Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacuf.
& Wol. Psych&. Copyright
2000, Vol. 24, pp. 535-546 0 2000 Elsevier Science
Printed in the USA. 027%5846/00/$kee
ELSEWER
Inc.
All rights reserved front matter
PIk 80278-5846(00)00091-9
EFFECTS OF TOBACCO SMOKING OR TOPOGRAPHIC EEG AND STROOP TEST IN SMOKIIUG DEPRIVED SMOKERS
XIAOJUAN XU1 AND EDWARD F. DOMINO’ ‘Department of Psychology, Grand Valley State University; *Department of Pharmacology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA (Final form, April 2000)
Abstract Xu, Xiaojuan and Edward F. Domino: Effects of Tobacco Smoking on Topographic EEG and Stroop test in smoking deprived smokers. Prog. Nemo-Psychopharmacol. & Biol. Psychiat. 2000,24, pp. 535-546.02000 %evier Science Inc. 1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
Quantitative electroencephalography (EEG) was used to measure human brain electrical changes produced by tobacco deprivation and smoking. Sixteen scalp cortical recording sites monitored regional changes in brain activity. The quantitative EEG was subdivided into delta, theta, alpha,, alph$, beta, and beta, bands for topographic mapping. A demanding version of the Stroop test was used to determine tobacco smoking effects on attention. The version used was more difficult than that used in previously reported studies. Healthy drug and substance free adult male and female volunteers were divided into nonsmoker (n=7) and smoker (n=7) groups according to their smoking status. They were instructed to abstain from tobacco products for at least 12 hr overnight before the next morning’s experiment. EEG was recorded before and after smoking either a fake placebo cigarette for nonsmokers or the cigarette of their choice for smokers. Subjects were also asked to perform the Stroop test before and after smoking the placebo or tobacco cigarette. The results showed that tobacco smoking significantly depressed delta and increased alpha, and beta, activity and slightly increased the Stroop effect. Although smoking one cigarette stimulated brain electrical activity of smoking deprived smokers, it did not improve performance on a difficult Stroop test.
Kevwords: attention, topographic EEG.
Stroop test, tobacco smoking, tobacco smoking abstinence,
Abbreviation: electroencephalogram
(EEG), electrocardiogram 535
(ECG)
536
X. Xu and E.F. Domino
Introduction Studies have shown that administration smokers
cause changes
of nicotine and nicotine deprivation
in the scalp recorded
electroencephalogram
noninvasive measure of changes in brain electrical activity. and Warburton, reported
1983; Pickworth
that 24 hr deprivation
(Comin,
et al., 1989; Knott, 1989). of nicotine
theta power.
in heavy
These effects
(EEG),
a
1980; Edwards
Ulett and Itil (1969)
smokers
decreased
frequency
and increased
cigarettes.
Knott and Venables (1977) found that 13 to 15 hr deprivation
in heavy smokers also decreased the alpha frequency.
in heavy
were reversed
alpha
by smoking of nicotine
Herning et al. (1983) showed
an increase in the theta power of heavy smokers after 4 hrs of deprivation.
Knott
(1988) and Golding (1988) reported
alpha
frequencies
that smokers showed a shift to higher
Thus, the results of several studies
after some form of nicotine intake.
suggest that short term nicotine deprivation
accompanies changing to slow activity in
EEG, and tobacco smoking increases brain wave frequencies. In the present study, the human EEG was further divided into six frequency delta (l-3.75
Hz), theta (4-7.5 Hz), alpha, (7.7510 Hz), and beta, (20.25-30
(12.75-20 frequencies
into one bandwidth.
Hz).
Hz), alpha, (10.25-12.5
Previous
studies grouped
bands:
Hz), beta, the alpha
Thus, it is possible that a decrease in the power of
lower alpha frequencies masked an increase in the power of higher alpha frequencies. One misses frequencies
important into broader
power
shifts
bandwidths.
within
the
A previous
alpha
bandwidth
by grouping
study employing
six frequency
bands showed that tobacco smoking shifted alpha EEG activity from alpha, to alp4 (Domino et al., 1992).
The present study employed six frequency
EEG was recorded
from
16 scalp positions to monitor regional changes in brain activity and topographic
EEG
precise changes in different frequency bands.
Furthermore,
bands to measure
mapping was used to characterize which regions of the brain show changes. Behavioral
studies have shown that tobacco smoking and nicotine
intake affect
human attention, the process that enables an individual to ignore the vast majority of information
and focus only on the relevant information.
