Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female Long-Evans hooded rats

Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female Long-Evans hooded rats

Accepted Manuscript Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female Long-Evans hooded rats Nathan S. Pentkowski, Yoav Litvin, D. Caroli...

893KB Sizes 0 Downloads 39 Views

Accepted Manuscript Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female Long-Evans hooded rats

Nathan S. Pentkowski, Yoav Litvin, D. Caroline Blanchard, Robert J. Blanchard PII: DOI: Reference:

S0031-9384(18)30209-9 doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.04.028 PHB 12179

To appear in:

Physiology & Behavior

Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:

26 November 2017 20 April 2018 20 April 2018

Please cite this article as: Nathan S. Pentkowski, Yoav Litvin, D. Caroline Blanchard, Robert J. Blanchard , Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female LongEvans hooded rats. The address for the corresponding author was captured as affiliation for all authors. Please check if appropriate. Phb(2018), doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.04.028

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female Long-Evans hooded rats. Nathan S. Pentkowskia,1* , Yoav Litvina, D. Caroline Blanchardb,c and Robert J. Blancharda,b a

Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa, 2530 Dole Street, Sakamaki C 400, Honolulu, Hawaii USA 96822

b

IP

T

Pacific Biosciences Research Center, University of Hawaii at Manoa, 1993 East-West Road, Honolulu, Hawaii USA 96822 c

CR

John A. Burns School of Medicine, University of Hawaii at Manoa, 651 Ilalo Street, Honolulu, Hawaii USA 96813

AN M

ED

Corresponding author: Nathan S. Pentkowski, Ph.D. Department of Psychology University of New Mexico 1 University of New Mexico MSC03 2220 Logan Hall Albuquerque, NM USA 87131 Phone: (505) 277-1928 Email: [email protected]

US

*

AC

CE

PT

Present address: 1 Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, 1 University of New Mexico, MSC03 2220, Logan Hall, Albuquerque, New Mexico USA 87131

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Abstract This study investigated the influence of the estrus cycle in mediating cat odor-induced unconditioned and conditioned defensive behaviors in female Long-Evans hooded rats. Unconditioned defensive behaviors were assessed during predatory cue exposure;

T

conditioned defensive behaviors were examined twenty-four hours after threat exposure.

IP

Estrus phases were determined by microscopic examination of vaginal smears within 10

CR

min of completing the behavioral tests. Compared to no-odor controls, female rats exposed to cat odor exhibited both unconditioned and conditioned defensive behaviors,

US

including elevated levels of freezing, risk assessment and avoidance. Rats in proestrus

AN

and estrus exhibited reduced levels of defensive behavior during the unconditioned test trial compared to subjects in diestrus and metestrus. Specifically, estrus stages

M

characterized by high levels of circulating estrogens and progesterone were associated

ED

with reduced immobility (i.e. freezing) and enhanced active defense (i.e. risk assessment), profiles that may enable mate seeking and subsequent reproduction in

PT

potentially dangerous or novel environments. These results suggest a specific role for

CE

ovarian hormone fluctuations in mediating unconditioned fear- and anxiety- like defensive

AC

behaviors during exposure to predatory odors.

Keywords

Defense, Anxiety, Fear, Estrus, Female, Estrogen, Predator, Freezing, Risk assessment

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1. Introduction Anxiety and stress disorders are the most prevalent psychiatric illnesses, with several forms such as generalized anxiety disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder and panic disorder disproportionately affecting women more than men [1-4]. Fluctuations in levels

T

of estrogen and progesterone during the menstrual cycle may be related to depression and

IP

anxiety susceptibility [5-8]. Proestrus (P) and estrus (E) are have higher concentrations of

CR

circulating progesterone and estrogen, with peak levels occurring during P followed by fluctuations throughout estrus stages [9, 10]. In rats, P and E stages are characterized by

US

reduced anxiety- like behavior [11]. For instance, rats in P and E exhibit increased time

AN

spent in the open arms of an elevated plus-maze (EPM) [12, 13] and obtain more punished reinforcers compared to rats in diestrus (D) and/or metestrus (M) [14]; the latter

M

deficits in D and M are prevented by diazepam. Studies have linked anxiolysis with P,

ED

reporting reduced anxiety- like behaviors in the light-dark test [15], EPM [16-18], punished responding [19], social interaction test [16] and defensive burying test [16, 20].

PT

In stark contrast, other studies indicate that rats in P and E exhibit enhanced anxiety-like

CE

behavior in the EPM [21, 22], or report no effect of estrus stage on anxiety- like behaviors in the EPM [23, 24] or defensive burying test [24]. Based on these equivocal reports,

AC

further characterization of neuroendocrine influences on anxiety-like behavior using novel animal models is needed in order to develop more effective pharmacological treatments for anxiety disorders in women. In order to understand the neurobiological mechanisms underlying fear- and anxietyrelated pathologies, considerable efforts have been made to develop animal models for human anxiety disorders [25]. In laboratory settings, evaluation of antipredator defensive

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT behaviors allows for an ethological analysis of anxiety- like behavior by exposing rats to natural predatory cues, such as cat odor. In particular, some of the defensive behaviors elicited by these naturalistic threats, e.g., risk assessment and freezing, [26, 27] are responsive to traditional anxiolytics such as diazepam and 5-HT1A agonists [28-31].

