Does In Vivo Catastrophizing Engage Descending Modulation of Spinal Nociception?

Does In Vivo Catastrophizing Engage Descending Modulation of Spinal Nociception?

The Journal of Pain, Vol 8, No 4 (April), 2007: pp 325-333 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Does In Vivo Catastrophizing Engage Descending M...

137KB Sizes 0 Downloads 73 Views

The Journal of Pain, Vol 8, No 4 (April), 2007: pp 325-333 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

Does In Vivo Catastrophizing Engage Descending Modulation of Spinal Nociception? Jamie L. Rhudy, Lauren J. Maynard, and Jennifer L. Russell Department of Psychology, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, Oklahoma.

Abstract: Prior research has found that pain catastrophizing measured before pain testing is not correlated with the nociceptive flexion reflex (NFR) threshold (a measure of spinal nociception), suggesting that catastrophizing does not alter pain through descending modulatory mechanisms. However, recent evidence suggests that in vivo catastrophizing (measured during or after pain testing) is a better predictor of pain outcomes. In the present study, NFR threshold and pain sensation ratings were assessed in 78 healthy participants by delivering electric stimulations to the sural nerve. After pain testing, participants were asked to rate their affective reaction (displeasure, arousal) to electric stimuli and to report on their pain catastrophizing. Hierarchical regression analyses controlling for participant sex, pre-experiment affect, depressive symptoms, and self-efficacy were used to predict pain-related outcomes (NFR threshold, pain sensation, displeasure ratings, arousal ratings) from in vivo catastrophizing scores. Results indicated that in vivo catastrophizing was related to pain sensation but not to NFR thresholds or arousal reactions. The relation between in vivo catastrophizing and displeasure ratings was not significant after other variables were controlled. These data support prior research suggesting that catastrophizing does not alter pain by engaging descending modulatory mechanisms. Perspective: Pain catastrophizing is an important psychological predictor of pain and pain-related functioning. The present study confirms prior reports suggesting that catastrophizing does not work by engaging mechanisms that alter pain transmission in the spinal cord before the signal travels to the brain. © 2007 by the American Pain Society Key words: Coping, nociceptive flexion reflex, pain perception, shock, nociception, descending pain modulation.

P

ain catastrophizing has emerged as one of the strongest predictors of pain-related outcomes, showing positive associations with pain report, pain behaviors, analgesic use, length of hospital stay, length of rehabilitation, pain-related disability, and supraspinal responses to noxious stimulation.19,26,49,52 In addition, interventions aimed at reducing catastrophizing have been found to improve pain.22,24,25 Accordingly, it is believed that catastrophizing enhances pain perception. Received June 14, 2006; Revised September 18, 2006; Accepted October 1, 2006. This work was partially supported by a Faculty Development Summer Fellowship from The University of Tulsa awarded to Jamie L. Rhudy. Presented in part at the 2006 American Pain Society conference held in San Antonio, Texas. Address reprint requests to Dr. Jamie L. Rhudy, The University of Tulsa, Department of Psychology, 600 South College, Tulsa, OK 74104. E-mail: [email protected] 1526-5900/$32.00 © 2007 by the American Pain Society doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2006.10.003

Several psychosocial models (for example, coping, appraisal, attentional, schema-activation, communal/interpersonal) have been used to conceptualize the catastrophizing-pain relationship52; yet, little is known about the mediating physiological mechanisms. One potential mechanism is supraspinal initiation of descending central nervous system (CNS) circuitry that can inhibit or facilitate ascending nociception within the spinal cord.11,52 Catastrophizing could activate this circuitry, leading to increased nociceptive facilitation, decreased nociceptive inhibition (disinhibition), or both. Moreover, repeated or tonic activation of such mechanisms could result in chronic sensitization of central nociceptive pathways, lowered pain thresholds, and hyperalgesia.9,20,33 Indeed, such processes could provide a mechanism through which catastrophizing promotes the development of chronic pain.16,37 If catastrophizing exerts its influence at the spinal level, then markers of spinal nociception should be enhanced. Alternatively, cata325