One of the well established
paradigms
to study attention is the Stroop test (Battig,
1985; Battig, 1991; Estler,
1982).
In the Stroop test, a subject is required to identify the color of the inks in
Effects of tobacco smoking on which a series of color names are printed.
537
EEG and attention
The color names correspond
to the color Subjects
of the inks in which they are printed in some trials, but not in other trials.
find the incongruity of color names and the color of the inks in which they are printed The
very distracting and take longer to identify the color of the inks in such trials. performance
difference between processing conflicting information
Green printed in red letters) and processing nonconflicting
(such as the word
information
(such as the
word Green printed in green letters) is known as the Stroop effect, which reflects a failure of the attention process to ignore the irrelevant shown that nicotine improves
the performance
information.
Studies have
on the Stroop test and reduces the
Stroop effect (Wesnes and Warburton, 1978; Hasenfratz and Battig, 1992). whether nicotine improves question.
the attention process on a more difficult
The present study also investigated
attention by utilizing a computerized
the effects of tobacco
However,
task is still a smoking on
Stroop test, which is a more difficult
than those in existing studies. Furthermore,
version
the study explored any parallel effects of
tobacco use on the brain electrical activity and on the attention process.
Methods Subjects Healthy adult male and female subjects, ranging in age from 20-40 years, and free from any medication
or drug use, were recruited
through
advertisements.
Subjects
were divided into two groups according to their smoking status. One group (n=7, including 4 females and 3 males) consisted of nonsmokers who inhaled air through a sham cigarette during the experimental
session. The other group (n=7, including 4
females and 3 males) consisted of moderate to heavy tobacco smokers (l-2 packs/day). Subjects were instructed
to abstain from
drinking
any caffeinated
beverages
or
alcohol, using any medications or drugs, or any form of tobacco for the 12 hr period before the experiment
which was conducted in the morning.
On the morning
of the
experiment, subjects were questioned about their compliance with the no drinking, no drugs and no smoking requirement, and the informed consent was then obtained.
538
X. Xu and E.F. Domino
EEG and ECG Recording The experiment
utilized
16 cortical
recording
reference lead per the lo-20 International of Clinical Neurophysiology,
sites linked to A, and A, as the
System (Recommendations
1983). An electrode cap (Electrode
for the Practice Cap International,
Eaton, OH 45320) was placed on each subject’s head with Grass electrode gel applied to each electrode. The EEG recordings taken from F7, F,, T,, T,, T,, T,, FP,, FP,, F,, F,, C,, C,, P,, P,, 0,, electroencephalograph taken from recordings
and 0,
were
recorded
on channels
(Model 8-24D, Grass Instruments,
the right arm and left leg was recorded were transferred
1-16 of a Grass
Quincy, MA 02169). ECG on channel
to a Zenith 386/25 microcomputer
17. The EEG
directly
and were
analyzed by the computer software package RHYTHM 7.1 (Stellate Systems, Quebec, Canada). Color topographic
maps were printed by a Hewlett-Packard
Paintjet (Model
3630A, Hewlett-Packard
Co., Rolling Meadows, IL 60008). The EEG was analyzed in
each of six bandwidths:
l-3.75 Hz (delta), 4-7.5 Hz (theta), 7.75-10 Hz (alpha ,),
10.25-12.5 Hz (alpha ,), 12.75-20 Hz (beta ,), and 20.25-30 Hz (beta J. The Stroou Test The Stroop test used a Macintosh computer presenting nonconflicting information.
The information
was made up of six color words: red, brown, yellow,
green, blue, and purple; six colors corresponded of nonconflicting
information:
to the words. There were six types
red, brown, yellow, green, blue, and purple written in
red, brown, yellow, green, blue, and purple, respectively. conflicting
information:
60 trials,
and so on.
30 trials with nonconflicting
conflicting information.
There were 30 types of
the word red written in five colors except red, the word
brown written in five colors except brown, contained
or conflicting
One Stroop test session
information
and 30 trials with
The subjects were instructed by the computer to identify the
color and press key 1 for color red, 2 for brown, 3 for yellow, 4 for green, 5 for blue, and 6 for purple. Each key was labeled with a small piece of corresponding color paper.
