Journal Pre-proof Effects of chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during early and late adolescence on anxiety-like behaviors and behavioral flexibility in adulthood ´ Trevor Towner, David F. Elena I. Varlinskaya, Dominika Hosova, Werner, Linda P. Spear
PII:
S0166-4328(19)30472-3
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112292
Reference:
BBR 112292
To appear in:
Behavioural Brain Research
Received Date:
26 March 2019
Revised Date:
19 August 2019
Accepted Date:
7 October 2019
Please cite this article as: Varlinskaya EI, Hosova´ D, Towner T, Werner DF, Spear LP, Effects of chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during early and late adolescence on anxiety-like behaviors and behavioral flexibility in adulthood, Behavioural Brain Research (2019), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112292
This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier.
Effects of chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during early and late adolescence on anxiety-like behaviors and behavioral flexibility in adulthood. Elena I. Varlinskaya, Dominika Hosová, Trevor Towner, David F. Werner, Linda P. Spear Neurobiology of Adolescent Drinking in Adulthood Consortium (NADIA) Center for Development and Behavioral Neuroscience Department of Psychology Binghamton University
of
Binghamton, NY 13902-6000
Corresponding Author: David Werner
ro
Email:
[email protected] Phone: 607-777-5782
re
-p
Fax: 607-777-4890
lP
The authors have no conflict of interest to disclose.
The work presented in this manuscript was funded by grants U01 AA019972 (Neurobiology of Adolescent Drinking in Adulthood Consortium - NADIA Project) and T32 AA025606 (
Abstract
ur na
Development and Neuroadaptation in Alcohol and Addictions – DNAA Project)
Although both humans and laboratory rodents demonstrate cognitive and affective alterations
Jo
associated with adolescent alcohol exposure, it is still unknown whether the consequences of early initiation of alcohol use differ from those of later binge drinking within the adolescent developmental period. The present study was designed to assess the effects of early and late AIE on (1) anxiety-like behavior under social (modified social interaction test) and non-social test circumstances (modified light/dark box test, elevated plus maze), and (2) behavioral flexibility, indexed via set shifting in males and females. Early-mid adolescent intermittent exposure (early
AIE) occurred between postnatal days (P) 25 and 45, whereas late adolescent intermittent exposure (late AIE) was conducted between P45 and P65, with behavioral testing initiated not earlier than 25 days after repeated exposure to ethanol (4.0 g/kg intragastrically, every other day for a total of 11 exposures). Anxiety-like behavior on the EPM was evident in males and females following early AIE, whereas only males demonstrated non-social anxiety on the EPM following late AIE. Social anxiety-like alterations and deficits in behavioral flexibility were
of
evident only in males following early AIE. Taken together, the results of the present study
ro
demonstrate a particular vulnerability of young adolescent males to long-lasting detrimental effects of repeated ethanol and an insensitivity of older adolescent females to the intermittent
-p
ethanol exposure paradigm.
1. Introduction
lP
Exposure timing; Sex differences
re
Keywords: Adolescent ethanol exposure; Anxiety; Behavioral flexibility; Social interaction;
ur na
Alcohol use typically begins during early adolescence [1], with this use potentially contributing to the risk of later alcohol abuse/dependence. Early initiation of use is especially concerning given that adolescents who begin drinking at 14 years of age or earlier are four times more likely to become alcohol-dependent relative to those who started drinking at 20 years of
Jo
age or later [2-4]. According to recent data from the Monitoring the Future survey, annual prevalence rates for alcohol use are 21%, 42% and 58% for 8th, 10th and 12th graders, respectively [5]. Over 20% of high school seniors report consumption of 5 or more drinks per occasion (i.e., binge levels of drinking) within the past 2 weeks, with 10% endorsing consumption of 10 or more drinks, and over 5% reporting consumption of 15 or more drinks over the same period [6]. Binge patterns of alcohol consumption (five or more drinks in males, or
four or more drinks in females within a 2-hour period that result in blood alcohol levels of 80 mg/dL and higher) are thought to be particularly harmful to the developing adolescent brain [7, 8]. Binge patterns of drinking among early adolescents (ages from 10 to 14) are even more alarming, since estimated peak blood alcohol concentrations (BECs) following 5 drinks are around 280 mg/dL for 10-year-olds and about 160 mg/dL for 14-year-old adolescents [see: 9]. Adverse consequences of adolescent alcohol use in humans are not limited to an increased
of
vulnerability for alcohol use disorders. Although few studies have investigated the impact of
ro
adolescent alcohol exposure on other adult psychiatric disorders, the research available reports relatively strong associations between alcohol use during adolescence and adult anxiety and
-p
depression disorders [10, 11]. Alcohol-related problems are especially common in adolescents
re
with social anxiety [12, 13], although it is still not clear whether alcohol use during adolescence can enhance or even elicit social anxiety. Alcohol exposure during adolescence in humans has
lP
also been associated with poor performance on a variety of cognitive measures, ranging from deficits in attention, memory and visuospatial function to impaired executive functions [14-17].
ur na
Much of this work has been cross-sectional and hence cannot be used to draw strong conclusions about causality.
In contrast, substantial animal research has emerged to assess long-lasting, causal, behavioral, cognitive, affective, neural and molecular consequences of adolescent ethanol
Jo
exposure [see: 18 for review]. In such studies, ethanol has been intermittently administered to adolescent rodents through a variety of routes, including intragastric gavage (for advantages and limitations see [19]) for varying lengths of time during the broad adolescent developmental period ranging from postnatal day (P) 25 up to P65 (see [18, 20, 21] for references and review). In rats, a conservative age range during which both males and females exhibit adolescent-typical
neurobehavioral characteristics initially was characterized as P28–42 [22, 23]. More recently, based on physiological and hormonal changes, the age span has been expanded to consider the period between P25 and P45 as the pre- and peri-pubertal period of early-mid adolescence and the interval between P45 and P65 as late adolescence/emerging adulthood [21, 24]. These age spans correspond to approximately 10–18 and 18–25 years of age in humans, respectively [21]. Research conducted by the Neurobiology of Adolescent Drinking in Adulthood
of
Consortium (NADIA) [reviewed in: 18, 20, 21] has revealed highly specific cognitive
ro
impairments as well as anxiety-like behavioral alterations in adulthood following adolescent ethanol exposure in rodents. For instance, although adolescent intermittent ethanol exposure
-p
(AIE) had little impact on simple spatial learning tasks [25] and more challenging five-choice
re
serial reaction time tests [26], AIE-associated deficits have been reported on tasks requiring behavioral flexibility, including impaired set shifting [27, 28], decreased reversal acquisition [29,
lP
30], and delayed context fear extinction [31]. Enhancements of anxiety-like behavior following AIE have also been reported for non-social (elevated plus maze, light/dark box) as well as social
ur na
tests of anxiety [32-34].
