Prq.
Neuro-Psycbpharmacd.
& Bid.
Psychic&
1997. Vol. 2 1. pp. 5X3-590
Copyright 0 1997 Elsevter Science Inc. PrInted in the USA.
AU ltghts reserved
027%5646/97
ELSEVIER
$32.00
+ .oO
PII 80278-5846(97)00033-X
DIFFERENTIAL EFFECTS OF DOPAMINERGIC DRUGS ON ANXIETY AND AROUSAL IN HEALTHY VOLUNTEERS WITH HIGH AND LOW ANXIEm YASUSHI MIZUKI, MASATOMO SUETSUGI, ITSUKO USHIJIMA and MICHIO YAMADA Department of Neuropsychiatry, Yamaguchi University School of Medicine, Yamaguchi, Japan
Final form, January iw7)
Abstract
Mizuki, Yasushi, Masatomo Suetsugi, Itsuko Ushijima and Michio Yamada: Differential Effects of Dopaminergic Drugs on Anxiety and Arousal in with LOW Anxiety. Volunteers High and Prog. NeuroHealthy Psychophannacol. & Biol. Psychiat. 1997, I& Pp.573-590. 0 1997ElsevierScienceInc.
1. The appearance of frontal midline theta activity (J?m6),the distinct EEG theta rhythm in the frontal midline area during performance of a mental task, indicates relief from anxiety in humans. 2. The authors examined the effects of bromocriptine and sulpiride on anxiety and arousal in 24 male university students with (Pm0 group, n=12) and without (non-Em9 group, n=12) F'm9. Subjects were given placebo, 2.5 mg bromocriptine and 100 mg sulpiride in a double-blind crossover design. 3. Blood samples were obtained, STAI scores were determined, and EEGs were recorded before and during the performance of an arithmetic addition task. The test was repeated twice: before and 1 hr after drug administration. 4. Bromocriptine reduced the HVA concentration in both groups; sulpiride caused an increase in both groups. In the Pm9 group, bromocriptine did not alter the appearance time of E'mO, the state anxiety score or the task performance; sulpiride increased the Pm9 amount and reduced the state anxiety but did not affect the task performance. In the non-Em9 group, bromocriptine increased the Pm9 duration and reduced the state anxiety score but did not influence the task performance, while sulpiride reduced F'm9 and increased the state anxiety but had no effect on the task performance. 5. These results suggest that the sensitivity of presynaptic D2 receptors is higher in high-anxiety subjects compared with lowanxiety subjects, and that anxiolytic effects in high-anxiety humans and those in low-anxiety humans may be caused by decreased and increased DA activity, respectively. In addition, the stimulation of DA function may cause anxiogenic effects in high-anxiety individuals. 573
574
Y.
MizukietaL
Kevwords: anxiety, bromocriptine, activity (Fm8), stress, sulpiride
dopamine,
frontal
midline
theta
Abbreviations: cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), decibel (dB), dopamine (DA), electroencephalogram (EEG), frontal midline theta activity (Fme), highperformance liquid chromatography (HPLC), homovanillic acid (EVA), state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI)
Introduction Dopamine (DA) activity is increased in panic disorder (Pitchot et al., 1992), and low doses of neuroleptics are efficacious in the treatment of In animals, various types of
anxiety (Taylor et al., 1983).
stress
(Roth et al., 1988) and administration of anxiogenic drugs such as fi-CCE and FG-7142 (McCullough and Salamone, 1992) can activate DA function. This enhancement of DA function can be partially blocked by diazepam (Coca et
al.,
following direct
1992).
Anxiogenic-like
stimulation
of
DA
also
effects
receptors
are
(Simon et
observed
al.,
1993).
However, administration of bromocriptine, a D2 receptor agonist, or low doses
of
sulpiride,
a
D2
receptor
antagonist,
mediates
anxiolytic
effects during stress tests in rats (Bruhwyler et al., 1991).
At higher
doses, sulpiride has an anxiogenic effect (Simon et al., 1992).
A distinct electroencephalogram midline area during performance
(EEG) theta rhythm from the of
a
mental
task
Ishihara and Yoshii (1972) and named Fm8 (Fig. 1).
was
frontal
identified by
The appearance of Fm8
differs among individuals (Mizuki et al., 1980).
It is more marked in subjects who are less neurotic and less anxious, while more neurotic and
more anxious subjects display less or no Fm8 When anxiolytic drugs
such
without Fm8, the authors
as
diazepam
were
to
normal
1984). humans
found a significant correlation between the
appearance of FmO and a decrease in anxiety 1983).
(Mizuki et al., given scores
(Mizuki et
al.,
Thus, the appearance of Fm0 may coincide with a decrease in
anxiety, hence Fm8 could be a useful tool in predicting the clinical effects of anxiolytic drugs.
