Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Clinical Neurophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph
Electroencephalographic activity of preterm infants is increased by Family Nurture Intervention: A randomized controlled trial in the NICU Martha G. Welch a,b,c,d,⇑,1, Michael M. Myers a,b,d,1, Philip G. Grieve b,e, Joseph R. Isler b, William P. Fifer a,b,d, Rakesh Sahni b, Myron A. Hofer a,d, Judy Austin f, Robert J. Ludwig a, Raymond I. Stark b, the FNI Trial Group2 a
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University College of Physicians & Surgeons, New York, NY 10032, USA Department of Pediatrics, Columbia University College of Physicians & Surgeons, New York, NY 10032, USA c Department of Pathology & Cell Biology, Columbia University College of Physicians & Surgeons, New York, NY 10032, USA d Department of Developmental Neuroscience, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY 10032, USA e Department of Biomedical Engineering, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA f Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, NY 10032, USA b
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history: Accepted 17 August 2013 Available online 17 October 2013 Keywords: Maternal separation Mother–infant intervention EEG Premature birth Brain maturation
h i g h l i g h t s Family Nurture Intervention (FNI) is designed to facilitate affective connections between prematurely
born infants and their mothers. At term age, prematurely born infants with FNI had robust increases in frontal brain activity. Promoting early nurturing interactions may improve neurobehavioral outcomes of preterm infants.
a b s t r a c t Objective: To assess the impact of Family Nurture Intervention (FNI) on electroencephalogram (EEG) activity in preterm infants (26–34 weeks gestation). Methods: Two groups were tested in a single, level IV neonatal intensive care unit (NICU; standard care or standard care plus FNI) using a randomized controlled trial design. The intervention consists of sessions designed to achieve mutual calm and promote communication of affect between infants and their mothers throughout the NICU stay. EEG recordings were obtained from 134 infants during sleep at 35 and 40 weeks postmenstrual age (PMA). Regional brain activity (power) was computed for 10 frequency bands between 1 and 48 Hz in each of 125 electrodes. Results: Near to term age, compared to standard care infants, FNI infants showed robust increases in EEG power in the frontal polar region at frequencies 10 to 48 Hz (20% to 36% with p-values <0.0004). Effects were significant in both quiet and active sleep, regardless of gender, singleton-twin status, gestational age (26–30 or 30–35 weeks) or birth weight (<1500 or >1500 g). Conclusion: FNI leads to increased frontal brain activity during sleep, which other investigators find predictive of better neurobehavioral outcomes. Significance: FNI may be a practicable means of improving outcomes in preterm infants. Ó 2013 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction ⇑ Corresponding author. Address: New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1051 Riverside Drive, Unit 40, New York, NY 10032, USA. Tel.: +1 212 543 5101; fax: +1 212 253 4234. E-mail address:
[email protected] (M.G. Welch). 1 These authors contributed equally to the work. 2 The FNI Trial Group members: Ladan Afifi, Howard F. Andrews, David A. Bateman, Amy Bechar, Beatrice S. Beebe, Susan A. Brunelli, Kathryn E. Carnazza, Christine Y. Chang, Patricia A. Farrell, Ewelina S. Fiedor, Amie A. Hane, Qais Karim, Shanna Kofman, Yasmine A. Koukaz, John M. Lorenz, Mary T. McKiernan, David P. Merle, Richard A. Polin, Sorana Sopterian.
Preterm birth at <37 weeks gestational age (GA) in the United States has an incidence of 12% (Hamilton et al., 2013). The problem addressed by this current study is that accompanying early preterm birth is a period of prolonged physical and emotional separation of mothers and infants during neonatal care with increased risk for adverse outcomes during subsequent growth and development. This vulnerability manifests as neurobehavioral disorders that include attention deficits (Johnson and Marlow,
1388-2457/$36.00 Ó 2013 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.08.021
676
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
2011), executive dysfunction (Baron et al., 2012; Peterson et al., 2000), depression and psychotic disorders (Nosarti et al., 2012), and autism spectrum disorder (Pinto-Martin et al., 2011). Numerous preclinical and clinical studies over the course of the past fifty years have confirmed that early mother infant separation leads to a wide array of adverse physiological and behavioral consequences that persist throughout life (Coe et al., 1988; Hofer, 1996; Sanchez et al., 2001). However, other preclinical studies indicate that increasing mother/infant interactions, such as buffered stress responses, can have positive effects on outcome (Meaney et al., 1996). Over the past two decades, specific interventions directed at premature infant care in Neonatal Intensive Care Units (NICU), such as Newborn Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP) (Als et al., 1994, 2012), skin-to-skin care (Conde-Agudelo et al., 2011, Feldman et al., 2002) and massage therapy (Field et al., 2006, 2010, Vickers et al., 2004), have shown significant effects on outcomes. However, these interventions are not universally accepted and further studies are necessary to document efficacy in preterm infants (Conde-Agudelo et al., 2011, Ohlsson and Jacobs, 2013). This research study tests a new approach for improving development of preterm infants, Family Nurture Intervention (FNI). The distinguishing feature of FNI is that the intervention staff facilitates communication of affect and physiologic co-regulation between mother and infant. The intervention starts early and continues over the full course of hospitalization, thereby minimizing the effects of mother–infant separation in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU). The intervention includes mother and infant scent cloth exchange, sustained touch and vocal soothing in the isolette with eye contact, wrapped or skin-to-skin holding and family-based support sessions (Welch et al., 2012) (see Methods below). FNI was based both on a method used to treat emotional, behavioral and developmental disorders in older children (Welch et al., 2006) and on basic research in animal models that revealed sensorimotor, olfactory and nurture-based activities within normal mother–infant relationships (Hofer, 1975, 1994, 1996; Myers et al., 1989a,b). In recent laboratory studies, central neural mechanisms have been found for these effects, involving epigenetic regulation of gene expression patterns in specific brain regions, from infancy to adulthood, as a result of variations in early mother-infant interactions (Meaney et al., 1996; Meaney, 2001; Jensen and Champagne, 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Caldji et al., 2011). FNI can be safely and feasibly implemented in a level IV NICU without any negative impact on length of stay (Welch et al., 2013). Quantitative characterization of features of the EEG unique to early development provides state-dependent indices for tracking changes in specific neurophysiologic mechanisms critical for normal development of cortical function (Grieve et al., 2005, 2007, 2008; Isler et al., 2005; Myers et al., 1997, 1993, 2012; Sahni et al., 1995, 2005). The impact of prior NICU interventions on brain development has been assessed using electroencephalogram (EEG) measures of complexity and coherence of brain activity during sleep (Als et al., 2012; Kaffashi et al., 2012; Ludington-Hoe et al., 2006). Prior studies have found that preterm infants suffer from functional abnormalities mediated by the frontal cortex, including attention, emotion regulation, language and cognition (Baron et al., 2012; Johnson and Marlow, 2011; Nosarti et al., 2012; Peterson et al., 2000; Pinto-Martin et al., 2011). Other studies have found that activity (power) at higher frequencies measured during sleep in preterm and term infants is predictive of Bayley neurobehavioral scores later in development (Scher et al., 1996). This current study obtained high-density, 124-lead, EEG recordings as a novel means to assess changes in brain activity from a NICU intervention. The current findings show near term age that the FNI group shows highly significant increases in EEG power in high frequency bands in the frontal cortex.
