Behaviourd Elsevier
Processes,
1I (1985) 113-121
113
DISPLAY VIGOUR AND SUBSEQUENT FIGHT PERFORMANCE FIGHTING FISH, BETTA SPLENDENS.
IN THE SIAMESE
C. S. EVANS’ ept. of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England f Now at Dept. of Psychology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA (Accepted
13
June
1984)
ABSTRACT Evans, C. S., 1985. Display vigour and subsequent fighting fish, Betta splendens. Behav. Processes
fight performance L1 : 113 - 13 1.
The display of 24 individual male Siamese fighting conspecific was measured, and the fish were then placed 12 pairs, the outcome of the aggressive interaction which cover erection durations obtained in the pre-fight isolate result for theories of display function are discussed.
in the Siamese
fish to an unresponsive stimulus together in pairs. For 11of the ensued was predicted by the gill tests. The implications of this
INTRODUCTION The behaviour of many studies,
of the Siamese
and the sequence
fighting
of events
between
conspecifics
has been described
together
will undergo
profound
characteristic
orientation,
by a stereotyped mouthlocking, & Peeke,
occurring
in detail
displays,
considerable
damage
(Braddock
mirrors,
behaviours
or live fish behind a barrier
Bols, 1977; Hogan & Bols, 1980; Bronstein, display eliciting described
techniques
a correlation
as a pre-fight
with subsequent
that the fish which display
encounters
with conspecifics
antagonists,
into biting and
& Braddock,
1955; Klein, Figler
(Thompson,
(Baenninger,
subsequently
proved
fishes which had the opportunity they were p!aced tc;ether,
relationships
1963, 1969; Simpson,
have employed
measurement
behaviour,
of agonistic
fight outcome.
For example, to mirrors
1975; these
and have
it has been
are dominant
in later
1968; Meliska et al., 1975) and that the fishes to a barrier
which separated
to be dominant to display
them from their
in seven out of eight cases. to a mirror,
than its pairmember.
after
five minutes
the winner-to-be
It has also been reported
to display to both mirrors took less time to cstahlich
and their future
opponents
rfnminant!sllhordinate
(IMeliska, 1974 in Meliska & Meliska, 1976; Meliska et al., 1980).
0376-6357/85/$03.30
1966,
1983). Some investigators
in the same tank, and allowed
to face its image more often
fish by means
1970; Rhoad, Kalat & Klopfer,
Simpson (1968) has shown that in five pairs of fish separated
of interaction
before
This is usually followed
escalates
may be evoked in isolated
most vigourously
the most time in close proximity
Similarly, turned
their fins, and adopt a
1976).
of models,
future
interaction
1968). Two males placed
spread
which often
1968; Hogan, 1968; Hogan & Roper, 1978; Baeninger,
spending
has been the subject
during an aggressive
(Simpson,
in colouration,
of threat
Many or all of these agonistic
reported
splendens)
often at right angles to each other.
sequence inflicting
changes
fish (Betta
Q 1985 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. (biomedical
Division)
that
114
These findings ing aggressive
are strikingly
interactions
between
Brock et al., 1979; Clutton-Brock often be predicted
similar
to those reported
rutting
red deer stags (Cervus elaphus
& Albon, 1979). The outcome
by the rate at which the opposing
that this is a case of “honest advertisement”, highly correlated displays
with fighting
by Clutton-Brock,
ability.
describ-
1.) (Clutton-
of these contests
males roar, and the authors
where an energetically
Similar suggestions
since Simpson (1968) has shown that the effect
suggest
costly signal is
have been made about visual
in fish (Zahavi, 1981). These models may also apply to the aggressive
Bettas,
can
display of
of one fish’s behaviour
on another
is highly predictable. If Bettas their fighting
may profitably
prowess
vigour to a non-reactive before
be viewed as disseminating
by means of their threat stimulus
they are introduced
Several
study examined
fight outcome,
and subordinate of mirrors
fish.
to provide
objections
centre
authors
should “bluff” and conceal
affect
stimulus
properties
of isolate
of the aggressive
encounter
the problems
feedback
for
on both dominant
presented
by the use
1975; Peeke & Peeke,
19701, their
the fish being tested
the test fish’s display in an uncontrolled over time as a result of fatigue
some of the problems
that this stimulus
display rates
obtains
with this
which can see the test fish or their own mirror image will
study was designed
way, and may change
or habituation.
