Accepted Manuscript Effects of hay grass level and its physical form (full length versus chopped) on standing time, drinking time, and social behavior of calves Muhammad Aziz ur Rahman, Xia Chuanqi, Su Huawei, Cao Binghai PII:
S1558-7878(17)30120-X
DOI:
10.1016/j.jveb.2017.06.004
Reference:
JVEB 1064
To appear in:
Journal of Veterinary Behavior
Received Date: 17 February 2017 Revised Date:
26 April 2017
Accepted Date: 15 June 2017
Please cite this article as: Aziz ur Rahman, M., Chuanqi, X., Huawei, S., Binghai, C., Effects of hay grass level and its physical form (full length versus chopped) on standing time, drinking time, and social behavior of calves, Journal of Veterinary Behavior (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2017.06.004. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Original full length paper Effects of hay grass level and its physical form (full length versus chopped) on standing time, drinking time, and social behavior of calves Muhammad Aziz ur Rahman ab, Xia Chuanqi a, Su Huawei a, Cao Binghai a* State Key Laboratory of Animal Nutrition, College of Animal Science and Technology, China
RI PT
a
Agricultural University, Beijing, China b
SC
Institute of Animal and Dairy Sciences, University of Agriculture Faisalabad, Faisalabad, Pakistan
M AN U
For correspondence: *Prof. Dr. Cao Binghai, State Key Laboratory of Animal Nutrition, College of Animal Science and Technology, China Agricultural University,Beijing 100193, China. Tel/Fax: +86-10-62814346; E-mail:
[email protected],
[email protected]*, Muhammad Aziz ur Rahman and Xia Chuanqi contributed equally Abstract
Two 73 day experiments were done on growing calves to evaluate the effect of different forage particle
TE D
length of hay grass on standing time, drinking time and social behavior. Hay grass level was changed by increasing and decreasing forage to concentrate ratio (F:C). Hay was the only source of fiber in the diet. In experiment 1,F:C were fed at full length (FL) hay in the total mix ration (TMR): low F:C (50%
EP
forage) with FL, high F:C (65% forage) with FL. In experiment 2, calves were fed the same TMR with same ingredients and nutrient composition but differing in particle length of hay: treatments were lower
AC C
F:C (50% forage) with short length (SL), higher F:C (65% forage) with SL. At the end of 2nd montha one week of adaptation period was provided to allow adjustment of social and feeding behaviour with pen-mates. Four color cameras supported with infrared anti nozzle technology were used to record standing, drinking and and social behavior. In experiment 1, treatment and hour interaction was found for standing time (P<0.05). Longer standing time was observed for the 9th hour after feed delivery. Animals with low F:C showed longer standing time as compare to high F:C (P<0.05). Social behavior was altered with hour of observation (P<0.05). Social activity was highest during the 1st to 3rd and 9th
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT hour after feed delivery. In experiment 2, standing time was affected by hour of observation in both treatment groups (P<0.05). Drinking water intake was affected by dietary treatment (P<0.05). Longest drinking time was observed in the low F:C group. Social activity was highest during 1st to 2nd and 9th hour after feed delivery (P<0.05). This study suggests that forage level should be carefully considered
RI PT
to promote normal behavior and positive welfare especially during the growing phase of fattening calves.
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
Keywords: hay grass level, hay particle size, standing time, social behavior
AC C
28
2
28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Introduction
Animals for meat production are intensively raised by offering concentrate and fiber sources
30
from crop residues (Artioli et al., 2015; Muhammad et al., 2015). Concentrate intakes supply energy
31
for growth (Hill et al., 2008) and forage intake alongside concentrate improves efficiency of feed
32
(Coverdale et al., 2004), the rumen environment of growing animals (Khan et al., 2011) and can
33
reduce expression of abnormal behaviors (Muhammad et al., 2015).