The plasma homovanillic acid central turnover of DA
(HVA) is regarded as an index of the
(Bacopoulos et al.,
1979).
Blood and plasma
levels of DA metabolites have been found to be highly correlated with cerebrospinal .fluid (CSF) levels and to (Heninger et al., 1979).
reflect
central
DA
activity
In the present study, the effects of bromo-
criptine and sulpiride on anxiety and arousal in healthy male university
Anxiety.dopamineandFm6l
575
students with and without FM3 were examined by monitoring the plasma HVA concentration, the
F'm0 amount, and the state-trait
anxiety
inventory
(STAI) score.
Methods
Subjects
On 3 consecutive days, 50 male university students had an EEG recorded for 5 min during the performance of an arithmetic addition test in order Students in whom the F'meappeared
to determine which students had a Fm8.
on the second or third day were excluded.
The study group consisted of
12 subjects who exhibited FmO on all 3 days, and 12 subjects in whom Fmf3 did not appear on any of the 3 days.
The former was termed the E'me
group and the latter the non-Fmf3group.
Subjects ranged in age from 20
to 25 (mean age: 23.2) years, and their resting EEGs were normal.
All
subjects were screened, and those who had any type of medical disorder or who were taking any medication were excluded.
Prior to the study,
informed consent was obtained from all subjects.
Druqs
Two drugs, 2.5 mg bromocriptine and 100 mg sulpiride, and placebo were used.
Capsules containing the drugs and their placebos, were prepared.
Drug doses were determined from data obtained in human studies (Seeman, 1995) and from minimal effective doses in clinical use (EPIC, 1995).
Experimental Procedure
Subjects were evaluated on the STAI-11 (trait anxiety) at lo:30 hr, and then ate lunch at 11:00 hr. subjects rested Blood
samples
in a quiet were
drawn
Scalp electrodes were attached, and the room reading a newspaper from
the
antecubital
vein
or at
a magazine. 13:00
hr.
Subjects then completed the STAI-I (state anxiety), and rested for 3 min with their eyes closed.
They performed the arithmetic addition task for
5 min as rapidly and accurately as possible.
Thereafter, the subjects were administered one of the active drugs or the placebo according to a double-blind, crossover, randomized design. hr following drug
administration.
between administrations.
The tests were repeated 1
There was
a l-day washout
period
EEGs were continuously recorded throughout the
576
Y.
entire
procedure,
except
Mizukietal
during
vein
puncture
and
the
60-min
rest
period.
Assessments Mental Task
The subjects performed the Uchida-Kraepelin test, a test
of continuous arithmetic addition (Kuraishi et al., 1957), as a mental task.
The subject adds horizontally arranged sequential pairs of single
digits aligned vertically.
He writes the last digit of the sum of each
pair in a space just below each pair.
Formerly, the numbers on the test
sheet were written in a manner such that the subject changed to a new line of numbers every minute.
In the present study, the test paper was
lengthened to a band-paper 4 cm high and 240 cm wide on which 8 lines containing 464 numerals each were printed.
Continuous addition could
thus be performed without changing lines.
Psychological Testinq
The STAI
(Spielberger et al.,
1970) used in
this study consists of separate self-report scales for measuring two distinct anxiety
concepts,
state
anxiety
(STAI-I) and
trait
anxiety
(STAI-II). The higher the score on each scale, the greater the anxiety level.
Laboratory Instruments
EEGs
These were
recorded
not
only
directly
on
paper
for visual
analysis but also simultaneously on an analog tape recorder for further computerized automatic analysis before and during the task performance. Disc electrodes were reference.
placed at Fpz, Fz, and Cz, using Al+AZ as
The recording conditions were as follows: paper speed of 1.5
cm/set, time constant of 0.1 set, and sensitivity of 10 pV/mm. high-cut filter was spectra
the
requirements
Transformation.
The
used. by
a
resting EEG was
digital
computer
analyzed using
A 60-HZ
for power
Fast
Fourier
Each lo-set epoch was analyzed with 102.4 points/set,
which permitted the analysis of frequencies up to 50 Hz.
Time samples
of 60 set were analyzed.
EEGs during the task were evaluated by computer analysis, and only the theta rhythm recorded from the Fz electrode was detected by the interval histogram method
(Kuwahara et al.,
1988).
performed for the final evaluation of Fm8.
Visual analysis was also The theta rhythm selection
Anxiety,dopamineandFmO
577
algorithm employed a criterion which specified whether the signal slope following a peak
or trough was
sufficiently negative or positive to
qualify the peak or trough as a theta rhythm.