326 strophizing may simply bias pain report/behavior or engage cortical mechanisms that facilitate supraspinal but not spinal nociception.38 Two recent studies by France and colleagues,14,15 using the nociceptive flexion reflex (NFR), have failed to establish a link between catastrophizing and enhanced spinal nociception. The NFR is a spinal reflex resulting from nociceptor activation,48,50,59 and the level of stimulation that elicits the NFR (NFR threshold) is used as a measure of spinal nociceptive sensitivity. Importantly, enhanced NFR is associated with enhanced subjective pain4,43,48,61 and chronic pain.1,35,47 Thus, if catastrophizing activates descending modulation to facilitate spinal nociception, then greater pain catastrophizing should be associated with lower NFR threshold (enhanced sensitivity). In France and colleagues’ studies, however, catastrophizing did not correlate with NFR threshold despite significant correlations with supraspinally mediated outcomes (for example, subjective pain, pain threshold, pain tolerance).14,15 Therefore, pain catastrophizing does not appear to activate processes that alter spinal nociception. Recent research suggests that the context in which catastrophizing is measured may influence the catastrophizing-pain relation.6,10 Most studies, including those mentioned above,14,15 assessed catastrophizing by asking participants to report on their catastrophic thoughts before pain testing. However, catastrophizing assessed before pain testing does not predict catastrophizing measured during or after a painful situation (that is, in vivo catastrophizing).6 Moreover, in vivo catastrophizing correlates more strongly with experimental pain outcomes.6,10 Thus, assessment of catastrophizing during or after participants have experienced a common painful event may provide greater accuracy in the report of catastrophic emotions/cognitions associated with pain coping, especially in persons without chronic pain (for example, healthy college students).6 Given this, it remains unclear whether in vivo catastrophizing activates descending modulation and would therefore correlate with NFR threshold. The present study assessed NFR threshold, pain sensation ratings, and affective pain reactions (displeasure, arousal), after which in vivo pain catastrophizing was assessed. Regression analyses were used to predict painrelated outcomes from catastrophizing after controlling for variables that could influence or confound the catastrophizing-pain relation. Participant sex was controlled because of known sex differences in pain and catastrophizing,6,8 depressive symptoms and pre-experiment affect were controlled because of potential confounds between catastrophizing and negative affectivity,18,23,51,52 and self-efficacy was controlled to account for efficacy beliefs.46,52,55 Given that in vivo catastrophizing may better assess pain-related catastrophic thoughts, it was predicted that in vivo catastrophizing would be positively correlated with subjective ratings of noxious electric stimulation (pain sensation, displeasure, arousal) and negatively correlated with NFR threshold, over and above control variables.

In Vivo Catastrophizing, Pain, and NFR

Materials and Methods Data for the present study were collected during 2 recent experiments that examined the influence of emotion on the NFR.43,44 Only those methods directly relevant to the present study will be described.

Participants Participants were 90 healthy male (28%) and female students who received course credit for their participation. Most were white non-Hispanic (68%), single (87%), and employed (59%), with an average age of 22.06 (SD ⫽ 6.22). Participants were excluded for age ⬍18 years, cardiovascular, neurological, and/or circulatory problems, recent use of analgesic, antidepressant, or antihypertensive medication, recent psychological trauma, specific phobia of snakes or spiders (due to pictures of spiders and snakes presented during picture-viewing, see “Procedure”), problems healing, Raynaud disease, or any medical problem exacerbated by stress. Twelve participants reached pain tolerance before NFR threshold was achieved and discontinued participation. Therefore, a final sample of 78 participants is included in the present analyses. All procedures were fully approved by The University of Tulsa Ethics Review Board, and participants gave informed consent before participating.

Apparatus All data acquisition was computer-controlled, using a PC equipped with dual monitors, A/D board (National Instruments, PCI-6036E, Austin, TX), and LabVIEW software. Computerized versions of questionnaires were presented by using LabVIEW that automatically saved data to spreadsheets. Noxious electrical stimuli were delivered by means of a Grass Instruments stimulator (Model S88, West Warwick, RI), stimulus isolation unit (Model SIU8T), constant-current unit (Model CCU1), and bipolar stimulating electrode (Nicolet, 019-401400, Madison, WI). The onset/offset of the stimulator was controlled by computer, and a computer-controlled voltage regulator varied the current to the participant (maximum current ⫽ 40 mA). Psychophysiological signals were sampled at 1000 Hz and collected/filtered with Grass Instruments Model 15LT Bipolar Amplifier with Quad AC (15A54) and Dual DC (15A12) modules. Experimenters monitored participants by video from an adjacent room.

Nociceptive Flexion Reflex Threshold The NFR is a spinally mediated, protective reflex elicited primarily by A␦-fiber activation, although C fibers also contribute.48,50,59 The stimulation intensity that reliably elicits the NFR (that is, NFR threshold) is correlated with pain threshold (r ⫽ 0.70),21,60 and the magnitude of the NFR correlates with subjective pain intensity (r ⫽ 0.84).4,43 Thus, the NFR is used as a measure of spinal nociception.48,50 Although NFR threshold is near pain threshold on average, there is significant individual variability in the subjective evaluation of the stimulus level associated with NFR threshold.14 This is not surprising, given the multiple modulatory influences at varying levels of the neuraxis that can inhibit

ORIGINAL REPORT/Rhudy et al or facilitate nociception before it is registered in consciousness as pain.11,32,58 To elicit the NFR, a bipolar surface stimulating electrode was attached to the left ankle over the retromalleolar pathway of the sural nerve. Two active Ag-AgCl electrodes were placed over the left biceps femoris muscle 10 cm superior to the popliteal fossa to record biceps femoris electromyogram (EMG) associated with the NFR. A common ground electrode was placed over the left lateral epicondyle of the femur. To facilitate relaxation of the leg muscles during the experiment, participants were seated in a recliner with a small pillow positioned under their ankle. For this procedure, reflex detection was defined as a mean biceps femoris EMG response in the 90- to 150-ms post-stimulus interval that exceeded mean EMG activity during the 60-ms pre-stimulus baseline interval by at least 1.0 standard deviation. Using the 90- to 150-ms time frame avoids potential contamination by the non-nociceptive RII reflex, startle responses, and/or voluntary movements.7 The stimulus intensity necessary to reliably elicit the NFR (that is, NFR threshold) was assessed according to procedures adapted from France and colleagues.14 Electric stimulation of the sural nerve was repeated according to a variable interval of 8 to 12 seconds to prevent habituation and predictability. For each stimulation, 5 rectangular wave pulses of 1-ms duration and 3-ms interstimulus interval were delivered. The trial began with 0 mA (current) and increased in 1.5 mA steps until an NFR was detected. Stimulus intensity was then decreased in 0.75-mA steps until a reflex was no longer detected. This up-down staircase procedure was repeated 2 more times, but with 0.5-mA steps. The average stimulus intensity (in mA) of the last 2 peaks and troughs was used to define NFR threshold. Participants for which a reflex could not be reliably obtained were given the choice of repeating the threshold assessment procedure or discontinuing the experiment. The raw biceps femoris EMG signal was amplified by 20,000, and frequencies below 10 Hz and above 300 Hz were filtered. The signal was rectified and reduced online after each stimulation.