2. Methods 2.1. FNI trial design Data for this study were obtained in a single-center, parallel group, randomized controlled trial (RCT) in the level IV NICU at Morgan Stanley Children’s Hospital of New York at Columbia University Medical Center. ClinicalTrials.gov has registered this trial (#NCT01439269). Columbia University Medical Center Institutional Review Board approved all recruitment, consent, and study procedures. The target enrollment for this study was 150 infants. The original protocol restricted the gestational ages of the infants to 26 to 32 weeks 6 days. After eight months of enrollment, we obtained approval to increase the age to 34 weeks 6 days in order to increase the rate of enrollment, but the target enrollment of 150 infants was not changed. Excluded were mothers that did not understand and speak English, or had a history of drug addiction or severe mental illness. Entry into the study required that families have at least one adult other than the mother in the home. Excluded infants were those with a birth weight below the third percentile for gestational age or significant congenital defects. Shown in the consort chart (Supplementary Figure S1) are the number of eligible infants and consents obtained.
2.2. FNI activities and procedures Following consent, mothers assigned to the FNI group met with Nurture Specialists who worked with the mothers and their families throughout the study to facilitate all aspects of the intervention. Mothers of infants assigned to the SC condition received the care that is standard for infants admitted to the NICU. The timeline of the intervention and the sequence of mother-infant interactions are depicted in Fig. 1. The first FNI activities took place with the infant confined to isolette care. As soon as possible after birth, the mothers started reciprocal scent cloth exchanges. Two small cotton scent cloths were given to the mothers, one to wear in their bra and the other to place under the head of their infant. Mothers were encouraged to repeat this cycle at each visit to the NICU. As infants became more stable, Nurture Specialists facilitated FNI mothers in making affective contact with their infants through the ports of the isolette, using firm and sustained touch and ‘containment’, speaking emotionally to their infants in their native language and when possible, making eye contact. When infants were medically able to leave the incubator, Nurture Specialists helped FNI mothers to engage in skin-to-skin, or non-skin-to-skin holding to continue vocal soothing and eye to eye contact. Mothers were facilitated in engaging regularly in these activities for a minimum of 1 h at a time. When family members were available, the Nurture Specialist
Fig. 1. FNI Timeline. (A) Scent Cloth exchange began immediately after enrollment. (B) Sustained Touch and Vocal Soothing began in the isolette as soon as infant was clinically stable and continued throughout stay. (C) Wrapped and Skin to Skin Holding took place as soon as it was clinically appropriate for the infant to come out of the incubator. (D) Family sessions occurred mainly near the end of the NICU stay.