RI PT
29
The provision of concentrate and forage enhances the intake of concentrate component at the
35
time of feeding before consuming forage in growing heifers (Devries and von Keyserlingk, 2009). It
36
has been documented that inadequate intake patterns may lead to improper nutrient intake (Devries and
37
von Keyserlingk, 2009), lowered ruminal pH, and ultimately enhance the risk of ruminal acidosis
38
(Beauchemin et al., 2002). The ruminal acidosis prevent the normal display of natural behaviours such
39
as eating and chewing, alter lying patterns, and stimulate the development of abnormal oral behaviours
40
(Artioli et al., 2015; Selemani and Eik, 2016). It is documented in literature that forage particle length
41
(FPL) can effect feeding patterns in dairy cows (Maulfair and Heinrichs, 2013) and sorting. Sorting
42
activity can cause intake of nutritional imbalance feed (Muhammad et al., 2015). Greater forage
43
particle length (FPL) can influence sorting (Maulfair and Heinrichs, 2013), feeding time, chewing time
44
and reduce non-nutritive oral behaviours of animals (Muhammad et al., 2015)
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
34
It is well established that ruminant animals forage intake (Muhammad et al., 2015), and feeding
46
behavior (Rustas et al., 2010; Muhammad et al., 2015) are influenced by the forage and concentrate
47
amount and also by FPL. It is generally considered that restricted forage intake are insufficient in
48
preventing the development of stereotypical behavior (Webb et al., 2012). Stereotypic behaviors in
49
cattle includes excessive oral manipulation of trough, bucket, and pen structures; tongue playing and
50
rolling; sham chewing; and grazing of the coat of other animals (Webb et al., 2012; Miller-Cushon et
51
al., 2013; Muhammad et al., 2015). Therefore, most of the behavioral studies to date have focused on
52
the effect of forage supplementation, particle size, and amount of forage and concentrate on the
AC C
45
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
performance and overall behavior of animals (Redbo and Nordblad, 1997; Broom and Fraser, 2007;
54
Muhammad et al., 2015; Suarez-Mena et al., 2015). To our knowledge no previous study has been
55
conducted to check the forage level and particle size effect on feeding pattern, stereotypic and social
56
behavior of growing Chinese Holstein claves after feed delivery. We evaluated the influence of forage
57
level by changing forage to concentrate ratio (F:C) at different FPL on drinking time, standing time
58
and social behavior of growing Chinese Holstein claves after feed delivery.
59
Materials and Method
61
Animals were managed by following the principles and specific guidelines of the State Key
SC
60
RI PT
53
Laboratory of Animal Nutrition, China Agricultural University, Beijing.
63
M AN U
62
Experiment 1: Effect of forage concentration at full length of grass Fourteen weaned calves of variable body weight (Mean ± SD: 176.91 ± 40.78kg) were used in
65
the first experiment. Calves were housed separately and free moving space (approximately 2x6 m) was
66
given. Bamboos were used to partition calves from each other in such a way that they could still
67
physically contact each other. Calves were divided into two groups. Two treatments were given: 1)
68
lower F:C (50% forage) at full length (FL) of grass hay, and 2) higher F:C (65% forage) at FL of
69
forage. Concentrate was formulated by using, corn (58%), soybean meal (27%), wheat bran (12%),
70
calcium carbonate (2%), Ca(HPO4)2 (1%), and NaCl (1%). Dry matter content (DM), crude protein
71
(CP), neutral detergent fiber (NDF), and acid detergent fiber (ADF) contents of total mix ration (TMR)
72
at lower F:C were 91.10 %, 12.05, 62.81, and 23.10 respectively. While, DM, CP, NDF, and ADF of
73
TMR at higher F:C were 91.32 , 10.33, 66.17, and 28.15 respectively. Total mix ration was offered ad
74
libitum (minimum 5% refusal was maintained) and offered at 8:00 h and 17:00 h of the day. Daily
75
fresh feed samples were taken for laboratory analysis. At the end of 2nd month partitions were removed
76
in such a way that two groups of seven calves were established per treatment. A week long adaptation
77
period was provided to animals to enable them to adjust to social linkages and feeding behavior with
AC C
EP
TE D
64
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
their pen mates. DH-CA-FW48J color cameras supported with infrared anti nozzle technology were
79
used to record video. Four cameras were used and each camera was installed at specific height
80
(approximately 4 m from the floor) to observe each group, and every group was monitored for six
81
days. Nine hours of data (from 8:00 h to 17:00 h of day) from the recorded video were used to analyse
82
the frequency of standing time, drinking time and social behavior. Definitions of the behaviors
83
observed are provided in Table 1. Data were originated from the video using a scan sampling method
84
(Webb, et al., 2012; Miller-Cushon et al., 2013). Three-minute scan sampling procedure was used to
85
record any activity. Activities which were recorded were standing time, drinking time and social
86
behavior. Allogrooming and butting was measured as a social behavior as described in previous
87
research (Faleiro et al., 2011). Social behavior was considered when allogrooming or butting was done
88
by calves at any scan. Total time for given behavior was estimated by total scan obtained for given
89
behavior multiplied by three (Endres et al., 2005).
