Dysfunctional feedback processing in adolescent males with conduct disorder

Dysfunctional feedback processing in adolescent males with conduct disorder

    Dysfunctional feedback processing in adolescent males with conduct disorder Yidian Gao, Haiyan Chen, Huiqiao Jia, Qingsen Ming, Jinya...

509KB Sizes 0 Downloads 47 Views

    Dysfunctional feedback processing in adolescent males with conduct disorder Yidian Gao, Haiyan Chen, Huiqiao Jia, Qingsen Ming, Jinyao Yi, Shuqiao Yao PII: DOI: Reference:

S0167-8760(15)30050-7 doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.11.015 INTPSY 11058

To appear in:

International Journal of Psychophysiology

Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:

8 June 2015 23 November 2015 23 November 2015

Please cite this article as: Gao, Yidian, Chen, Haiyan, Jia, Huiqiao, Ming, Qingsen, Yi, Jinyao, Yao, Shuqiao, Dysfunctional feedback processing in adolescent males with conduct disorder, International Journal of Psychophysiology (2015), doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.11.015

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Dysfunctional feedback processing in adolescent males with conduct disorder

IP

T

Yidian Gao1, Haiyan Chen1, Huiqiao Jia1, Qingsen Ming1, Jinyao Yi1, Shuqiao Yao1, 2,

1

SC R

3,*

Medical Psychological Institute, Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University,

Changsha, Hunan 410011, China

National Province Technology Institute of Psychiatry, Central South University,

NU

2

3

MA

Changsha, Hunan 410011, China

Key Laboratory of Psychiatry and Mental Health of Hunan Province, Central South

CE P

Email addresses:

TE

D

University, Changsha, Hunan 410011, China

YG: [email protected]

AC

HC: [email protected] HJ: [email protected] QM: [email protected] JY: [email protected] SY: [email protected]

*

Corresponding author. Psychological Institute, Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, Hunan 410011, China. Tel.: 86 731 85292126; fax: 86 731 85361328. E-mail address: [email protected] 1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Abstract Abnormalities in neural feedback-processing systems may play a role in the

IP

T

development of dysfunctional behavior in individuals diagnosed with conduct

SC R

disorder (CD). The present study investigated the relation between CD adolescents and feedback processing by measuring event-related potentials (ERPs) in a single outcome gambling task, which included reward valence (loss and gain) and reward

NU

magnitude (10 and 50 cents) as outcomes. N2 and P3 components have been

MA

established as effective indicators in studies of behavioral disinhibition, reward processing, and decision making. Eighteen adolescent males (age: 13–17 years)

D

diagnosed with CD and 19 healthy age-matched male controls were recruited.

TE

Compared to healthy controls, CD individuals exhibited reduced N2 amplitudes in

CE P

response to loss condition. There was also a significant decreased P3 amplitude in all conditions. The amplitudes of P3 were negatively correlated with impulsivity scores

AC

across both groups, and the amplitudes of N2 were positively correlated with impulsivity scores across both groups. Our findings suggest that adolescents with CD may be impaired in neural sensitivity feedback and the processing of environmental cues compared to healthy controls. Moreover, N2 and P3 may be reliable indices to detect different sensitivity in reward and punishment feedback processing.

Keywords: conduct disorder; feedback processing; ERPs

2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1. Introduction

T

Conduct disorder (CD) is a psychiatric disorder that emerges during childhood or

IP

adolescence. Affected individuals show repetitive and persistent patterns of violating

SC R

the basic rights of others and difficulty in following major age-appropriate societal norms (APA, 2000). CD is accompanied by various behavioral problems, such as

NU

impulsivity, aggression, and risk-taking (Dougherty et al., 2000; Fairchild et al., 2009; Mathias et al., 2007), which represent latent externalizing traits. Previous studies have

MA

shown that individuals with CD lack empathy and compassion, and that they display

D

impaired control over their emotions and impulses (Dougherty et al., 2000). Similar to

TE

patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), individuals with CD show deficits in tasks requiring sustained attention and cognitive function

CE P

(Banaschewski et al., 2003). These characteristics are indicative of potential deficits in feedback-evaluation and self-monitoring systems (Sterzer et al., 2007).

AC

With the ability to predict and respond to feedback cues, we learn to survive in and adapt to the environment (Flores et al., 2015). The experiences that occur in anticipation and response to feedback appear to have important implications for our interpersonal adaption and intrapsychic regulation (Cole et al., 1994). A number of studies have addressed the influences of positive and negative feedback on cognition, motivation and behavior (Schultz, 2007). In particular, anticipation of positive feedback (reward or gain) facilitates approach behavior, whereas anticipation of negative feedback (punishment or loss) facilitates avoidance behavior (Young, 1959). The Reinforcement Sensitivity model developed by Gray et al. provides one of the 3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT most cited theory for explaining reward and punishment processing (Bjork and Pardini, 2015; Byrd et al., 2014; Colder and O'Connor, 2004; Goodnight et al., 2006;

IP

T

Gray, 1991). Gray proposed two main systems: the behavioral inhibition system (BIS)

SC R

and behavioral approach system (BAS). According to Gray, BIS serves to inhibit behavior in response to aversive stimuli or punishment, while BAS is thought to be sensitive to reward feedback or nonpunishment (Gray, 1991). Convergent findings

NU

from neurobiological studies imply that CD adolescents exhibit an aberrant difficulty

MA

in feedback-evaluation and adapting their behavior accordingly (Brazil et al., 2009; Brazil et al., 2013; Salim et al., 2015).

D

Recent neuroimaging studies of individuals with CD have demonstrated structural

TE

abnormalities in various brain regions, including the bilateral temporal lobes, the

CE P

orbitofrontal cortex, and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which are brain structures associated with decision-making and reward processing (De Brito et al.,

AC

2009; Fairchild et al., 2011; Huebner et al., 2008; Jiang et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2014). Generally, individuals benefit from these feedback-evaluations and feedback-monitoring systems to guide their decision making and to implement optimal behavior. Impaired processing or dysfunction in these brain areas may account for the lack of empathy and self-monitoring observed in CD individuals. People with CD may find it difficult to construct a socially adaptable behavior system (Gelhorn et al., 2007; Morcillo et al., 2012; Nock et al., 2006). Previous studies have examined electrophysiological correlates (e.g., ERPs) of feedback processing, including P2, N2 and P3 components (Franken et al., 2010;

4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Pfabigan et al., 2011; Salim et al., 2015). These components are particularly sensitive to feedback valence (gain/loss), feedback magnitude

IP

T

(large/small), and behavioral outcome evaluations (positive/negative) (van Meel et al.,

SC R

2005; Wu and Zhou, 2009). They have been proposed to encode different features of feedback evaluation and reflect motivational significance of feedback (Gentsch et al., 2013). For instance, P2 is implicated in attention selection and salience detection

NU

(Potts et al., 2006), and is related to reward system of brain (Riis et al., 2009; San

MA

Martin et al., 2010). Furthermore, the N2 and P3 components are used to examine cognitive processes that have been correlated with decision-making and

D

feedback-evaluation processing (Baker and Holroyd, 2011; Kam et al., 2012;

TE

Kamarajan et al., 2010). Specifically, the P3 reflects a later, top-down controlled

CE P

feedback evaluation process (Cui et al., 2013), whereas the N2 was thought to reflect the binary evaluation of resultant good versus bad outcomes (Holroyd et al., 2006;

AC

Kamarajan et al., 2009). These observations suggest that the two aspects underlying outcome evaluation are processed separately and rapidly in the brain (Wu and Zhou, 2009).