The criteria
for Fm9
were: continuity of more than 4 consecutive waves, frequency of 5.0-7.5 Hz, and amplitude of more than 20 pV, for computer analysis; rhythmical sinusoidal configuration,
markedly
higher
amplitude
as
compared with
background activity, duration exceeding 1 set, and frequency of 5.0-7.5 Hz, for visual analysis. Artifact segments containing eye blinking and eye movement were excluded from analysis, but there were few of these episodes as shown in Fig. 1.
Biochemical Determination
Ten-ml blood samples were collected on two occasions with heparinized syringes and transferred into siliconized tubes containing 0.1 ml of 10% Na2-EDTA.
The blood was
mixed,
immediately placed on
centrifuged at 3000 rpm for 10 min at 4°C.
ice, and then
The top layer of platelet-
rich plasma was transferred to pipette tubes and stored at -80 "C.
RVA
concentrations in 2-ml plasma samples were measured by high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) with electrochemical detection according to the technique of Sailer and Salama
(1984) with
slight modifications.
HVA values were obtained from a single assay.
Fig 1. A sample of EEG during performance of the Uchida-Kraepelin test. Fm8 is underlined.
Statistical Analysis
Resting EEG, plasma WA
concentration, appearance time of Fm8, STAI-I
578 and
Y.Mizukieta1 STAI-II scores,
number of
performed
tasks,
and
number
of
errors
before the drug and placebo administration were averaged and used as the The
control values.
values
obtained
following drug
or
placebo
in-
gestion were averaged and compared with controls except for the STAI-II scores.
The control and treatment values in the Fm0 group were also
compared with
the
corresponding
values
in
the
non-Fm0
group.
The
comparison of resting EEG between the two groups and between the control EEG and the EEG following drug administration in both groups was analyzed by the Student's t-test and the paired t-test, respectively.
The
comparison of remaining items between the groups was analyzed by the Ftest for equal variance, and a probability level of 0.05 was set for statistical significance.
The Student's t-test was
used .for further
post-hoc analysis in the case of Fz2.82 (PgO.05); the Aspin-Welch t-test was adopted at Fp2.82
(P~0.05).
As
for the
comparison
between
control and experimental values following drug administration
the
in both
groups, the chi-square test for goodness of fit was employed for normality, and a probability level of 0.05 was required for statistical significance.
The
paired t-test was
used when x223.84 (PgO.05); the
Wilcoxon signed-ranks test was adopted when x2>3.84 (PCO.05).
Results
Restinq EEGs
In the frontal area (Fig. 2), the power values of the control EEG were greater at the whole bands in the Fm8 group as compared to the non-Fm8 group.
Following administration of placebo, the power values in the Fm8
group were higher at 10, 11, 14-17 and 29-32 Hz but lower at 40 and 41 Hz compared to the non-Fm8 group.
Following administration of 2.5 mg
bromocriptine, the power values in the Fm8 group were higher at 3, 5, 6 and
39-42
Hz
but
lower
at
22
Hz
compared
to
the
non-Fm8
group.
Following administration of 100 mg sulpiride, the power values in the Fm0 group were
higher at 4-6 and
12 Hz but lower at
38 and
41 Hz
compared to the non-Fm0 group. Placebo was associated with an increase in the power at 12, 16 and 32 Hz and a decrease at 41 Hz in the Fm8 group, but an increase at 6 and 8 Hz and a decrease at 24,
33-36 and 45 Hz in the
compared with the respective control values
non-Fm8 group, as
(Fig. 3).
Bromocriptine decreased the power at 6 and 7 Hz and increased the power at 21 and 23 Hz in the Fm8 group, but decreased the power at
3, 5 and
6 HZ
and
Anxiety, dopamineand Fm8
579
(dB)
Fig. 2: Resting EEGs at the frontal area in the Fm8 group and the non-Fm8 group. The figures represent the EEG power spectra in the Fm8 and nonThe power values during control conditions and following Fm8 groups. placebo, bromocriptine and sulpiride administration are shown from the upper to lower rows. The values of EEG frequency during control conditions and following drug administration from the Fm8 group were The compared with the corresponding values from the non-Fme group. solid line shows the Fm8 group [Fm8 (+)I, and the dotted line the non-Fm8 Ordinate: logarithmic power values (dB); abscissa: group tFm8 (-)I. frequency of EEG (Hz). A: PcO.10; *: PCO.05; **: P
increased the power at 12 and 22-25 Hz in the non-Fm8 group compared with the control values.
Sulpiride reduced the power at 4, 6 and I Hz
and increased the power at 13, 18, 29-32 and 38-41 Hz in the Fm8 group, but reduced the power at 5 and 6 Hz and increased the power at 25-28 and 39
Hz
in the non-Fm8 group compared with the control values.