90
Experiment 2: Effect of forage concentration at short length of grass
M AN U
SC
RI PT
78
Fourteen weaned calves of body weight (Mean ± SD: 136.59 ± 26.14kg) were used in the
92
second experiment. Calves were raised separately and divided by bamboo using the same experimental
93
design as experiment one. Low F:C (50% forage) with short length (SL) of hay grass, high F:C (65%
94
forage) with SL of hay were used as treatments. Concentrate formulation and chemical composition
95
was the same as described in experiment one except for the FPL of hay. Short length of hay was
96
achieved by using a total mix ration mixer. Hay grass was chopped in TMR mixer at 1900 r.p.m for
97
twenty minutes to get short particle sizes ie more than 50% forage particles measured 19 mm on the
98
screen.
99
Chemical and Statistical analysis
AC C
EP
TE D
91
100
Samples were analyzed for DM (135 °C; AOAC, 2000; method 930.15), NDF with α-amylase
101
(heatstable) and sodium sulphite (Vansoest et al., 1991), ADF (AOAC, 2000; method 973.18), and CP
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(AOAC, 2000; method 990.03). Statistical analyses were identical in both experiments. Data for
103
standing time, drinking time and social behavior were summarized on an hourly basis for each calve
104
on each F:C both on FL and SL particle size. Differences among F:C in drinking time, standing time
105
and social behavior over a 9-h period were analyzed using the MIXED procedure of SAS (v. 9.1, SAS
106
Institute Inc. Cary, NC, USA, 2008) treating hour as a repeated measure. The model included the fixed
107
effects of hour, F:C level, and hour by F:C level interaction, and the random effect of replicate. The
108
variance-covariance error structure was first-order autoregressive, according to the best fit with
109
Schwarz’s Bayesian information criterion. All values reported are least squares means. Significance
110
was declared at P ≤ 0.05.
SC
RI PT
102
M AN U
111
Results
113
In experiment 1
114
Treatment and hour interaction was found for standing time (P<0.05). Longest standing time was
115
observed on 9th hour after feed delivery and animals which were on low F:C showed a longer standing
116
time compared to high F:C (Fig. 1). Shortest standing time was observed in the first hour after feed
117
delivery (Fig 1). Drinking water intake was not affected by dietary treatment, hour and treatment and
118
hour interaction (P>0.05) (Fig. 2).
119
Social behavior was affected by hour of observation (P<0.05), with the highest level of social activity
120
recorded between the 1st and 3rd hours after feed delivery and one hour before evening feed delivery
121
(Fig. 3). Animals which were on low F:C animals showed more social activities during this period of
122
time (Fig. 3).
123
In experiment 2
124
Standing time was affected by hour and in both treatments trends for increasing standing time was
125
observed (P<0.05). The first increase was observed from the 1st to 3rd hour after feed delivery (Fig. 4).