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
engaged them in sessions that discussed the importance of the FNI activities between mother and infant, and offered support to the families with ongoing problems and with their needs arising from the arrival of their new infant. 2.3. EEG acquisition and sleep state coding EEG recording used 128-electrode data acquisition systems (EGI, Inc., Eugene, Oregon); however, only 124 leads are used in infants. Data were obtained between 11 am and 4 pm within 30 min after a normally scheduled feeding. A study session of 90 min duration was required to acquire 60 min of EEG data. The details of EEG acquisition are described in prior work (Grieve et al., 2008) Following consultation with the manufacturer of the EEG system, we used special methods to maintain electrode/scalp contact, including covering the electrode net with plastic wrap to diminish evaporation of the saline in the electrode sponges and covering the net and plastic wrap with an elastic material (Surgilast). Application of the net and these extra measures usually took about 5 min. These procedures generally allowed impedances to be kept below 50 kilo ohms per manufacturer’s recommendation. The EEG was sampled at 1000 samples/s using a vertex reference. The voltage from each lead was band-pass filtered from 0.1 to 400 Hz and then digitized with 16 bits per sample at the rate of 1000 samples per second. Then, in software, data were re-referenced to the average of the 124 lead EEG montage and processed to obtain measures of power (lV2) at specific frequencies for each electrode. The negative of the average reference approximates the voltage at the vertex lead as referenced to a point at infinity, thus producing the 125th lead. Research assistants performed visual coding of quiet and active sleep, indeterminate, awake and crying states once each minute. This coding was done continuously throughout the EEG study with behavioral criteria previously shown to be appropriate for preterm infants (Stefanski et al., 1984). 2.4. EEG signal artifact rejection
677
power in the highest frequency band (37–48 Hz), then data for all frequency bands for that lead were set to missing. With elimination of outliers and acceptance of a minimum of 3 min of EEG data within each sleep state to increase the stability of measures of EEG power, the median power for all epochs of active sleep and quiet sleep for each lead and frequency band was determined. This allowed the inclusion of 293 early and near term studies (129 SC, 164 FNI) for AS analyses and 290 early and late term studies (127 SC, 163 FNI) in the QS analyses. In both AS and QS, there was a trend for a greater fraction of SC studies to be excluded as compared with the fraction of FNI studies that were excluded. However, in neither state were these group differences in exclusions significantly different (AS: X2 = 2.52, p = 0.11; QS: X2 = 3.04, p = 0.08). EEG studies were divided into 2 age groups; preterm age (33.8– 36.9 weeks PMA) and a near term age (37.2–44.4 weeks PMA). 80% of the preterm age studies and 25% of the near term age studies were conducted in the NICU. The remaining studies were done in the follow-up clinic. These percentages did not differ between the study groups. For overall descriptive purposes, the first phase of the analyses performed t-tests to characterize differences between FNI and SC for each lead, for each frequency band, for each sleep state, and for each of the two study ages. To determine whether group differences were statistically significant, ANOVAs of log EEG power for each study age group were conducted for each of the five regions (frontal polar, frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital) (Fig. 2) for each of the 10 frequency bands and for each sleep state. These analyses had one between-subjects factor (FNI vs. SC) and one within-subjects factor (left hemisphere, right hemisphere). Gestational age at birth and PMA at the time of the EEG studies were included as covariates. When a given subject had more than one study within an age range, results were averaged over the multiple studies within that range. Approximately one third of the infants had multiple studies within the early range and another third within the near to term age range. The distribution of multiple versus single studies was not significantly different between the two groups.
Multiple steps were taken to diminish inclusion of EEG data contaminated by movement-related or other sources of non-cortical electrical activity. The standard deviation of voltage was computed for each 30-s epoch of each lead. Individual lead data were excluded if the standard deviation of the lead exceeded 50 lV. Epochs were excluded if more than 20 of the 125 leads exceeded the standard deviation criterion and entire studies were excluded if more than 80% of their epochs were excluded. 2.5. EEG power analysis, outlier rejection and statistical analyses After dropping all defined artifacts, data were available from at least one EEG study for each of 134 infants (63 SC, 71 FNI). From these data, EEG power was computed using 1 s FFTs for each of the 125 leads. Average power for each of 10 frequency bands (1– 3 , 4–6, 7–9, 10–12, 13–15, 16–18, 19–21, 22–24, 25–36 and 37– 48 Hz) was computed for 30-s epochs throughout the session. To define outliers in the power data, regression analyses of log power versus PMA at the time of the recording were conducted for each lead, frequency band and sleep state (2500 total analyses; 125 leads 10 bands 2 states). Outliers from the regression line were defined when residuals with Studentized value exceeded 1.98 (i.e. p 0.05 with df 300). These values were set to missing. Finally, muscle activity can contribute to outlier values of power in a lead at nearly all frequencies. As a percentage of EEG power in a band, these outliers are larger in high frequency bands (Shackman et al., 2009). Accordingly, to limit the contribution of muscle activity, if data from a lead were set to missing because of excessive
Fig. 2. Schematic of high density EEG net. The 124 electrodes in the EEG net are distributed within ten standard brain regions for the regional analyses of the study. Note 10 midline leads (open circles) were excluded from statistical analyses by region.
678
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
Within each age group 100 tests were conducted (10 frequency bands 5 regions 2 sleep states), producing p-values that reflected the main effects of treatment. To control for multiple testing, we employed the False Discovery Rate procedure (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) with a threshold set at 10%. We performed additional analyses on subsets of infants with EEG studies who had EEG studies at both ages (33 SC, 43 FNI). These were group by repeated measures (age) ANOVAs and were limited to left frontal polar power in the 19–21 Hz band.
the early and near term studies. Results obtained at the near to term age range indicate there were highly significant effects of the intervention (red to dark brown electrodes, lower sets of electrodes in Figs 3 and 4). In both sleep states, the intervention led to regional increases in power, particularly in the most frontal electrodes at frequencies above 10 Hz. At the early study age range, there were no robust differences in power between the two groups.
3. Results
To test whether the individual electrode differences in Figs 3 and 4 were regionally significant, we conducted a series of ANOVAs in which the main effect of FNI vs. SC was determined across ten regions (Fig. 2), ten frequency bands, two sleep states, and both age ranges. Data from left and right hemispheres were included as a within-subjects factor in these analyses and gestational age at birth and PMA at the time of EEG study were covariates. ANOVAS found no significant intervention group by hemisphere interactions (Supplementary Tables S5–S8). Thus, results only focus on the main effect of intervention group. Identification of the subset of p-values that passed the 10% False Discovery Rate threshold revealed that near to term age, power above 10 Hz in the leads over the frontal polar cortex was significantly affected by the intervention. As shown in Table 1, there was increased power in this region in FNI infants that ranged from 19% to 36% greater than that in SC infants. The effects of FNI appeared to be more pronounced in QS, although robust changes were found in both sleep states. At frequencies above 10 Hz, seven of the QS and six of the AS ANOVA tests passed the 10% FDR threshold.
