C. 11975). Male transfer in olive baboons. Nature 255.219-220. *Pusey, ‘A. (in press). Intercommunity transfer of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park. In (J. Goodall & D. A. Hamburg, eds) The Great Apes. (Perspectives in Human Evolution, vol. IV.) Menlo Park: W. A. Benjamin. Reynolds, V. (1975). How wild are the Gombe chimpanzees? Man 10,123-125. Reynolds, V. & Reynolds, F. (1965). Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest. In (I. DeVore, ed) Primate Behavior : Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. *Ransom, T. W. (1971). Ecology and social behavior of baboons (Papio an&s) at the Gombe National Park, Unpublished Dissertation. Berkeley: University of California. Riopelle, A. J. (1963). Growth and behavioral changes in the chimpanzee. Zeitschrift fur Morphologic und Anthropologic 53, 53-6 1. Rodman, P. S. (1973). Population composition and adaptive organization among orangutans of the Kutai Reserve. In (R. P. Michael & J. H. Crook, eds) Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates. London: Academic Press. *Roy, A. D. (1974). Rhinophycomycosis enteromorphae occurring in a chimpanzee in the wild in East Africa. American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 23, 935. *Roy, A. D. & Cameron, H. M. (1972). Rhinophycomycosis enteromorphae occurring in a chimpanzee in the wild in East Africa. AmericanJournal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 21, 234-237. Rudran, R. (1973). Adult male replacement in one-male troops of purple-faced langurs (Presbytis senex senex) and its effect on population structure. Folia primatologica 19,166-192. Sabater Pi, J. & Groves, C. (1972). The importance of the higher primates in the diet of the Fang of Rio Muni. Man 7, 239-243. Stott, D. H. (1969). Cultural and natural checks on population growth. In (A. P. Vayda, ed) Enviroment and CutturaE Behavior. New York: Natural History Press. Struhsaker, T. T. (1969). Correlates of ecology and social organization among African cercopithecines. Folia primatologica 11,80-l 18. Sugiyama, Y. (1968). Social organization of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Primates 9, 225-258. Sugiyama, Y. (1969). Social behavior of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Primates 10, 197225. Sugiyama, Y. (1973). The social structure of wild chimpanzees: a review of field studies. In (R. P. Michael & J. H. Crook, eds) Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates. London: Academic Press. Suzuki, A. (1969). An ecological study of chimpanzees in a savanna woodland. Primates 10, 103-148. Suzuki, A. (197la). Carnivority and cannibalism observed among forest-living chimpanzees. Journal of the Anthropological Society of Nippon 79, 30-48. Suzuki, A. (19716). On the problems of conservation of the chimpanzees in East Africa and the preservation of their environment. Primates 12,415418. *Teleki, G. (1973a). The Predatory Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees. Lewisburg: Bucknell University Press. *Teleki, 6. (19733). The omnivorous chimpanzee. Scientific American 228, 32-42. *Teleki, G. (1973~). Group responses to the accidental death of a chimpanzee in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Folia primatologica 20, 81-94. “Teleki, G. (1973d). Notes on chimpanzee interactions with small carnivores in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Primates 14, 407-411. *Teleki, G. (1974). Chimpanzee subsistence technology: materials and skills. Journal of i&man Evolution 3, 575-594. Teleki, G. (1975a). The evolution of primate subsistence patterns. Paper presented at 14lst Annual Meeting of the American Association for the advancement of Science. New York. “Teleki, G. (1975b). Primate subsistence patterns: collector-predators and gatherer-hunters. Journai of Human Evolution 4, 125-184.

598

G. TELEKI

ET

AL.

Teleki, G . (1976). Spatial and temporal dimensions of routine activities performed by chimpanzees of An ethological study of adaptive strategy. Dissertation. Gombe National Park. Tanzaniai -. lJn&ublished _ University Park: Pennsylvania State Univers&. . _ Teleki, G. & Baldwin, L. A. (1975). Breeding programs aim to keep this a planet of the apes. Smithsonian 5, 76-81. Weiss, K. M. (1973). Demographic models for anthropology. Memoirs of the Society of American Archeologists 27, 1-186. “Wrangham, R. W. (1974). Artificial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their natural habitat. Animal Behaviour 22,83-93. “Wrangham, R. W. (1975). The behavioural ecology of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Unpublished Dissertation. University of Cambridge, England.