126
The second increase was observed from the 7th to 9th hour after feed delivery (Fig. 4). For both trends
AC C
EP
TE D
112
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
animals which were on low F:C showed longer standing time (Fig. 4). Drinking water intake was
128
affected by dietary treatment (P<0.05). The longest drinking time was observed in the low F:C group
129
(Fig. 5).
130
Social behavior was affected by the hour of observation (P<0.05), with social activity longest during
131
1st to 2nd hour after feed delivery and one hour before evening feed delivery in both treatments (Fig. 6).
132
Animals which were on low F:C showed more social activities during this period of time (Fig. 6).
133
Discussion
134
Standing data were coincident in the two experiments, with longer standing times recorded for low F:C
135
animals, for the last few hours after feed delivery. animals in the second experiment demonstrated a
136
trend for longer standing during the 1st to 3rd hour after feed delivery, which was not significant.
137
DeVries et al. (2007) stated that a higher intake rate of the low forage diet is present during the time
138
period following feed delivery due to high sorting activity in stall fed dairy cows. In situations where
139
animals are consuming low forage diet in a highly competitive environment, dominant animals may be
140
rapidly consuming increased amounts of fermentable carbohydrates and low amounts of physically
141
effective fiber. This prevents subordinate animals from feeding and they demonstrate more standing
142
behaviours without doing other activities and may be forced to access feed at a later time. It seems that
143
the higher standing time reported in the 2nd experiment during 1st to 3rd hour after feed delivery at low
144
F:C level was only due to higher competition. However, a higher standing time at the 9th hour at low
145
F:C before evening feed delivery in both experiments was perhaps an indicator of calves waiting for
146
fresh feed. Traditionally, animals devote a large part of their day time on grazing, (Hafez, 1975) so it
147
could be claimed that higher time spent on standing without doing any specific activity at low F:C may
148
be as a result of calves trying to involve in grazing activity without a desirable, available forage
149
source. Furthermore, it has also been predicted that standing without doing any specific activity may
150
be an indicator of feed searching behavior due to hunger (D'Eath et al., 2009). In the current research,
151
calves were given ad libitum access to TMR to prevent hunger. Nevertheless, hay grass may possibly
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
127
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
not have been an appropriate feedstuff and, thus, calves may have continued to search for alternative
153
feed sources. Increased inactive standing time may enhance the susceptibility to claw horn lesions,
154
which have the potential to cause pain and discomfort, leading to reduced mobility and lameness
155
(Chapinal et al., 2009; Faleiro et al., 2011). Therefore, standing time in the current study suggests that
156
welfare of the calves may decrease as forage level decreased.
157
Water intake time increased with decreasing the forage concentration in the second experiment. Lower
158
forage levels may require more water requirements to increase the digesta flow which ultimately
159
enhance the water intake time. These results agree with previous research summarized by Lascano and
160
Heinrichs (2011) who reported that voluntary water intake increase with the low forage diet.
161
Furthermore, recent studies also provide the evidence that greater water intake observed at low forage
162
level (Suarez-Mena et al., 2015; Muhammad et al., 2016).
163
Social behavior was recorded when a calf was licking or nosing a neighboring calf with the muzzle, or
164
butting. Time spent in performing social behaviors in both experiments was increased at low F:C. Our
165
findings concur with those of Faleiro et al. (2011) that time spent in performing social behaviors
166
increases when fiber level decreases by removing straw from the feed. Faleiro et al. (2011) further
167
reported that increases in social behaviour occur mainly as a result of more time spent in allogrooming,
168
although each component of social activity was not registered. We postulate that increases in social
169
behavior in our study were also due to allogrooming. Allogrooming in cattle is an important behavior
170
pattern with functional significance for the formation and maintenance of social bonds, the
171
stabilization of social relationships (Sato et al., 1993) and a good indicator of general health or
172
thriftiness (Albright and Arave, 1997). However, extreme grooming for longer periods of time could
173
also be considered as poor welfare or a sign of stress as shown in other species (Uvnas-Moberg, 1998).