327 subscale measures negative/catastrophic thoughts and ideations about pain and has been shown to have good internal and test-retest reliabilities.46

Affect Ratings Pre-experiment affect and affective responses to electric stimulations were rated with the use of a computerized version of the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM).29 The SAM consists of 2 sets of 5 pictographs depicting affective valence (unpleasant-pleasant) and arousal (calm-excited). The SAM has been shown to have good validity, with correlations between SAM valence and arousal ratings and the multi-item Semantic Differential scale being r ⫽ 0.96 and r ⫽ 0.95, respectively.29 Further, SAM ratings have been shown to be reliable, with coefficients for valence and arousal being r ⫽ 0.94 and r ⫽ 0.94, respectively.28 Participants dragged an indicator on or between the 5 pictographs for each scale and submitted their answers by computer mouse. Ratings ranged from 1 to 9 for each dimension. The valence dimension was reversescored so that higher scores reflected greater negative affect (displeasure). To rate pre-experiment affect, instructions were: “Please rate how you feel right now . . .”. To rate responses to electric stimuli, instructions were: “Please rate how the electrical stimulations made you feel . . .”.

Center for Epidemiological StudiesDepression Scale The Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D)40 is a 20-item self-report measure of depressive symptomatology. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale indicating the degree to which the respondent has experienced each symptom over the past 2 weeks. The CES-D has been shown to have good internal consistency and test-retest reliability and adequate discriminative and convergent validity.40

Self-Efficacy for Pain Reduction

A previously validated computer-presented numerical rating scale was used to rate each electric stimulation.14 Starting from the bottom, the scale was labeled 0 (no sensation), 1 (just noticeable), 25 (uncomfortable), 50 (pain), 75 (very painful), and 100 (maximum tolerable). To respond, participants dragged an indicator along the scale and submitted their answers by computer mouse. Average ratings from the last 2 stimuli during the NFR threshold assessment were used as a measure of pain sensation.

The 5-item Self-Efficacy for Pain Reduction (SEPR) was used to measure participants’ beliefs that they could make reductions in different levels of pain (mild, discomforting, distressing, horrible, excruciating) without taking medications.41 Ratings were made on 11-point Likert scales ranging from 0 (uncertain) to 10 (certain). Items were summed to achieve a score ranging from 0 to 50, with higher scores indicating greater self-efficacy. Research has suggested a 1-day test-retest reliability ranging from 0.45 to 0.70, and correlation with cold pressor pain tolerance was r ⫽ 0.48, suggesting adequate validity (Rhudy JL, Dubbert PM, Parker JD, Burke RS, Williams AE, Affective modulation of pain in substance dependent veterans, Pain Med [in press]). Internal consistency in the present study was ␣ ⫽ 0.81.

In Vivo Catastrophizing

Procedure

The 6-item subscale of the Coping Strategies Questionnaire (CSQ) was used to assess in vivo catastrophizing after all exposure to noxious electric stimulations. This

Participants were provided an overview of the experiment, informed consent was obtained, and all electrodes were then applied. Before pain testing, participants

Self-Report Measures Pain Sensation Rating Scale

328 filled out demographics, depressive symptoms, preexperiment affect, and self-efficacy questionnaires. There were 2 phases to the original experiments: NFR threshold assessment and emotional picture-viewing. Phase 1 involved sending electric stimuli to the ankle to determine the stimulation intensity that reliably elicited the NFR (NFR threshold). During this phase, participants viewed the computerized pain rating scale positioned next to a digital light that illuminated when they were to make a rating (after every electric stimulation). Phase 2 involved viewing 24 emotionally charged pictures, during which electric stimuli were randomly delivered. During this phase, 8 pleasant (couples in erotic poses), 8 neutral (household objects, mushrooms), and 8 unpleasant (human and animal attack scenes) pictures were each presented for 6 seconds (with varying 12- to 22-second interpicture interval). Presentation order was randomized across participants with the limitation that not more than 2 pictures of similar valence were shown consecutively. This picture-viewing paradigm was used to examine the influence of emotional valence (pleasant, neutral, unpleasant) on NFR magnitude and pain ratings resulting from noxious electric stimuli delivered during pictures. Data from phase 2 are reported elsewhere and thus will not be discussed further.43,44 Once phase 2 was concluded, the participants completed the pain catastrophizing measure, rated their affective reactions to electric stimulations (displeasure and arousal), were debriefed, and were thanked for their participation. It was decided to measure pain catastrophizing after phase 2 to keep from influencing coping responses (through priming, suggestion, and so forth) during phase 2 pain testing. Thus, these ratings were taken 15 to 20 minutes after the NFR threshold testing procedure.