of mirror and model designs.
also differs
from the natural
situation
and consequently
their
The apparatus
to allow the test fish to view a non-displaying
to the test fish display,
1983, personal
accuracy
have described
on the inappropriate
similarly
“indifferent”
of a pair
have argued that this should
animals
stimuli (Rhoad, Kalat & Klopfer,
Live conspecifics
avoiding
the winning member
authors
the predictive
and the effects
Several
method.
the present
about
to win fights (Dawkins & Krebs, 1978).
The present subsequent
information
then it follows that display
could be used to predict
to each other.
not be the case, and that under some circumstances their potential
accurate
displays,
used
Betta,
in
thus
It should be noted, however,
because
the stimulus
fish is
is not driven away (Bronstein,
communication).
METHODS Subjects Twenty-four maintained
male Betta
in jars, visually
were fed freeze-dried apparatus
contained
splendens
isolated,
were obtained
with the temperature
Tubifex worms once daily. chloamphenicol
(an antibiotic)
as adults from local dealers, regulated
and
at 27% f 1°C. Fish
The water in both the jars and the test at 2.4 mg. per litre.
Apparatus A plastic smaller
tank
35 rm.
Y 7lr K -m.
Y 7fl rm.
wz15
divided intn Tao C-r;.-r+ments,
of which (12.5 cm. x 24.5 cm. x 20 cm.) housed the stimulus
the subject
fish.
The partition
consisted
of a “sandwich”
of Plexiglas
the
fish and the larger of sheet containing
115
two sheets of “opposite” bly was held together
one-way
plastic
(Rank Strand Electric
by means of brass bolts at the corners,
The tank was then filled
with aged tap water,
was added to the bottom.
The stimulus
from the observer, dark.
arranged
observer,
whereas
be a black wall. observer
fish was then placed
was illuminated
the stimulus
from the stimulus It was necessary
end) to prevent
after
Data were recorded well of Glasgow,
into place.
in the smaller
compartment
of the study. by two 6OW lamps at the opposite
fish was visible,
end
The room was otherwise
both to the subject
fish side of the apparatus,
the partition
and the appeared
to
to lightly sand the sides of the tank (other than the
the fish seeing their reflected
the same size as the test apparatus both tanks was changed
and cemented
at an angle of 45’ to the partition.
Under these conditions
The whole assem-
and a 4 cm deep layer of washed gravel
of the tank, and was housed there for the duration During tests the apparatus
Ltd.).
images.
was used for aggressive
A second
plastic
interactions.
tank of
The water in
each group of three pairs had been run.
on a 12 pen event recorder
made by Edgoumbe
with a paper speed of 2.5 cm. per minute.
was done with a Pye Instruments
Peebles
Observation
Both-
period timing
stopclock.
Procedure Each fish was allowed test), and was then placed nant/subordinate tested
relationship
with the stimulus
to display to the non-responding
together
with its pairmember
was established
fish 24 hours after
(fight).
as Thompson
for evoking to control
and Sturm (1965) have pointed
display in Bettas. for variation
necessitated
Further,
in activity
testing
(Hogan,
running the experiment
tank until a domi-
Both pairmembers
the fight (post-fight
Fish which were to be paired were matched possible,
stimulus fish (pre-fight
in a separate
were then re-
test).
for size and colouration out the importance
was carried
as far as
of these factors
out at the same time each day
1961 in Hogan, 1968); this latter procedure
as a series of replications,
using three pairs at a
time. The first of the fish to be paired was introduced the test apparatus
and observed
this was characterised colouration display
and spreading
toward
of the dorsal,
within ten minutes were removed.
the trauma associated which subsequently fish immediately.
later paired,
with introduction
proved
ventral
This flexible
and returned
erection
in the distance
of
the onset of display behaviour, fish, a deepening
and caudal
fins.
prevented
was necessary
since
even those fish
display from reacting
to its home jar; the second
of body
Fish which did not
initial period
into the tank often
to have a very vigourous
was then tested
Gill cover
following
the stimulus
At the end of the ten minute test period
from the apparatus
increase
for ten minutes
by orientation
into the large compartment
to the stimulus
the first fish was removed fish, with which the first was
in the same way.