T

Importantly, female rats show enhanced antipredator defensive behavior (e.g., increased

IP

risk assessment and decreased contact) in response to potential (e.g., cat odor) as opposed

CR

to distinct (e.g., live cat exposure) threat sources, as well as differential reactivity in such situations to classic anxiolytics [32].

US

To further clarify the influence of the estrus cycle on anxiety- like defensive behavior,

AN

this study examined unconditioned and conditioned behavioral responses in female rats exposed to cat odor. We hypothesized that fluctuations in levels of ovarian hormones

M

typical of the various estrus stages would differentially modulate defensive behavior,

ED

with higher levels associated with reduced anxiety-like responsivity. Accordingly, we predicted that this decrease in defensiveness would manifest during P and E, stages that

PT

are characterized by comparatively higher levels of estrogen and progesterone compared

CE

to M and D.

2. Materials and Methods

AC

2.1 Subjects

Subjects were adult female Long-Evans hooded rats born and reared in the Snyder Hall breeding colony at the University of Hawaii at Manoa. Following weaning (21 days), all rats were singly housed in standard home cages (21.6 x 45.7 x 17.8 cm) under controlled temperature (21 ± 2 ◦C) and illumination (12-h light/dark cycle, lights on at 06:00 a.m.) with free access to food (rat chow) and water. Rats (n=53) were 11 to 13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT weeks of age at the start of behavioral testing. All husbandry and experimentation adhered to the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Research Council, 2011), and all experimental procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at The University of Hawaii.

T

2.2 Cat Odor Exposure

IP

Detailed procedures for the cat odor test have been previously described [33-35].

CR

Briefly, the test apparatus consisted of a white Plexiglas runway (100 x 12 x 50 cm) with a clear front panel to permit observation and recording. Separate cloth-wrapped solid

US

plastic blocks (9 x 9 x 2 cm) served as the cat odor and control stimuli. The cat odor

AN

block was rubbed for 5 min against the fur of a laboratory-housed domestic male cat for 3 consecutive days prior to use. In order to control for potential novelty effects, control rats

M

were exposed to an identical cloth-wrapped plastic block never exposed to cat odor. All

ED

rats were habituated to the apparatus for 10 min over 3 consecutive days without the presence of a block stimulus. On the following day (unconditioned behavior test), the

PT

appropriate block stimulus was placed at one end of the runway and a test subject was

CE

placed at the opposite end. Twenty-four hours later each rat was tested in the same apparatus without the cat odor stimulus (cue+context conditioning test). A cloth-wrapped

AC

solid plastic block never exposed to cat odor served as the cue during the conditioned test trial. All sessions were 10 min in duration and were conducted under red light in order to ensure that the threat stimulus remained as ambiguous as possible. Rats were tested in the same order across days, with testing occurring between 11:00 a.m. and 3:00 p.m. In order to insure that control rats were not exposed to cat odor, control and cat odor exposed

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT subjects were tested in separate rooms, using identical test chambers. Between trials each apparatus was thoroughly cleaned using a 10% ethanol solution. 2.3 Behavioral Measures Defensive behaviors analyzed in each test situation included: crouch freeze-complete

T

cessation of movement other than respiration; risk assessment behaviors including stretch

IP

approach-forward ambulation with flat back and stretched neck, stretch attend-standing

CR

on all 4 paws with flat back and stretched neck orientated toward the threat source, and crouch sniff-olfactory investigation evidenced by vertical or lateral head movements

US

(scoring initiated when nose visibly moved more than 1 cm); avoidance-duration of time

AN

spent in the far compartment relative to the threat- or control-block stimulus; and contactdirect contact with the block stimulus, measured as direct paw or head contact.

M

Locomotor activity-crossing of lines marking the far, medium and near thirds of the

ED

apparatus was measured to examine potential nonspecific changes in ambulation. Behavioral measures represent the durations of events in seconds (or numbers of line

PT

crossings) during a 10 min observation period.

CE

2.4 Behavioral Analysis

All test trials were recorded using Pioneer DVD recorders, and were later analyzed

AC

using the behavioral analysis software Observer 5.0 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands) by a highly trained observer blind to test conditions. Use of this software allowed for a detailed, frame-by-frame analysis. 2.5 Determination of Estrus Cycle Estrus cycle phases were determined by the consensus of three research assistants using bright-field microscopic examination (10x and 40x magnification) of vaginal

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT smears collected within 10 min of testing in both the unconditioned and conditioned cat odor tests as previously described [9, 36, 37]. Proestrus was identified by the predominance of nucleated epithelial cells, E was identified by the presence of dense sheets of cornified epithelial cells, M was identified by the presence of scattered,

T

nucleated, or cornified epithelial cells and leukocytes, and D was identified by the

IP

presence of leukocytes [11].