580
Y.Mizuki etal
FmO(+)
(de)
Fme( -)
(dBJ
I 0
10
20
30
40
50(HZ)
-10
(dB)
10
20
10
20
30
40
50 (Hz)
40
50 (HZ)
(dE)
I
-’ 0
10
20
30
40
1 -10
50 (HZ)
(dB)
I -’ 0
30
(dB)
10
20
30
40
AW)
-1M
SO(HZ)
Fig. 3: Resting ERGS at the frontal area during control conditions and The following placebo, bromocriptine and sulpiride administration. figures represent the power spectra of EEGs before and after drug The power values during administration in the Fm0 and non-I%@ groups. control conditions are respectively compared with those following The left side of placebo, bromocriptine and sulpiride administration. the figure: the FmB group [Fme (+)I; the right side: the non-Fm0 group [FmB (-)I. The solid line indicates the control, and the dotted line placebo (PL), bromocriptine (BRC) and sulpiride (SLR), respectively. The details are given in the legend for Fig. 2.
Plasma HVA Concentration
The HVA concentration was lower at baseline and following sulpiride administration in the FmO group compared with the non-Fm0 group, but there were no significant differences in the HVA concentration following bromocriptine and placebo ingestion between the groups (Table 1). The increased by sulpiride compared with the control values in both groups. Placebo had
HVA
concentration
was
decreased
by
bromocriptine
and
no effect on the HVA concentration in either group.
Appearance Time Of Fme The appearance time of Fm(!l was greater in the Fmtl group than in the non-Fm0 group at any time (Table 1).
Sulpiride increased the FmO amount
in the FmO group compared with the control value, but bromocriptine and
FmB(+)
Fm8(-)
2.33*0.29
2.18kO.20
360.33k5.47
Fm8(-)
me(+)
46.26&O-86
Fme(+)
361.17*5.35
42.6720.82 **
Fm8(-)
mef-b
42.52kO.88
me(+)
FmBf-)
2.00zt0.27
2.03iO.25
362.88k5.34
362.69r5.45
40.39AO.87
37.69kO.92 *
1.52i0.38 C
0.46*0.10
me(+)
39.38kO.99 *
9.92kO.68 10.79i1.70 ***
10.7Ok1.32 ***
8.59kO.59
10.09iO.62
8.38*0.50 *
Fm8(-)
m(+)
Placebo
1.82kO.29
1.65kO.26
363.19k5.23
363.55i4.86
39.55kO.98 C
41.70*0.90
1.75~0.48 C
10.3523.84 *
7.50*0.70 c
6.89AO.45 C
Bromocriptine
2.84kO.31
2.71kO.27
365.95k5.43
364.3225.53
45.42i1.03 C
35.32kO.82 B ***
0.20*0.07 C
17.93e2.02 B ***
11.85i0.52 C
10.06*0.61 c *
Sulpiride
The values for the control and those following placebo, bromocriptine or sulpiride administration are shown as actual measurements (meaniS.E.M.). The BVA values are expressed in ng/ml, and Fm8 amounts in sec. Fm8(+): the Fm8 group; Fme(-): the non-Fm8 group. Statistically significant differences between the groups *: PCO.05; **: P
Error
Task
STAI-11
STAI-I
FmB
EVA
Control
Effects of Placebo, Bromocriptine and Sulpiride on the BVA Concentration, Appearance Time of Fm8, STAI Scores, the Number of Performed Tasks and the Number of Errors.
Table 1
$
1
9
582
Y.Mizukiet&.
placebo had no effect.
In the non-Fm8 group, bromocriptine and placebo
increased the Fin0 amount compared
with
the
control,
while
sulpiride
and
following
caused a decrease.
STAI Scores
The
state
anxiety
scores
lower
were
at
baseline
sulpiride and placebo administration in the Fm8 group as compared to the There were no significant differences between
non-Fm8 group (Table 1).
the groups following bromocriptine administration. the state anxiety scores
Sulpiride reduced
in the Fm8 group compared with
values, but bromocriptine and placebo had no effect.
the control
In the
group, bromocriptine decreased the state anxiety scores, sulpiride caused an increase and placebo had no effect.
non-FmH whereas
The trait anxiety scores were lower at baseline in the Fm8 group than in the non-Fm8 group.
Number of Performed Tasks and Number of Errors
There were no significant differences in the task performance and the number
of
errors
at
any
time
between
the
groups
(Table
1).
No
significant change in the task performance level or the number of errors was noted in either
group when
compared with
the respective control
values.