174
Longer time spent in social behavior was particularly evident during the active feeding times ie first
175
few hours after feed delivery and one hour before evening feed delivery, this was also the case for
176
stereotypic behaviors (supplementary Fig. 1 and Fig. 2). Stereotypies consist of a few simple
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
152
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
movements that are repeated in the same way over and over again, although they seem to lack any
178
function in the context in which they are performed (Faleiro et al., 2011). These so-called stereotypic
179
behaviors consist of a repeated tongue playing and rolling; excessive oral manipulation of trough,
180
bucket, and pen structures; and self-grooming in calves (Webb et al., 2012; Miller-Cushon et al., 2013;
181
Muhammad et al., 2015). Chewing activity is associated with the stereotypic behaviors, higher levels
182
of chewing activity can reduce expression of stereotypic behaviors (Faleiro et al., 2011). The hourly
183
data of our adjacent published study (Muhammad et al., 2016) on stereotypic behavior found
184
(supplementary Fig. 1 and Fig. 2) that less stereotypic behaviors occurred during the 1st to 5th hour in
185
the 1st experiment, and the 1st to 4th hour in the 2nd experiment after feed delivery in high F:C animal.
186
This was due to higher chewing activity in calves during this period of time (supplementary Fig. 3 and
187
Fig. 4). If this is the case, a lower chewing activity of high F:C during the 6th to 8th hour in the 1st
188
experiment and during the 5th to 7th hour in the 2nd experiment after feed delivery (supplementary Fig.
189
3 and Fig. 4) should enhance the stereotypic behaviors in both experiment but our results provide
190
evidence that reverse happened. Our results indicate that chewing alone does not control the
191
expression of stereotypic behaviors. In our study the only fiber source provided was hay and hay may
192
not have been a desirable feedstuff and, thus, the use of hay as sole source of fiber might cause
193
abomasal damage. Animals probably do not like to eat hay as sole source of fiber but here it was the
194
only source for animal to fulfill their fibrous needs, so they have no choice but to eat it. It has been
195
reported in previous studies that maize silage pellets, straw, and grains have been associated with
196
abomasal damage (Mattiello et al., 2002; Brscic et al., 2011). Proposed factors involved in the etiology
197
of abomasal damage in beef and veal calves include the abrasive action of coarse feed stuffs
198
(Katchuik, 1992; Mattiello et al., 2002). Stereotypic behavior occurred during the 6th to 8th hour in the
199
1st experiment and during the 5th to 7th hour in the 2nd experiment after feed delivery in high F:C diet,
200
which might result in abomasal damage. As in our adjacent paper (Muhammad et al., 2016) the results
201
here revealed that regardless of dietary treatment stereotypic behavior occurred in both experiments
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
177
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
with low and high forage diets, indicating that calves suffered some welfare problems. It is important
203
to emphasize that the peaks of stereotypic behavior were also observed at the 9th hour after feed
204
delivery in both experiments and corresponded with the peaks of standing time. The increased
205
standing time and stereotypic behavior observed at the 9th hour after feed delivery in both experiments
206
supports that animals were attempting to engage in grazing behavior without desirable forage and
207
showing frustration at the available forage source. In the current study, animals were given ad libitum
208
access to feed to prevent hunger therefore the frustration demonstrated may be due to the availability
209
of a forage source. However, hay may not have been a desirable feedstuff and, thus, the animals may
210
have continued to show stereotypic behavior after feeding.
211
Conclusion
M AN U
SC
RI PT
202
Irrespective of dietary treatment, allogrooming and stereotypic behavior occurs in group
213
housed calves. The frequency of stereotypical and standing time behaviours in the current study
214
suggest that welfare of calves may improve as forage level increase. The current study results suggest
215
that offering alternative forage sources should be considered carefully to promote normal behavior and
216
positive welfare especially when a sole source of forage is fed as the fiber component of the diet in the
217
growing phase of fattening calves.