CR

2.6 Statistical Analysis

To examine the effects of cat odor on unconditioned and conditioned defensive

US

behaviors separate t-tests were performed on each dependent measure with data from

AN

each estrus cycle stage pooled across groups. To examine the influence of each estrus stage on cat odor-induced defensive behaviors separate one-way analysis of variance

M

(ANOVA) were performed on each dependent measure. Significant ANOVAs were

ED

followed by post-hoc Tukey’s HSD tests to provide pairwise comparisons; α was set at 0.05 for all statistical comparisons.

PT

3. Results

CE

3.1 Estrus Determination

Of the 32 rats exposed to the predatory cat odor, 12 were tested during diestrus, 6

AC

during metestrus, 6 during proestrus and 8 during estrus. One-way ANOVA failed to demonstrate any significant behavioral effects between estrus stages in rats exposed to the control block. Thus, for ease of comparison control rats from each estrus stage were pooled into a single control group (n=21). 3.2 Effects of Cat Odor on Unconditioned Defensive Behavior

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 1 illustrates the effects of predator odor exposure on unconditioned defensive behaviors during the cat odor test with data from each estrus cycle stage pooled. Exposure to cat odor produced robust increases in defensive behavior. Compared to controls, rats exposed to cat odor exhibited increased levels of crouch freezing

T

[t(51)=5.27, p<0.001] and suppressed locomotor activity [t(51)=8.10, p<0.001; control:

IP

55.67 ± 3.49, cat odor: 16.69 ± 3.15]. Rats demonstrated a clear aversion to the cat odor

CR

stimulus, exhibiting higher levels of avoidance [t(51)=7.48, p<0.001] and less contact [t(51)=11.56, p<0.001] with the cat odor block compared to controls. Cat odor exposure

US

also potentiated risk assessment behaviors, including a reliable increase in stretch attend

AN

[t(51)=2.88, p<0.01] and a trend toward increased stretch approach [t(51)=1.92, p=.06], but no effect on crouch sniffing (p>0.05).

M

3.3 Influence of Estrus stage on Cat Odor-Induced Unconditioned Defensive Behavior

ED

Figure 2 illustrates the influence of estrus stage on predator-induced defensive behaviors during the unconditioned cat odor test. The ANOVA indicated a significant

PT

effect of estrus stage on the duration of crouch freezing [F(3, 28)=6.68, p<0.005]. Post-

CE

hoc analyses revealed that rats in D exhibited higher durations of freezing compared to rats in P and E (Tukey’s, p<0.05 in each case). Cat odor-elicited risk assessment was also

AC

influenced by estrus stage. The ANOVA indicated a difference in the duration of crouch sniffing [F(3, 28)=6.15, p<0.005], with rats in P and E exhibiting higher levels compared to rats in D (Tukey’s, p<0.05 in each case). There was no effect of estrus stage on the durations of avoidance, contact, stretch approach, stretch attend or or the number of line crossings (P: 19.00 ± 8.14 , E: 21.75 ± 9.38, M: 17.83 ± 5.74, D: 11.58 ± 3.26). 3.4 Effects of Cat Odor on Conditioned Defensive Behavior

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 3 illustrates the effects of predator odor-induced conditioned defensive behaviors during the cat odor cue+context test with data from each estrus cycle stage pooled. Exposure to the cat odor cue+context induced a characteristic pattern of enhanced conditioned defensiveness. Compared to controls, rats previously exposed to cat odor

T

exhibited increased levels of conditioned crouch freezing [t(51)=3.46, p<0.005] and

IP

suppressed locomotor activity [t(51)=3.05, p<0.005; control cue: 49.24 ± 3.66, cat odor

CR

cue: 31.03 ± 4.19]. Rats previously exposed to cat odor demonstrated a clear aversion to the cat odor cue stimulus, exhibiting higher levels of conditioned avoidance [t(51)=8.67,

US

p<0.001] and less contact [t(51)=8.66, p<0.001] with the cue control block compared to

AN

controls never exposed to cat odor. Cat odor cue+context exposure also potentiated conditioned risk assessment behaviors, including reliable increases in stretch attend

M

[t(51)=2.53, p<0.05] and stretch approach [t(51)=2.27, p<.05], but no effect on crouch

ED

sniffing (p>0.05).