Data Analysis To examine the relations between in vivo catastrophizing and nociceptive reactions, 4 hierarchical regression analyses were conducted that predicted NFR threshold, pain sensation ratings, displeasure ratings, and arousal ratings. Analyses controlled for participant sex because of the well-established sex differences in pain,6,8,12,45 whereas depressive symptoms, self-efficacy, and preexperimental affect were controlled to ensure that these factors did not mediate the relation between catastrophizing and pain outcomes.52 Therefore, participant sex, depressive symptoms (CES-D), self-efficacy (SEPR), and pre-experiment affect (SAM displeasure and arousal ratings) were entered into step 1, and in vivo catastrophizing scores were entered into step 2. In the 3 models predicting subjective reactions to electric stimuli (pain, displeasure, and arousal ratings), NFR threshold (stimulus intensity level) was also entered into step 1 to control for individual differences in stimulus level. All assumptions for regression were met.

Results Preliminary analyses indicated that participants who discontinued the experiment were not significantly

In Vivo Catastrophizing, Pain, and NFR different from completers on self-efficacy ratings, depressive symptoms, or pre-experiment affect ratings (P ⬎ .05). Table 1 provides descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations for all variables in this study. Table 2 depicts results of all 4 regression models. The regression model predicting NFR threshold was not significant [F(6, 71) ⫽ 0.372, P ⫽ .894], nor was the zero-order correlation or the ␤-weight for in vivo catastrophizing. Moreover, no other predictor was significantly related to NFR threshold in this sample. This suggests that in vivo catastrophizing is not related to spinal nociception. The regression model predicting pain sensation ratings was significant [F(7, 70) ⫽ 2.83, P ⫽ .01], and in vivo catastrophizing predicted pain sensation ratings over and above control variables (R2 change ⫽ 0.05, F change ⫽ 4.46, P ⫽ .038). This suggests that in vivo catastrophizing was related to the subjective evaluation of noxious electric stimuli. Stimulus level (NFR threshold) and selfefficacy were also significant predictors in the model, predicting 8% and 7% unique variance in pain sensation ratings, respectively. This suggests that persons with higher NFR thresholds also tended to have higher ratings of the threshold-level stimulus and that increased selfefficacy was associated with lower pain ratings. The regression model predicting affective displeasure reactions to electric stimulations was significant [F(7, 70) ⫽ 2.49, P ⫽ .024]. Although the zero-order correlation between catastrophizing and displeasure ratings was significant, after controlling for other variables in the regression model, in vivo catastrophizing was not significant (R2 change ⫽ 0.04, F change ⫽ 3.74, P ⫽ .057). Stimulus level (NFR threshold) was a significant predictor in the model (P ⫽ .018), explaining 7% unique variance in ratings of displeasure. Again, persons with higher NFR thresholds also tended to have higher ratings of the threshold-level stimulus. Self-efficacy approached significance as a unique predictor (P ⫽ .072) and explained 4% of the variance in displeasure ratings. The regression model predicting affective arousal reactions to electric stimulations was not significant [F(7, 70) ⫽ 1.16, P ⫽ .335], nor was the zero-order correlation or the ␤-weight for in vivo catastrophizing.

Discussion The present study was the first, to our knowledge, to examine the relation between in vivo catastrophizing (measured after pain testing) and NFR threshold, pain sensation ratings, and affective pain reactions (displeasure, arousal). Results suggested that in vivo catastrophizing was related to subjective pain but not to NFR threshold or subjective arousal reactions. The zero-order correlation between in vivo catastrophizing and affective displeasure was positive and significant, but the relation was not significant when other variables were controlled by multiple regression. We did not specifically test for mediation in our models2; however, we did control for pre-experiment affect, depressive symptoms, and self-efficacy and still found a significant relation be-

ORIGINAL REPORT/Rhudy et al Table 1.

329

Descriptive Statistics for Variables in Regression Models CORRELATION MATRIX (PEARSON R)

VARIABLE

MEAN

SD

OBSERVED RANGE

1

1. In vivo catastrophizing (0–36) 2. NFR threshold (0–40 mA) 3. Pain sensation rating (0–100) 4. Displeasure rating of electric stimuli (1–9) 5. Arousal rating of electric stimuli (1–9) 6. Depressive symptoms (0–60) 7. Self-efficacy (0–50) 8. Pre-experiment displeasure (1–9) 9. Pre-experiment arousal (1–9) 10. Participant sex (1 ⫽ female)

8.21

5.65

0–25



7.33

5.25

2–26

0.05



50.24

25.08

4–99

0.28*

0.26*

7.38

1.47

4–9

0.30*

0.25*

0.41*



6.77

2.03

1–9

0.05

0.21

0.21

0.48*

12.83

8.83

0–45

0.36*

0.03

0.08

0.14

0.01

23.90 3.90

8.72 1.44

3–50 1–7

⫺0.24* ⫺0.20 0.01 0.00

0.09 ⫺0.04

3.91

2.31

1–9

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

0.04 0.10

– ⫺0.26*



9



– –

⫺0.09 ⫺0.06

0.08 0.08

0.10

⫺0.06

0.03

0.05

0.20

0.20

0.11

⫺0.01



0.09

0.05

⫺0.05

0.15

⫺0.10

⫺0.07

⫺0.17

⫺0.05

⫺0.04

Abbreviation: NFR, nociceptive flexion reflex. NOTE. Actual ranges of the scales are listed in parentheses next to the variable labels. *Two-tailed P ⬍ .05.