(GCE) was selected between
as the index of threat display.
the distal edge of the operculum
GCE is an
and the body, which is
116
often accompanied
by extension
of the branchiostegal
membranes
end of a bout of GCE is marked by a sudden movement toward the body even if they are not completely a number of authors Figler & Peeke,
to describe
aggressive
1976; Braddock
known to occur in most other anabantid Simpscn (1968) has pointed greater
duration
two female
The observer
Ten minutes removed
after
caused both individuals ists.
the conclusion to display,
Braddock, returned
of lateral
of pre-fight
testing
together
procedure
and were tested
between
fish GCE bouts
both paired
fish were
led to biting by one or both antagonbecoming
colouration
at the approach
to their home jars as soon as a definite
been established,
and a
in the large tank; this invariably
and sometimes
fleeing
1957). Further,
recorder.
lines of the original
1955) and repeatedly
(Forselius,
of subject
The end of the fight was marked by the subordinate
with the appearance
1968; Klein,
during an interaction
the duration
to the event
from their home jars and placed
has been used by
(e.g., Simpson,
with facing the opponent,
the winner-to-be
(GCED), using a key panel connected
This movement
fish, and some cichlids
recorded
1968). The
1955; Lobb & McCain, 1976), and is also
out that GCE coincides
of GCE characterises
Bettas.
closed.
display in Bettas
& Braddock,
(Simpson,
of the distal edge of the opercula
extremely
of the dominant.
dominant/subordinate
with the stimulus
pale, often
(e.g., Braddock
&
The fish were relationship
had
fish 24 hours later using the same
asbefore.
RESULTS Fish can be compared test periods. was greater
according
to GCED during the 10 minute pre- and post-fight
In 11out of 12 pairs, the display duration than that of the future
subordinate
of the future
pairmember
dominant
(binomial
pairmember
test 2-tailed;
P =
.016) (see Table I). Further, interactions
the group of fish which subsequently
had a greater
group of future although
subordinate
hours after
ing to the test stimulus displaying,
vigourously. ranked before
appear
(Table I). The effect
unexpectedly
z = 3.059,s
than the
= 19, E = .0022)
the two groups.
on dominants
of changes
fish had ceased
was more variable,
lower values than pre-fight
of non-displaying
none of the fin spreading
to correlate
in aggressive
testing
displaythree
and two displayed
if the dominant
more
GCED scores are
the fight experience.
into the apparatus
While pre-fight
test 2-tailed;
the fight I1 of the 12 subordinate
There is no obvious pattern and after
proved to be dominant fish during pre-fight
overlap between
seven produced
The behaviour introduction showed
fish (Mann-Whitney
there was considerable
Twenty-four ceased
display to the stimulus
these
fish in the post-fight fish frequently
or colour changes
oriented
long interactions
is of interest;
toward
of fight outcome,
Some of the least vigourously
when placed with their pairmembers,
on
the stimulus,
which are associated
test data were a good predictor with fight duration.
tests
but
with display. they do not displaying
whereas
fish had
some much
117
more vigourous definitive
displayers
subordinate
capitulated
behaviour
after
a short time.
It was also noted that
was never seen immediately
or mouthlocking,
but that a period of individual
quite ephemeral,
was always the last event in the interaction.
after
or mutual display,
an exchange although
of bites
sometimes
TABLE 1 A Comparison per minute)
of pre- and post-fight for both dominant
scores (mean seconds
GCED
and subordinate
Pre-Fight
pairmembers.
Test
Post-Fight
Subordinate
Dominant
Dominant
Test Subordinate
G
27.5
J
4.6
G
0
J
u
26.7
V
12.3
U
18.5
V
0
F
24.8
D
11.1
F
18.6
D
4.0
C
21.4
E
0
C
10.8
E
0
Q
20.5
R
6.6
Q
0
R
0
K
19.6
L
16.3
x
10.1
L
0
0
X
14.9
W
0
X
6.6
W
0
Z
14.4
Y
2.3
Z
34.1
Y
0
T
12.4
S
2.9
T
7.1
S
0
N
11.2
M
18.6
N
6.5
M
0
P
10.9
0
4.2
P
0
0
0
H
6.5
I
0
H
7.9
I
0
Note.