3.5 Influence of Estrus stage on Cat Odor-Induced Conditioned Defensive Behavior

PT

Figure 4 illustrates the influence of estrus stage during initial threat exposure on

CE

conditioned defensive behaviors during the cat odor cue+context test. Estrus stage during the unconditioned test trial did not significantly alter any defensive behaviors during the

AC

cat odor cue+context conditioning test (p>0.05 in each case) and there was no effect on locomotor activity (P: 34.50 ± 11.71, E: 34.38 ± 9.00, M: 43.00 ± 10.41, D: 21.08 ± 5.03). Additionally, conditioned defensive behaviors did not differ between groups when the data were re-analyzed according to the estrus stage determined immediately (within 10 min) following cue+context testing (data not shown). 4. Discussion

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Results from the present experiments demonstrate for the first time that fluctuations in ovarian hormones during stages of the estrus cycle modulate unconditioned defensive behaviors during exposure to an ethologically relevant predatory threat source (cat odor). Regardless of estrus stage, cat odor increased defensive responsivity in female rats,

T

increasing freezing, risk assessment and avoidance, while suppressing locomotion and

IP

contact with the predatory threat stimulus (see Figure 1). This defensive profile is

CR

partially estrus stage-dependent, as rats in P and E exhibited reduced freezing and increased risk assessment (crouch-sniffing) compared to rats in D and M (see Figure 2), a

US

shift in defensive responsivity similar to reducing the level of environmental threat (i.e., a

AN

gradual shift from immobility to cautious exploration, eventually leading to a return to non-defensive behaviors). These results extend upon previous research indicating that

M

rats in E and/or P exhibit reduced anxiety- like behaviors in the EPM [12, 13, 16-18],

ED

punished responding conflict models [14, 19], light-dark test [15], social interaction test [16] and defensive burying test [16, 20] compared to rats in D and/or M.

PT

Similar to previous reports examining conditioning in 18-38-day old female pups [35]

CE

or female adults [38], rats in the present study demonstrated heightened conditioned defensiveness during exposure to the cat odor-paired cue+context including increased

AC

freezing, risk assessment and avoidance, as well as reduced locomotion and cue contact (see Figure 3). However, in contrast to the influence of estrus stage on unconditioned defensive behaviors, there were no reliable differences between estrus groups on defensive responsivity during the cat odor cue+context test. Non-significant effects were detected when estrus cycle stages were measured either immediately following unconditioned threat exposure (see Figure 4) or immediately following cue+context re-

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT exposure (data not shown), suggesting that estrus stage did not impact conditioned fear acquisition or expression. These null effects of estrus stage during conditioned test trials are consistent with a previous report that failed to find an effect of estrogen replacement in ovariectomized females on cat odor-induced conditioned fear [38].

T

The influence of estrus stage on conditioned behavioral responsivity appears to be

IP

threat-source specific as estrus stage has been implicated in the expression of conditioned

CR

defensive behavior and fear extinction using footshock [39, 40] as the threat source [for review see: 41]. To confirm this hypothesis, future studies examining estrus stage effects

US

on predator odor-induced conditioning should test for conditioned defensive behaviors

AN

more than 24 hours after initial threat exposure to minimize potential carryover stress effects. Additionally, although previous studies have firmly established that cat odor

M

induces conditioned defensive behaviors in adult male rats by examining the effects of

ED

novel cues and/or contexts across several days of extinction [42], experiments utilizing adult female rats have not included these controls.

PT

Rodent antipredator defensive behaviors are valid indices of anxiety-related behaviors

CE

in humans [43, 44]. Therefore, the shift in cat odor-induced defensive responsivity demonstrated by rats in P and E suggests that estrogen and progesterone mediate

AC

antipredator defensive behaviors related to anxiety. In support of this notion, situations in which rats are exposed to potential or anticipated threat (cat odor, cat-paired contexts) evoke anxiety- like behaviors [45], an assertion supported by pharmacological data [29]. Indeed, classic anxiolytics such as the benzodiazepines diazepam and chlordiazepoxide [46], alcohol [47], the 5-HT1A agonists 8-OH-DPAT and Gepirone [48], and the tricyclic antidepressant imipramine [49] decrease freezing and avoidance while increasing risk

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT assessment during exposure to contexts associated with a cat, while the benzodiazepine midazolam decreases freezing and avoidance during cat odor exposure [50]. Similarly, in the present study we detected a shift from passive (freezing) to active (risk-assessment) defensiveness in females with higher levels of ovarian hormones (P and E). Although this

T

shift in defensiveness could result from high levels of ovarian hormones producing

IP

behavioral disinhibition, this explanation is unlikely as the changes in the defensive

CR

repertoire were independent of effects on locomotor activity (line crossings). The neurobiological mechanisms mediating the estrus stage dependent expression of

US

antipredator defensive behavior may involve estrogen and/or progesterone signaling in

AN

the accessory olfactory system. The accessory olfactory system mediates responsivity to both conspecific and predatory olfactory cues [50, 51]. Estrogens affect social

M

recognition in mice via interaction with the oxytocinergic system in the medial amygdala

ED

[52, 53], a component of the accessory olfactory system that preferentially modulates innate defensive behaviors to cat odor [51, 54]. Importantly, oxytocin binding in the

PT

medial amygdala promotes prosocial behaviors by attenuating anxiety [52, 53].