The N2 component of ERP has been used to study patients who exhibit traits consistent with behavioral disinhibition abnormalities. N2 is a fronto-central negative wave, occurring around 200 ms after stimulus onset, which indexes cognitive processes of stimulus evaluation, classification, decision-making and executive function, and these may be important for responding to environmental cues (Luck, 2005). Several researchers have focused on studying the behavioral disinhibitory

5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT processing in adolescents with externalizing problems using N2. The amplitude of N2 has been reported to be positively correlated with callous/unemotional temperament

IP

T

traits which may denote a more severe form of CD (Sumich et al., 2012). Compared

SC R

to healthy controls, adolescents with comorbid ADHD+CD display significantly prolonged latency of both N2 and P3 (van Meel et al., 2005). Albrecht et al. found that the ADHD-only and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD)/CD-only groups

NU

displayed reduced Stop-N2 amplitude using a stop-task. Adolescents with comorbid

MA

ADHD+ODD/CD also showed similar or less disinhibition prominent deficits than other groups (Albrecht et al., 2005). Additionally, it has also been suggested that the

D

N2 amplitude was reduced in the externalizing spectrum or juvenile non-psychopathic

TE

offenders compared with the control group (Anjana et al., 2010; Dikman and Allen,

CE P

2000; Kroger et al., 2014; Vila-Ballo et al., 2014). P2, a medial frontal positive component at approximately 200 ms poststimulus is

AC

associated with the identification of task-relevant perceptual representations (Potts et al., 2006). The spatio-temporal distribution of P2 is similar with N2. P2 is elicited to error choices or responses resulting in monetary loss (Potts et al., 2006). It has been argued that amplitude of P2 is sensitive to feedback information which has a motivational value, and has been found to reflect sensitivity toward reward (Martin and Potts, 2009; Salim et al., 2015). Previous study reported that P2 amplitude in psychopathic individuals was enhanced for predicted rewards and reward omissions, but not for unpredicted feedback using a passive gambling task (Salim et al., 2015). When using an Emotional Stroop task, researchers found that individuals scoring high

6

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT on psychopathic traits display reduced P2 amplitude to negative feedback compared to controls (Carolan et al., 2014). Larger P2 amplitude has also been reported in ADHD

IP

T

children, indicating deficiencies in early sensory processing in ADHD (Wiersema et

SC R

al., 2006).

The P3 wave is a stimulus-evoked centro-parietal positivity that occurs approximately 300 to 400 ms after a stimulus. The P3 is most commonly associated

NU

with expectation violation. It has been linked to a broadly distributed neural network

MA

involving the locus-coeruleus norepinephrine system, and it is sensitive to motivationally important events (Ito and Bartholow, 2009; Wu and Zhou, 2009).

D

Reduced P3 amplitude has been consistently linked to a spectrum of externalizing

TE

disorders, such as CD (Cappadocia et al., 2009; Iacono et al., 2002), illicit substance

CE P

abuse (Iacono et al., 2002), antisocial behavior (Bauer and Hesselbrock, 2001, 2003; Costa et al., 2000) and ADHD (Iacono et al., 2002). In a comprehensive study, Iacono

AC

et al. found that reduced P3 amplitude was associated with several behavior disinhibition disorders, including ADHD, ODD, CD, antisocial personality disorder, alcoholism, as well as illicit drug abuse and dependence (Morcillo et al., 2012). In sum, these previous studies have shown that P2, N2 and P3 components are all linked to patients with impaired feedback processing which is found underlying the behavioral adaptation deficits. However, the majority of neuropsychologic studies related to CD adolescents have rarely considered the confounding effects of comorbid psychiatric conditions (e.g. ADHD). Few studies have directly examined the feedback evaluation in adolescents

7

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT with CD. It also remains unknown as to whether the symptoms of CD are associated with reward-processing abnormalities. Therefore, the present study aimed to examine

IP

T

whether CD adolescents have abnormalities in the evaluation of monetary reward and

SC R

punishment while performing a simple outcome gambling task. This study also investigate whether the early N1, P2, later N2 and P3 components correlate with externalizing factors, such as impulsivity. Since there were distinct gender differences

NU

observed in the ERP indices of affective stimuli, the participants in present study were

MA

designed to be male adolescents only (Criado and Ehlers, 2007). Therefore, our first hypothesis is that adolescents with CD will show decreased amplitude in both N2 and

D

P3 components. Second, we posited that adolescents with CD will exhibit increased

TE

amplitude in P2 component. Third, we think that adolescents with CD will

CE P

demonstrate higher impulsivity scores on the behavioral measures. And lastly, the decreased N2 and P3 amplitude will be correlated with increased impulsivity scores.

AC

2. Methods

2.1 Participants 40 participants were recruited, 3 participants were excluded due to excessive artifacts in the EEG recording. Finally, CD group consisted of 18 male adolescents (age range: 13–17 years, mean: 15.4 years, standard deviation [SD]: 1.3 years), who were recruited from the outpatient clinics affiliated with the Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University in Changsha, Hunan, China. Controls included 19 healthy age-matched male adolescents (mean age: 15.5 years, SD: 1.3 years), recruited from a

8

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT school in the same city. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University. Written informed consent was

IP

T

obtained from all participants and their parents. All participants received monetary

SC R

compensation upon completion of the study.

CD was diagnosed independently by two psychiatrists, according to the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders-Patient Edition (SCID-I/P)

NU

(First, 2002), which has been translated into Chinese and adapted for use in both

MA

patients and healthy individuals (Cassidy et al., 2011; Shi et al., 2005). Additionally, one parent of each subject was interviewed to obtain detailed information. The

D

psychiatrists made the final decision in the case that information obtained from the

TE

patients and parents was inconsistent.