3.1. Demographics and clinical characteristics There were no significant differences between SC and FNI groups with regard to parent demographics, maternal or infant birth or clinical conditions (Supplementary Tables S1–S3). 3.2. EEG study characteristics One hundred thirty-four infants (63 SC, 71 FNI) from 105 (51 SC, 54 FNI) pregnancies including singletons and twins provided EEG data for these analyses (Supplementary Table S4). The recordings were 1 h in duration in both study groups and in both age groups. In the early EEG studies, FNI infants had fewer epochs identified as outliers in both active sleep (AS) and quiet sleep (QS) states than did SC infants. 3.3. Electrode by electrode differences in EEG power at the two age ranges To evaluate the effect of FNI on EEG power, we conducted ttests for each electrode, within each sleep state and PMA range and for each of 10 frequency bands. Electrode maps of the p-values from these t-tests are shown in Fig. 3 for quiet sleep and in Fig. 4 for active sleep. Included in the maps are data for each of 10 frequency bands across the spectral range from 1 to 48 Hz for both
3.4. Regional effects of FNI on EEG power
3.5. FNI increased EEG power in both the first and second half of study period To test whether the effects of FNI were consistent over the course of the trial, we divided the data into the first half and second half of the study period for enrollment in the RCT. We focused on the data obtained at near to term age in QS. Although multiple
Fig. 3. EEG power during quiet sleep. In quiet sleep FNI had effects on EEG power at the older age range (after 6 weeks of FNI) but not the early range. These effects were most apparent in frontal regions. Shown are electrode maps of the p-values for the differences in power between FNI and SC infants in t-tests for each electrode, across the spectral frequency range from 1 to 48 Hz and PMA range for each of 10 frequency bands across all 30 s epochs that were behaviorally coded as quiet sleep.
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
679
Fig. 4. EEG power during active sleep. In active sleep FNI had effects on EEG power at the older age range (after 6 weeks of FNI) but not the early range. These effects were most apparent in frontal regions. Shown are electrode maps of the p-value for the differences in power between FNI and SC infants in t-tests for each electrode, across the spectral frequency range from 1 to 48 Hz and PMA range for each of 10 frequency bands across all 30 s epochs that were behaviorally coded as active sleep.
Table 1 Results from analyses of Frontal Polar EEG power from the near to term studies (PMA 37–44.4 weeks). For each sleep state (active and quiet) analyses were repeated measures ANOVAs of log EEG power for each of 10 frequency bands. These analyses had one between subjects factor (Group: FNI vs. SC) and one within subjects factor (hemisphere). The p-values shown are for the main effect by Group. Note the number of subjects across frequency bands and states was not constant because outliers were set to missing for individual cells in these analyses. There were no significant interactions between treatment group and hemisphere. In both quiet and active sleep in all frequency bands, the power was greater in FNI than in SC infants. Presented in bold are the p-values which passed a 10% False Discovery Rate threshold based on p values from 200 tests (5 regions 10 frequency bands 2 ages 2 states). Active sleep
3.6. FNI increased EEG power in both males and females A gender by group comparison determined whether the effects of FNI treatment on EEG power were similar in males and females. The ANOVA showed a robust main effect of group with FNI greater than SC (F(1, 92) = 20.17, p < 0.00003), but no significant interaction between group and sex (p = 0.60). The mean power for males and females in each group is shown (Fig. 5A). Post-hoc t-tests showed a significant effect of FNI in both males and females (p < 0.001(SC/FNI, n = 20/28) and p < 0.007 (n = 25/25), respectively).
Frequency band (Hz)
Quiet sleep FNI vs. SC (%)
p (Group Ns)
FNI vs. SC (%)
p (Group Ns)
3.7. FNI increased EEG power in both singletons and twins
1–3 4–6 7–9 10–12 13–15 16–18 19–21 22–24 25–36 37–48
+8 +16 +19 +21 +21 +30 +32 +32 +36 +31
0.26438 (44/51) 0.07060 (44/53) 0.03022 (44/52) 0.00364 (44/53) 0.00501 (44/50) 0.00018 (44/52) 0.00003 (44/52) 0.00006 (44/52) 0.00007 (44/53) 0.00121 (44/53)
+11 +5 +12 +20 +20 +20 +19 +20 +23 +20
0.19612 (44/50) 0.38996 (44/50) 0.10840 (44/50) 0.00360 (44/51) 0.00459 (44/49) 0.00351 (44/50) 0.00375 (44/49) 0.00525 (44/49) 0.00307 (44/51) 0.01474 (44/51)
We also conducted an ANOVA to determine if the effects of FNI were similar in singletons and twins (Fig. 5B). There was a highly significant effect of group with FNI greater than SC (F(1, 92) = 21.19, p < 0.00002) but there was also a significant interaction between twin/singleton status and group (F(1, 92) = 6.18, p < 0.02). Post-hoc tests showed that the effects of FNI were significant in both singletons and twins (p < 0.03 (SC/FNI, n = 27/29) and p < 0.01 (n = 18/24), respectively). 3.8. FNI increased EEG power independent of age or weight at birth
frequency bands showed highly significant differences, we focused on the band that had the most robust results (19–21 Hz). Inspection of the electrode by electrode results in Figs. 3 and 4 suggested somewhat more robust results on the left side. Thus, we conducted these post hoc tests using data from the left frontal polar region. An ANOVA for effects of group (FNI vs. SC) and study period half showed there was a highly significant difference between groups with FNI greater than SC (F(1, 92 = 20.06, p < 0.00003), but no significant group by study half interaction (p = 0.99). Post-hoc t-tests showed a significant effect of group in both halves of the intervention (p < 0.007 (SC/FNI, n = 24/26) and p < 0.001 (n = 21/27), respectively).
Next, we divided the infants into two groups based on gestational age at birth. There were 28 infants born 630 weeks (12 SC, 16 FNI) and 70 infants born >30 weeks (33 SC, 37 FNI). There was a significant main effect of age group with FNI greater than SC (F(1, 94) = 14.43, p < 0.0003) but no significant interaction between intervention group and birth age group (p = 0.48). The mean power for each birth age group is shown in Fig. 5C. Post-hoc t-tests showed there was significantly greater power in the FNI group in both age groups (630 weeks PMA, p < 0.03;>30 weeks PMA, p < 0.001). The effect of the intervention in the left frontal polar region at 19–21 Hz was also significant for infants born <1500 g (p < 0.005(SC/FNI, n = 18/32)) as well as infants born P1500 g (p < 0.001(n = 27/21)) with greater power in the FNI group.