218
Acknowledgements
AC C
219
EP
TE D
212
220
The authors are grateful to undergraduate and graduate students at research station for their
221
help during sample collection. Particularly, we are thankful to master students, Yang Yong Zai, and
222
Wang Chang Shui, for their assistance. We also acknowledge the financial support received from
223
National Beef Cattle Industry and Technology System, Beijing, China.
224
Conflict of Interest
225
No conflict of interest exits in the submission of this manuscript, and manuscript is approved by
226
all authors for publication. I would like to declare on behalf of my co-authors that the work described
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
227
is original research that has not been published previously, and ever not under consideration for
228
publication elsewhere
229
Ethics/Permission Animals were managed by following the principles and specific guidelines of the State Key
230
Laboratory of Animal Nutrition, China Agricultural University, Beijing.
232
Author’s contributions
RI PT
231
Muhammad Aziz ur Rahman carried out the experiment, finished data analysis, and drafted the
234
manuscript. Xia Chuan Qi participated in the experiments and helped with data collection and analysis.
235
Su Huawei and Binghai Cao conceived the experiment and finished the manuscript. All authors
236
approved the final version of the manuscript for publication.
237
References
238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266
AOAC., 2000. Official Methods of Analysis. 17th Ed. Association of Official Analytical Chemists, Gaithersburg, MD, USA. Albright, J. L., Arave, C. W., 1997. The behaviour of cattle. CAB international, Wallingford, UK. Artioli, L. F. A., Moriel, P., Poore, M. H., Marques, R. S., Cooke, R. F., 2015. Decreasing the frequency of energy supplementation from daily to three times weekly impairs growth and humoral immune response of preconditioning beef steers. J. Anim. Sci. 93, 5430-5441.10.2527/jas.2015-9457 Beauchemin, K. A., Maekawa, M., Christensen, D. A., 2002. Effect of diet and parity on meal patterns of lactating dairy cows. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 82, 215-223 Broom, D., Fraser, A., 2007. Abnormal behaviour 1: stereotypies. Domestic animal behaviour and welfare, 226234 Brscic, M., Heutinck, L. F. M., Wolthuis-Fillerup, M., Stockhofe, N., Engel, B., Visser, E. K., Gottardo, F., Bokkers, E. A. M., Lensink, B. J., Cozzi, G., Van Reenen, C. G., 2011. Prevalence of gastrointestinal disorders recorded at postmortem inspection in white veal calves and associated risk factors. J. Dairy Sci. 94, 853863.10.3168/jds.2010-3480 Chapinal, N., de Passillé, A. M., Weary, D. M., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G., Rushen, J., 2009. Using gait score, walking speed, and lying behavior to detect hoof lesions in dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 92, 43654374.http://dx.doi.org/10.3168/jds.2009-2115 Coverdale, J. A., Tyler, H. D., Quigley, J. D., Brumm, J. A., 2004. Effect of various levels of forage and form of diet on rumen development and growth in calves. J. Dairy Sci. 87, 2554-2562 D'Eath, R. B., Tolkamp, B. J., Kyriazakis, I., Lawrence, A. B., 2009. 'Freedom from hunger' and preventing obesity: the animal welfare implications of reducing food quantity or quality. Anim. Behav. 77, 275288.10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.028 DeVries, T. J., Beauchemin, K. A., Keyserlingk, M. A. G. v., 2007. Dietary forage concentration affects the feed sorting behaiour of lactating cow. J. Dairy Sci. 90, 5572–5579.10.3168/jds.2007-0370 Devries, T. J., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G., 2009. Short communication: Feeding method affects the feeding behavior of growing dairy heifers. J. Dairy Sci. 92, 1161-1168.10.3168/jds.2008-1314 Endres, M. I., DeVries, T. J., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G. Weary, D. M., 2005. Effect of feed barrier design on the behavior of loose housed lactating dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 88, 2377–2380. Faleiro, A. G., Gonzalez, L. A., Blanch, M., Cavini, S., Castells, L., de la Torre, J. L. R., Manteca, X.,
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
233