Discussion
Bioloqical Aspects of DA Function
The
mental
established
arithmetic as
a
task
stressor
used
in
the
present
(Gorman
et
al.,
1987).
study The
has been stressful
manipulations activate mesolimbic and mesocortical DA neurons (Moore and Lookingland, 1995) and stress clearly changes the DA metabolism in the ventral striatum as well as in the frontal cortex (Le Moal and Simon, 1991).
A specific increase in DA turnover is observed in the nucleus
accumbens 1 hr after stress (D'Angio et al., 1987), and the increased DA utilization
under
the
stressful
condition
is
a
general
phenomenon
Anxiety. dopamineand FmB related to various types of anxiety
583 This
(Le Moal and Simon, 1991).
anxiogenic-like effect is also observed following the stimulation of DA receptors (Simon et al., 1993). both
and
pre-
Although D2 receptors are those
post-synaptically,
located
at
localized
presynaptic
nerve
terminals function as autoreceptors controlling the release of DA in the nigrostriatal
and
mesolimbic
DA
systems
(Roth and
Elsworth,
1995).
Acute administration of bromocriptine decreases DA synthesis and release by stimulating presynaptic D2 receptors, while that of sulpiride increases DA synthesis and release by blocking presynaptic D2 receptors (Roth and Elsworth, 1995).
Pharmaco-EEG of Bromocriptine and Sulpiride
In the present study, bromocriptine reduced the power of 0 activity and
increased
the
power
of
low-fast
$
activity
in
both
Bromocriptine also decreased S activity in the non-Fm8 group.
groups. Sulpiride
reduced the power of 8 activity and increased middle- and high-fast @ activity in both groups. activities
in
the
Sulpiride also increased fast a and low-fast fi EEG
F'm8 group.
profiles
following
the
acute
administration of 2.5 mg bromocriptine in humans are characterized by a decrease in slow (6 and 8) waves and an increase in fast a, low- and 'middle-fast fi activities
(Suitsu, 1992).
On
the
other
hand,
acute
administration of 400 mg sulpiride in normal volunteers has been found to increase the proportion of slow wave activity as well as decrease CI and fl activities
(McClelland et al.,
(1982) have reported that
three
1990).
types
of
However,
EEG
changes
Saito were
et
al.
observed
following the administration of 200 mg sulpiride in normal subjects: i) increased slow wave and fiactivity associated with decreased CI activity (thymo-leptic type), ii) increased slow wave
and decreased fl activity
(neuroleptic type), iii)and decreased slow wave and increased p activity (psychostimulant
type).
Therefore,
the
present
EEG
findings
are
consistent with the results of previous studies and suggest that the EEG effects of 2.5 mg bromocriptine and those 1992),
of
nootropic
and
vigilance
psychostimulants,
100 mg
enhancers, such
as
sulpiride are similar to
such
as
amphetamine
piracetam
(Suitsu,
(Herrmann,
1982),
respectively.
Stress, Anxietv and DA Turnover In the present study, the control values of BVA concentration in the
584
Y MizukietaI_
Fm8 group were lower than in the non-Fm8 group. decreased and
sulpiride
increased WA
However, bromocriptine
concentration
in
both
groups.
Furthermore, the sulpiride-induced HVA decrease was greater in the nonFm8 group than in the Fm8 group. is increased by psychological
Although DA turnover in normal humans
stress, subjects with
a high
level of
anxiety exhibit an increased DA turnover in both relaxed and stressful situations compared with subjects with
low anxiety levels
(Mizuki et
Increased DA turnover correlates with the probability of
al., 1992).
panic attacks, and this elevation has been associated with
increased
Fm8 is more marked in subjects who
anxiety (Roy-Byrne et al., 1986).
are less neurotic and less anxious; subjects who are more neurotic and more anxious show a reduced or absent Fm8 (Mizuki et al., 1984). administration
of
bromocriptine
reduces
DA
function
by
Acute
stimulating
presynaptic D2 receptors, while that of sulpiride increases DA function by blocking presynaptic
D2 receptors
(Roth and Elsworth,
1995).
In
addition, the sensitivity of DA receptors is increased in patients with Therefore, data on HVA levels
panic disorders (Pitchot et al., 1992).
after sulpiride administration in the present
study
suggest
that D2
receptor sensitivity may be higher in individuals with high levels of anxiety compared with those with low levels of anxiety. bromocriptine
for
D2
receptors
is
greater
(Billard et al., 1984; Creese, 1987). groups are
normal
humans
in
the
than
The affinity of
that
of
sulpiride
Furthermore, the subjects in both
present
study.
Therefore,
it
is
possible that the strong affinity of bromocriptine for D2 receptors may mask
the
slight
differences
in D2
receptor
sensitivity
between
the
groups.