In each trial, the computer presented either one color word written in
the color it indicated or written in other colors. When the subject responded, computer immediately time and accuracy.
the
presented the next trial. The computer recorded both reaction
Effects of tobacco
539
smoking on EEG and attention
Procedure The subjects were Macintosh computer
instructed
to follow
the instructions
on the screen
and practice the Stroop test after they arrived
area. After they finished practicing
the Stroop test, electrodes
of the
in the research
were affixed to the
skull and ears for EEG, right arm and left leg for ECG. Then they were instructed to relax in a reclined position and the light was switched off in the room. After two twominute practice perform
recordings
of eyes-closed
EEG and ECG, subjects were asked to
the Stroop test in a sitting position. When they finished the Stroop test,
subjects were told to close their eyes and relax in a reclined position. One two-minute session of eyes-closed
EEG was recorded.
Then the subjects were
minutes to smoke either a sham cigarette or a cigarette minute session of eyes-closed EEG was recorded subjects were then instructed to perform
allowed
five
of their choice. One two-
immediately
after smoking.
The
the Stroop test again. The Stroop test was
always performed with the lights on and the EEG was always recorded with the lights off. Statistical Analysis The differences
between EEG before smoking and after smoking were tested using
two-way ANOVAs with two within factors (2 conditions x 16 channels) on each of the six bands for each group. The differences between before and after smoking at each channel were tested using multiple mean comparisons
associated with the two-way
ANOVAs. The differences between the EEG of nonsmokers and smokers before smoking were tested using two-way
ANOVAs with one between factor and one within factor
groups x 16 channels) on each of the six bands. measures
(16 channels)
and one factor
(2
One-way MANOVAs with repeated
(2 groups)
were
also used to test the
differences between the two groups at each channel on each of the six bands.
Results The topographic maps of the mean EEG of nonsmokers before and after sham
After
I
05.56 81.13 76.69 72.25 67.81 63.38 58.94 54.58 58.06 45.63 41.19 36.75 32.3i 27.68 23.44 19.m
90.88 85.56 81.13 76.69 72.25 67.01 63.38 5B.94 54.58 50.9b 45.63 4L.19 X.75 32.31 27.88 23.44
Fig 1: Topographic EEG maps of nonsmokers and smokers. The top of the head is projected to a surface map with the nose above and the left hemisphere on the reader’s left, etc. Thus, the frontal cortex is above and the occipital cortex below. The upper series of colored maps shows the mean EEG before and after sham smoking in seven nonsmokers. The lower series shows the mean EEG before and after cigarette smoking in seven smokers, The colored calibrations represent voltage normalization for each frequency band.
SMOKERS
NONSMOKERS
Effects of tobacco smoking on EEG and attention
smoking show no significant change [Fig 1: upper panels].
541
However, the topographic
maps of the mean EEG of smokers before and after cigarette smoking show changes [Fig. 1: lower panels]. Two-way ANOVAs with repeated measure carried out on data for each band showed that tobacco smoking significantly decreased delta [F (1, 15) = 11.524, p < 0.011, and increased alpha, [F (1, 15) = 4.4931 and beta, activity [F (I, 1s) = 10.368, p < 0.011. activity
decreased
positions:
For the delta band, multiple comparisons
significantly
following
smoking
FP,, FP,, F,, F,, C,, C,, P, and P,.
comparisons
at the following
beta, activity increased
recording
For the alpha, band, multiple
showed that alpha, activity increased significantly
all sixteen recording positions.
showed that delta
following
smoking at
For the beta, band, multiple comparisons showed that
significantly
following
smoking at the following
recording
positions: T,, T,, FP,, FP,, F,, F,, C,, C,, P,, P,, 0, and Oz. A comparison
of the topographic
maps of the mean EEG of nonsmokers
before
sham smoking and the topographic maps of the mean EEG of smokers before smoking [Fig 1: left panel] suggested that tobacco deprivation decreased high frequency EEG activity.
increased low frequency
and
However, both ANOVAs and MANOVAs on
the mean EEG of nonsmokers before sham smoking and the mean EEG of smokers before smoking revealed no significant differences on each of the six bands. The results of the Stroop tests are shown in Fig 2. The upper two panels of Fig 2 show the mean reaction times and accuracy for nonconflicting
information
[Fig. 2:
upper panels], while the middle two panels show the mean reaction times and accuracy for conflicting information
[Fig. 2: middle panels]. Comparisons of the reaction times
indicate that the reaction times were shorter for nonconflicting for conflicting
information,
except the reaction
time of smokers
information
before smoking.
nonsmokers
made fewer mistakes for nonconflicting
conflicting
information.
for
and
conflicting
Comparisons of accuracy show that both smokers and information
between reaction
times to conflicting
and the Stroop effects measured by differences
conflicting
for
than those
than they did for
The lower two panels of Fig 2 show the Stroop effects
measured by differences information,
information
nonconflicting
information
[Fig.
and nonconflicting
between percent errors
2: lower
nonsmokers, the Stroop effects did not change after sham smoking.
panels].