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Calsamiglia, S., Ferret, A., 2011. Performance, ruminal changes, behaviour and welfare of growing heifers fed a concentrate diet with or without barley straw. Animal 5, 294-303.Doi 10.1017/S1751731110001904 Hafez, E. S. E., 1975. The Behaviour of Domestic Animals. Baillière Tindall. Hill, T. M., Bateman, H. G., II, Aldrich, J. M., Schlotterbeck, R. L., 2008. Effects of the amount of chopped hay or cottonseed hulls in a textured calf starter on young calf performance. J. Dairy Sci. 91, 26842693.10.3168/jds.2007-0935 Katchuik, R., 1992. Abomasal Disease in Young Beef-Calves - Surgical Findings and Management Factors. Can. Vet. J. 33, 459-461 Khan, M. A., Weary, D. M., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G., 2011. Hay intake improves performance and rumen development of calves fed higher quantities of milk. J. Dairy Sci. 94, 3547-3553.10.3168/jds2010-3871 Lascano, G. J., Heinrichs, A. J., 2011. Effects of feeding different levels of dietary fiber through the addition of corn stover on nutrient utilization of dairy heifers precision-fed high and low concentrate diets1. J. Dairy Sci. 94, 3025-3036.http://dx.doi.org/10.3168/jds.2010-3831 Mattiello, S., Canali, E., Ferrante, V., Caniatti, M., Gottardo, F., Cozzi, G., Andrighetto, I., Verga, M., 2002. The provision of solid feeds to veal calves: II. Behavior, physiology, and abomasal damage. J. Anim. Sci. 80, 367-375 Maulfair, D. D., Heinrichs, A. J., 2013. Effects of varying forage particle size and fermentable carbohydrates on feed sorting, ruminal fermentation, and milk and component yields of dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 96, 30853097.10.3168/jds.2012-6048 Miller-Cushon, E. K., Bergeron, R., Leslie, K. E., Mason, G. J., DeVries, T. J., 2013. Effect of feed presentation on feeding patterns of dairy calves. J. Dairy Sci. 96, 7260-7268.10.3168/jds.2013-7013 Muhammad, A. U., Xia, C. Q., Cao, B. H., 2016. Dietary forage concentration and particle size affect sorting, feeding behaviour, intake and growth of Chinese holstein male calves. J Anim Physiol Anim Nutr (Berl) 100, 217-223.10.1111/jpn.12349 Redbo, I., Nordblad, A., 1997. Stereotypies in heifers are affected by feeding regime. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 53, 193-202.Doi 10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01145-8 Rustas, B. O., Norgaard, P., Jalali, A. R., Nadeau, E., 2010. Effects of physical form and stage of maturity at harvest of whole-crop barley silage on intake, chewing activity, diet selection and faecal particle size of dairy steers. Animal 4, 67-75.Doi 10.1017/S1751731109990887 Sato, S., Tarumizu, K., Hatae, K., 1993. The influence of social factors on allogrooming in cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 38, 235-244.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(93)90022-H Selemani, I. S., Eik, L. O., 2016. The effects of concentrate supplementation on growth performance and behavioral activities of cattle grazed on natural pasture. Trop. Anim. Health Prod. 48, 229232.10.1007/s11250015-0935-z Suarez-Mena, F. X., Lascano, G. J., Rico, D. E., Heinrichs, A. J., 2015. Effect of forage level and replacing canola meal with dry distillers grains with solubles in precision-fed heifer diets: Digestibility and rumen fermentation. J. Dairy Sci. 98, 8054-8065.10.3168/jds.2015-9636 Uvnas-Moberg, K., 1998. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 819-835 Vansoest, P. J., Robertson, J. B., Lewis, B. A., 1991. Methods for Dietary Fiber, Neutral Detergent Fiber, and Nonstarch Polysaccharides in Relation to Animal Nutrition. J. Dairy Sci. 74, 3583-3597 Webb, L. E., Bokkers, E. A. M., Engel, B., Gerrits, W. J. J., Berends, H., van Reenen, C. G., 2012. Behaviour and welfare of veal calves fed different amounts of solid feed supplemented to a milk replacer ration adjusted for similar growth. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 136, 108-116.10.1016/j.applanim.2011.12.004
AC C
267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
311 312
Fig. 1. Hourly averages - Standing time (min) in growing Chinese Holstein male calves fed full length
313
hay with lower forage to concentrate ratio (F:C) (50% forage) and higher F:C (65% forage). Data are
314
averaged over 6 d for 7 animals on each treatment. Error bars indicate standard error.