680
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
Fig. 5. Effects of FNI on EEG power are found in multiple subsets of infants. EEG power (mean ± SE) in electrodes located over the left frontal cortex in the frequency band from 19 to 21 Hz for subgroups of SC and FNI infants. Results are presented for comparisons of subjects grouped by (A) gender, (B) birth status, twin vs. singleton, (C) gestational age (GA) at birth, (D) Pre- or post-discharge EEG obtained near to term age.
3.9. FNI increased EEG power independent of testing before or after discharge The majority of near to term EEG studies were conducted postdischarge in our follow-up clinic (34 SC, 34 FNI) while a few were obtained in the NICU prior to discharge (10 SC, 12 FNI). Analyses showed a main effect of group with FNI greater than SC (F(1, 86) = 14.54, p < 0.0003) but no significant interaction between group and discharge status (p = 0.56) (Fig. 5D). Moreover, there was a significant effect of FNI in both of these subsets (tested in NICU p < 0.02; tested in follow-up clinic p < 0.001). 3.10. FNI alters developmental trajectories of EEG power Next, we examined developmental trajectories of EEG power by analyzing changes in power with age (controlling for gestational age at birth and PMA at testing). Repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted using data from subjects with measurements both early at 33.8–36.9 PMA and near-term at 37.2–44.4 PMA (43 FNI, 33 SC). The developmental trajectories for EEG power in the 19–21 Hz band during QS for the left and right sides of five brain regions are shown in Fig. 6. Results showed that left frontal polar power increased in FNI infants with PMA, whereas SC infants showed no significant change with PMA (Fig. 6A). This pattern of change with age resulted in a significant age by group interaction (p < 0.0001). A significant age by group interaction was also found for right frontal polar power (p < 0.001). Even though the differences in frontal power at term age were not significant, the change from the early PMA studies to the near to term age studies was greater in FNI infants in both hemispheres (interaction term left frontal p < 0.0005, right frontal p < 0.008; Fig. 6B). Significant age by group interactions were also found for the left temporal (p < 0.03; Fig. 6C), as well as for the left and right parietal regions (p < 0.008, p < 0.005, respectively; Fig. 6D), and for the right occipital region (p < 0.01; Fig. 6E). Post-hoc tests showed that power increased significantly (p < 0.02 to p < 0.00001) in FNI in all but the left temporal region.
In contrast, power increased in SC infants only in the left occipital region (p < 0.05). Moreover, in SC, EEG power in the left frontal region decreased with age (p < 0.02). 4. Discussion Preterm infants are a growing segment of the population. These infants are taken from their uterine environment before fetal development is complete and are exposed to an artificial environment. Advances in technology have made it possible to reduce mortality at early gestational ages. However, adverse outcomes in preterm infants remain high. There are several reports of positive impact of non-medical interventions on outcomes following neonatal intensive care (Als et al., 1994, 2012, Conde-Agudelo et al., 2011, Feldman et al., 2002, 2006, 2010, Vickers et al., 2004), although comprehensive assessments of these interventions suggest that they have limited usefulness in resolving problems of long-term outcome (Ohlsson and Jacobs, 2013, Symington and Pinelli, 2006). The Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP) focuses on providing care of NICU infants tailored to their observed neurobehavioral characteristics (Als et al., 1994). NIDCAP results in improved medical and neurobehavioral outcomes (Als et al., 2004; Buehler et al., 1995; McAnulty et al., 2009). These effects are correlated with MRI- and EEG-based evidence of increased connectivity between frontal and occipital brain regions following NIDCAP intervention (Als et al., 2004). Encouraging mothers to engage in skin-to-skin care of their preterm infants has positive effects upon growth and clinical course in the NICU (Conde-Agudelo et al., 2011), as well as longer term neurobehavioral outcomes (Feldman et al., 2002). Skin-to-skin care has also been reported to increase EEG-based measures of complexity, interpreted as enhanced maturation of brain function (Kaffashi et al., 2012; Scher et al., 2009). Massage intervention in preterm infants has been shown to increase growth rates and reduce length of stay in the NICU (Field et al., 2010; Field et al., 2006, Vickers et al., 2004). Two reports
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
681
Fig. 6. Developmental trajectories of EEG power are altered by FNI. EEG power (loge 19–21 Hz (lV2/Hz); mean ± SE) for left and right sides of the (A) frontal polar, (B) frontal, (C) temporal, (D) parietal and (E) occipital brain regions recorded during quiet sleep for SC (circles/dashed line) and FNI (Square/ solid line) infants measured between 34 and <37 weeks PMA vs. P37 to 44 weeks PMA. The p-values are from ANOVAs and reflect the significance level of the interaction between age and intervention group controlling for gestational age at birth and PMA at testing.