Although both humans and laboratory rodents demonstrate cognitive and affective alterations associated with adolescent alcohol exposure, it is still unknown whether the consequences of alcohol use during early adolescence differ from those of later drinking within
Jo
the adolescent developmental period. Indeed, early drinking onset individuals are often continuing drinking later in adolescence [35], making it almost impossible to separate adverse consequences of early exposure from those associated with heavy drinking later in adolescence. In rats, cognitive and anxiety-like alterations associated with AIE have emerged following
repeated intermittent exposure to ethanol initiated during early adolescence and continuing through the entire adolescent period [27, 28, 32, 36, 37]. Our recent work has shown, however, that social anxiety-like alterations following AIE are sex- and exposure timing-dependent [34]. Specifically, when effects of AIE during early-mid adolescence (P25–45) versus late adolescence/emerging adulthood (P45–65) were assessed, adult male rats exposed to ethanol during early-mid adolescence (early AIE) but not late adolescence
of
(late AIE) were found to exhibit long-lasting social anxiety-like alterations (indexed via
ro
decreases in social investigation and social preference); these effects were not evident in early exposed females [33, 34]. Given these findings, it appears that the cognitive and affective
re
with ethanol exposure initiated later in adolescence.
-p
consequences of ethanol exposure during early adolescence may differ from those associated
The proposed differences in the adverse consequences of early and late AIE may stem
lP
from differential maturation of brain regions critical for emotional responding and those implicated in cognitive, top-down control. Prefrontal cortical regions implicated in behavioral
ur na
flexibility show delayed maturation [38, 39] relative to more rapidly maturing limbic regions that are critically involved in processing of emotional and rewarding stimuli [40]. Therefore, affective alterations may be associated with early AIE as a result of disrupted limbic maturation, whereas cognitive alterations may be more pronounced following late AIE as a result of
Jo
disrupted maturation of prefrontal cortical regions. The present study was designed to test this hypothesis by assessing the effects of early and late AIE on (1) anxiety-like behavior under social and non-social test circumstances, and (2) behavioral flexibility, indexed via set shifting in males and females.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Subjects Male and female Sprague Dawley rats bred and reared at Binghamton University were used in all experiments. Animals were housed in a temperature controlled (22°C) vivarium on a 12/12-hour light/dark cycle (lights on at 0700) with ad libitum access to food and water prior to the start of testing in adulthood. Litters were culled to 8-10 pups, maintaining a sex ratio of six
of
males and four females whenever possible. At P21 animals were weaned and pair-housed either
ro
with a same-sex littermate (for assessments of anxiety-like behavior) or a same-sex nonlittermate (for assessment of behavioral flexibility). Animal use and maintenance was in
-p
accordance with the guidelines for animal care established by the National Institutes of Health,
re
and all protocols used were approved by the Binghamton University Institutional Animal Care
lP
and Use Committee.
2.2 Intermittent Ethanol Exposure
ur na
Males and females were exposed to ethanol intragastrically (i.g.) at a dose of 4 g/kg (25% ethanol solution in tap water, v/v) every other day between 1100 and 1500 hours (11 exposures). Control animals were given an isovolumetric amount of tap water i.g. on each exposure day. Early-mid adolescent animals were exposed to water or ethanol from P25 to P45 (early AIE),
Jo
whereas late adolescent animals were given water or ethanol from P45 to P65 (late AIE). Following the final exposure, all animals were left undisturbed for 20-25 days, depending on the specific experiment (see below), thereby allowing animals to mature into adulthood prior to behavioral testing. Intragastric ethanol exposure at this level produces peak BECs well into the binge range at the start (~ 200 mg/dl) and end (~ 130 mg/dl) of the chronic exposure period [41].
These BECs are also in line with estimates for early adolescents following 5 standard drinks [9] and are well within the range of BECs obtained from adolescents drinking in a field setting where levels up to 300 mg/dl have been observed [e.g., 42]. Cage-mates were assigned to the same exposure condition, with only one male and one female subject from a given litter assigned to each exposure/timing/testing condition to minimize litter effects [43, 44].
of
2.3. Anxiety-like Behavior
ro
2.3.1. Experimental Design
Effects of AIE on anxiety-like behavior were assessed using three different paradigms:
-p
light/dark box, modified social interaction test, and elevated plus maze. The design was a 2
re
(Exposure: water, ethanol) X 2 (Timing: early adolescence, late adolescence) X 2 (Sex: male, female) factorial (n = 7-9 per condition), with data analyzed separately for males and females
lP
given known sex differences following early AIE [e.g., 33, 34]. In this study, animals were pairhoused with same-sex littermates, since one animal from each littermate pair was tested in the
ur na
light/dark box while the other animal was assessed in the modified social interaction test. These housing conditions allowed for utilization of all animals in a given litter, with all experimental conditions for a certain exposure timing presented within a litter. Testing occurred on P70 for the early-exposed animals and P90 for animals in the late adolescent exposure group. For testing,
Jo
subjects were first habituated to the light/dark box testing room in their home cage for one hour. One animal was then tested in the light/dark box while the other was immediately placed into the social interaction chamber in an adjacent room, to habituate there. Following testing of both animals, they were returned to the colony room. Two days later, all subjects were tested using the elevated plus maze (P72 or P92).
2.3.2. Modified Light/Dark Box (LDB) Test The apparatus was composed of a larger, brightly lit (30 lux) chamber (40.64 cm wide x 49.5 cm long x 29.85 cm high) with a clear Plexiglas lid, connected to a smaller, dark (0-2 lux) chamber (40.64 cm wide x 40.64 cm long x 29.85 cm high) with an opaque lid; both chambers had Plexiglas flooring which was left bare for testing. Animals were free to cross between the two sides of the apparatus through a circular aperture (7.6 cm diameter). Testing occurred on P70
of
or P90 between 1400-1700, lasted five minutes, was conducted in the presence of a white noise
ro
generator, and recorded by video camera. Measures scored from the recordings included latency to enter the other apparatus side (all four paws), total number of crosses between sides, and time
-p
spent on each side.
re
Unlike many studies using the LDB, each test session was initiated by placing the animal into the dark compartment, rather than the light one. We found recently (unpublished data) that
lP
initially placing animals onto the light side of the apparatus resulted in a substantial portion of animals freezing and remaining there for the duration of the session with clear signs of
ur na
discomfort (e.g., large amounts of defecation) as has been noted elsewhere [45, 46]. Between animals, the LDB was cleaned with 6% hydrogen peroxide and wiped dry. 2.3.3. Modified Social Interaction (SI) Test The modified social interaction (SI) test was conducted on P70 or P90 between 1400-
Jo
1700 hours using standard procedures in our laboratory [34]. Testing was conducted under dim light (15-20 lux) with white noise, in a Plexiglas chamber (46 x 30.3 x 30.5 cm) containing pine shavings and divided into two equally-sized compartments with an aperture in the partition to allow free movements of the animals between compartments. At the onset of this procedure, each experimental animal was placed alone inside the chamber for a 30-minute period of habituation
to the novel environment. Then, an unfamiliar social partner of the same sex and age was placed on the opposite side of the chamber from the experimental animal for a ten-minute social interaction test. The partner was non-manipulated, drug-naive, non-socially deprived prior to the test and weighed approximately 10-20 g less than the experimental animal. In order to differentiate experimental animals from their social partners during the test, each experimental animal was marked with a line on its back with an indelible marker.
of
Test sessions were recorded by a video camera and scored at a later date by a trained
ro
experimenter without knowledge of the experimental condition of any given animal. Two anxiety-sensitive behavioral measures, namely social investigation and the coefficient of social
-p
preference/avoidance [47-49], were scored and analyzed. Social investigation frequency was
re
defined as the sniffing of any part of the body of the partner whereas social preference/avoidance was indexed via movements of the experimental animal through the aperture toward or away
lP
from the non-manipulated partner using a social preference/avoidance coefficient calculated as: (crossovers to the partner – crossovers away from the partner) / (total number of crossovers both
ur na
to and away from the partner) x 100 [50]. The total number of crossovers (movements between compartments) demonstrated by each experimental subject was used as an index of general locomotor activity under social test circumstances. The apparatus was cleaned with 6% hydrogen peroxide and bedding replaced between animals.