tween in vivo catastrophizing and pain sensation ratings. Thus, these data are consistent with other research that suggests the effect of catastrophizing on pain is not due solely to the mediating effects of depressive symptoms or negative affect19,23,53 and extends this work to suggest that self-efficacy does not fully explain the relation. Although the majority of studies examining the influence of pain catastrophizing have been in clinical populations, experimental studies have noted a positive association between catastrophizing and reactions to noxious stimuli including pain threshold, pain tolerance, temporal summation, and subjective reports.13-15,17,20 In an attempt to elucidate the mechanisms mediating this relation, the present study used the NFR—a measure of spinal nociception. Prior studies have found that the NFR and subjective pain are modulated in parallel by psychological factors such as emotion,43,44 stress,62 attention,61 and hypnotic suggestion.27 Ostensibly, psychological (supraspinal) factors influence the NFR through circuitry that includes the periaqueductal gray (PAG), rostroventral medulla (RVM), dorsolateral pons/tegmentum (DLPT), and descending projections through the dorsolateral funiculus—a modulatory circuit with numerous connections to brain centers involved with cognition and emotion.11,42,57 It is assumed that this spinal gating mechanism alters the ascending flow of nociception before registration at supraspinal centers associated with pain perception. At least part of the variability in subjective pain would then reflect alterations in spinal nociception (as indexed by the NFR), hence accounting for the covariation between these measures. We reasoned that

if supraspinal processes associated with catastrophic thinking modulate subjective pain by activating descending circuitry, the NFR would covary with catastrophizing (that is, greater catastrophizing equals enhanced NFR). This should be true whether pain catastrophizing reflects a trait-like disposition that results in tonic activation of descending processes, or whether it results in state-like activation during, or in anticipation of, pain.52 However, our study corroborates prior work suggesting that descending modulation does not mediate the catastrophizing-pain relation because the NFR did not covary with in vivo pain catastrophizing.14,15 As France and colleagues14 point out, though, “a failure to reject the null hypothesis can never establish that such a relationship does not exist. . .” (p. 463). Nevertheless, this is the third empirical study suggesting that catastrophizing does not significantly correlate with NFR threshold. Moreover, the effect size for the relation is consistently small across studies regardless of the method of catastrophizing assessment. In the present study, in vivo catastrophizing explained 0.3% of the variance in NFR thresholds, an effect size similar to that found in a sample of osteoarthritis patients when catastrophizing was measured before pain testing (0.4%).15 Accordingly, our findings are not likely to stem exclusively from low power or characteristics specific to our sample. Together, these data suggest that catastrophizing modulates pain by mechanisms other than descending controls that alter spinal nociception. It is possible that

330

In Vivo Catastrophizing, Pain, and NFR

Table 2. ␤-Weights, Semi-Partial Correlations (sr ), and Inferential Statistics for Final Regression Models Predicting Pain-Related Outcomes 2

CRITERION (MODEL R2) NFR threshold (R2 ⫽ 0.03)

Pain sensation ratings (R2 ⫽ 0.26)

Displeasure ratings (R2 ⫽ 0.20)

Arousal ratings (R2 ⫽ 0.10)



PREDICTORS

SR

2

Sex (0 ⫽ male, 1 ⫽ female) Depressive symptoms Self-efficacy Pre-exp displeasure rating Pre-exp arousal rating In vivo catastrophizing

0.074 0.010 0.132 0.115 ⫺0.078 0.063

Sex (0 ⫽ male, 1 ⫽ female) Depressive symptoms Self-efficacy Pre-exp displeasure rating Pre-exp arousal rating NFR threshold In vivo catastrophizing

⫺0.138 ⫺0.016 ⫺0.290† ⫺0.074 0.056 0.289‡ 0.245†

0.018 0.000 0.072† 0.005 0.003 0.081‡ 0.050†

Sex (0 ⫽ male, 1 ⫽ female) Depressive symptoms Self-efficacy Pre-exp displeasure rating Pre-exp arousal rating NFR threshold In vivo catastrophizing

0.078 0.058 ⫺0.211* ⫺0.062 0.059 0.262† 0.227*

0.006 0.003 0.038* 0.003 0.003 0.067† 0.043*

Sex (0 ⫽ male, 1 ⫽ female) Depressive symptoms Self-efficacy Pre-exp displeasure rating Pre-exp arousal rating NFR threshold In vivo catastrophizing

⫺0.110 ⫺0.067 0.031 ⫺0.037 0.208* 0.224* 0.051

0.011 0.004 0.001 0.001 0.040* 0.049* 0.002

0.005 ⬍0.001 0.015 0.012 0.006 0.003

Abbreviations: NFR, nociceptive flexion reflex; Pre-exp, pre-experiment ratings. sr2 provides the unique shared variance between a predictor and the criterion. *P ⬍ .10. †P ⬍ .05. ‡P ⬍ .01.