Anxiety and D2 Recenters Bromocriptine and sulpiride had different effects on Fmf3 amount and state anxiety scores in Fm8 and non-Fm8 subjects in the present study. The
appearance of
Fm8
is
negatively
correlated
with
a
decrease
in
anxiety and is closely related to the personality of subjects (Mizuki et al., 1984). The present results suggest that bromocriptine produced an anxiolytic effect but sulpiride caused the opposite effect in highanxiety subjects under stressful conditions, only sulpiride reduced the anxiety
level
of
low-anxiety
subjects
in
a
stressful
situation.
(1991) have reported that bromocriptine demonstrated
Bruhwyler et al.
anxiolytic effects at moderate doses but had no effects on anxiety at high
doses
in
administration
the of
open-field
bromocriptine
test may
for treat
rats. anxiety
Clinically, in
the
acute cocaine
Anxiety.dopam~neandF& withdrawal syndrome caused by (Markou and
Koob,
activation of DAergic neurotransmission
In
1992).
585
low doses
addition,
of
sulpiride
effective in preventing anxiety induced by the conflict test
are
in rats
(Pith and Samanin, 1986), and clinical efficacy for sulpiride has also been established in patients with anxiety (Peselow and Stanley, 1982). Therefore, the previous studies on the anxiolytic effects of the two drugs coincide well with
the present data on bromocriptine
in high-
However, it
anxiety subjects and sulpiride in low-anxiety subjects.
recently has been reported that sulpiride can exhibit an anxiogenic-like effect in the two-compartment exploratory test for mice 1992).
In
addition,
individuals with
high
increased anxiety
DA
turnover
compared with
(Simon et al.,
has
been
observed
those
with
low
in
anxiety
(Mizuki et al., 1992), and the present results of HVA levels suggest that
the
sensitivity
individuals than
in
of
D2
receptors
low-anxiety
is
higher
in
high-anxiety
Therefore,
individuals.
anxiolytic
effects in high-anxiety humans may be caused by decreased DA activity (stimulation of presynaptic D2 receptors), while those
in low-anxiety
humans may be caused by increased DA function (inhibition of presynaptic D2 receptors).
Arousal and D2 Receptors
No significant differences were found in the task performance or the number of errors between groups in the present study, no were there significant changes in these parameters in either group when compared with the respective control values.
Performance of an arithmetic task
reflects the level of arousal rather than the level of attention, and the number of errors is correlated with major changes in the arousal level but
is not affected by
small changes
(Mixuki, 1987).
In the present study, no drug had an effect on arousal in either high-anxiety or
low-anxiety
subjects.
Recently,
it
has
been
reported
that
stimulation of Dl receptors led to some desynchronization in baseline EEG and a general decrease usually regarded as 1993).
in EEG power
spectra,
signs of arousal in rats
results which
are
(Kropf and Kuschinsky,
The probable mechanism of this phenomenon is that increased DA
in the synaptic cleft activates Dl receptors that stimulate release of acetylcholine, which
initiates the arousal response
(Yoshida et
al.,
1993).
On the other hand, a characteristic increase of power in the slow a activity is apparently due to activation of postsynaptic D2 receptors (Kropf and Kuschinsky, 1991, 1993). that pre-synaptic autoreceptors mediate
However, the hypothesis an increase in the EEG power
following a low dose of a DA receptor agonist is doubtful
(Kropf and
586
Y.Mizuki etal.
Kuschinsky, 1993).
In the present study, the doses of bromocriptine and
sulpiride were small, acute administration of the drugs was used, and it was
hypothesized
these
drugs
autoreceptors predominantly. ther bromocriptine
nor
had
an
influence
on
presynaptic
Therefore, it is not surprising that nei-
sulpiride
affected
the
arousal
level
of
the
subjects in both groups.
Conclusions
The present results suggest that the sensitivity of pre-synaptic D2 receptors was higher in high-anxiety subjects compared with low-anxiety subjects. anxiety
Our results also suggested that anxiolytic effects in high-
humans
decreased DA addition,
the
and
those
activity
and
stimulation
in
low-anxiety
increased DA of
DA
humans
function,
transmission
may
may
be
caused
respectively. cause
by In
anxiogenic
effects in high-anxiety individuals.
Acknowledgment
A part of this research was supported by a grant-in-aid for scientific research from the Ministry of Education, Science and Culture, Japan (No. 60480262).