Jo
2.3.4. Elevated Plus Maze (EPM)
EPM testing occurred on P72 or P92 between 1100 – 1300 hours. The maze consisted of
two open arms and two closed arms, with each arm being 17.5 cm wide and 48.3 cm in length. The open arms had small plastic edges (1.3 cm high) located along each side and end of the open arms to prevent the animals from slipping off the edge. The closed arms were surrounded by
walls 29.2 cm tall, with the entire maze elevated 50.0 cm above the floor. Prior to testing, animals were brought from the vivarium in their home cages into a room adjacent to the testing room, with dim overhead lighting (15-20 lux) and white noise. At that time, cage-mates were separated by a mesh divider in the home cage where they remained for one hour to decrease initial anxiety levels [47]. EPM testing was conducted in an adjacent testing room, with lighting of 15-20 lux in the open arms, 5-10 lux in the closed arms, and a white noise generator to help
of
mask extraneous noise. Each animal was allowed to explore the maze for 5 minutes while being
ro
video recorded. Cage-mates were tested sequentially, with the apparatus cleaned with 6% hydrogen peroxide between animals. Measures scored from the video recordings include time
-p
spent in the open arms, open arm entries, and closed arm entries. Percent open arm time and
re
percent open arm entries were analyzed as the most common measures of anxiety-like behavior on the EPM, with closed arm entries used to index general locomotor activity [51]. Since prior SI
lP
testing or testing in the LDB had no effects on the EPM measures, means from each pair of littermates tested on the EPM were calculated and used for statistical analyses to avoid including
ur na
more than one animal of each sex from a given litter in a given test condition.
2.4. Behavioral Flexibility (BF) 2.4.1. Experimental Design
Jo
The consequences of AIE and timing of exposure on behavioral flexibility were assessed
using a 2 (Exposure: water, ethanol) X 2 (Timing: early adolescence, late adolescence) X 2 (Sex: male, female) factorial experimental design (n=12 per each condition), with data analyzed separately for each sex, given our prior findings of sex differences in the consequences of AIE. In contrast to measures of anxiety, animals that underwent adolescent exposure and behavioral
flexibility testing in adulthood were pair-housed with non-littermates at the time of weaning (P21). Whereas multiple tests of anxiety were used, only a single measure of behavioral flexibility (set-shifting) was administered and rehousing with non-littermates allowed for both animals in a cage to undergo the same adolescent exposure and adulthood operant procedure.
2.4.2. Procedure
of
Behavioral flexibility was assessed using an operant set-shifting task as described
ro
elsewhere [52]. Five days prior to the initiation of the operant procedure (P65 or P85), animals were gradually food restricted to 85% of free-feeding weights. Animals were exposed to the
-p
reinforcer (banana-flavored sucrose pellets, Bio-Serv) used in the operant procedure for three
re
days prior to the initiation of operant training by placing roughly 20 pellets per animal in their home cages. The operant procedure was initiated on P70 or P90. In brief, the set-shifting task
lP
requires animals to first associate a light cue with a rewarded lever (set) and, upon meeting criterion, switch to a location-based rule. A criterion of 10 consecutive correct responses was
ur na
used for both the cue and location rules [52, 53]. The number of trials required for acquiring the cue rule (cue acquisition) was used as an indication of baseline differences in cognitive ability. The number of trials required to acquire the location rule (set shift) was used to index behavioral flexibility. Errors made during the shift were broken into three categories: perseverative
Jo
(responding on the cue rule), regressive (responding on the cue rule following a number of correct responses on set shift), and never reinforced (responding on lever that was not associated with the cue or location rules) [52]. Omissions (defined as trials for which no response on either lever was recorded) were excluded from consideration. Animals that failed to meet criterion at any point during the set-shifting task (training, acquisition, or shift) and those that scored two
standard deviations beyond the mean on trials to completion during cue acquisition were characterized as outliers and removed from the study (resulting in the elimination of 1 early water-exposed male, 1 early ethanol-exposed male, 2 late ethanol-exposed males, 2 late waterexposed females, 1 early ethanol-exposed female, and 1 late ethanol-exposed female).
2.5. Statistical analyses
of
All anxiety and behavioral flexibility measures were initially assessed using separate 2
ro
(Exposure: ethanol, water) X 2 (Timing: early adolescence, late adolescence) analyses of variance (ANOVAs) within each sex. Males and females were analyzed separately due to
-p
previous research showing AIE-induces social anxiety-like alterations in males, but not females
re
(Varlinskaya et al. 2014, 2017). Results of these analyses are presented in Table 1. When main effects of exposure or timing were evident or approached significance, Student’s one-tailed t-
lP
tests or Mann-Whitney U tests (in the cases of a non-normal distribution) were subsequently used to assess whether these main effects were driven by one age only. This approach allowed us
ur na
to assess the alterations resulting from the specific timing of adolescent exposure rather than exposure per se and to decrease concerns regarding false negatives. Significant results in all figures are direct comparisons between ethanol- and water-exposed groups within each
Jo
timing/sex condition made with Student’s one-tailed t-tests or Mann-Whitney U tests.
3. Results
3.2 Light/Dark Box (LDB)
When littermates of animals assessed socially were tested in the LDB, an overwhelming majority (86.3%) of subjects remained in the dark compartment for the entire test session (see Table 2). Due to floor effects, these data were not analyzed further.
3.2. Social Interaction (SI) Test When separate 2-way ANOVAs for each behavioral measure -- social investigation,
of
social preference, and total number of crossovers – were conducted within each sex, a significant
ro
effect of exposure emerged in males, but not females for social investigation, with ethanolexposed males, in general, demonstrating less social investigation than water-exposed males
-p
(172.7 ± 11.2 for water-exposed males; 139.7 ± 8.6 for ethanol-exposed males). Although no
re
significant main effect or interactions were evident for the social preference/avoidance coefficient in males (see Table 1), the main effect of exposure approached significance (p =
lP
0.07), reflecting a tendency for ethanol-exposed males to show lower social preference than water-exposed males (34.2 ± 5.1 for water-exposed males; 19.3 ± 6.1 for ethanol-exposed Further analyses that were restricted to comparisons between water- and ethanol-
ur na
males).
exposed animals separately for each timing period of adolescent exposure clearly demonstrated that exposure effects were driven by the early adolescent exposure in males only, with early AIE significantly reducing the frequency of social investigation (t15 = 2.343, p = 0.033) as well as the
Jo
preference/avoidance coefficient (t 15 = 2.685, p = 0.017), with no significant differences between water- and ethanol-exposed males evident for these measures (social investigation: t 13 = 0.618, p = 0.547; social preference: t13 = 0.888, p = 0.391) following late AIE (see Figure 1a,b,c,). In females, social investigation, social preference, and total number of crossovers did not differ as a function of exposure or exposure timing (see Table 1, Figure 1a,b,c).