from a small trial (N = 10/group) found that massage therapy accelerated developmental decreases in inter-burst intervals in EEG activity, shortened latencies in visual evoked responses, and caused a transient enhancement of visual acuity at three months of age (Guzzetta et al., 2009, 2011). These studies also showed that massage therapy led to higher levels of EEG power during active sleep. Although FNI incorporates some aspects of the above interventions, it differs in central focus. FNI specifically facilitates the pairing of mother’s communication of emotion with exchange of scent, comforting touch, vocal soothing, holding and the experience of mutual calming. This pairing of affect communication and sensory interaction may underlie another mechanism that could explain the effects of FNI on brain function;
namely, learning. The neural substrates for learning exist very early in development and have been demonstrated in early motor learning paradigms in rat fetuses (Robinson et al., 2008). Other studies have demonstrated that the human fetus learns to recognize the mother’s voice (DeCasper and Fifer, 1980; Fifer and Moon, 1994; Moon and Fifer, 2000) and scent (Marlier et al., 1997; Schaal et al., 2000). New learning from olfactory stimuli (Sullivan et al., 1986a,b) guides behavior during the early postnatal period, with temperature and touch being key determinants of affiliative learning (Kojima and Alberts, 2011). Learning in infants can occur during a wide range of circumstances, such as feeding (Delaunay-El Allam et al., 2006) and even during sleep (Fifer et al., 2010). Based on this body of early learning studies and on the design of our intervention, it seems
682
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
very likely that some form of learning may underlie the effects on brain development of infants following FNI. Indeed, our results show that near to term age infants who receive facilitated FNI exhibit robust differences in brain activity (EEG power), when compared to infants receiving standard care. The increases in EEG power in FNI infants ranged from 19% to 36% in frequencies from 10 to 48 Hz. The greatest percent change (+36%) occurred during quiet sleep in the low gamma frequency range (25–36 Hz). These findings are particularly convincing because they were obtained in a large randomized controlled trial that employed fine grained high density EEG records with repeated measurements over early development, and held true regardless of gender, singleton or twin status, gestational age at birth, or birth weight. Further, they held for infants enrolled in the first or second half of the study, as well as for infants studied near to term age while still in the NICU or after discharge. FNI exerts its most highly significant effects in the frontal polar regions at beta and low gamma frequencies. Results from other studies have shown that increased high frequency power predicts better developmental outcomes at later ages. Results from our prior study in term infants with cardiac anomalies showed a positive correlation between left frontal beta power, during sleep, and Bayley scores at 18 months (Williams et al., 2012). This is consistent with prior work in both term and preterm infants (Scher et al., 1996; Tarullo et al., 2012). In older awake infants, frontal gamma power, thought to be related to level of arousal and attention, was correlated with cognitive abilities (Benasich et al., 2008). Resting frontal EEG gamma power measured in awake infants at 16 months of age predicted language capabilities at 3 years of age (Gou et al., 2011). These studies support the hypothesis that the increases in high frequency power we demonstrated in preterm infants following FNI presage improved neurobehavioral outcomes at later stages of development. Based on studies in older infants, one might have expected to find effects of FNI on the early development of EEG power left/right asymmetry in our subjects in the 7–9 Hz frequency range. These studies showed asymmetry in EEG power in frontal areas, particularly at frequencies between 6 and 9 Hz in association with maternal depression (Dawson et al., 1999a,b), responses to fearful stimuli (Diaz and Bell, 2012), orphanage rearing (McLaughlin et al., 2011), and quality of maternal care (Hane and Fox, 2006; Hane et al., 2010). In contrast to all of the prior published asymmetry work, our data was obtained during sleep. In our study, we did not find significant left/right group differences in power, or any group by hemisphere interactions in the 7–9 Hz band, in postmenstrual age group, sleep state, or brain region. It is possible that left/right differences will emerge at later ages, but we did not record EEG beyond the near to term age range or in the awake state. Although we found that main developmental effects of FNI were localized to frontal polar power, we also found significant groupby-age interactions in multiple brain regions. These results suggest that there are widespread changes in developmental trajectories of brain activity that could be mediated by FNI. Since this study was limited to a specific age range during sleep, it is also conceivable that increases in EEG power might be seen in the FNI group in other brain regions and/or at earlier ages under other test conditions, such as during the awake state or following stimuli that evoke EEG responses. It is important to note that the effects of FNI on infant brain activation occurred with an apparently small ‘‘dose’’ of the intervention. Scent cloth exchange was made approximately 2–3 times per week, and Vocal Soothing and Sustained Touch with Eye Contact sessions took place approximately three times per week. Skin to Skin Holding sessions took place an average of two times per week for at least one hour each time. In total, Nurture Specialistfacilitated intervention sessions occurred on average approxi-
mately 4 h per week. This relatively small amount of time spent in nurturing activities, however, may not be an accurate indicator of the intervention effect. In other words, dose and effect may not be directly correlated. Behavioral and emotional effects might have been sustained in both mother and infant beyond each session. Sensory stimulation and associated brain activation during early development can elicit cascades of hormone and neurochemical events that can shape infant brain development (Johnston et al., 2001; Kuhn and Schanberg, 1998; Park and Poo, 2013). Thus, in addition to learning mechanisms, we speculate that the increased brain activity we see following FNI may serve to shape activitydependent aspects of brain development and plasticity. In replication studies we plan to adapt EEG-based measures (Clapp et al., 2012) to track changes in brain plasticity as both markers and mechanisms for the effects of FNI. Our ongoing clinical research will assess the proximal physiological and behavioral effects of paired nurturing activities, and explore underlying developmental mechanisms. In follow-up of this study cohort extending to two years of age and longer, we will determine the effects of FNI on cognition, language, attention, and emotion regulation. 5. Conclusions Family Nurture Intervention is a novel intervention strategy for the neonatal intensive care unit that pairs communication of affect and interactions designed to calm both mother and infant. Compared to the SC group, the FNI group showed robust increases in high frequency EEG brain power in the frontal polar region at near to term age. These effects were independent of gestational age or weight at birth, gender, twin status, or discharge status when assessed. Such region and frequency-specific increases have been shown by others to correlate with better neurodevelopment at later ages. Results from this study support the importance of early mother–infant nurture and of facilitating family nurture in the NICU and suggest that FNI may provide an effective and practicable approach to improving outcomes in preterm infants. 6. Financial disclosures Funding for this project was provided by the Einhorn Family Charitable Trust and the Fleur Fairman Family to the BrainGut Initiative at Columbia University Medical Center. Funding in part was also provided by Columbia University’s CTSA from NCRR/NIH (UL1RR024156). Author MMM received salary support from the New York State Office of Mental Health Research Scientist position. The authors have no competing financial interests. Acknowledgments We would like to thank the NICU staff at the Morgan Stanley Children’s Hospital of New York as well as the participating families for their invaluable assistance to our program of research. We are grateful for the support of the Columbia University Irving Institute for Clinical and Translational Research, where the bulk of EEG recordings were performed. We also want to acknowledge and thank our Performance and Safety Monitoring Board for their thoughtful contributions to the conduct of this study (Bruce Levin, Ph.D., Michael Weitzman, M.D. and Deborah E. Campbell, M.D.). Appendix A. Supplementary data Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013. 08.021.