CE P

For the recruitment of healthy control individuals, students, selected randomly from class rosters, were matched with CD subjects by age and gender. Participants who

AC

agreed to be interviewed by the psychiatrists were subjected to the SCID-I/P and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Chinese revision (C-WISC) examinations (Gong, 1993). Information provided by the students was verified by their parents on an as-needed basis. None of the healthy control participants met the criteria for CD. Subjects were excluded from both groups if they reported a history of any of the following: ADHD or any other psychiatric or emotional disorder; diagnosis of any pervasive developmental or chronic neurological disorder, Tourette syndrome, posttraumatic stress disorder, or obsessive compulsive disorder; persistent headaches; head trauma; alcohol or substance abuse in the past year; or an IQ of ≤ 80 on the

9

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT C-WISC. All subjects were right-handed according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). All subjects had normal or normal corrected vision. We

IP

T

also used the Chinese version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depress Scale

SC R

(CES-D) and the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) to rate the depression and anxiety severity respectively (Radloff, 1991; Yao et al., 2007a). To assess the impulsivity trait, participants were required to complete the Chinese version

NU

of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) (Yao et al., 2007b), which is a reliable

MA

and valid assessment tool to measure impulsivity in Chinese adolescents. The BIS-11 is a widely used self-reporting questionnaire that contains 30 items and includes three

D

subscales: attentional impulsiveness (making decisions quickly), motor impulsiveness

TE

(acting without thinking), and unplanned impulsiveness (lack of prior planning or

CE P

future orientation). The sum of scores for all items provides an overall impulsiveness score, with higher scores indicating greater impulsiveness.

AC

Table 1 summarizes the demographic and background characteristics of the two groups. All CD individuals were treatment-naive and met the criteria for adolescent-onset CD, demonstrating at least one sign of CD after 10 years of age (APA, 2000).

2.2 Simple Outcome Gambling task The simple outcome gambling task used in this study is illustrated in Fig 1. It is a modified version of the classical gamble task initially developed by Gehring and Willough (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002; Kamarajan et al., 2009). Using the

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT gambling task, we may be able to examine emotional responses to monetary reward and punishment in adolescents with CD, and to study the processing of feedback

IP

T

(Cappadocia et al., 2009). In each trial, the participant was presented with a choice

SC R

stimulus of two numbers (corresponding to an equivalent monetary value in Chinese cents) on a computer screen. A box on the left-hand side of the screen contained the number 10 (10 cents, with which one can buy a ballpoint pen refill), and a box on the

NU

right-hand side displayed the number 50 (50 cents, with which one can buy a ballpoint

MA

pen). These two numbers were shown for 800 ms, followed by a random interval of 700 to 1700 ms. The participant pressed the right or left button to choose the number

D

on the same side of the screen during a period of 200 to 1000 ms.

TE

If the participants did not react during the response period, then the program would

CE P

proceed to the next trial without feedback (reward or punishment) (Kamarajan et al., 2009). If the participants did react during the response period, then the feedback

AC

stimulus appeared at the center of the screen. The feedback stimulus lasted 800 ms and was followed by an interval of 700 to 1700 ms. The feedback stimulus of “+10” or “+50” indicated “gain”, whereas the feedback stimulus of “-10” or “-50”, indicated “loss”. Thus, there were four possible feedback stimuli, depending on the participant's preceding choice: small loss (-10) and small gain (+10) if "10" was chosen, large loss (-50) and large gain (+50) if "50" was chosen. Participants started with 10 Yuan (with which one can buy a story book) and were told to try to win as much money as possible. Unbeknownst to the participants, no matter which option (10/50) was chosen, the probabilities of loss or gain feedback in

11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT each block were equal (50%). The order of loss or gain feedback was also pseudo-randomized. A total of 344 trials were presented, which were divided into four

IP

T

equal blocks of 86 trials each. Participants took a break of approximately 5 min

SC R

between each block. To increase motivation, participants were told they would be paid the amount of money that they won during the experiment. Participants performed the gambling task with the STIM-2 software package (Neuroscan, Inc.) (Yi et al., 2012).

MA

NU

The reaction time for the task conditions and responses was calculated (Table 1).

2.3 Event-related potential recordings

D

EEG readings were obtained with 63 Ag/AgCl ring electrodes, in accordance with

TE

the Extended International System, using 10-20 electrode caps. Two electrodes were

CE P

placed on the left and right mastoids, and a ground electrode was secured on the forehead. Electrodes were subsequently re-referenced to the average of the right and

AC

left mastoids. Vertical electrooculography (EOG) data were recorded from electrodes placed above and below the left eye. Horizontal EOG data were recorded from the outer canthi of each eye. All electrode impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ. EEG data were recorded continuously with a bandpass filter of 0.05 to 100 Hz and digitized at 250 Hz.

2.4 Electroencephalography data analysis All ERP analyses were done offline with the Neuroscan 4.3 Edit software package. Ocular artifacts were corrected with an eye-movement correction algorithm

12

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT (Semlitsch et al., 1986). Before further analysis, the ERP data were digitally filtered with a low-pass filter at 30 Hz (24 dB/ octave). Continuous EEG output was

IP

T

segmented into intervals of 200 ms before the feedback stimulus and 800 ms after

SC R

feedback onset. Epochs were baseline-corrected by subtracting the average activity of that channel during the baseline period from each sample. After baseline correction, sweeps in which amplitudes exceeded ±100 μV were rejected automatically as

NU

artifacts.

MA

ERPs were obtained by averaging the EEG signals according to feedback type (gain vs. loss) and reward condition (10 vs. 50) for each channel. We measured the N1, P2

D

in the time windows of 100-150 ms and 150-220 ms, respectively (Salim et al., 2015).

TE

The N2 amplitude was scored as the averaged amplitude of a 100 ms interval ranging

CE P

from 200-300 ms (Groen et al., 2013; van Meel et al., 2011). Analyses of N1, P2 and N2 amplitudes were limited to the anterior frontal midline electrode (Fz), the

AC

frontal-central midline electrode (FCz), and the central midline electrode (Cz) in all conditions. The P3 amplitude was measured as the averaged amplitude within 300-450 ms after feedback onset at the Pz electrode (Schulreich et al., 2013; Yeung and Sanfey, 2004). These time-windows were determined according to the largest differences based on visual inspection of grand average waveforms and previous studies. The average number of trials under the four feedback conditions did not significantly differ between the CD and the control groups [F =0.04, p = 0.84]. The averaged numbers of trials were 66.61 for the CD group and 65.58 for the control

13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT group. The average numbers of trials in the four conditions are shown in Table 1. 2.5 Statistical analysis

IP

T

All statistical analyses were carried out in SPSS 19.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

SC R

Independent sample t-tests were used to examine differences in age, IQ, BIS-11 scores, and reaction time. The ERP data were analyzed with repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). The amplitude of the N1, P2, N2 and P3 were evaluated

NU

statistically with reward valence (gain vs. loss), reward magnitude (10 vs. 50), and

with

group

(CD

vs.

MA

electrodes (topographic factor; only not for P3) as repeated-measurement factors, and healthy control)

as

a

between-group

factor.

The

D

Greenhouse-Geisser correction for non-sphericity was applied, when appropriate, for

TE

repeated measures (Holroyd and Coles, 2002). Subsequently, Pearson’s correlations

CE P

were calculated between BIS scores and N1/P2/N2/P3 amplitudes. P-values < 0.05

AC

were considered statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1 Demographic and behavioral results There were no significant differences in age, IQ, or reaction time in response to the guess prompt between CD individuals and healthy controls (Table 1). There same results were also found in CES-D or MASC scores. Compared to the healthy control group, the CD group exhibit higher scores for attention impulsiveness [t (35) = 3.4, p = 0.002], motor impulsiveness [t (35) = 2.8, p = 0.008], and unplanned impulsiveness

14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [t (35) = 2.4, p = 0.025], as well as for the BIS total score [t (35) = 3.5, p = 0.001].