catastrophic thinking simply biases pain report/behavior without altering the nociceptive signal. However, recent imaging research suggests catastrophizing during painful stimulation is correlated with activation of brain regions associated with attention to, and anticipation of, pain.19,49 Therefore, catastrophizing may engage cortico-cortical mechanisms that enhance (or remove inhibition of) nociception at the supraspinal level rather than within the spinal cord. Future research should attempt to experimentally manipulate catastrophic thinking while imaging supraspinal reactions to noxious stimulation to help delineate potential brain circuitry associated with pain catastrophizing. The present study is not without its limitations, however. First, instructions for the pain catastrophizing subscale were not altered to specifically ask participants to reflect on their catastrophic thoughts during pain testing. Rather, we assumed that pain testing would provide a salient and recent experience on which participants could base their self-report. Therefore, it remains possi-

ble that catastrophizing scores would have significantly correlated with NFR thresholds had we altered the instructions, or had we assessed catastrophizing during pain testing. Second, the method of catastrophizing assessment (pre-test vs post-test) was not experimentally manipulated; thus, our design provided a weaker test of the influence of in vivo catastrophizing because we were unable to compare the 2 methods.54 Nonetheless, recent studies that did measure catastrophizing before and after pain testing suggests that pre-test and post-test assessments are poorly correlated and that post-test ratings are more strongly correlated with pain outcomes.6,10 Therefore, our study is likely to provide important insights into the relation of in vivo catastrophizing and spinal nociception. The third limitation stems from our measure of spinal nociception. The NFR is an indirect measure of spinal nociception assessed from muscle activity corresponding to a nociception-initiated withdrawal reflex.48,50,59 As such, the NFR may not provide a complete picture of the complex processes associated with nociceptive process-

ORIGINAL REPORT/Rhudy et al

331

ing in the spinal cord. Indeed, numerous factors could influence motoric output of the NFR without influencing nociceptive transmission. To address these shortcomings, future research is needed that provides corroborating evidence from other measures of spinal nociception (for example, fMRI of dorsal horn activity). Finally, methodology associated with the larger studies from which these data were drawn may have influenced our outcomes. These data came from 2 studies that examined the influence of emotional picture-viewing (phase 2) on nociception and pain. During pictureviewing, participants were exposed to 24 pictures varying in emotional content (for example, couples in sexual acts, animals attacking, household objects). NFR threshold and pain sensation ratings were assessed before picture-viewing, whereas pain catastrophizing and affective reactions to electric stimulation (displeasure, arousal) were measured after. Therefore, emotion-induction may have biased memory for, or the reporting of, catastrophizing and pain-related affective reactions.31,34,39 There are reasons to believe picture-viewing did not confound their measurement, however. Biases caused by emotional valence (pleasant vs unpleasant) should be washed out because there were equal numbers of pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral pictures presented, and all pictures were viewed by every participant. The order of picture presentation was randomized between subjects, so any effects resulting from the first or last picture (primacy or recency effects, respectively) should be washed out. Further, each picture was presented only briefly (6 seconds), and studies have shown that there is very little emotional carry-over effects after picture offset.3 Moreover, any bias caused by emotion-induction should have similarly affected the report of catastrophizing and affective reactions (given they were both measured after picture-viewing). Yet, the association between these variables was nonsignificant in the regression models. Nonetheless, it remains possible that emotion-induction had some effect on the measurement of catastrophizing and affective reactions to electric stimulation.

It is important that future research determine whether these findings replicate in clinical pain populations. Although 1 study has suggested that these results should generalize,15 a number of psychological and physiological changes occur in the development of chronic pain that could alter the catastrophizing-pain relation. For example, as pain becomes intractable, negative coping may increase, leading to greater and more frequent catastrophizing. The range of catastrophizing scores was large in our no-pain sample; however, clinical samples evidence higher mean catastrophizing and a wider range of scores.5,30,46 Catastrophizing may have a greater impact on pain and nociception at the upper tail of the catastrophizing distribution. Further, evidence suggests that persistent pain can be associated with changes in central nociceptive processing.36,56,63 These changes could modify the CNS such that neural mechanisms associated with catastrophizing gain access to descending circuitry, perhaps through deactivation of opioid receptors (given their importance in descending circuitry).11,13,64 In sum, this study suggests that in vivo pain catastrophizing is positively associated with subjective pain, even after controlling for depressive symptoms, current affect, self-efficacy, and participant sex. However, in vivo catastrophizing does not amplify the pain experience through descending modulation because catastrophizing scores did not correlate with the NFR (a measure of spinal nociception). Therefore, psychological interventions for the management of pain that target catastrophic thoughts are not likely to influence ascending nociception but rather supraspinal nociception or its interpretation.

References

5. Crisson JE, Keefe FJ: The relationship of locus of control to pain coping strategies and psychological distress in chronic pain patients. Pain 35:147-154, 1988

1. Banic B, Petersen-Felix S, Andersen OK, Radanov BP, Villiger PM, Arendt-Nielsen L, Curatolo M: Evidence for spinal cord hypersensitivity in chronic pain after whiplash injury and in fibromyalgia. Pain 107:7-15, 2004 2. Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51: 1173-1182, 1986 3. Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ: Pictures as prepulse: Attention and emotion in startle modification. Psychophysiol 30:541-545, 1993 4. Chan CW, Dallaire M: Subjective pain sensation is linearly correlated with the flexion reflex in man. Brain Res 479:145150, 1989

Acknowledgments The authors thank Amy E. Williams, Klanci M. McCabe, Philip Rambo, and Mary Nguyen for help with data collection and 3 anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