CE

Furthermore, in ovariectomized rats estradiol administration reduces anxiety- like defensive behaviors during cat odor exposure [38], as well as testing in the open field

AC

[39] and EPM [55]. Lastly, rats in P displaying reduced anxiety in the EPM have higher levels of estradiol compared to D, and estradiol administration to rats in D eliminated their anxiogenic profile [18]. Although not directly tested in the present study, our results may involve progesterone conversion to allopregnanolone and subsequent anxiolysis via agonism at GABAA receptors [56-59]. Importantly, these processes occur in the amygdala [60] during both

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT behavioral E [58] and P [16]. An alternative view of the role of estrogen and progesterone effects on defensive behavior should also be considered. Rather than elevated estrogen and/or progesterone signaling during P and E reducing anxiety- like defensive behavior, enhanced defensiveness in D and M may result from falling levels of progesterone and

T

allopregnanolone coupled with low stable levels of estrogen in late D [10]. Indeed,

IP

previous reports indicate that these types of fluctuations in ovarian hormones enhance

CR

anxiety- like behavior [61-64].

In summary, during predator odor exposure we suggest that estrogen and progesterone

US

signaling, likely involving the medial amygdala, mediates a transition from passive

AN

immobility/freezing to active risk assessment by attenuating fear and promoting high levels of investigative defenses that enable approach, while maintaining vigilance. The

M

adaptive utility of such a mechanism is evident; high levels of ovarian hormones needed

ED

for reproduction would also sustain mate seeking in the face of a potential predatory threat. From an evolutionary perspective, this behavioral profile could promote mate

PT

seeking and subsequent reproduction in natural environments that contain a persistent, yet

CE

ambiguous level of predatory threat. Here, reproduction serves as an evolutionary motivation, in addition to the acquisition of resources (i.e. food and shelter) only during

AC

times of receptivity, and therefore a reduction in fear (defensive) levels enables efficient exploratory behaviors that would otherwise be unnecessary. The precise neural mechanisms that regulate these behavioral processes, as well as the potential involvement of ovarian hormone modulation of accessory olfactory information and defense systems require further investigation. 5. Conclusions

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The present results indicate that estrus stages characterized by higher levels of circulating estrogen and progesterone (P and E) are associated with reduced freezing and enhanced risk assessment, profiles that may promote mate seeking and subsequent reproduction in novel and/or potentially dangerous environments. These results implicate

CR

Acknowledgements

IP

like defensive behaviors during exposure to predatory odors.

T

fluctuations in ovarian hormones in the expression of unconditioned fear- and anxiety-

The authors thank Chris Markham, Amy Vasconcellos, Joel Sabugo and Mark Ellis

US

for their technical assistance.

AN

Funding source

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public,

M

commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

ED

Competing Interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report or any involvement to disclose,

PT

financial or otherwise, that may bias the conduct, interpretation or presentation of this

AC

CE

work.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure captions Figure 1.Effects of cat odor exposure on unconditioned defensive behaviors with data from each estrus cycle stage pooled. Rats exposed to cat odor (filled bars) exhibited enhanced defensiveness compared to controls (clear bars). Defensive behaviors include

T

freezing (A), stretch attend (B), stretch approach (C), crouch sniffing (D), and odor block

IP

avoidance (E) and contact (F). Asterisk (*) represents a difference compared to controls

CR

(t-test, p<0.05 in each case). Behavioral measures represent the durations of events in

US

seconds (s) during the 10 min test period.

AN

Figure 2.Effects of estrus stage on cat odor-induced unconditioned defensive behaviors. Rats in proestrus (P) and estrus (E) exhibited reduced defensive behavior compared to

M

rats in diestrus (D) and metestrus (M). Defensive behaviors include freezing (A), stretch

ED

attend (B), stretch approach (C), crouch sniffing (D), and odor block avoidance (E) and contact (F). Asterisk (*) represents a difference compared to diestrus (Tukey’s HSD test,

PT

p<0.05 in each case). Behavioral measures represent the durations of events in seconds

CE

(s) during the 10 min test period.

AC

Figure 3.Effects of cat odor cue+context exposure on conditioned defensive behaviors with data from each estrus cycle stage pooled. Rats previously exposed to cat odor (filled bars) exhibited enhanced conditioned defensiveness compared to controls (clear bars). Defensive behaviors include freezing (A), stretch attend (B), stretch approach (C), crouch sniffing (D), and odor block avoidance (E) and contact (F). Asterisk (*) represents a

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT difference compared to controls (t-test, p<0.05 in each case). Behavioral measures represent the durations of events in seconds (s) during the 10 min test period.