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
References Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty G, Butler SC, Lightbody L, Kosta S, et al. NIDCAP improves brain function and structure in preterm infants with severe intrauterine growth restriction. J Perinatol 2012;32:797–803. Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Rivkin MJ, Vajapeyam S, Mulkern RV, et al. Early experience alters brain function and structure. Pediatrics 2004;113:846–57. Als H, Lawhon G, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Gibes-Grossman R, Blickman JG. Individualized developmental care for the very low-birth-weight preterm infant. Medical and neurofunctional effects. JAMA 1994;272:853–8. Baron IS, Kerns KA, Muller U, Ahronovich MD, Litman FR. Executive functions in extremely low birth weight and late-preterm preschoolers: effects on working memory and response inhibition. Child Neuropsychol 2012;18:586–99. Benasich AA, Gou Z, Choudhury N, Harris KD. Early cognitive and language skills are linked to resting frontal gamma power across the first 3 years. Behav Brain Res 2008;195:215–22. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B 1995;57:289–300. Buehler DM, Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Liederman J. Effectiveness of individualized developmental care for low-risk preterm infants: behavioral and electrophysiologic evidence. Pediatrics 1995;96:923–32. Caldji C, Hellstrom IC, Zhang TY, Diorio J, Meaney MJ. Environmental regulation of the neural epigenome. FEBS Lett 2011;585(13):2049–58. Clapp WC, Hamm JP, Kirk IJ, Teyler TJ. Translating long-term potentiation from animals to humans: a novel method for noninvasive assessment of cortical plasticity. Biol Psychiatry 2012;71:496–502. Coe CL, Rosenberg LT, Levine S. Effect of maternal separation on the complement system and antibody responses in infant primates. Int J Neurosci 1988;40:289–302. Conde-Agudelo A, Belizan JM, Diaz-Rossello J. Kangaroo mother care to reduce morbidity and mortality in low birthweight infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011. CD002771. Dawson G, Frey K, Panagiotides H, Yamada E, Hessl D, Osterling J. Infants of depressed mothers exhibit atypical frontal electrical brain activity during interactions with mother and with a familiar, nondepressed adult. Child Dev 1999a;70:1058–66. Dawson G, Frey K, Self J, Panagiotides H, Hessl D, Yamada E, et al. Frontal brain electrical activity in infants of depressed and nondepressed mothers: relation to variations in infant behavior. Dev Psychopathol 1999b;11:589–605. DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP. Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science 1980;208:1174–6. Delaunay-El Allam M, Marlier L, Schaal B. Learning at the breast: preference formation for an artificial scent and its attraction against the odor of maternal milk. Infant Behav Dev 2006;29:308–21. Diaz A, Bell MA. Frontal EEG asymmetry and fear reactivity in different contexts at 10 months. Dev Psychobiol 2012;54:536–45. Feldman R, Weller A, Sirota L, Eidelman AI. Skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo care) promotes self-regulation in premature infants: sleep-wake cyclicity, arousal modulation, and sustained exploration. Dev Psychol 2002;38:194–207. Field T, Diego M, Hernandez-Reif M. Preterm infant massage therapy research: a review. Infant Behav Dev 2010;33:115–24. Field T, Diego MA, Hernandez-Reif M, Deeds O, Figuereido B. Moderate versus light pressure massage therapy leads to greater weight gain in preterm infants. Infant Behav Dev 2006;29:574–8. Fifer WP, Byrd DL, Kaku M, Eigsti IM, Isler JR, Grose-Fifer J, et al. Newborn infants learn during sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010;107:10320–3. Fifer WP, Moon CM. The role of mother’s voice in the organization of brain function in the newborn. Acta Paediatr 1994;397(Suppl):86–93. Gou Z, Choudhury N, Benasich AA. Resting frontal gamma power at 16, 24 and 36 months predicts individual differences in language and cognition at 4 and 5 years. Behav Brain Res 2011;220:263–70. Grieve PG, Isler JR, Izraelit A, Peterson BS, Fifer WP, Myers MM, et al. EEG functional connectivity in term age extremely low birth weight infants. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:2712–20. Grieve PG, Myers MM, Stark RI, Housman S, Fifer WP. Topographic localization of electrocortical activation in newborn and two- to four-month-old infants in response to head-up tilting. Acta Paediatr 2005;94:1756–63. Grieve PG, Stark RI, Isler JR, Housman SL, Fifer WP, Myers MM. Electrocortical functional connectivity in infancy: response to body tilt. Pediatr Neurol 2007;37:91–8. Guzzetta A, Baldini S, Bancale A, Baroncelli L, Ciucci F, Ghirri P, et al. Massage accelerates brain development and the maturation of visual function. J Neurosci 2009;29:6042–51. Guzzetta A, D’Acunto MG, Carotenuto M, Berardi N, Bancale A, Biagioni E, et al. The effects of preterm infant massage on brain electrical activity. Dev Med Child Neurol 2011;53(Suppl. 4):46–51. Hamilton BE, Hoyert DL, Martin JA, Strobino DM, Guyer B. Annual Summary of Vital Statistics: 2010–2011. Pediatrics 2013. Hane AA, Fox NA. Ordinary variations in maternal caregiving influence human infants’ stress reactivity. Psychol Sci 2006;17:550–6. Hane AA, Henderson HA, Reeb-Sutherland BC, Fox NA. Ordinary variations in human maternal caregiving in infancy and biobehavioral development in early childhood: a follow-up study. Dev Psychobiol 2010;52:558–67. Hofer MA. Studies on how early maternal separation produces behavioral change in young rats. Psychosom Med 1975;37:245–64.