6. Dixon KE, Thorn BE, Ward LC: An evaluation of sex differences in psychological and physiological responses to experimentally-induced pain: A path analytic description. Pain 112:188-196, 2004 7. Dowman R: Possible startle response contamination of the spinal nociceptive withdrawal reflex. Pain 49:187-197, 1992 8. Edwards RR, Haythornthwaite JA, Sullivan MJ, Fillingim RB: Catastrophizing as a mediator of sex differences in pain: differential effects for daily pain versus laboratory-induced pain. Pain 111:335-341, 2004 9. Edwards RR, Fillingim RB, Maixner W, Sigurdsson A, Haythornthwaite J: Catastrophizing predicts changes in

332

In Vivo Catastrophizing, Pain, and NFR

thermal pain responses after resolution of acute dental pain. J Pain 5:164-170, 2004

osteoarthritic knee pain, II: Follow-up results. Behavior Therapy 21:435-447, 1990

10. Edwards RR, Campbell CM, Fillingim RB: Catastrophizing and experimental pain sensitivity: Only in vivo reports of Catastrophic cognitions correlate with pain responses. J Pain 6:338-339, 2005

26. Keefe FJ, Rumble ME, Scipio CD, Giordano LA, Perri LM: Psychological aspects of persistent pain: Current state of the science. J Pain 5:195-211, 2004

11. Fields HL, Basbaum AI: Central nervous system mechanisms of pain modulation, in Wall PD, Melzack R (eds): Textbook of Pain. Edinburgh, UK, Churchill Livingston, 1999, pp 309-329

27. Kiernan BD, Dane JR, Phillips LH, Price DD: Hypnotic analgesia reduces R-III nociceptive reflex: Further evidence concerning the multifactorial nature of hypnotic analgesia. Pain 60:39-47, 1995

12. Fillingim RB, Edwards RR, Powell T: The relationship of sex and clinical pain to experimental pain responses. Pain 83:419-425, 1999

28. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN: International affective picture system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-4: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida; 1999

13. Fillingim RB, Hastie BA, Ness TJ, Glover TL, Campbell CM, Staud R: Sex-related psychological predictors of baseline pain perception and analgesic responses to pentazocine. Biol Psychol 69:97-112, 2005

29. Lang PJ: Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment, in Sidowski JB, Johnson JH, Williams TA (eds): Technology in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 1980, pp 119-137

14. France CR, France JL, al’Absi M, Ring C, McIntyre D: Catastrophizing is related to pain ratings, but not nociceptive flexion reflex threshold. Pain 99:459-463, 2002

30. Lawson K, Reesor KA, Keefe FJ, Turner JA: Dimensions of pain-related cognitive coping: Cross-validation of the factor structure of the Coping Strategy Questionnaire. Pain 43:195204, 1990

15. France CR, Keefe FJ, Emery CF, Affleck G, France JL, Waters S, Caldwell DS, Stainbrook D, Hackshaw KV, Edwards C: Laboratory pain perception and clinical pain in postmenopausal women and age-matched men with osteoarthritis: Relationship to pain coping and hormonal status. Pain 112:274-281, 2004 16. Gebhart GF: Descending modulation of pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27:729-737, 2004 17. Geisser ME, Robinson ME, Pickren WE: Differences in cognitive coping strategies among pain-sensitive and paintolerant individuals on the cold-pressor test. Behavior Therapy 23:31-41, 1992 18. Geisser ME, Robinson ME, Keefe FJ, Weiner ML: Catastrophizing, depression and the sensory, affective and evaluative aspects of chronic pain. Pain 59:79-83, 1994 19. Gracely RH, Geisser ME, Giesecke T, Grant MA, Petzke F, Williams DA, Clauw DJ: Pain catastrophizing and neural responses to pain among persons with fibromyalgia. Brain 127:835-843, 2004 20. Granot M, Granovsky Y, Sprecher E, Nir RR, Yarnitsky D: Contact heat-evoked temporal summation: Tonic versus repetitive-phasic stimulation. Pain 122:295-305, 2006 21. Hennighasuen E, Mylius V, Kunz M, Schepelmann K: Attention and distraction have no modulatory effect on the nociceptive withdrawal reflex. Klinische Neurophysiologie 37:A90, 2006 22. Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM: Changes in beliefs, catastrophizing, and coping are associated with improvement in multidisciplinary pain treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol 69:655-662, 2001 23. Keefe FJ, Brown GK, Wallston KA, Caldwell DS: Coping with rheumatoid arthritis pain: Catastrophizing as a maladaptive strategy. Pain 37:51-56, 1989 24. Keefe FJ, Caldwell DS, Williams DA, Gil KM, Mitchell D, Robertson D, Robertson C, Martinez S, Nunley J, Beckham JC, Helms M: Pain coping skills training in the management of osteoarthritic knee pain: A comparative study. Behavior Therapy 21:49-62, 1990 25. Keefe FJ, Caldwell DS, Williams DA, Gil KM, Mitchell D, Robertson C, Martinez S, Nunley J, Beckham JC, Crisson JE, Helms M: Pain coping skills training in the management of