IP

T

3.2 Event-related potential results

SC R

Figure 2. shows the grand-average ERP waveforms elicited by four outcome stimuli (-10, +10, -50 and +50) at electrode sites Fz, Cz and Pz. Topographic scalp maps depicted in Figure 2. show differences between the two groups in four feedback

NU

conditions at the indicated time windows. Figure 3. plots the values of the mean

results are compiled in Table 2.

D

3.2.1 N1 brainwave responses

MA

amplitudes of N1, P2, N2 and P3 at each electrode site. Repeated-measures ANOVA

TE

Repeated-measures ANOVAs found a significant differences in N1 amplitudes

CE P

between CD group and healthy control group [F (1, 35) = 5.6, p = 0.023], showing a reduced N1 amplitude in CD adolescents compared to healthy controls. The site of

AC

electrode placement demonstrate a significant effect [F (2, 70) = 5.8, p = 0.008], with a larger N1 response at the FCz than at the Fz or Cz electrode site. No other significant effects or interactions were observed.

3.2.2 P2 brainwave responses We found a main effect of magnitude [F (1, 35) = 14.3, p = 0.001]. P2 amplitude was larger in response to large magnitude compared to small magnitude feedback. Furthermore, we found a main effect of electrode [F (2, 70) = 15.9, p < 0.001]. The amplitude of P2 component at the Cz was larger than at the Fz or FCz electrode site.

15

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Repeated-measures ANOVA of P2 showed a significant group effect [F (1, 35) = 4.4, p = 0.043], as well as significant interactions between group and feedback valence [F

IP

T

(1, 35) = 4.4, p = 0.044]. Follow-up analyses revealed that in the gain condition, P2

SC R

amplitudes did not differ between two groups (p >0.05), whereas P2 amplitude in the CD group was larger than healthy control group in loss condition (p = 0.019).

NU

3.2.3 N2 brainwave responses

MA

Repeated-measures ANOVAs found a significant differences in N2 amplitudes between CD patients and healthy controls [F (1, 35) = 6.6, p = 0.015], showing a

D

reduced N2 amplitude in CD group compared to healthy controls. N2 amplitudes were

TE

also significantly affected by reward valence [F (1, 35) = 8.0, p = 0.008 ], but this

CE P

effect was qualified by the Group×Valence interaction [F (1, 35) = 5.2, p = 0.029]. Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that healthy controls displayed larger N2

AC

amplitudes in response to loss feedback compared to gain feedback (p = 0.001). In contrast, N2 amplitudes did not differ between loss and gain conditions in the CD group (p >0.05). The site of electrode placement had a significant effect [F (2, 70) = 48.2, p < 0.001], with a larger N2 response at the Fz than at the Cz or FCz electrode site. The interaction of Group × Electrode was found significant [F (2, 70) = 7.0, p = 0.007]. Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that the differences of N2 amplitudes between two groups were the largest at the Fz electrode (p = 0.001). The interaction of Magnitude × Electrode was also significant [F (2, 70) = 4.0, p = 0.030], demonstrating the differences of N2 amplitudes between electrodes were larger in

16

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT large magnitude (50) than in small magnitude (10) (p < 0.001) (Table 2).

IP

T

3.2.4 P3 brainwave responses

SC R

There was a significant difference in P3 amplitude between two groups [F (1, 35) = 5.0, p = 0.032], indicating more positive-going ERP responses in the healthy control group than in the CD group. We found a main effect of valence [F (1, 35) = 17.7, p <

NU

0.001]. P3 amplitude was more pronounced following gain as compared to loss

MA

feedback. Reward magnitude also had significant effects on the P3 amplitude [F (1, 35) = 14.6, p = 0.001], indicating more positive P3 responses to large magnitude than

D

to small magnitude. Repeated-measures ANOVAs revealed no significant interactions

CE P

TE

(Table 2).

3.3 Correlation analyses

AC

Pearson’s correlation analyses revealed that N2 responses evoked by various conditions were positively correlated to the BIS attentional impulsiveness except for a small-magnitude gain (+10). N2 amplitudes after loss conditions (-10/-50) were positively correlated to the BIS total scores (Table 3). Additionally, the P3 amplitudes after some conditions were negatively correlated with the scores of attentional impulsiveness except for a small-magnitude loss (-10) (Table 3).

4. Discussion Conduct disorder has been widely associated with deficits in cognitive control,

17

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT feedback evaluation and behavioral inhibition. The present report is aimed at studying the electrophysiological correlates of feedback processing using a gambling task in

IP

T

adolescent males with CD. Specifically, we examined whether CD individuals show

SC R

altered ERP responses to signals of gain and loss when compared to controls. The key findings of several analyses are that CD and healthy adolescents differ in their feedback processing under conditions of gain and loss.

NU

Gain and loss feedback processing has been conceptualized in terms of behavioral

MA

activation system (BAS) and behavioral inhibition system (BIS) (Byrd et al., 2014; Gray, 1991). Our data suggest that CD adolescents showed aberrant P2 and N2

D

amplitudes in response to loss feedback, and a decreased P3 amplitude in all

TE

conditions compared to controls. Our results are partially consistent with past findings,

CE P

lending further empirical evidence to the idea that individuals with disinhibition syndrome have a less sensitive BIS system (Gray, 1991). The group differences found

AC

in our study suggest that adolescents with CD exhibited less concern and monitoring towards the feedback during trials on which they were punished in contrast to reward. A decreased response to punishment feedback indicates a lower level of activation in the BIS system, which in turn suggests hyposensitivity to punishment or negative feedback, leading to increased aggression or antisocial tendency. The abnormalities of self-monitoring ongoing behaviors, while ignoring negative outcomes, increase the likelihood of engaging in harmful behaviors. The P3 wave component has been reported to play an important role in enabling individuals to differentiate good from bad outcomes during decision making and

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT allowing them to optimize their actions (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). Reduction of the P3 amplitude is seen in some mental and neurological disorders, such as

IP

T

schizophrenia (Jeon and Polich, 2003) and disinhibition disorders (Polich et al., 1994).

SC R

Our findings are in line with previous reports about the ERP abnormalities of the P3 in CD adolescents. Previous reports have likewise described reduced P3 amplitudes in individuals with CD compared to their healthy peers (Cappadocia et al., 2009). In CD

NU

patients, observed reduction of the P3 amplitude to both reward and punishment

MA

conditions implies a low physiological arousal which may be caused by an overactive reward system (BAS) , requiring them to seek external stimulation in order to increase

D

their internal arousal (Byrd et al., 2014; Cappadocia et al., 2009).