Figure 4.Effects of estrus stage on conditioned defensive behaviors during cat odor

T

cue+context exposure. There were no significant differences between estrus groups

IP

[proestrus (P), estrus (E), diestrus (D), metestrus (M)]. Defensive behaviors include

CR

freezing (A), stretch attend (B), stretch approach (C), crouch sniffing (D), and odor block

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

AN

seconds (s) during the 10 min test period.

US

avoidance (E) and contact (F). Behavioral measures represent the durations of events in

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

AN

US

CR

IP

T

References 1. Collegium Internationale Neuro-psychopharmacologicum. Workshop (1993 : Monte-Carlo Monaco) and G. Darcourt, Current therapeutic approaches to panic and other anxiety disorders : Collegium Internationale Neuropsychopharmacologicum, Regional Workshop, Monte Carlo, November 20-22, 1993. 1994, Basel ; New York: Karger. vi, 206 p. 2. Pigott, T.A., Anxiety disorders in women. Psychiatr Clin North Am, 2003. 26(3): p. 621-72, vi-vii. 3. Kessler, R.C., et al., Lifetime and 12-Month Prevalence of Dsm-Iii-R PsychiatricDisorders in the United-States - Results from the National-Comorbidity-Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1994. 51(1): p. 8-19. 4. Cloitre, M., Yonkers, K.A., Pearlstein, T., Altemus, M., Davidson, K.W., Pigott,, S. T.A., M.K., Pine, D., Ross, J., Howell, H., Brogan, K., Rieckmann,, and C. N., L., Women and anxiety disorders: implications for diagnosis and treatment. Proceedings of a conference, November 19-21, 2003, Chantilly, Virginia, USA. CNS Spectr, 2004. 9(9): p. 1-16. 5. Rupprecht, R. and F. Holsboer, Neuropsychopharmacological properties of neuroactive steroids. Steroids, 1999. 64(1-2): p. 83-91. 6. Kessler, R.C., et al., Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 1995. 52(12): p. 1048-60. 7. Bekker, M.H. and J. van Mens-Verhulst, Anxiety disorders: sex differences in prevalence, degree, and background, but gender-neutral treatment. Gend Med, 2007. 4 Suppl B: p. S178-93. 8. Fink, G., et al., Estrogen control of central neurotransmission: effect on mood, mental state, and memory. Cell Mol Neurobiol, 1996. 16(3): p. 325-44. 9. Whishaw, I.Q.a.K., B., The Behavior of the Laboratory Rat: A Handbook with Tests. 2005: Oxford University Press. 10. Butcher, R.L., W.E. Collins, and N.W. Fugo, Plasma concentration of LH, FSH, prolactin, progesterone and estradiol-17beta throughout the 4-day estrous cycle of the rat. Endocrinology, 1974. 94(6): p. 1704-8. 11. Freeman, M.E., The ovarian cycle of the Rat. In: Knobil E, de Neil J, editors. The physiology of reproduction. The physiology of reproduction, ed. E.a.N. Knobil, JD. 1988: New York: Raven Press. 12. Diaz-Veliz, G., et al., Ketanserin and anxiety levels: influence of gender, estrous cycle, ovariectomy and ovarian hormones in female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1997. 58(3): p. 637-42. 13. Mora, S., N. Dussaubat, and G. Diaz-Veliz, Effects of the estrous cycle and ovarian hormones on behavioral indices of anxiety in female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1996. 21(7): p. 609-20. 14. Molina-Hernandez, M., C.M. Contreras, and P. Tellez-Alcantara, Diazepam increases the number of punished responses in a conflict-operant paradigm during late proestrus and estrus in the Wistar rat. Neuropsychobiology, 2001. 43(1): p. 29-33. 15. Zuluaga, M.J., et al., Experimental anxiety in the black and white model in cycling, pregnant and lactating rats. Physiol Behav, 2005. 84(2): p. 279-86.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

23.

24.

25. 26. 27. 28.

29.

30.

31.

T

IP

CR

US

22.

AN

21.

M

20.

ED

19.

PT

18.

CE

17.