N and M were deviant
in that the future
subordinate
had the greater
pre-fight
test
score.
DISCUSSION The remarkable in agreement
predictive
et al (1975) using the future that these findings to elicit reasonable by-second
dispiay.
despite
If this information
to propose matching
of pre-fight
by Baeninger
pair members
are similar
Hence the aggressive
as stimuli
considerable
display of Bettas
to some extent as measured
is
and Meliska
it should be noted
in the stimuli employed
by human observers,
it seems
to the fish even in the second1980. pers. comm.).
by GCED would seem to be a candi-
class of signals indicative
holding power (RHP) (Glutton-Brock
for fight outcome
for each other; differences
is so easily garnered
that it is available
test scores
(1968) using mirror stimuli,
of GCE which occurs during a fight (Simpson,
date for the “honest advertisment” resource
accuracy
with the data reported
of fighting
& Albon, 1979; Parker,
ability
or
1974; Maynard-Smith
118
& Parker, species
1976) which now appear
(Davies & Halliday, A few observations
evidence
favouring
“broadside” observed
postures
for information
transfer,
the appearance
interpreted preceded
fleeing
Recent
between similarity although
(Zahavi,
statement
if minimally required
they are direct
effect
Finally,
a confusion
and true “bluffing”
the post-fight
between
than subordinates,
fish obtained
in the present
of the fight experience.
of the varying
low rates of valid,
amounts
it has been suggested of aggressive
1973). This idea has been extended (1975) who have suggested “fish whose aggressive
responding
habituates
to perform”;
high rates of tail
Baeninger
reinforced
and the relative
incurred
might determine
a similar
be interpreted
to the large range of fight by individual
“is the major process
territorial
(1970) has
display rates of domin-
these data cannot
corresponding
1955).
by the presentation
study would seem to reflect
conspecifics”
by Meliska, Meliska, Hoyenga,
that habituation
ability
are often hard to
& Braddock,
test results.
of damage
between
of fighting
and hence costly involving
response
that habituation
behaviour
which is accurate,
used in assessment
However,
levels of fatigue
has
(Hinde, 1981). That is, the
1968; Braddock
an operant
citing the
This approach
with regard to the display of Bettas
trials of strength
to interpret
and
the
that the contest
with the exaggeration
(Simpson,
and the uncontrolled
the reduction
the end of an interaction.
alternations
of a mirror image more often
because
in 25 of the 35 pairs studied.
fish is plainly exhausting,
fish will perform
ant and subordinate
& Braddock
GCE), immediately
both bluff and assessment”
that “displays
or indirect
fighting
It is more difficult
differential
release
they also suggest
shown that dominant
durations,
“involves
should not be confused
and rapid position
may also be
Dawkins & Krebs (1978) have suggested
The Dawkins and Krebs position
the display of Siamese
in part on an
with Braddock
“will stay and can display at least this vigourously”,
because
bluff because beating
and is in agreement
1981) and seems to reflect
informative,
sequence
dealing with the topic of animal signals in general
information
by “bluff”.
is puzzling
depends
in the 12 pairs studied
by one pairmember
in Betta spendens
non-specific,
“facing” and
of roaring
temporal
(an index of display including
of display rates until just before
been criticised
between
(Dawkins and Krebs, 1978; Hinde, 1981) have discussed
of “bluff” or “deceit”. conspecifics
general
articles
in particular
alternate
During the
that display was always the element
behaviour
terms,
and colour changes
to be circumstantial
seem to be a near-optimal
The observation
transfer
of several
in Bettas.
to the alternation
if the “reply” to be emitted
signal.
that “challenging”,
theoretical
displays
possibility
further
the fish sequentially
of subordinate
in information
(1955) who reported
would appear
of display function
1968); this is analogous
especially
of the incoming
preceding
threat
interaction
(Simpson,
behaviour
1982).