TE

To some extent, individuals with CD appear to be less aware of the importance of

CE P

negative feedback compared to healthy controls. The observed features in self-monitoring capabilities in CD individuals may reflect diminished neural activity

AC

in key brain regions necessary to evaluate external feedback responses compared to their healthy peers (Yi et al., 2012). Consistent with Gray’s theory, the reduced amplitudes of N2 and P3 in adolescents with CD may reflect abnormalities in environmental cues processing and sensation seeking (Gray, 1991). The impaired monetary reward and punishment processing in CD adolescents may result from diminished

neural

activity

in

reward-evaluation

pathways

or

deficits

in

decision-making systems, thereby increasing the tendency of repeating a previously rewarded behavior even when such behaviors is no longer producing reward but punishment instead (Patterson and Newman, 1993).

19

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT In addition, we found that there was an early difference in the N1 component between the two groups. The anterior N1 was reduced in the CD group. Previous

IP

T

studies suggested that the high impulsive individuals differ on early sensory and

SC R

attention-related components, with reduced N1 amplitudes, indicating reduced inhibiting and enhanced orienting (Houston and Stanford, 2001). It is reported that larger amplitudes of N1 in high impulsive individuals indicate enhanced attention

NU

orienting (Houston and Stanford, 2001). The difference in N1 amplitude in the current

MA

study may suggest that individuals with conduct disorder are less engaged by stimulus information about the consequences of their decisions and lack of inhibition.

D

Despite the differences exhibited in adolescents with CD compared to healthy

TE

controls, several aspects of the neural mechanisms of feedback processing were

CE P

similar in both groups. Our correlation analyses showed that the amplitudes of N2 and P3 were significantly associated with BIS-11 attentional scores, which is a measure of

AC

concentration capacity. Specifically, N2 amplitude was positively associated with BIS-11 total scores. The relations between N2, P3 amplitudes and BIS-11 scores speak to participants’ concentration during the tasks (Dikman and Allen, 2000). The decreased amplitudes of N2 and P3 support the aforementioned idea that CD individuals may be less concerned about the consequences of having punishment. Furthermore, the N2 and P3 amplitudes vary with reward valence and their magnitude may due to the different motivational or affective significance of reward and punishment, reflecting a meaningful change in neural processing. There are several potential limitations that should be mentioned. First, it's important

20

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT to note, there may be inter-individual variation among healthy controls, and as well as disinhibited individuals will probably show these effects (Dikman and Allen, 2000).

IP

T

Earlier studies have demonstrated that the P3 amplitude was related to the severity of

SC R

the “rules violation” subtype, but not related to aggression, deceitfulness, or theft (Bauer and Hesselbrock, 2003). The current study did not differentiate each subtype of CD due to limited sample size. Future studies are needed to further improve the

NU

homogeneity of subjects and explore the individual differences within controls and

MA

subtypes of CD. The second is that the study included no females, so future studies should validate our findings in CD individuals of different subtypes and genders.

D

Third, our study is cross-sectional which is common in neuroimaging studies of

TE

psychiatric disorders. Therefore, our findings are limited in making any causal claims

CE P

about the etiology of the problem behaviors in CD individuals. In addition, the participants recruited in the present study are CD-only. In order to gain more insight

AC

on potential confounding effects in feedback processing, future studies should include the comparison between CD and/or ADHD adolescents. In conclusion, the present study provides insights into the electrophysiological processing of differential responses to reward and punishment between adolescent with CD and healthy controls. This study demonstrates a decreased N2 amplitude following a loss as well as an overall decreased P3 responses in CD individuals. These findings suggest an underlying deficit in feedback processing, which may increase the propensity for behavioral disinhibition in CD individuals.

21

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Competing interests

IP

T

All authors declare that they have no competing interests.

SC R

Authors’ contribution

Author SY and JY designed the study and wrote the protocol. YG, HC and HJ carried out the studies and participated in the data collecting. YG and HC managed statistical

NU

analysis and drafted the manuscript. QM and JY helped to draft the manuscript. All

MA

authors read and approved the final manuscript.

D

Acknowledgments

TE

This study was supported by the grants from the National Nature Science Foundation

CE P

of China (grant no. 81471384), National Key Technologies R&D Program in China’s 11th 5-year plan (grant no. 2009BAI77B02), Specialized Research Fund for the

AC

Doctoral Program of Higher Education (SRFDP, no. 20130162110043) and the construct program of the key discipline in Hunan Province.

22

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT References Albrecht, B., Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., Heinrich, H., Rothenberger, A., 2005. Response inhibition deficits in externalizing child psychiatric disorders: an ERP-study with the Stop-task.

T

Behavioral and Brain Functions 1, 22. disorder. Functional Neurology 25, 87-92.

IP

Anjana, Y., Khaliq, F., Vaney, N., 2010. Event-related potentials study in attention deficit hyperactivity American Psychological Association, 2000. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4

SC R

edition. American Psychiatric Association, Washington DC.

Baker, T.E., Holroyd, C.B., 2011. Dissociated roles of the anterior cingulate cortex in reward and conflict processing as revealed by the feedback error-related negativity and N200. Biological Psychology 87, 25-34.

NU

Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., Heinrich, H., Albrecht, B., Brunner, E., Rothenberger, A., 2003. Association of ADHD and conduct disorder - brain electrical evidence for the existence of a distinct subtype. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 44, 356-376.

MA

Bauer, L.O., Hesselbrock, V.M., 2001. CSD/BEM localization of P300 sources in adolescents "at-risk": evidence of frontal cortex dysfunction in conduct disorder. Biological Psychiatry 50, 600-608. Bauer, L.O., Hesselbrock, V.M., 2003. Brain maturation and subtypes of conduct disorder: interactive effects on p300 amplitude and topography in male adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of

D

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 42, 106-115.

TE

Bjork, J.M., Pardini, D.A., 2015. Who are those “risk-taking adolescents”? Individual differences in developmental neuroimaging research. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 11, 56-64. Brazil, I.A., de Bruijn, E.R., Bulten, B.H., von Borries, A.K., van Lankveld, J.J., Buitelaar, J.K., Verkes,

CE P

R.J., 2009. Early and late components of error monitoring in violent offenders with psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry 65, 137-143. Brazil, I.A., Maes, J.H., Scheper, I., Bulten, B.H., Kessels, R.P., Verkes, R.J., de Bruijn, E.R., 2013. Reversal deficits in individuals with psychopathy in explicit but not implicit learning conditions.

AC

Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 38, 120152. Byrd, A.L., Loeber, R., Pardini, D.A., 2014. Antisocial behavior, psychopathic features and abnormalities in reward and punishment processing in youth. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 17, 125-156. Cappadocia, M.C., Desrocher, M., Pepler, D., Schroeder, J.H., 2009. Contextualizing the neurobiology of conduct disorder in an emotion dysregulation framework. Clinical Psychology Review 29, 506-518. Carolan, P.L., Jaspers-Fayer, F., Asmaro, D.T., Douglas, K.S., Liotti, M., 2014. Electrophysiology of blunted emotional bias in psychopathic personality. Psychophysiology 51, 36-41. Cassidy,

C.M.,

Joober,

R.,

King,

S.,

Malla,

A.K.,

2011.