3.3. Elevated Plus Maze (EPM) Two-way ANOVAs for percent open arm time and percent open arm entries revealed significant main effects of exposure for both measures in males (see Table 1), with ethanolexposed males demonstrating lower percent of open arm time (5.1 ± 1.2 % for water-exposed males; 0.5 ± 0.2% for ethanol-exposed males) and open arm entries (1.5% for water-exposed; 2.6 ± 0.7% for ethanol-exposed males) than their water-exposed counterparts. Further analyses
of
assessing the effects of AIE within each adolescent timing period in males revealed that late AIE
ro
significantly decreased percent open arm entries (t 15 = 2.70, p = 0.016; see Figure 1d) and percent open arm time on the EPM (U = 16.00, p = 0.028; Figure 1e). Males exposed to ethanol
-p
during early adolescence also demonstrated a significant decrease in percent open arm entries (U
re
= 19.00, p = 0.05, Figure 1d, e), with a decrease in percent arm time being non-significant (U = 20.00, p = 0.06). Although no main effects for the EPM anxiety measures were evident in
lP
females, (see Table 1), Student t- test showed that early AIE significantly reduced percent open arm time (t 16 = 2.284, p = 0.018, see Figure 1d) and percent open arm entries (t 16 = 2.199, p =
ur na
0.022, see Fig 1e), whereas late AIE did not affect either of these anxiety-related measures (percent open arm time: t 16 = 0.498, p = 0.312; percent open arm entries: t 16 = 0.123, p = 0.452). Closed arm entries were not affected by early- and late-AIE in either males or females (Table 1, Figure 1f). Overall, these data suggest that unlike social anxiety, early AIE enhances non-social
Jo
anxiety in both sexes, with males continuing to be vulnerable to AIE-associated non-social anxiety into late adolescence. 3.4. Behavioral Flexibility (BF) Two-way ANOVAs of the acquisition data (i.e, number of trials required for acquiring the cue rule) did not reveal significant main effects or interactions in either males or females (see
Table 1, Figure 2a). Similarly, no main effects or interactions were evident in either sex when the number of trials required to acquire the location rule (set shift) as well as the number of total errors made during set shift were analyzed (Table 1, Figure 2b). When the three error types during the set shift were analyzed, a significant main effect of exposure emerged for regressive errors in males (6.83 ± 0.92 for water-exposed, 11.00 ± 1.31 for ethanol-exposed males). In females, significant main effects of timing were evident for both
of
regressive (7.30 ± 1.12 for early exposure, 11.71 ± 1.67 for late exposure) and never reinforced
ro
(2.17 ± 0.43 for early exposure, 3.91 ± 0.41 for late exposure) errors (see Table 1), with older females, in general, demonstrating more errors than their less mature counterparts. Further
-p
comparisons within each timing/sex condition demonstrated that the number of regressive errors
re
was significantly increased in males following early AIE (t 20 = 2.235, p = 0.019, see Figure 2e), whereas late AIE in males (t20 =1.430, p = 0.084) as well as early (t21 =1.230, p = 0.116) or late
ur na
4. Discussion
lP
(t19 = 0.104, p = 0.459) AIE in females had no effects on these measures.
The results of the present study demonstrate that long-lasting detrimental effects of repeated ethanol exposure were sex- and timing of exposure-dependent. In males, but not females, early adolescent ethanol exposure resulted in the emergence of social anxiety-like
Jo
alterations and some deficits in behavioral flexibility. In contrast, males and females exposed to ethanol during early-mid adolescence, as well as males exposed to ethanol during late adolescence, demonstrated anxiety-like behavior on the EPM. These findings suggest that, unlike social alterations, early AIE induces non-social anxiety regardless of sex, with males also showing non-social anxiety when AIE exposure was delayed until late adolescence-emerging
adulthood. Similar anxiety-like alterations on the EPM have been reported in Sprague Dawley males following intraperitoneal administration of ethanol (2 g/kg) during early-mid adolescence [32, 36, 37]. In contrast, no behavioral changes on the EPM were reported in male Wistar rats following intragastric ethanol exposure (3 g/kg, 6 exposures between P37 and P44; Torcaso et al., 2017), while other studies have shown decreased anxiety-like behavior on the EPM following early AIE [27, 54]. For instance, intermittent exposure to ethanol by vapor inhalation of Long-
of
Evans adolescent male rats (P28-P42) was found to increase percent open arm time and open
ro
arm entries, suggesting an early AIE-associated reduction in non-social anxiety [27]. Similarly, ethanol self-administration early in adolescence (P28-P42) was reported to decrease anxiety-like
-p
behavior on the EPM in male Wistar rats tested in adulthood [54]. These discrepancies may be
re
associated with procedural differences between the studies that include, but are not limited to, different strains, different routes of ethanol administration, breeding in-house versus shipping of
lP
young animals, different levels of ethanol exposure, pre-test manipulations, testing during light versus dark part of the light/dark cycle, and handling duration [55, 56]. In the two studies
ur na
reporting significant decreases in anxiety-like behavior on the EPM, animals were tested during the dark part of the light/dark cycle under extremely low light and hence extremely low anxietyprovoking test conditions [27, 54]. It seems likely that the observed increases in open arm exploration under these non-anxiety-provoking test circumstances may reflect AIE-associated
Jo
disinhibition rather than decreased anxiety-like behavior, since the behavioral expression of anxiety and disinhibition can compete depending on the characteristics of the test situation [57]. The social consequences of AIE differed as a function of exposure timing and sex.
Similar to our earlier findings [33, 34, 58], late AIE did not affect social behavior, whereas socially anxiogenic effects of early AIE were evident only in males. The confirmed differences
in the social consequences of early versus late AIE suggest that neural systems affected by repeated ethanol exposure differ in early-mid and late adolescent males. Social anxiety-like alterations may be associated with ethanol-induced adaptations in neural systems implicated in social behavior and anxiety. For instance, recent studies have shown involvement of frontal cortical regions, the amygdala, and the hippocampus in peer-directed social interactions in juvenile and adolescent animals [59-62], with these regions also implicated in the modulation of
of
non-social anxiety-like behavior [63-66]. Substantial remodeling of these brain areas during
ro
adolescence [22, 67-70] suggests high vulnerability of these regions to disruption by repeated ethanol exposure at the time of early AIE [18].