interpretation
in red deer, and would similarly
assessment
in the agonistic
from this and other studies
the proposed
course of an aggressive
to be involved
1978; Maynard-Smith,
fish. involved
in
(Peeke & Peeke,
Hoyenga,
fight outcome
at a slow rate should dominate
and Ward in Bettas; fish whose
119
aggressive pointed
responding
habitutes
rapidly”.
out that while habituation
However,
Rhoad, Kalat and Klopfer
is usually defined
in a purely descriptive
(1975) have
manner,
often used as if it were an explanation,
and the Peeke and Peeke hypothesis
fore be “completely
when Meliska and Meliska (1976) compared
fight performance their future opponents
Further,
of pairmembers
opponent
display via visual stimulation
subordinate pre-fight
than an unhabituated
experiences.
with which they were later paired, of this latter isolated.
After
of the nine isolate
fewer between
threat
that there
display and attack commence
biting soon after
(Braddock
& Braddock
mirror group and both members to their opponents
before
be assessed
experience,
by Bettas
ant/subordinate
a third.
sessions”, the isolate
being placed
fish
of dissociation” newly paired Bettas
together,
but rather
display for
1968); since one member
viewing group had observed
together,
only two
they bit less and formed
However,
1955; Simpson,
of the
and displayed
the higher levels of biting and jawlock-
with an information
transfer
from its display behaviour
relationships
in pairs; the mutually
and the isolates
be a “degree
in these groups should not have been unexpected.
are also compatible
or the images
while a fourth group were
note that although
in Bettas.
of the mutually
being placed
Meliska,
fish and six out of seven mirror pairs
may therefore
behaviours
that
to the conspecific
in ten 15 minute “combat
as the fish with pre-fight
they suggest
some time initially
ing observed
viewing
fish pairs did so. The authors
as vigourously
jawlocks;
do not typically
results
either
the fish were placed together
relationships
of their
from pairs of Bettas
side of a one way mirror,
one group, the two mirror groups a second,
While five out of six pairs of mutually
displayed
More recently, obtained
The fish were exposed
124 hours of exposure
dominant/subordinate
image or that of
would make a fish more likely to
animal”.
the fight lengths
the reflecting
the
they found no evidence
group viewed from the other side of the mirror
viewing fish formed formed
to their own mirror
be disadvantaged)
Meliska and Peeke (1980) have compared with different
either
may there-
being that fish with visual experience
and therefore
of aggressive
socially
exposed
(the expectation
would habituate
“habituation become
circular”.
it is
view.
Interestingly,
these
If an opponent’s
then the rapid resolution
RHP can
of domin-
in the two groups with 124 hours of mutual or one-member
viewing would be predicted. In summary,
the aggressive
display of Bettas
tion about RHP, and since this is, in some cases, explanation
of the data, accounts
that habituation
is of primary
of agonistic
importance
seems to contain
an alternative
accurate
informa-
(and more parsimonious)
behaviour
in this species
in determining
fight outcome
which suggest may need to be
re-examined. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author for providing ment.
Pilot studies
the materials
would like to thank the Zoology Department
space and facilities, were conducted
of Cambridge
University
and Dr. M. J. A. Simpson for his help and encouragewith S. C. Savill who helped design the apparatus,
for which were provided
by J. J. Clark.
Dr. S. J. Gaioni, J. C. Hanson and
120
an anonymous
referee
all made suggestions
which improved
earlier
versions
of the manu-
script.