For
For smokers, the
542
X. Xu and E.F. Domino
Times to NonconflictingInformations
Reaction
23
Nonsmokers Smokers
ReactionTimes to ConflictingInformation
Nonsmokers
Smokers
Accuracyfor NonconflictingInformation
Nonsmokers Smokers Accuracyfor Conflicting Information
Nonsmokers Smokers
600
c
g
500
;
400
5 k ;
200
8
100
R
300
0 -100
Nonsmokers Smokers Nonsmokers Smokers Groups Groups Fig 2: The upper four panels show the mean reaction times to and accuracy for Each bar in the reaction time panels nonconflicting and conflicting information. represents the mean reaction time f SE for 5-6 subjects, whereas each bar in the accuracy panels represents the mean percent errors f SE for 5-6 subjects. The lower two panels show the mean Stroop effects. Each bar in the left lower panel represents the Stroop effects measured by a difference between the reaction times to conflicting and nonconflicting information + SE for 5-6 subjects. Each bar in the right lower panel represents the Stroop effects measured by a difference between percent errors for conflicting and nonconflicting information & SE for 5-6 subjects. Stroop effects measured by differences
in reaction times slightly increased following
smoking, while the Stroop effects measured by differences in accuracy did not change following smoking.
However, the increase in the Stroop effects of smokers was not
543
Effects of tobacco smoking on EEG and attention
significant by a correlated t-test. The recording
of ECG indicated that sham smoking did not change heart rate of
nonsmokers whereas smoking significantly increased the heart rate of smokers [Fig 31.
lOO8060-
I
40-
l&p&e.
20O!
Nonsmokers
Smokers Groups
Fig 3: Heart rates of nonsmokers and smokers before and after smoking a sham cigarette or a cigarette. Each bar represents the mean heart rates per minute f SE for seven subjects. ** p < 0.01 Discussion Tonographic
EEG
The effects of tobacco smoking on EEG in the present study confirmed findings that tobacco smoking decreases low frequency
and increases high frequency
EEG activity (Knott, 1988; Golding, 1988; Domino et al., 1992). showed that tobacco
smoking significantly
the previous
decreased
The present study
delta activity,
alpha, and beta, activity in smoking deprived smokers.
but increased
Decreases in delta activity
occurred mainly in the frontal and parietal regions, while increases in beta, occurred mainly
in
Interestingly,
the
frontal,
parietal,
occipital,
and
the
right
increases in alpha, occurred in all recorded regions.
temporal
regions.
544
X. Xu and E.F. Domino
A comparison
of the topographic
maps of the mean EEG of nonsmokers
before
sham smoking and the topographic maps of the mean EEG of smokers before smoking suggested that smokers showed increased low frequency and decreased high frequency EEG activity following EEG of nonsmokers
tobacco deprivation,
although the differences
between the
and smokers before smoking were not statistically
significant.
Such differences in EEG may reflect the effects of nicotine withdrawal, suggesting the addictive nature of nicotine. reflect
individual
nonsmoker
It is also possible that such differences
differences
since subjects were not randomly
and smoker groups.
time will be able to address
in EEG may
assigned to the
Future studies focus on the EEG of smokers over the effects
of nicotine
withdrawal
Nonetheless, the present study suggested that tobacco deprivation
with certainty.
seemed to increase
low frequency and decrease high frequency EEG activity. Stroon Effects To make certain that the effects of tobacco smoking lasted throughout the Stroop test after smoking, recorded
another
two-minute
session of eyes-closed
following the Stroop test after smoking.
EEG and ECG were
Both EEG and ECG showed the
effects of tobacco smoking (data not shown). Thus, the Stroop test after smoking was carried out under the influences of tobacco smoking.
The results of the Stroop test
indicated that sham smoking did not affect the Stroop effects whereas tobacco smoking increased
the Stroop
effect
conflicting and nonconflicting
measured
by differences
information.
between
significant.
Comparisons
between
performances
smoking was not of
nonsmokers before smoking show that smokers responded to conflicting slightly faster than nonsmokers, nonconflict and conflict information smokers experience smokers identified names were printed.
times to
Because of great variations in the Stroop
effects among smokers, the increase in the Stroop effect following statistically
reaction
smokers
information
but they made slightly more mistakes for than nonsmokers.
and
both
Such results may suggest that
restless effects of nicotine withdrawal.