References BACOPOULOS, N.G., HATTOX, S.E. and ROTH, R.H. (1979) 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid and homovanillic acid in rat plasma: possible indicators of central dopaminergic activity. Eur. J. Pharmacol., 56: 225-236. Billard, W., Ruperto, V., Crosby, G., Iorio, L.C., Barnett, A. (1984) Characterization of the binding of 3H-SCH 23390, a selective Dl receptor antagonist ligand, in rat striatum. Life Sci., 29: 1885-1893. BRUHWYLER, J., CHLEIDE, E., LIEGEOIS, J-F., DELARGE, J. and MERCIER, M. (1991) Effects of specific dopaminergic agonists and antagonists in the open-field test. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 39: 367-371. COCO, M.L., KUHN, C.M., ELY, T.D. and KILTS, C.D. (1992) Selective activation of mesoamygdaloid dopamine neurons by conditioned stress: attenuation by diazepam. Brain Res., 590: 39-47. Creese, I. (1987) Biochemical properties of CNS dopamine receptors. In:
Anxiety. dopamine and FmB
Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation of (Ed.), pp. 251-264, Raven Press, New York.
587 Progress,
H.Y.
Meltzer
D'ANGIO, M., SERRANO, A., RIVY, J-P. and SCATTON, B. (1987) Tail-pinch stress increases extracellular DOPAC levels (as measured by in vivo voltammetry) in the rat nucleus accumbens but not frontal cortex: antagonism by diazepam and zolpidem. Brain Res., 409: 169-174. GORMAN, J.M., FYER, M.R., LIEBOWITZ, M.R. and KLEIN, D.F. (1987) Pharmacologic provocation of panic attacks. In: Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation of Progress, H.Y. Meltzer (Ed.), pp. 985-993, Raven Press, New York. HENINGER, G., BACOPOULOS, N., ROTH, R.H., BOWERS, M.Jr. and SWEENEY, D. (1979) Plasma and CSF dopamine metabolites in psychiatric patients: effects of probenecid and haloperodol. In: The Fourth International Catecholamine Symposium, E. Usdin (Ed.), pp. 1887-1889, Pergamon Press, New York. HERRMANN, W.M. (1982) Development and critical evaluation of an objective procedure for the electroencephalographic classification of psychotropic drugs. In: Electroencephalography in Drug Research, W.M. Herrmann (Ed.), pp. 405-508, Gustav Fischer, Stuttgart. ISHIHARA, T. and YOSHII, N. (1972) Multivariate analytic study of EEG and mental activity in juvenile delinquents. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., 33: 71-80. JAPAN PHARMACEUTICAL INFORMATION CENTER Ethical Drugs, Yakugyo Jiho, Tokyo.
(JPIC) (1995) Drugs
in Japan:
KROPF, W. and KUSCHINSKY, K. (1991) Electroencephalographic correlates of the sedative effects of dopamine agonists presumably acting on autoreceptors. Neuropharmacol., 0: 953-960. KROPF, W. and KUSCHINSKY, K. (1993) Effects of stimulation of dopamine Dl receptors on the cortical EEG in rats: different influences by a blockade of Dz receptors and by an activation of putative dopamine autoreceptors. Neuropharmacol., 2: 493-500. KURAISHI, S., KATO, M. and TSUJIOKA, B. (1957) Development of the Uchida-Kraepelin psychodiagnostic test in Japan. Psychologia, L: 104109. KUWAHARA, H., HIGASHI, H., MIZUKI, Y., MATSUNARI, S., TANAKA, M. and INANAGA, K. (1988) Automatic real-time analysis of human sleep stages by an interval histogram method. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., 70: 220-229. LE MOAL, M. and SIMON, H. (1991) Mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic network: functional and regulatory roles. Physiol. Rev., 2: 155-234.
588
Y.Mizuki eta1
MARKOU, A. and KOOB, G.F. (1992) Bromocriptine reverses the elevation in intracranial self-stimulation thresholds observed in a rat model of cocaine withdrawal. Neuropsychophannacol., z: 213-224. MCCLELLAND, G.R., COOPER, S.M. and PILGRIM, A.J. (1990) A comparison of the central nervous system effects of haloperodol, chlorpromazine and sulpiride in normal volunteers. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol., 30: 795-803. MCCULLOUGH, L.D. and SALAMONE, J.D. (1992) Anxiogenic drugs beta-CCE and FG 7142 increase extracellular dopamine levels in nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacol., 109: 379-382. MIZUKI, Y., TANAKA, M., ISOZAKI, H., NISHIJIMA, H. and INANAGA, K. (1980) Periodic appearance of theta rhythm in the frontal midline area mental performance of a task. Electroenceph. during Clin. Neurophysiol., e: 345-351. MIZUKI, Y., HASHIMOTO, M., TANAKA, T., INANAGA, K. and TANAKA, M. (1983) A new physiological tool for assessing anxiolytic effects in humans: frontal midline theta activity. Psychopharmacol., 80: 311-314. MIZUKI, Y., KAJIMURA, N., NISHIKORI, S., IMAIZUMI, J. and YAMADA, M. (1984) Appearance of frontal midline theta rhythm and personality traits. Folia Psychiat. Neurol. Jpn., 3: 451-458. MIZUKI, Y. (1987) Frontal lobe: mental functions and EEG. Am. J. EEG Technol., 21: 91-101. SUETSUGI, M., USHIJIMA, I. and MIZUKI, Y., KAJIMURA, N., KAI, s., YAMADA, M. (1992) Differential responses to mental stress in high and low anxious normal humans assessed by frontal midline theta activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol., 12: 169-178. MOORE, K-E. and LOOKINGLAND, K.J. (1995) Dopaminergic neuronal systems in the hypothalamus. In: Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress, F.E. Bloom and D.J. Kupfer (Eds.), pp. 245-256, Raven Press, New York. PESELOW, E.D. and STANLEY, M. (1982) Clinical trials of benzamide in psychiatry. Adv. Biochem. Psychopharmacol., 35: 163-194. PICH, E.M. and SAMANIN, R. (1986) Disinhibitory effects of buspirone and low doses of sulpiride and haloperidol in two experimental anxiety models in rats: possible role of dopamine. Psychopharmacol., !3J: 125130. PITCHOT, W., ANSSEAU, M., MORENO, A-G., HANSENNE, M. and VON FRENCKELL, R. (1992) Dopaminergic function in panic disorder: comparison with major and minor depression. Biol. Psychiat., 32: 1004-1011.