REFERENCES Baeninger, R., 1970. Catechol amines and social relations in Siamese fighting fish. Anim. Behav., 16: 442-447. Baeninger, R., 1970. Visual reinforcement, habituation and prior social experience of Siamese fighting fish. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 71: l-5. Bols, R. J., 1977. Display reinforcement in the Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens: Aggressive motivation or curiosity? 3. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 91(2): 233-244. Braddock, J. C., and Braddock, 2. I., 1955. Aggressive behavior among females of the Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Physiol. Zool., 28: 152-172. Bronstein, P. M., 1983. Onset of combat in male Betta splendens. J. Comp. Psychol. 97(2): 135-139. Clayton, F. L. and Hinde, R. A., 1968. The habituation and recovery of aggressive displays in Betta splendens. Behaviour, 30: 96-106. Clutton-Brock, T. H. and Albon, S. D., 1979. The roaring of red deer and the evolution of honest advertisement. Behaviour, 69: 145-170. Clutton-Brock, T. H., Albon, S. D., Gibson, R. M. and Guinness, F. E., 1979. The logical stag: Adaptive aspects of fighting in red deer (Cervus elaphus 1.1. Anim. Behav., 27: 211-225. Davies, N. 8. and Halliday, T. R., 1978. Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature, 274: 683-685. Dawkins, R. and Krebs, J. R., 1978. Animal signals: Information or manipulation? In: J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Editors), Behavioural Ecology. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, England, pp. 282-309. Figler, M. H., 1972. The relationship between eliciting stimulus strength and the habituation of the threat display in the male Siamese fighting fish Betta splendens. Behaviour, 42: 63-96. Forselius, S., 1957. Studies of anabantid fishes. I. 2001. Bidrag Fran Uppsala, 32: 93-301. approraches are not Hinde, R. A., 1981. Animal signals: Ethological and games-theory incompatible. Anim. Behav., 29: 535-542. Hogan, J. A., 1968. Fighting and reinforcement in the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). 3. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 64: 356-359. Hogan, J. A. and Bols, R. J., 1980. Priming of aggressive motivation in Betta splendens. Anim. Behav., 28: 135-142. Hogan, J. A. and Roper, T. J., 1978. A comparison of the properties of different reinforcers. Adv. St. Behav., 8: 155-255. Klein, R. M., Figler, M. H. and Peeke, H. V. S., 1976. Modification of consummatory (attack) behavior resulting from prior habituation of appetitive (threat) components of the agonistic sequence in male Betta splendens (Pisces, Belontiidae). Behaviour, 58: l-25. Maynard-Smith, J., 1982. Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England. Maynard-Smith, J. and Parker, G. A., 1976. The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim. Behav., 24: 159-175. Meliska, J, A. and Meliska, C. J., 1976. Effects of habituation on threat display and dominance establishment in the Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Anim. Learn. Behav., 4(2): 167-171. Meliska, C. J., Meliska, J. A. and Peeke, H. V. S., 1980. Threat displays and combat aggression in Betta splendens following visual exposure to conspecifics and one-way mirrors. Behav. Neur. Biol.. 28: 473-480. Meliska, J. A., Meliska, C. J., Hoyenga, K. T., Hoyenga, K. B. and Ward, E. F., 1975. Approach tendency and threat display as related to social status of Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Anim. Learn. Behav., 3(2): 135-139. Parker, G. A., 1974. Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. J. Theor. Biol., 47: 223-243. Peeke, H. V. S. and Peeke, S. C., 1970. Habituation of conspecific aggressive responses in
121
the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Behaviour, 36: 232-245. H. V. S. and Peeke, S. C., 1973. Habituation in fish with special reference to intraspecific aggressive behavior. In: H. V. S. Peeke and M. 5. Herz (Editors), Habituation 1: -behavioral Studies. Academic Press, New York. Lobb, M. L. and McCain, G., 1976. Procedurally related differences in the aggressive behavior of Betta splendens (Regan). Anim. Learn. Behav., 4(4): 367-373. Rhoad, K. D., Kalat, J. W. and Klopfer, P. H., 1975. Aggression and avoidance by Betta splendens toward natural and artificial stimuli. Anim. Learn. Behav., 3(3): 271-276. Simpsdn, M. 3. A., 1966. An experimental analysis of the threat display of -the Siamese fighting fish Betta splendens. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Cambridge University, England. Simpson, M. J. A., 1968. The display of the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Anim. Behav. Monog., 1: l-73. Thompson, T. I., 1963. Visual reinforcement in Siamese fighting fish. Science, 141: 55-57. Thompson, T. I. and Sturm, T., 1965. Visual reinforcer colour and operant behavior in Siamese fighting fish. J. Exp. Anal. Behav., 8: 341-366. Behav. Zahavi, A., 1981. The lateral display of fishes: Bluff or honesty in Signalling? Anal. Letters, 1: 233-235.
Peeke,