-p
Brain oxytocin (OXT) and vasopressin (AVP) systems are among the neural systems that
re
are involved in social alterations evident in adult males following early AIE. These neuropeptide systems play a substantial role in the regulation of social behaviors, are sexually dimorphic, and
lP
are differentially involved in the modulation of anxiety-like behavior, especially anxiety evident under social circumstances [71, 72].
Whereas the brain OXT system is critical for social
ur na
approach and social preference [73], activation of the AVP system typically exerts anxiogenic effects [see: 72 for references and review]. Both V1a [74, 75] and V1b [76] types of AVP receptors are involved in the modulation of anxiety-related behaviors, with V1a receptors playing a more substantial role in the modulation of non-social anxiety [74] and V1b receptors
Jo
contributing to social anxiety [76]. Given the sex differences in OXT and AVP brain systems and the separable roles of these peptides in modulating anxiety-like alterations in social contexts, our recent study tested the hypothesis [see: 72] that social anxiety evident in adult males following early AIE is associated with alterations in OXT and AVP systems [58]. This hypothesis was supported by our confirmed, with the data showing early AIE-induced changes in OXT and AVP
receptor surface expression in the hypothalamus only in males, characterized by significant decreases in hypothalamic neuronal surface exposure of OXT receptors along with increases in V1b receptor expression [58]. Importantly, these social anxiety-like alterations seen in adult males following early AIE were separately reversed either by pharmacological stimulation of OXT or by blockade of V1b receptors; V1a receptor blockade was ineffective [58]. These findings support an involvement of OXT and V1b receptors (but not V1a) receptors) in social
of
sex-specific consequences of early AIE. In contrast, given evidence for a role of V1a receptors in
ro
the modulation of non-social anxiety [74], it is possible that these receptors might play a role in the non-social anxiety evident in the EPM in both males and females following early AIE and in
-p
males after late AIE. This possibility has yet to be tested.
re
Relatively mild alterations in behavioral flexibility were evident in males exposed to ethanol during early adolescence, with no effects observed in females. That is, when using a set-
lP
shifting operant task as an index of behavioral flexibility, the early AIE males demonstrated a significant increase in the number of regressive during the set-shifting task (i.e., responding on
ur na
the original cue rule following a number of correct responses on the set shift) than their waterexposed counterparts, suggesting certain difficulties in switching from the cue to the location rule. These difficulties suggest alterations in functioning of prefrontal cortical regions associated with early AIE, given that inactivation of the prelimbic region of the medial prefrontal cortex
Jo
(mPFC) impairs the shift to the new rule [77]. In contrast to males, females showed no deficits in behavioral flexibility following early AIE. The observed vulnerability of males, but not females, during early-mid adolescence to ethanol-associated alterations in behavioral flexibility may be related to later maturation of the mPFC in males than in females [see: 78 for references and review]. For instance, in rats, dendritic remodeling of the mPFC during adolescence occurs later
in males than in females, with no sex differences evident in the basolateral amygdala [69]. Since the brain is especially vulnerable to drugs during its development, with periods of pronounced developmental changes in brain regions sensitive to a given drug defining critical periods for induction of lasting drug-induced changes in those and associated regions [79], it is not surprising that males are more vulnerable than females to ethanol-associated cognitive alterations when exposure to ethanol occurs during early adolescence.
of
The results of the present study suggest that early adolescence, particularly in males, is a
ro
period of enhanced vulnerability to ethanol-associated long-term affective alterations. To a certain extent, these experimental findings are in agreement with human data. For instance,
-p
recent human research suggests that adolescents, especially males, who reported a shorter
re
interval between their first experience with alcohol and the onset of binging as well as firstoccasion binge users are more likely to have alcohol-related problems [80, 81]. Furthermore,
lP
BECs achieved with intragastric administration of the relatively high dose of ethanol in the rat model of adolescence are in line with estimates for early adolescents following 5 standard drinks
ur na
[9] and are also well in the range of those obtained from breathalyzer data in a local field studies of responsive alcohol drinkers in a college bar setting [42]. In humans, affective disorders, especially social anxiety, are comorbid with alcohol use disorders [82, 83], and our experimental findings are in agreement with the human research. It is still not clear, however, whether alcohol
Jo
use during adolescence can enhance social anxiety or whether pre-existing social anxiety put adolescents at risk for of alcohol use disorder [84], with alcohol perhaps being more appealing for these socially anxious adolescents due to its anxiolytic properties. The results of the present study as well as our previous data [33, 34, 58] support the suggestion that initiation and rapid progression to binge drinking during early adolescence may be a risk factor for the emergence of
anxiety, especially under social circumstances. The extent to which these findings are applicable to human adolescent binge drinking and its consequences later in life is limited by the forced method of ethanol exposure. Forced ethanol exposure, giving the precise control of exposure level and timing, can produce neural adaptations different from those associated with voluntary ethanol consumptions. Nevertheless, the results of the present study demonstrate that affective and cognitive alterations associated with AIE are
of
sex- and exposure-timing-dependent. In general, males were more sensitive to ethanol-associated
ro
alterations than females, with early exposure being more harmful than late exposure. Young adolescent males were particularly vulnerable to long-lasting detrimental effects of chronic
-p
ethanol exposure, whereas older adolescent females were insensitive to intermittent ethanol
re
exposure. However, exact mechanisms underlying timing- and sex-specific effects of AIE are not well understood. Therefore, more studies are needed to focus on the mechanisms of enhanced
lP
vulnerability of young adolescent males and resilience of older adolescent females to detrimental
ur na
long-lasting effects of repeated ethanol exposure.
REFERENCES:
1. 2.
Jo
3.
Faden, V.B., Trends in initiation of alcohol use in the United States 1975 to 2003. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2006. 30(6): p. 1011-22. Dawson, D.A., et al., Age at first drink and the first incidence of adult-onset DSM-IV alcohol use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2008. 32(12): p. 2149-60. Ehlers, C.L., et al., Age at first intoxication and alcohol use disorders in Southwest California Indians. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2006. 30(11): p. 1856-65. DeWit, D.J., et al., Age at first alcohol use: a risk factor for the development of alcohol disorders. Am J Psychiatry, 2000. 157(5): p. 745-50. Johnston, L.D., et al., Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 19752015: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, . 2016, The University of Michigan. Patrick, M.E., et al., Extreme binge drinking among 12th-grade students in the United States: prevalence and predictors. JAMA Pediatr, 2013. 167(11): p. 1019-25. Bava, S. and S.F. Tapert, Adolescent brain development and the risk for alcohol and other drug problems. Neuropsychol Rev, 2010. 20(4): p. 398-413.
4. 5.
6. 7.
14.
15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23.
Jo
24.
of
13.
ro
12.
-p
11.
re
10.
lP
9.