Childhood

symptoms

of

inattention-hyperactivity predict cannabis use in first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 132, 171-176. Colder, C.R., O'Connor, R.M., 2004. Gray's reinforcement sensitivity model and child psychopathology: laboratory and questionnaire assessment of the BAS and BIS. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 32, 435-451. Cole, P.M., Michel, M.K., Teti, L.O., 1994. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: a clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, 73-100. Costa, L., Bauer, L., Kuperman, S., Porjesz, B., O'Connor, S., Hesselbrock, V., Rohrbaugh, J., Begleiter, H., 2000. Frontal p300 decrements, alcohol dependence, and antisocial personality disorder. Biological

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Psychiatry 47, 1064-1071. Criado, J.R., Ehlers, C.L., 2007. Electrophysiological responses to affective stimuli in Mexican Americans: Relationship to alcohol dependence and personality traits. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 88, 148-157.

T

Cui, J.F., Chen, Y.H., Wang, Y., Shum, D.H., Chan, R.C., 2013. Neural correlates of uncertain decision making: ERP evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 776.

IP

De Brito, S.A., Mechelli, A., Wilke, M., Laurens, K.R., Jones, A.P., Barker, G.J., Hodgins, S., Viding, traits. Brain 132, 843-852.

SC R

E., 2009. Size matters: increased grey matter in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional Dikman, Z.V., Allen, J.J., 2000. Error monitoring during reward and avoidance learning in high- and low-socialized individuals. Psychophysiology 37, 43-54.

Dougherty, D.M., Bjork, J.M., Marsh, D.M., Moeller, F.G., 2000. A comparison between adults with

NU

conduct disorder and normal controls on a continuous performance test: Differences in impulsive response characteristics. Psychological Record 50, 203-219.

Fairchild, G., Passamonti, L., Hurford, G., Hagan, C.C., von dem Hagen, E.A., van Goozen, S.H.,

MA

Goodyer, I.M., Calder, A.J., 2011. Brain structure abnormalities in early-onset and adolescent-onset conduct disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 624-633. Fairchild, G., van Goozen, S.H.M., Stollery, S.J., Aitken, M.R.F., Savage, J., Moore, S.C., Goodyer, I.M., 2009. Decision Making and Executive Function in Male Adolescents with Early-Onset or

D

Adolescence-Onset Conduct Disorder and Control Subjects. Biological Psychiatry 66, 162-168.

TE

First, M., Spitzer, R, Gibbon, M, Williams, J, 2002. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders-Patient Edtion (SCID-I/P, 11/2002 revision). New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.

CE P

Flores, A., Munte, T.F., Donamayor, N., 2015. Event-related EEG responses to anticipation and delivery of monetary and social reward. Biological Psychology 109, 10-19. Franken, I.H., Van den Berg, I., Van Strien, J.W., 2010. Individual differences in alcohol drinking frequency are associated with electrophysiological responses to unexpected nonrewards. Alcoholism,

AC

Clinical and Experimental Research 34, 702-707. Gehring, W.J., Willoughby, A.R., 2002. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 295, 2279-2282. Gelhorn, H.L., Sakai, J.T., Price, R.K., Crowley, T.J., 2007. DSM-IV conduct disorder criteria as predictors of antisocial personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry 48, 529-538. Gentsch, K., Grandjean, D., Scherer, K.R., 2013. Temporal dynamics of event-related potentials related to goal conduciveness and power appraisals. Psychophysiology 50, 1010-1022. Gong, Y., Cai, TS, 1993. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Chinese Revision (C-WISC). Hunan Maps Press, Changsha. Goodnight, J.A., Bates, J.E., Newman, J.P., Dodge, K.A., Pettit, G.S., 2006. The interactive influences of friend deviance and reward dominance on the development of externalizing behavior during middle adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 34, 573-583. Gray, J., 1991. The Neuropsychology of Temperament, in: Strelau, J., Angleitner, A. (Eds.), Explorations in Temperament. Springer US, pp. 105-128. Groen, Y., Tucha, O., Wijers, A.A., Althaus, M., 2013. Processing of continuously provided punishment and reward in children with ADHD and the modulating effects of stimulant medication: an ERP study. PLoS One 8, e59240.

24

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Holroyd, C.B., Coles, M.G.H., 2002. The neural basis. of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review 109, 679-709. Holroyd, C.B., Hajcak, G., Larsen, J.T., 2006. The good, the bad and the neutral: electrophysiological responses to feedback stimuli. Brain Research 1105, 93-101.

T

Houston, R.J., Stanford, M.S., 2001. Mid-latency evoked potentials in self-reported impulsive aggression. International Journal of Psychophysiology 40, 1-15.

IP

Huebner, T., Vloet, T.D., Marx, I., Konrad, K., Fink, G.R., Herpertz, S.C., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., 2008. Morphometric brain abnormalities in boys with conduct disorder. Journal of the American

SC R

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 47, 540-547.

Iacono, W.G., Carlson, S.R., Malone, S.M., McGue, M., 2002. P3 event-related potential amplitude and the risk for disinhibitory disorders in adolescent boys. Archives of General Psychiatry 59, 750-757. Ito, T.A., Bartholow, B.D., 2009. The neural correlates of race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13,

NU

524-531.

Jeon, Y.W., Polich, J., 2003. Meta-analysis of P300 and schizophrenia: patients, paradigms, and practical implications. Psychophysiology 40, 684-701.

MA

Jiang, Y., Guo, X., Zhang, J., Gao, J., Wang, X., Situ, W., Yi, J., Zhang, X., Zhu, X., Yao, S., Huang, B., 2015. Abnormalities of cortical structures in adolescent-onset conduct disorder. Psychol Med 45, 3467-3479.

Kam, J.W.Y., Dominelli, R., Carlson, S.R., 2012. Differential relationships between sub-traits of

D

BIS-11 impulsivity and executive processes: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology

TE

85, 174-187.

Kamarajan, C., Porjesz, B., Rangaswamy, M., Tang, Y.Q., Chorlian, D.B., Padmanabhapillai, A., Saunders, R., Pandey, A.K., Roopesh, B.N., Manz, N., Stimus, A.T., Begleiter, H., 2009. Brain

CE P

signatures of monetary loss and gain: Outcome-related potentials in a single outcome gambling task. Behavioural Brain Research 197, 62-76. Kamarajan, C., Rangaswamy, M., Tang, Y., Chorlian, D.B., Pandey, A.K., Roopesh, B.N., Manz, N., Saunders, R., Stimus, A.T., Porjesz, B., 2010. Dysfunctional reward processing in male alcoholics: an

AC

ERP study during a gambling task. Journal of Psychiatric Research 44, 576-590. Kroger, A., Hof, K., Krick, C., Siniatchkin, M., Jarczok, T., Freitag, C.M., Bender, S., 2014. Visual processing of biological motion in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an event related potential-study. PLoS One 9, e88585. Luck, S.J., 2005. An introduction to the event-related potential technique. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass. Martin, L.E., Potts, G.F., 2009. Impulsivity in Decision-Making: An Event-Related Potential Investigation. Personality and Individual Differences 46, 303. Mathias, C.W., Stanford, M.S., Marsh, D.M., Frick, P.J., Moeller, F.G., Swann, A.C., Dougherty, D.M., 2007. Characterizing aggressive behavior with the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale among adolescents with conduct disorder. Psychiatry Research 151, 231-242. Morcillo, C., Duarte, C.S., Sala, R., Wang, S., Lejuez, C.W., Kerridge, B.T., Blanco, C., 2012. Conduct disorder and adult psychiatric diagnoses: associations and gender differences in the U.S. adult population. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 323-330. Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., Cohen, J.D., 2005. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychological Bulletin 131, 510-532. Nock, M.K., Kazdin, A.E., Hiripi, E., Kessler, R.C., 2006. Prevalence, subtypes, and correlates of

25

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT DSM-IV conduct disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine 36, 699-710. Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97-113.