RI PT
315
Fig. 2. Hourly averages - Drinking water (min), in growing Chinese Holstein male calves fed full
317
length hay with lower forage to concentrate ratio (F:C) (50% forage) and higher F:C (65% forage).
318
Data are averaged over 6 d for 7 animals on each treatment. Error bars indicate standard error.
SC
316
319
Fig. 3. Hourly averages - Social behaviour (min) in growing Chinese Holstein male calves fed full
321
length hay with lower forage to concentrate ratio (F:C) (50 % forage) and higher F:C (65% forage).
322
Data are averaged over 6 d for 7 animals on each treatment. Error bars indicate standard error.
M AN U
320
323
Fig. 4. Hourly averages -Standing time (min) in growing Chinese Holstein male calves fed short length
325
hay with lower forage to concentrate ratio (F:C) (50 % forage) and higher F:C (65% forage).
326
Data are averaged over 6 d for 7 animals on each treatment. Error bars indicate standard error.
EP
327
TE D
324
Fig. 5. Hourly averages - Drinking water (min) in growing Chinese Holstein male calves fed short
329
length hay with lower forage to concentrate ratio (F:C) (50 % forage) and higher F:C (65% forage).
330
Data are averaged over 6 d for 7 animals on each treatment. Error bars indicate standard errorb
331
AC C
328
332
Fig. 6. Hourly averages - Social behaviour (min) in growing Chinese Holstein male calves fed short
333
length hay with lower forage to concentrate ratio (F:C) (50 % forage) and higher F:C (65% forage).
334
Data are averaged over 6 d for 7 animals on each treatment. Error bars indicate standard error.)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Chewing
Animal having its head in the feeder and being Activity engaged in chewing Chewing was defined as mastication movements Activity other than eating either animal is standing or laying
M AN U
Activity Eating
SC
RI PT
Table1. Behavior Definitions Behavior Description Category Posture Body elevated from floor and weight supported by Posture Standing legs and without doing any specific activity like (eating, chewing, walking, fighting, running, licking any surface, tongue rolling, consuming wood shavings, butting, and rubbing any surface) Brisket in contact with floor with no chewing, licking Posture Lying any surface, tongue rolling, consuming wood shavings, and rubbing any surface with head was performed by animals
Stereotypic behavior Tongue playing1
AC C
EP
TE D
Tongue playing/rolling; repeatedly turning, rolling or Abnormal unrolling tongue extended outside or inside mouth Abnormal Oral manipulation of Licking, nibbling or sucking trough 2 trough Abnormal Oral manipulation of Licking, nibbling, or sucking portions of pen 3 pen Tongue is extended and shifted across own body Abnormal Groom4 repeatedly Social licking, and nosing coat or other part of another calf Social Allogrooming in the pen with the muzzle When the animal pressed any body part of another Social Butting calf with its head or thigh 2, 3, 4, behavior were collectively considered as oral manipulation 1, 2, 3, 4 behavior were collectively considered as stereotypic behavior
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Highlights
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Regardless of level of hay grass, calves show allogrooming and stereotypic behavior Higher levels of hay grass improve welfare of calves Chewing can overcome the stereotypic behavior to a degree
AC C
• • •