31. Levine LJ, Safer MA: Sources of bias in memory for emotions. Curr Directions Psychol Sci 11:169-173, 2002 32. Meagher MW, Grau JW, King RA: Frontal cortex lesions block the opioid and nonopioid hypoalgesia elicited by brief shocks but not the nonopioid hypoalgesia elicited by long shocks. Behav Neurosci 103:1366-1371, 1989 33. Melzack R: From the gate to the neuromatrix. Pain Suppl 6:S121-S126, 1999 34. Mineka S, Sutton SK: Cognitive biases and the emotional disorders. Psychol Sci 3:65-69, 1992 35. Nappi G, Sandrini G, Alfonsi E, Cecchini AP, Micieli G, Moglia A: Impaired circadian rhythmicity of nociceptive reflex threshold in cluster headache. Headache 42:125-131, 2002 36. Neugebauer V, Li W, Bird GC, Han JS: The amygdala and persistent pain. The Neuroscientist: A Review Journal Bridging Neurobiology, Neurology, and Psychiatry 10:221-234, 2004 37. Picavet HS, Vlaeyen JW, Schouten JS: Pain catastrophizing and kinesiophobia: Predictors of chronic low back pain. Am J Epidemiol 156:1028-1034, 2002 38. Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, Clare S, Menon RS, Matthews PM, Rawlins JN: Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science 284:1999 39. Pyszczynski T, Hamilton JC, Herring FH, Greenberg J: Depression, self-focused attention, and the negative memory bias. J Pers Soc Psychol 57:351-357, 1989 40. Radloff LS: The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1:385-401, 1977 41. Rhudy JL, Meagher MW: Fear and anxiety: Divergent effects on human pain thresholds. Pain 84:65-75, 2000 42. Rhudy JL, Meagher MW: The role of emotion in pain modulation. Curr Opin Psychiatry 14:241-245, 2001 43. Rhudy JL, Williams AE, McCabe K, Nguyen MA, Rambo P: Affective modulation of nociception at spinal and supraspinal levels. Psychophysiol 42:579-587, 2005 44. Rhudy JL, Williams AE, McCabe KM, Rambo PL, Russell JL: Emotional modulation of spinal nociception and pain: The impact of predictable noxious stimulation. Pain (Epub online August 4, 2006)

ORIGINAL REPORT/Rhudy et al 45. Riley JL III, Robinson ME, Wise EA, Myers CD, Fillingim RB: Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: A meta-analysis. Pain 74:181-187, 1998 46. Rosenstiel AK, Keefe FJ: The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: Relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain 17:33-44, 1983

333 tion of sex differences in pain catastrophizing and response to experimental pain. Clin J Pain 20:275-282, 2004 55. Turner JA, Clancy S: Strategies for coping with chronic low back pain: Relationship to pain and disability. Pain 24: 355-364, 1986 56. Urban MO, Gebhart GF: Supraspinal contributions to hyperalgesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:7687-7692, 1999

47. Sandrini G, Antonaci F, Lanfranchi S, Milanov I, Danilov A, Nappi G: Asymmetrical reduction of the nociceptive flexion reflex threshold in cluster headache. Cephalalgia: an international Journal of Headache 20:647-652, 2000

57. Villemure C, Bushnell MC: Cognitive modulation of pain: How do attention and emotion influence pain processing? Pain 95:195-199, 2002

48. Sandrini G, Serrao M, Rossi P, Romaniello A, Cruccu G, Willer JC: The lower limb flexion reflex in humans. Prog Neurobiol 77:353-395, 2005

58. Watkins LR, Mayer DJ: Multiple endogenous opiate and non-opiate analgesia systems: Evidence of their existence and clinical implications. Ann N Y Acad Sci 467:273-299, 1986

49. Seminowicz DA, Davis KD: Cortical responses to pain in healthy individuals depends on pain catastrophizing. Pain 120:297-306, 2006

59. Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, Hallin RG, Persson A: Do large diameter cutaneous afferents have a role in the transmission of nociceptive messages? Brain Res 311:375-379, 1984

50. Skljarevski V, Ramadan NM: The nociceptive flexion reflex in humans: review article. Pain 96:3-8, 2002

60. Willer JC: Comparative study of perceived pain and nociceptive flexion reflex in man. Pain 3:69-80, 1977

51. Sullivan MJ, D’Eon JL: Relation between catastrophizing and depression in chronic pain patients. J Abnorm Psychol 99:260-263, 1990

61. Willer JC, Boureau F, Albe-Fessard D: Supraspinal influences on nociceptive flexion reflex and pain sensation in man. Brain Res 179:61-68, 1979

52. Sullivan MJ, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA, Keefe F, Martin M, Bradley LA, Lefebvre JC: Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clin J Pain 17:52-64, 2001

62. Willer JC, Dehen H, Cambier J: Stress-induced analgesia in humans: Endogenous opioids and naloxone-reversible depression of pain reflexes. Science 212:689-691, 1981

53. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J: The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation. Psychol Assess 7:524-532, 1995 54. Thorn BE, Clements KL, Ward LC, Dixon KE, Kersh BC, Boothby JL, Chaplin WF: Personality factors in the explana-

63. Woolf CJ, Salter MW: Neuronal plasticity: increasing the gain in pain. Science 288:1765-1769, 2000 64. Zubieta J-K, Ketter TA, Bueller JA, Xu Y, Kilbourn MR, Young EA, Koeppe RA: Regulation of human affective responses by anterior cingulate and limbic mu-opioid neurotransmission. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60:1145-1153, 2003