Anxiety.dopamineandFmtl
589
ROTH, R-H., TAM, S-Y., IDA, Y., YANG, J-X. and DEUTCH, A.Y. (1988) dopamine systems. In: and the mesocorticolimbic The stress Mesocorticolimbic Dopamine System, P.W. Kalivas and C.B. Nemeroff (Eds.), pp. 138-147, The New York Academy of Sciences, New York. ROTH, R.H. midbrain Generation 243, Raven
and ELSWORTH, J.D. (1995) Biochemical pharmacology of Psychopharmacology: dopamine neurons. In: The Fourth of Progress, F.E. Bloom and D.J. Kupfer (Eds.), pp. 227Press, New York.
ROY-BYRNE, P.P., UHDE, T.W., SACK, D.A., LINNOILA, M. and POST, R.M. (1986) Plasma HVA and anxiety in patients with panic disorder. Biol. Psychiat., 21: 849-853. (1982) The orientation of SAITO, M., KITA, S. and KAGONO, Y. induced changes psychotropic drug properties based on the in zimelidine and studies on sulpiride. In: electroencephalogram: Electroencephalography in Drug Research, W.M. Herrmann (Ed.), pp. 5174, Gustav Fischer, Stuttgart. SALLER, C.F. and SALAMA, A-1. (1984) Rapid automated analysis of biogenic amines and their metabolites using reversed-phase highperformance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection. J. Chromatogr., 309: 287-298. (1995) Dopamine receptors: Clinical P. correlates. In: Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress, F.E. Bloom and D.J. Kupfer (Eds.), pp. 295-302, Raven Press, New York.
SEEMAN,
COSTENTIN, PANISSAUD, C. and SIMON, P., anxiogenic-like effect inhibition by a antagonist. Neuroreport, 2: 941-942.
J. Dl
(1992) dopamine
Sulpiride receptor
SIMON, P., PANISSAUD, C. and COSTENTIN, J. (1993) Anxiogenic-like effects induced by stimulation of dopamine receptors. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., Qs: 685-690. SPIELBERGER, C.D., GORSUCH, R.L. and LUSHENE, R.E. (1970) State-Trait Psychologists Anxiety Inventory, Consulting Press, Palo Alto, California. SUITSU, N. (1992) A computer-assisted EEG study on psychotropic properties of antiparkinsonian drugs. Psychiat. Neurol. Jpn., 94: 238262. TAYLOR, D-P., RIBLET, L.A. and STANTON, H.C. (1983) Dopamine and anxiolytics. In: Anxiolytics: Neurochemical, Behavioral, and Clinical Perspectives, J.B. Malick, S.J. Enna and H.I. Yamamura (Eds.), pp. 7791, Raven Press, New York. YOSHIDA, K., GJERDE, D-K., CARINO, M.A., HALPERN, L.M. and HORITA, A.
590
Y. Mizuki etd
(1993) The effects of SCH 23390 and raclopride on cocaine-induced analepsis and EEG arousal in rabbits. Neuropharmacol., 32: 487-492. Inquiries and reprint requests should be addressed to Yasushi Mizuki, M.D. Associate Professor Department of Neuropsychiatry, Yamaguchi University School of Medicine, 1144 Kogushi, Ube, Yamaguchi 755, Japan Phone Number: 81-836-22-2255 Fax Number: 81-836-22-2253