Silveri, M.M., Adolescent brain development and underage drinking in the United States: identifying risks of alcohol use in college populations. Harv Rev Psychiatry, 2012. 20(4): p. 189-200. Donovan, J.E., Estimated blood alcohol concentrations for child and adolescent drinking and their implications for screening instruments. Pediatrics, 2009. 123(6): p. e975-81. Jeanblanc, J., et al., Light alcohol intake during adolescence induces alcohol addiction in a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia. Addict Biol, 2015. 20(3): p. 490-9. Rohde, P., et al., Natural course of alcohol use disorders from adolescence to young adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 2001. 40(1): p. 83-90. Buckner, J.D., A.M. Eggleston, and N.B. Schmidt, Social anxiety and problematic alcohol consumption: the mediating role of drinking motives and situations. Behav Ther, 2006. 37(4): p. 381-91. Gilles, D.M., C.L. Turk, and D.M. Fresco, Social anxiety, alcohol expectancies, and selfefficacy as predictors of heavy drinking in college students. Addict Behav, 2006. 31(3): p. 388-98. Hanson, K.L., et al., Impact of Adolescent Alcohol and Drug Use on Neuropsychological Functioning in Young Adulthood: 10-Year Outcomes. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse, 2011. 20(2): p. 135-154. Jacobus, J., et al., White matter integrity, substance use, and risk taking in adolescence. Psychol Addict Behav, 2013. 27(2): p. 431-42. Lisdahl, K.M., et al., Dare to delay? The impacts of adolescent alcohol and marijuana use onset on cognition, brain structure, and function. Front Psychiatry, 2013. 4: p. 53. Nguyen-Louie, T.T., et al., Learning and Memory in Adolescent Moderate, Binge, and Extreme-Binge Drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2016. 40(9): p. 1895-904. Spear, L.P., Effects of adolescent alcohol consumption on the brain and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci, 2018. 19(4): p. 197-214. Knapp, D.J. and G.R. Breese, Models of chronic alcohol exposure and dependence. Methods Mol Biol, 2012. 829: p. 205-30. Crews, F.T., et al., Adolescent Alcohol Exposure Persistently Impacts Adult Neurobiology and Behavior. Pharmacol Rev, 2016. 68(4): p. 1074-1109. Spear, L.P., Adolescent alcohol exposure: Are there separable vulnerable periods within adolescence? Physiol Behav, 2015. 148: p. 122-30. Spear, L.P., The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2000. 24(4): p. 417-63. Spear, L.P. and S.C. Brake, Periadolescence: age-dependent behavior and psychopharmacological responsivity in rats. Dev Psychobiol, 1983. 16(2): p. 83-109. Vetter-O'Hagen, C.S. and L.P. Spear, The effects of gonadectomy on age- and sex-typical patterns of ethanol consumption in Sprague-Dawley rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2011. 35(11): p. 2039-49. Acheson, S.K., et al., Effects of acute or chronic ethanol exposure during adolescence on behavioral inhibition and efficiency in a modified water maze task. PLoS One, 2013. 8(10): p. e77768. Semenova, S., Attention, impulsivity, and cognitive flexibility in adult male rats exposed to ethanol binge during adolescence as measured in the five-choice serial reaction time task: the effects of task and ethanol challenges. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2012. 219(2): p. 433-42.
ur na
8.
25.
26.
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
Jo
39.
of
32.
ro
31.
-p
30.
re
29.
lP
28.
Gass, J.T., et al., Adolescent alcohol exposure reduces behavioral flexibility, promotes disinhibition, and increases resistance to extinction of ethanol self-administration in adulthood. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2014. 39(11): p. 2570-83. Fernandez, G.M., et al., Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure leads to alterations in brain-derived neurotrophic factor within the frontal cortex and impaired behavioral flexibility in both adolescent and adult rats. Neuroscience, 2017. 348: p. 324-334. Coleman, L.G., Jr., et al., Adolescent binge drinking alters adult brain neurotransmitter gene expression, behavior, brain regional volumes, and neurochemistry in mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2011. 35(4): p. 671-88. Coleman, L.G., Jr., et al., Adolescent binge ethanol treatment alters adult brain regional volumes, cortical extracellular matrix protein and behavioral flexibility. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2014. 116: p. 142-51. Broadwater, M., E.I. Varlinskaya, and L.P. Spear, Effects of voluntary access to sweetened ethanol during adolescence on intake in adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2013. 37(6): p. 1048-55. Pandey, S.C., et al., Potential role of adolescent alcohol exposure-induced amygdaloid histone modifications in anxiety and alcohol intake during adulthood. Neurobiol Dis, 2015. 82: p. 607-619. Varlinskaya, E.I., E.U. Kim, and L.P. Spear, Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence: Effects on stress-induced social alterations and social drinking in adulthood. Brain Res, 2017. 1654(Pt B): p. 145-156. Varlinskaya, E.I., E. Truxell, and L.P. Spear, Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence: Effects on social behavior and ethanol sensitivity in adulthood. Alcohol, 2014. Hingson, R.W. and W. Zha, Age of drinking onset, alcohol use disorders, frequent heavy drinking, and unintentionally injuring oneself and others after drinking. Pediatrics, 2009. 123(6): p. 1477-84. Kokare, D.M., et al., Adolescent Alcohol Exposure-Induced Changes in AlphaMelanocyte Stimulating Hormone and Neuropeptide Y Pathways via Histone Acetylation in the Brain During Adulthood. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol, 2017. 20(9): p. 758-768. Kyzar, E.J., et al., Adolescent alcohol exposure alters lysine demethylase 1 (LSD1) expression and histone methylation in the amygdala during adulthood. Addict Biol, 2017. 22(5): p. 1191-1204. Caballero, A., R. Granberg, and K.Y. Tseng, Mechanisms contributing to prefrontal cortex maturation during adolescence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2016. 70: p. 4-12. Mills, K.L., et al., The developmental mismatch in structural brain maturation during adolescence. Dev Neurosci, 2014. 36(3-4): p. 147-60. Casey, B.J. and R.M. Jones, Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 2010. 49(12): p. 1189-201; quiz 1285. Kim, E.U., et al., Adolescent intermittent ethanol exposure: Effects on pubertal development, novelty seeking, and social interaction in adulthood. Alcohol, 2019. 75: p. 19-29. Celio, M.A., et al., Are we drunk yet? Motor versus cognitive cues of subjective intoxication. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2014. 38(2): p. 538-44.
ur na
27.
40.
41.
42.
49.
50.
51. 52. 53. 54.
55.
56.
Jo
57.
of
48.
ro
47.
-p
46.
re
45.
lP
44.