Frye, C.A., S.M. Petralia, and M.E. Rhodes, Estrous cycle and sex differences in performance on anxiety tasks coincide with increases in hippocampal progesterone and 3alpha,5alpha-THP. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2000. 67(3): p. 587-96. Sayin, A., et al., The effects of the estrus cycle and citalopram on anxiety-like behaviors and c-fos expression in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2014. 124: p. 180-7. Marcondes, F.K., et al., Estrous cycle influences the response of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Physiol Behav, 2001. 74(4-5): p. 435-40. Molina-Hernandez, M., et al., Estrus variation in anticonflict-like effects of the mGlu5 receptor antagonist MTEP, microinjected into lateral septal nuclei of female Wistar rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2006. 84(3): p. 385-91. Fernandez-Guasti, A. and O. Picazo, Anxiolytic actions of diazepam, but not of buspirone, are influenced by gender and the endocrine stage. Behav Brain Res, 1997. 88(2): p. 213-8. Nin, M.S., et al., Anxiolytic effect of clonazepam in female rats: grooming microstructure and elevated plus maze tests. Eur J Pharmacol, 2012. 684(1-3): p. 95-101. Sadeghipour, H.R., et al., Nitric oxide involvement in estrous cycle-dependent changes of the behavioral responses of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Behav Brain Res, 2007. 178(1): p. 10-7. Soares-Rachetti Vde, P., et al., Short term, low dose fluoxetine blocks estrous cycle-linked changes in responsiveness to diazepam in female rats. J Psychopharmacol, 2016. 30(10): p. 1062-8. Fernandez-Guasti, A., et al., Chronic treatment with desipramine induces an estrous cycle-dependent anxiolytic-like action in the burying behavior, but not in the elevated plus-maze test. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1999. 63(1): p. 13-20. Litvin, Y., et al., Unconditioned models of fear and anxiety. Handbook of Anxiety and Fear, 2008. 17: p. 81-99. Blanchard, D.C. and R.J. Blanchard, Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion. Annu Rev Psychol, 1988. 39: p. 43-68. Blanchard, R.J. and D.C. Blanchard, Antipredator defensive behaviors in a visible burrow system. J Comp Psychol, 1989. 103(1): p. 70-82. Blanchard, D.C., G. Griebel, and R.J. Blanchard, Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2001. 25(3): p. 205-18. Blanchard, D.C., G. Griebel, and R.J. Blanchard, The Mouse Defense Test Battery: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Eur J Pharmacol, 2003. 463(1-3): p. 97-116. Griebel, G., G. Perrault, and D.J. Sanger, Study of the modulatory activity of BZ (omega) receptor ligands on defensive behaviors in mice: evaluation of the importance of intrinsic efficacy and receptor subtype selectivity. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 1999. 23(1): p. 81-98. Griebel, G., et al., Behavioural profiles in the mouse defence test battery suggest anxiolytic potential of 5-HT(1A) receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1999. 144(2): p. 121-30.

AC

16.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

39.

40. 41.

42. 43.

44.

45. 46. 47. 48.

49.

T

IP

CR

US

38.

AN

37.

M

36.

ED

35.

PT

34.

CE

33.

Blanchard, D.C., et al., Sex effects in defensive behavior: baseline differences and drug interactions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 1991. 15(4): p. 461-8. Pentkowski, N.S., et al., Effects of lesions to the dorsal and ventral hippocampus on defensive behaviors in rats. Eur J Neurosci, 2006. 23(8): p. 2185-96. Pentkowski, N.S., et al., Effects of acidic-astressin and ovine-CRF microinfusions into the ventral hippocampus on defensive behaviors in rats. Horm Behav, 2009. 56(1): p. 35-43. Hubbard, D.T., et al., Development of defensive behavior and conditioning to cat odor in the rat. Physiol Behav, 2004. 80(4): p. 525-30. Marcondes, F.K., F.J. Bianchi, and A.P. Tanno, Determination of the estrous cycle phases of rats: some helpful considerations. Braz J Biol, 2002. 62(4A): p. 609-14. Goldman, J.M., A.S. Murr, and R.L. Cooper, The rodent estrous cycle: characterization of vaginal cytology and its utility in toxicological studies. Birth Defects Res B Dev Reprod Toxicol, 2007. 80(2): p. 84-97. Litvin, Y., et al., Estradiol regulates responsiveness of the dorsal premammillary nucleus of the hypothalamus and affects fear- and anxiety-like behaviors in female rats. Eur J Neurosci, 2014. 40(2): p. 2344-51. Hiroi, R. and J.F. Neumaier, Differential effects of ovarian steroids on anxiety versus fear as measured by open field test and fear-potentiated startle. Behav Brain Res, 2006. 166(1): p. 93-100. Milad, M.R., et al., Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience, 2009. 164(3): p. 887-95. Morgan, M.A., J. Schulkin, and D.W. Pfaff, Estrogens and non-reproductive behaviors related to activity and fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2004. 28(1): p. 5563. Blanchard, R.J., et al., Cue and context conditioning of defensive behaviors to cat odor stimuli. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2001. 25(7-8): p. 587-95. Blanchard, D.C., et al., Human defensive behaviors to threat scenarios show parallels to fear- and anxiety-related defense patterns of non-human mammals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2001. 25(7-8): p. 761-70. Shuhama, R., et al., Defensive responses to threat scenarios in Brazilians reproduce the pattern of Hawaiian Americans and non-human mammals. Braz J Med Biol Res, 2008. 41(4): p. 324-32. Blanchard, R.J., et al., The characterization and modelling of antipredator defensive behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 1990. 14(4): p. 463-72. Blanchard, D.C., et al., Diazepam changes risk assessment in an anxiety/defense test battery. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1990. 101(4): p. 511-8. Blanchard, R.J., D.C. Blanchard, and S.M. Weiss, Ethanol effects in an anxiety/defense test battery. Alcohol, 1990. 7(5): p. 375-81. Blanchard, D.C., et al., Evidence for differential effects of 8-OH-DPAT on male and female rats in the Anxiety/Defense Test Battery. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1992. 106(4): p. 531-9. Blanchard, R.J., et al., Attenuation of antipredator defensive behavior in rats following chronic treatment with imipramine. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1993. 110(1-2): p. 245-53.