One nonsmoker
and two
color names instead of the color of the inks in which the color Their data were not included in the results of the Stroop test.
Their mistakes further suggested the difficulty of the version of the Stroop test in the present study. Thus, tobacco smoking did not seem to improve attention process on a
Effects of difficult
Stroop test, rather
tobacco
545
smoking on EEG and attention
the present study suggested an increase in the Stroop
effects. The recording of ECG suggested that sham smoking did not affect the heart rates but tobacco smoking significantly increased the heart rate (Fig 3). Thus, tobacco smoking produced stimulant effects.
Although tobacco smoking stimulated the brain electrical
activity and the heart beat, it did not seem to improve attention process required for a difficult task.
Conclusions 1. Smoking cigarette significantly decreased EEG delta activity, and increased alpha, and beta, activity in smoking deprived smokers. 2. Smoking cigarette did not seem to improve performance
of a difficult Stroop test.
3. Smoking cigarette significantly increased the heart rate.
Acknowledgments Supported in part by NIDA grants DA-07226, DA-10992, the Psychopharmacology Research Fund 361024, and Grand Valley State University grant-in-aid.
References B_&TTIG, K. (1991) Coffee, cardiovascular and behavioral effects--current trends. Rev. Environ. Health. m, 53-84.
research
BATTIG, K. (1985) The physiological effects of coffee consumption. In: Coffee, Botany, Biochemistry and Production of Beans and Beverage, M. N. Clifford and K. C. Wilson (Eds.), pp 394-439, The Avi Publishing Company, Westport, CT. CONRIN, J. (1980) The EEG effects of tobacco smoking--A Electroencephalogram. 11, 180- 187.
review.
Clinical
DOMINO, E.F., RISKALLA, M., ZHANG, Y. AND KIM, E. (1992) Effects of tobacco smoking on the topographic EEG II. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. & Biol. Psychiat. 16, 463-482. EDWARDS, J.A. and WARBURTON, D.M. (1983) electrocortical activity. Pharmac. Ther. 19, 147-164.
Smoking,
nicotine,
and
546
X. Xu and E.F. Domino
ESTLER, C. J. (1982) Caffeine. In: Psychotropic Agents: Part III. Alcohol and Psychomimetics, Psychotropic Effects of Central Acting Drugs, F. Hoffmeister and G. Stille (Eds.), pp 369-389, Springer-Verlag, Berlin. GOLDING, J. F. (1988) Effects of cigarette smoking on resting EEG, visual evoked potentials and photic driving. Pharmacol. Biochem. & Behav. 29, 23-32. HASENFRATZ, M. and BATTIG, K. (1992) Action profiles of smoking and caffeine: Stroop effect, EEG, and peripheral physiology. Pharmacol. Biochem. & Behav. 42, 155-161. HERNING, R. I., JONES, R. T., and BACHMAN, J. (1983) EEG changes during tobacco withdrawal. Psychopharmacology. 20(51, 507-5 12. KNOTT, V.J. Dynamic (1988) Neuropsychobiology. fi, 54-60.
EEG
changes
during
KNOTT, V.J. (1989) Brain electrical imaging the dose-response smoking. Neuropsychobiology. 22, 236-242.
cigarette
smoking,
effects of cigarette
KNOTT, V.J. and VENABLES, P.H. (1977) EEG alpha correlates of non-smokers, smokers, smoking, and smoking deprivation. Psychophysiology. 14(2), 150-156. (1989) PICKWORTH, W.B., HERNING, R. I. and HENNINGFIELD, J.E. Spontaneous EEG changes during tobacco abstinence and nicotine substitution in human volunteers. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 25 l(2), 976-982. Recommendations for the Practice of Clinical Neurophysiology International Federation of Societies for Electroencephalograph Neurophysiology, Elsevier, New York. ULETT, J.A. and ITIL, T.M. (1969) and smoking deprivation. Science.
Quantitative electroencephalogram 164, 969-972.
The (1983) and Clinical in smoking
WESNES, K. and WARBURTON, D.M. (1978) The effects of cigarette smoking and In Smoking Behavior. R.E. Thornton, nicotine tablets upon human attention. (Ed.). pp 13 l- 147, Churchill Livingstone. Edinburgh.
Inquiries and reprint requests should be addressed to: Dr. Xiaojuan Xu Department of Psychology Grand Valley State University Allendale, MI 4940 1 USA E-mail:
[email protected]