Holson, R.R. and B. Pearce, Principles and pitfalls in the analysis of prenatal treatment effects in multiparous species. Neurotoxicol Teratol, 1992. 14(3): p. 221-8. Zorrilla, E.P., Multiparous species present problems (and possibilities) to developmentalists. Dev Psychobiol, 1997. 30(2): p. 141-50. Ardayfio, P. and K.S. Kim, Anxiogenic-like effect of chronic corticosterone in the lightdark emergence task in mice. Behav Neurosci, 2006. 120(2): p. 249-56. Steimer, T., P. Driscoll, and P.E. Schulz, Brain metabolism of progesterone, coping behaviour and emotional reactivity in male rats from two psychogenetically selected lines. J Neuroendocrinol, 1997. 9(3): p. 169-75. Doremus-Fitzwater, T.L., E.I. Varlinskaya, and L.P. Spear, Social and non-social anxiety in adolescent and adult rats after repeated restraint. Physiol Behav, 2009. 97(3-4): p. 484-94. Varlinskaya, E.I., T.L. Doremus-Fitzwater, and L.P. Spear, Repeated restraint stress alters sensitivity to the social consequences of ethanol in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2010. 96(2): p. 228-35. Varlinskaya, E.I. and L.P. Spear, Increases in anxiety-like behavior induced by acute stress are reversed by ethanol in adolescent but not adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2012. 100(3): p. 440-50. Varlinskaya, E.I., L.P. Spear, and N.E. Spear, Social behavior and social motivation in adolescent rats: role of housing conditions and partner's activity. Physiol Behav, 1999. 67(4): p. 475-82. Pellow, S., Can drug effects on anxiety and convulsions be separated? Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 1985. 9(1): p. 55-73. Brady, A.M. and S.B. Floresco, Operant procedures for assessing behavioral flexibility in rats. J Vis Exp, 2015(96): p. e52387. Butts, K.A., S.B. Floresco, and A.G. Phillips, Acute stress impairs set-shifting but not reversal learning. Behav Brain Res, 2013. 252: p. 222-9. Gilpin, N.W., C.A. Karanikas, and H.N. Richardson, Adolescent binge drinking leads to changes in alcohol drinking, anxiety, and amygdalar corticotropin releasing factor cells in adulthood in male rats. PLoS One, 2012. 7(2): p. e31466. Carobrez, A.P. and L.J. Bertoglio, Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2005. 29(8): p. 1193-205. Hogg, S., A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1996. 54(1): p. 21-30. Ennaceur, A., Tests of unconditioned anxiety - pitfalls and disappointments. Physiol Behav, 2014. 135: p. 55-71. Dannenhoffer, C.A., et al., Oxytocin and vasopressin modulation of social anxiety following adolescent intermittent ethanol exposure. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2018. 235(10): p. 3065-3077. Daenen, E.W., et al., The effects of neonatal lesions in the amygdala or ventral hippocampus on social behaviour later in life. Behav Brain Res, 2002. 136(2): p. 571-82. Flores, G., et al., Comparative behavioral changes in postpubertal rats after neonatal excitotoxic lesions of the ventral hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex. Synapse, 2005. 56(3): p. 147-53.
ur na
43.
58.
59. 60.
67. 68.
69. 70.
71.
72. 73.
74. 75.
Jo
76.
of
66.
ro
65.
-p
64.
re
63.
lP
62.
van Kerkhof, L.W., et al., Social Play Behavior in Adolescent Rats is Mediated by Functional Activity in Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2013. 38(10): p. 1899-909. Vanderschuren, L.J., E.J. Achterberg, and V. Trezza, The neurobiology of social play and its rewarding value in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2016. 70: p. 86-105. Calhoon, G.G. and K.M. Tye, Resolving the neural circuits of anxiety. Nat Neurosci, 2015. 18(10): p. 1394-404. Canteras, N.S., et al., Neuroanatomy of anxiety. Curr Top Behav Neurosci, 2010. 2: p. 77-96. Ishikawa, J., R. Nishimura, and A. Ishikawa, Early-life stress induces anxiety-like behaviors and activity imbalances in the medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala in adult rats. Eur J Neurosci, 2015. 41(4): p. 442-53. Shin, L.M. and I. Liberzon, The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2010. 35(1): p. 169-91. Crews, F., J. He, and C. Hodge, Adolescent cortical development: a critical period of vulnerability for addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2007. 86(2): p. 189-99. Ernst, M. and J.L. Fudge, A developmental neurobiological model of motivated behavior: anatomy, connectivity and ontogeny of the triadic nodes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2009. 33(3): p. 367-82. Koss, W.A., et al., Dendritic remodeling in the adolescent medial prefrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala of male and female rats. Synapse, 2014. 68(2): p. 61-72. Sturman, D.A. and B. Moghaddam, The neurobiology of adolescence: changes in brain architecture, functional dynamics, and behavioral tendencies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2011. 35(8): p. 1704-12. Dumais, K.M. and A.H. Veenema, Vasopressin and oxytocin receptor systems in the brain: Sex differences and sex-specific regulation of social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol, 2016. 40: p. 1-23. Neumann, I.D. and R. Landgraf, Balance of brain oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for anxiety, depression, and social behaviors. Trends Neurosci, 2012. 35(11): p. 649-59. Lukas, M., et al., Early life stress impairs social recognition due to a blunted response of vasopressin release within the septum of adult male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2011. 36(6): p. 843-53. Bleickardt, C.J., et al., Characterization of the V1a antagonist, JNJ-17308616, in rodent models of anxiety-like behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2009. 202(4): p. 711-8. Egashira, N., et al., Impaired social interaction and reduced anxiety-related behavior in vasopressin V1a receptor knockout mice. Behav Brain Res, 2007. 178(1): p. 123-7. Litvin, Y., G. Murakami, and D.W. Pfaff, Effects of chronic social defeat on behavioral and neural correlates of sociality: Vasopressin, oxytocin and the vasopressinergic V1b receptor. Physiol Behav, 2011. 103(3-4): p. 393-403. Floresco, S.B., A.E. Block, and M.T. Tse, Inactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat impairs strategy set-shifting, but not reversal learning, using a novel, automated procedure. Behav Brain Res, 2008. 190(1): p. 85-96. Hammerslag, L.R. and J.M. Gulley, Sex differences in behavior and neural development and their role in adolescent vulnerability to substance use. Behav Brain Res, 2016. 298(Pt A): p. 15-26.
ur na
61.
77.
78.
80.
81.
82. 83.
-p
ro
84.
Adams, J., et al., Workshop to identify critical windows of exposure for children's health: neurobehavioral work group summary. Environ Health Perspect, 2000. 108 Suppl 3: p. 535-44. Morean, M.E., J. Peterson, and A. L'Insalata, Predictors of quickly progressing from initiating alcohol use to engaging in binge drinking among adolescents. Addict Behav Rep, 2019. 9: p. 100165. Enstad, F., et al., Predicting hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood from early and excessive adolescent drinking - a longitudinal cross-national study of Norwegian and Australian adolescents. BMC Public Health, 2019. 19(1): p. 790. Buckner, J.D., et al., Implications of comorbid alcohol dependence among individuals with social anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety, 2008. 25(12): p. 1028-37. Morris, E.P., S.H. Stewart, and L.S. Ham, The relationship between social anxiety disorder and alcohol use disorders: a critical review. Clin Psychol Rev, 2005. 25(6): p. 734-60. Blumenthal, H., et al., The Links Between Social Anxiety Disorder, Insomnia Symptoms, and Alcohol Use Disorders: Findings From a Large Sample of Adolescents in the United States. Behav Ther, 2019. 50(1): p. 50-59.
of
79.
re
Figure 1. The impact of early and late adolescent intermittent ethanol exposure on (a) social investigation, (b) social preference, (c) locomotor activity during a 10-min social interaction test
lP
and on (d) percent open arm time, (e) percent open arm entries, and (f) number of closed arm entries during a 5-min elevated plus maze test in adult male (left panels) and female (right
ur na
panels) rats. A significant difference (p≤ 0.05) assessed with Student’s one-tailed t-tests or Mann-Whitney U tests between ethanol- and water-exposed animals within each timing/sex
Jo
condition is indicated with (*).