T

Patterson, C.M., Newman, J.P., 1993. Reflectivity and learning from aversive events: toward a psychological mechanism for the syndromes of disinhibition. Psychological Review 100, 716-736.

IP

Pfabigan, D.M., Alexopoulos, J., Bauer, H., Lamm, C., Sailer, U., 2011. All about the Money - External Performance Monitoring is Affected by Monetary, but Not by Socially Conveyed Feedback Cues in

SC R

More Antisocial Individuals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5.

Polich, J., Pollock, V.E., Bloom, F.E., 1994. Meta-analysis of P300 amplitude from males at risk for alcoholism. Psychological Bulletin 115, 55-73.

Potts, G.F., Martin, L.E., Burton, P., Montague, P.R., 2006. When things are better or worse than Neuroscience 18, 1112-1119.

NU

expected: the medial frontal cortex and the allocation of processing resources. Journal of Cognitive Radloff, L.S., 1991. The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents

MA

and young adults. J Youth Adolesc 20, 149-166.

Riis, J.L., Chong, H., McGinnnis, S., Tarbi, E., Sun, X., Holcomb, P.J., Rentz, D.M., Daffner, K.R., 2009. Age-related changes in early novelty processing as measured by ERPs. Biological Psychology 82, 33-44.

D

Salim, M.A., van der Veen, F.M., van Dongen, J.D., Franken, I.H., 2015. Brain activity elicited by Psychology 110, 50-58.

TE

reward and reward omission in individuals with psychopathic traits: An ERP study. Biological San Martin, R., Manes, F., Hurtado, E., Isla, P., Ibanez, A., 2010. Size and probability of rewards

CE P

modulate the feedback error-related negativity associated with wins but not losses in a monetarily rewarded gambling task. Neuroimage 51, 1194-1204. Schulreich, S., Pfabigan, D.M., Derntl, B., Sailer, U., 2013. Fearless Dominance and reduced feedback-related negativity amplitudes in a time-estimation task - further neuroscientific evidence for

AC

dual-process models of psychopathy. Biological Psychology 93, 352-363. Schultz, W., 2007. Multiple dopamine functions at different time courses. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30, 259-288. Semlitsch, H.V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P., Presslich, O., 1986. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23, 695-703. Shi, Q.C., Zhang, J.M., Xu, F.Z., Phillips, M.R., Xu, Y., Fu, Y.L., Gu, W., Zhou, X.J., Wang, S.M., Zhang, Y., Yu, M., 2005. Epidemiological survey of mental illnesses in the people aged 15 and older in Zhejiang Province, China. Zhonghua Yu Fang Yi Xue Za Zhi 39, 229-236. Sterzer, P., Stadler, C., Poustka, F., Kleinschmidt, A., 2007. A structural neural deficit in adolescents with conduct disorder and its association with lack of empathy. Neuroimage 37, 335-342. Sumich, A., Sarkar, S., Hermens, D.F., Kelesidi, K., Taylor, E., Rubia, K., 2012. Electrophysiological correlates of CU traits show abnormal regressive maturation in adolescents with conduct problems. Personality and Individual Differences 53, 862-867. van Meel, C.S., Heslenfeld, D.J., Oosterlaan, J., Luman, M., Sergeant, J.A., 2011. ERPs associated with monitoring and evaluation of monetary reward and punishment in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 52, 942-953. van Meel, C.S., Oosterlaan, J., Heslenfeld, D.J., Sergeant, J.A., 2005. Telling good from bad news:

26

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT ADHD differentially affects processing of positive and negative feedback during guessing. Neuropsychologia 43, 1946-1954. Vila-Ballo, A., Hdez-Lafuente, P., Rostan, C., Cunillera, T., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., 2014. Neurophysiological correlates of error monitoring and inhibitory processing in juvenile violent

T

offenders. Biological Psychology 102, 141-152. Wiersema, R., van der Meere, J., Roeyers, H., Van Coster, R., Baeyens, D., 2006. Event rate and

IP

event-related potentials in ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 47, 560-567.

SC R

Wu, Y., Zhou, X., 2009. The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation. Brain Research 1286, 114-122.

Yao, S., Zou, T., Zhu, X., Abela, J.R., Auerbach, R.P., Tong, X., 2007a. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the multidimensional anxiety scale for children among Chinese secondary school

NU

students. Child Psychiatry and Human Development 38, 1-16.

Yao, S.Q., Yang, H.Q., Zhu, X.Z., Auerbach, R.P., Abela, J.R.Z., Pulleyblank, R.W., Tong, X., 2007b. An examination of the psychometric properties of the chinese version of the Barratt Impulsiveness

MA

Scale, 11th version in a sample of chinese adolescents. Perceptual and Motor Skills 104, 1169-1182. Yeung, N., Sanfey, A.G., 2004. Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience 24, 6258-6264.

Yi, F., Chen, H., Wang, X., Shi, H., Yi, J., Zhu, X., Yao, S., 2012. Amplitude and latency of

D

feedback-related negativity: aging and sex differences. Neuroreport 23, 963-969.

TE

Young, P.T., 1959. The role of affective processes in learning and motivation. Psychological Review 66, 104-125.

Zhang, J., Zhu, X., Wang, X., Gao, J., Shi, H., Huang, B., Situ, W., Yi, J., Yao, S., 2014. Increased

CE P

structural connectivity in corpus callosum in adolescent males with conduct disorder. J Am Acad Child

AC

Adolesc Psychiatry 53, 466-475 e461.

27

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figures

T

Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the single outcome gambling task

SC R

IP

One of the two numbers (10 or 50) in the choice stimulus (800 ms) is displayed to be selected by the participants. The selected number results in feedback stimulus, which could indicate a “gain” (+10 or +50) or “loss” (-10 or -50). (A) Typical trial showing a

NU

loss of 10 in the box (-10). (B) Trial showing a gain of 50 in the box (+50). (C) Time

MA

window for task events: the selection window (1000 ms) wherein the subject selects either of the numbers, and the analysis window (200 ms prestimulus + 800 ms

TE

D

poststimulus) that was used for the ERP analyses.