AC

32.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

55.

56.

57.

62.

63.

64.

PT

61.

CE

60.

AC

59.

ED

M

58.

T

54.

IP

53.

CR

52.

US

51.

McGregor, I.S., et al., Neural correlates of cat odor-induced anxiety in rats: region-specific effects of the benzodiazepine midazolam. J Neurosci, 2004. 24(17): p. 4134-44. Takahashi, L.K., Olfactory systems and neural circuits that modulate predator odor fear. Front Behav Neurosci, 2014. 8: p. 72. Choleris, E., et al., An estrogen-dependent four-gene micronet regulating social recognition: a study with oxytocin and estrogen receptor-alpha and -beta knockout mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2003. 100(10): p. 6192-7. Choleris, E., et al., Steroidal/neuropeptide interactions in hypothalamus and amygdala related to social anxiety. Prog Brain Res, 2008. 170: p. 291-303. Takahashi, L.K., et al., The smell of danger: a behavioral and neural analysis of predator odor-induced fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2005. 29(8): p. 1157-67. Koss, W.A., D.R. Gehlert, and A. Shekhar, Different effects of subchronic doses of 17-beta estradiol in two ethologically based models of anxiety utilizing female rats. Horm Behav, 2004. 46(2): p. 158-64. Bitran, D., M. Shiekh, and M. McLeod, Anxiolytic effect of progesterone is mediated by the neurosteroid allopregnanolone at brain GABAA receptors. J Neuroendocrinol, 1995. 7(3): p. 171-7. Laconi, M.R., et al., The anxiolytic effect of allopregnanolone is associated with gonadal hormonal status in female rats. Eur J Pharmacol, 2001. 417(1-2): p. 1116. Rhodes, M.E. and C.A. Frye, Inhibiting progesterone metabolism in the hippocampus of rats in behavioral estrus decreases anxiolytic behaviors and enhances exploratory and antinociceptive behaviors. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci, 2001. 1(3): p. 287-96. Diaz-Veliz, G., et al., Gender, estrous cycle, ovariectomy, and ovarian hormones influence the effects of diazepam on avoidance conditioning in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2000. 66(4): p. 887-92. Gulinello, M., R. Orman, and S.S. Smith, Sex differences in anxiety, sensorimotor gating and expression of the alpha4 subunit of the GABAA receptor in the amygdala after progesterone withdrawal. Eur J Neurosci, 2003. 17(3): p. 641-8. Devall, A.J., Z.W. Liu, and T.A. Lovick, Hyperalgesia in the setting of anxiety: sex differences and effects of the oestrous cycle in Wistar rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2009. 34(4): p. 587-96. Devall, A.J., et al., Elevation of brain allopregnanolone rather than 5-HT release by short term, low dose fluoxetine treatment prevents the estrous cycle-linked increase in stress sensitivity in female rats. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol, 2015. 25(1): p. 113-23. Smith, S.S., et al., Steroid withdrawal in the mouse results in anxiogenic effects of 3alpha,5beta-THP: a possible model of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2006. 186(3): p. 323-33. Smith, S.S., et al., Withdrawal from 3alpha-OH-5alpha-pregnan-20-One using a pseudopregnancy model alters the kinetics of hippocampal GABAA-gated current and increases the GABAA receptor alpha4 subunit in association with increased anxiety. J Neurosci, 1998. 18(14): p. 5275-84.

AN

50.

AC

Fig. 1

CE

PT

ED

M

AN

US

CR

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Fig. 2

AC

CE

PT

ED

M

AN

US

CR

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC

Fig. 3

CE

PT

ED

M

AN

US

CR

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC

Fig. 4

CE

PT

ED

M

AN

US

CR

IP

T

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Highlights 

Cat odor exposure induced both unconditioned and conditioned defensive behaviors in female rats.



Rats in proestrus and estrus exhibited lower levels of unconditioned defensive

IP

Higher levels of estrogen and/or progesterone may enable mate seeking in

CE

PT

ED

M

AN

US

CR

potentially dangerous environments.

AC



T

behavior.