Elevated Plus Maze
Modified Social Interaction Test
d. Open Arm Time
a. Social Investigation
female
Percent
Percent
*
female
male
Frequency / 10 min
Frequency / 10 min
male
*
* Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
b. Social Preference
male
Adolescent Exposure
Percent
Percent
*
female
* Adolescent Exposure
* f. Closed Arm Entries
female
male
Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
-p
Adolescent Exposure
Number
Number
Crossovers
Crossovers
female
Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
c. Locomotor Activity
male
*
of
female Coefficient (%)
Coefficient (%)
male
Adolescent Exposure
e. Open Arm Entries
ro
Adolescent Exposure
Figure 1.
re
Figure 2. The impact of early and late adolescent intermittent ethanol exposure on (a) number of trials during acquisition, (b) number of trials during set-shifting, (c) total shift errors, (d)
lP
perseverative, (e) regressive, and (f) never reinforced errors during set-shifting in adult male (left panels) and female (right panels) rats. A significant difference (p≤ 0.05) assessed with Student’s
ur na
one-tailed t-tests or Mann-Whitney U tests between ethanol- and water-exposed animals within
Jo
each timing/sex condition is indicated with (*).
Behavioral Flexibility d. Perseverative Errors
a. Acquisition female
e. Regressive Errors
male
f. Never Reinforced Errors
female
male
ro
Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
re
Figure 2.
-p
Adolescent Exposure
Number
Number
Number
Number
female
Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
c. Total Shift Errors
of
Adolescent Exposure
female
*
Number
Number of Trials Adolescent Exposure
male
Adolescent Exposure
Adolescent Exposure
female
Number of Trials
male
Number
Number Adolescent Exposure
b. Set-shift
Number
Adolescent Exposure
female
male
Number of Trials
Number of Trials
male
lP
Table 1. ANOVA Results for Social Interaction, Elevated Plus Maze, and Behavioral Flexibility Measures.
Test/Behavioral Factors Measure Exposure SI test:
ur na
Social Investigation
Chamber Crosses
Timing Exposure X Timing Exposure Timing Exposure X Timing Exposure Timing
EPM:
Exposure X Timing Exposure
SI test:
Preference/ Avoidance
Jo
SI test:
% Open Arm Time
EPM:
Timing Exposure X Timing Exposure
% Open Arm Timing
Male
F(1,28) =5.06 =0.03* F(1,28) = 2.08 F(1,28) = 1.74 F(1,29) = 3.63 F(1,29) = 1.38 F(1,29) = 0.61 F(1,29) = 0.32 F(1,29) = 4.15 0.051 F(1,29) = 0.07 F(1,30) = 7.08 <0.01* F(1,30) = 0.15 F(1,30) = 0.03 F(1,30) = 10.36 0.01* F(1,30) = 0.13
Female F(1,29) = 0.01
p = 0.93
F(1,29) = 0.03 F(1,29) = 1.56 F(1,30) = 0.38 F(1,30) = 0.07 F(1,30) = 0.27 F(1,29) = 0.07 F(1,29) = 0.05
p = 0.87 p = 0.22 p = 0.54 p = 0.79 p = 0.61 p = 0.80 p = 0.83
p = 0.80 F(1,29) = 0.00 F(1,32) = 3.05 p
p = 0.99 p = 0.09
p = 0.70 F(1,32) = 0.33 p = 0.87 F(1,32) = 0.89 F(1,32) = 2.01 p<
p = 0.57 p = 0.35 p = 0.17
p = 0.73 F(1,32) = 0.47
p = 0.50
p p = 0.16 p = 0.19 p = 0.07 p = 0.25 p = 0.44 p = 0.57 p=
Exposure X Timing Exposure Timing Exposure X Timing Exposure Timing Exposure X Timing
F(1,30) = 0.16 p = 0.69 F(1,32) = 1.49 p = 0.23 F(1,30) = 0.01 p = 0.98 F(1,32) = 0.00 p = 0.96 EPM: F(1,30) = 0.10 p = 0.64 F(1,32) = 0.00 p = 0.96 Closed Arm F(1,30) = 0.22 p = 0.76 F(1,32) = 0.11 p = 0.75 Entries F(1,40) = 0.91 p = 0.35 F(1,40) = 3.82 p = 0.06 BF: F(1,40) = 2.95 p = 0.09 F(1,40) = 0.36 p = 0.55 Cue Acquisition F(1,40) = 0.00 p= F(1,40) = 0.20 p = 0.66 0.99 Exposure F(1,40) = 0.58 p = 0.45 F(1,40) = 1.26 p = 0.27 BF: Timing F(1,40) = 0.02 p = 0.90 F(1,40) = 2.00 p = 0.17 Set Shift Exposure X Timing F(1,40) = 2.34 p = 0.13 F(1,40) = 1.35 p = 0.25 Exposure F(1,40) = 1.85 p = 0.18 F(1,40) = 0.84 p = 0.37 BF: Timing F(1,40) = 0.00 p = 0.96 F(1,40) = 1.21 p = 0.28 Total Errors Exposure X Timing F(1,40) = 0.24 p = 0.63 F(1,40) = 0.65 p = 0.42 Exposure F(1,40) = 0.77 p = 0.39 F(1,40) = 2.79 p = 0.10 BF: Timing F(1,40) = 0.43 p = 0.52 F(1,40) = 1.93 p = 0.17 Perseverative Exposure X Timing F(1,40) = 0.02 p = 0.87 F(1,40) = 0.00 p = 0.96 Errors Exposure F(1,40) = 0.35 p = 0.56 F(1,40) = 6.66 p < BF: 0.05* Regressive Errors Timing F(1,40) = 1.37 p = 0.25 F(1,40) = 4.73 p < 0.05^ Exposure X Timing F(1,40) = 0.30 p = 0.61 F(1,40) = 0.59 p = 0.45 Exposure F(1,40) = 1.70 p = 0.20 F(1,40) = 0.00 p = 0.98 BF: Never F(1,40) = 0.04 p = 0.84 F(1,40) = 8.13 p < Reinforced Errors Timing 0.01^ Exposure X Timing F(1,40) = 0.50 p = 0.48 F(1,40) = 0.82 p = 0.37 Note: bold text denotes a significant main (p ≤ 0.05) effect of Exposure (*) or Timing (^).
ur na
lP
re
-p
ro
of
Entries
Jo
Table 2. Light/Dark Box. Percent of animals remained in the dark chamber for the entire test session. Timing of Adolescent Staying in Dark Adolescent Sex Exposure Chamber (%) Exposure Water 87.5 Male Ethanol 85.7 Early Water 100.0 Female Ethanol 77.8 Water 88.9 Male Ethanol 88.9 Late Water 85.7 Female Ethanol 75.0