CE P

Figure 2. Average ERP waveforms and Topographic scalp maps for both groups

ERP waveforms showing comparisons for each condition across groups on electrode sites of Fz, Cz and Pz, respectively. Gain condition-evoked potentials are depicted on

AC

the left side, and loss condition-evoked potentials on the right side. Solid and dashed blue lines represent the average ERP waveforms for the healthy controls group. Solid and dashed red lines represent the average waveforms for the CD group. Topographies delineate the differences between groups and all conditions in the indicated time windows.

Figure 3. Schematic illustration of the mean voltage and standard error (SE) of the N1, P2, N2 and P3 in both groups

(A) Comparison of mean voltage and SE of the N1 at FCz electrode site. (B) 28

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Comparison of mean voltage and SE of the P2 at Cz electrode site. (C) Comparison of mean voltage and SE of the N2 at Fz electrode site. (D) Comparison of mean voltage

IP

T

and SE of the P3 at Pz electrode site. Comparisons between CD individuals (black

SC R

bars) with healthy controls (grey bars) were elicited by negative (-10, -50) and

AC

CE P

TE

D

MA

NU

positive (+10, +50) feedback. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

29

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Tables

t

p

-0.19

0.85

-1.30

0.20

30.74 ± 11.1

-0.42

0.68

38.95 ± 15.2

-0.03

0.98

HC (n = 19)

15.44 ± 1.3

15.53 ± 1.3

IQ

101.20 ± 11.2

104.80 ± 9.3

CES-D

28.89 ± 15.6

MASC

38.78 ± 16.9

NU

Age

SC R

CD (n = 18)

IP

T

Table 1- Demographic and behavioral characteristics of study groups

Chinese version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) Attentional

19.00 ± 2.5

Motor

25.56 ± 5.1

Non-planned Total

3.42

0.002**

20.95 ± 4.8

2.83

0.008**

28.50 ± 3.9

25.21 ± 4.6

2.35

0.025*

73.06 ± 9.2

63.79 ± 8.9

3.46

0.001**

58.20 ± 22.9

59.00 ± 15.6

-0.12

0.90

72.60 ± 22.4

68.56 ± 17.3

0.62

0.54

59.75 ± 22.6

61.83 ±18.2

-0.31

0.76

Gain 50

75.90 ± 23.7

72.94 ±18.1

0.17

0.67

Reaction time

321.08 ± 55.3

297.14 ± 37.6

1.54

0.13

Loss 10 Loss 50

AC

Gain 10

CE P

Usable trials

TE

D

MA

16.11 ± 2.6

CD: conduct disorder group; HC: healthy control group; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depress Scale; MASC, Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children. Difference are statistically significant at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

30

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

N1 amplitude

P2 amplitude

N2 amplitude

P3 amplitude

F

p

F

p

F

F

p

Group

4.51

0.041*

4.41

0.043*

6.56

0.015*

T

4.99

0.032*

Valence

3.46

0.07

0.48

0.50

IP

Table 2- Mixed-model ANOVA results

7.98

0.008**

17.71

< .001***

Magnitude

0.20

0.66

14.34

0.001**

12.75

0.001**

14.57

0.001**

Electrode

5.57

0.006**

15.91

< .001***

48.24

< .001***





Group ×valence

0.09

0.76

4.35

0.044*

5.17

0.029*

0.81

0.38

Group ×magnitude

1.10

0.30

0.60

0.45

0.27

0.61

1.10

0.30

Group ×electrode

0.34

0.67

2.82

0.07

6.95

0.007**





Valence ×magnitude

1.65

0.21

0.80

0.38

2.19

0.15

0.37

0.55

Group ×valence ×magnitude

0.88

0.36

1.81

0.19

0.43

0.52

0.80

0.38

Valence ×electrode

3.48

0.06

2.39

0.12

1.79

0.18





Group ×valence ×electrode

0.26

0.66

0.50

0.55

1.03

0.35





0.46

0.57

1.56

0.22

4.04

0.030*





0.93

0.37

0.95

0.38

0.39

0.64





0.85

0.39

2.21

0.14

0.24

0.68





0.34

0.62

0.23

0.69

0.40

0.58





Variable or interaction

CE P

Group ×magnitude ×electrode

Valence ×magnitude ×electrode

SC R

NU

MA

D

TE

Magnitude ×electrode

p

×electrode

AC

Group ×valence ×magnitude

Difference is statistically significant at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

31

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 3 - Correlation analysis results

BIS

BIS

BIS

Attentional

Motor

Non-planned

Total

p

IP

r

p

SC R

0.87

0.14

0.42

-0.04

0.83

0.11

0.53

0.08

0.64

0.22

0.21

0.10

0.59

0.19

0.26

0.61

0.06

0.72

0.14

0.40

0.97

0.04

0.83

0.06

0.71

0.76

0.05

0.77

0.12

0.46

0.76

0.02

0.91

0.02

0.92

D

T

BIS

0.06

0.28

0.09

0.38

0.018*

0.16

0.34

0.21

0.21

0.24

0.14

0.30

0.07

0.32

0.05

0.40*

0.013*

0.048*

0.15

0.36

0.25

0.13

0.26

0.12

0.05

- 0.13

0.45

-.0.02

0.92

- 0.14

0.42

0.004**

- 0.23

0.19

- 0.01

0.99

- 0.24

0.17

- 0.39

0.021*

- 0.21

0.22

- 0.01

0.99

- 0.18

0.29

- 0.43

0.009**

- 0.21

0.22

- 0.10

0.57

- 0.25

0.15

p

r

p

r

N1 -10

0.34

0.047*

0.11

0.52

-0.03

N1 +10

0.28

0.09

0.06

0.74

N1 -50

0.27

0.11

0.20

0.26

N1 +50

0.30

0.09

0.17

0.34

P2 -10

0.21

0.22

0.09

P2 +10

0.11

0.52

0.01

P2 -50

0.23

0.17

0.05

P2 +50

0.14

0.40

-0.05

N2 -10

0.37

0.021*

N2 +10

0.26

0.11

N2 -50

0.45

0.005**

N2 +50

0.32

P3 -10

- 0.33

P3 +10

- 0.48

P3 -50 P3 +50

MA

TE

0.30

CE P

AC

NU

r

Correlation coefficient is statistically significant at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 (all p-values were uncorrected).

32

MA

NU

SC R

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC

CE P

TE

D

Fig. 1

33

AC

Fig. 2

CE P

TE

D

MA

NU

SC R

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

34

AC

Fig. 3

CE P

TE

D

MA

NU

SC R

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

35

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Highlights

SC R

We examined ERP correlates of reward processing in conduct disorder individuals. Participants chose between small- and large-amount options in a gambling task.

NU

Conduct disorder group shows decreased N2 following loss feedback. Conduct disorder group shows overall decreased P3 amplitudes. Feedback-related ERPs were modulated by effects of feedback valence and

AC

CE P

TE

D

MA

magnitude.

36