683
Hofer MA. Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1994;59:192–207. Hofer MA. On the nature and consequences of early loss. Psychosom Med 1996;58:570–81. Isler JR, Garland M, Stark RI, Grieve PG. Local coherence oscillations in the EEG during development in the fetal baboon. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:2121–8. Jensen CL, Champagne FA. Epigenetic and neurodevelopmental perspectives on variation in parenting behavior. Parent Sci Pract 2012;12:202–11. Johnson S, Marlow N. Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric disorders. Pediatr Res 2011;69:8R–11R. Johnston MV, Nishimura A, Harum K, Pekar J, Blue ME. Sculpting the developing brain. Adv Pediatr 2001;48:1–38. Kaffashi F, Scher MS, Ludington-Hoe SM. Loparo KA. An analysis of the kangaroo care intervention using neonatal EEG complexity: A preliminary study. Clin Neurophysiol; 2012. Kojima S, Alberts JR. Warmth from skin-to-skin contact with mother is essential for the acquisition of filial huddling preference in preweanling rats. Dev Psychobiol 2011;53:813–27. Kuhn CM, Schanberg SM. Responses to maternal separation: mechanisms and mediators. Int J Dev Neurosci 1998;16:261–70. Ludington-Hoe SM, Johnson MW, Morgan K, Lewis T, Gutman J, Wilson PD, et al. Neurophysiologic assessment of neonatal sleep organization: preliminary results of a randomized, controlled trial of skin contact with preterm infants. Pediatrics 2006;117:e909–23. Marlier L, Schaal B, Soussignan R. Orientation responses to biological odours in the human newborn. Initial pattern and postnatal plasticity. C R Acad Sci III 1997;320:999–1005. McAnulty G, Duffy FH, Butler S, Parad R, Ringer S, Zurakowski D, et al. Individualized developmental care for a large sample of very preterm infants: health, neurobehaviour and neurophysiology. Acta Paediatr 2009;98:1920–6. McLaughlin KA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Adverse rearing environments and neural development in children: the development of frontal electroencephalogram asymmetry. Biol Psychiatry 2011;70:1008–15. Meaney MJ, Diorio J, Francis D, Widdowson J, LaPlante P, Caldji C, et al. Early environmental regulation of forebrain glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: implications for adrenocortical responses to stress. Dev Neurosci 1996;18:49–72. Meaney MJ. Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;24:1161–92. Moon CM, Fifer WP. Evidence of transnatal auditory learning. J Perinatol 2000;20:S37–44. Myers MM, Brunelli SA, Shair HN, Squire JM, Hofer MA. Relationships between maternal behavior of SHR and WKY dams and adult blood pressures of crossfostered F1 pups. Dev Psychobiol 1989a;22:55–67. Myers MM, Brunelli SA, Squire JM, Shindeldecker RD, Hofer MA. Maternal behavior of SHR rats and its relationship to offspring blood pressures. Dev Psychobiol 1989b;22:29–53. Myers MM, Fifer WP, Grose-Fifer J, Sahni R, Stark RI, Schulze KF. A novel quantitative measure of Trace-alternant EEG activity and its association with sleep states of preterm infants. Dev Psychobiol 1997;31:167–74. Myers MM, Grieve PG, Izraelit A, Fifer WP, Isler JR, Darnall RA, et al. Developmental profiles of infant EEG: overlap with transient cortical circuits. Clin Neurophysiol 2012;123:1502–11. Myers MM, Stark RI, Fifer WP, Grieve PG, Haiken J, Leung K, et al. A quantitative method for classification of EEG in the fetal baboon. Am J Physiol 1993;265:R706–14. Nosarti C, Reichenberg A, Murray RM, Cnattingius S, Lambe MP, Yin L, et al. Preterm birth and psychiatric disorders in young adult life. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012;69:E1–8. Ohlsson A, Jacobs SE. NIDCAP: a systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials. Pediatrics 2013;131:e881–93. Park H, Poo MM. Neurotrophin regulation of neural circuit development and function. Nat Rev Neurosci 2013;14:7–23. Peterson BS, Vohr B, Staib LH, Cannistraci CJ, Dolberg A, Schneider KC, et al. Regional brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. JAMA 2000;284:1939–47. Pinto-Martin JA, Levy SE, Feldman JF, Lorenz JM, Paneth N, Whitaker AH. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in adolescents born weighing <2000 g. Pediatrics 2011;128:883–91. Robinson SR, Kleven GA, Brumley MR. Prenatal development of interlimb motor learning in the rat fetus. Infancy 2008;13:204–28. Sahni R, Schulze KF, Kashyap S, Ohira-Kist K, Fifer WP, Myers MM. Sleeping position and electrocortical activity in low birthweight infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2005;90:F311–5. Sahni R, Schulze KF, Stefanski M, Myers MM, Fifer WP. Methodological issues in coding sleep states in immature infants. Dev Psychobiol 1995;28:85–101. Sanchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM. Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: evidence from rodent and primate models. Dev Psychopathol 2001;13:419–49. Schaal B, Marlier L, Soussignan R. Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother’s diet. Chem Senses 2000;25:729–37. Scher MS, Ludington-Hoe S, Kaffashi F, Johnson MW, Holditch-Davis D, Loparo KA. Neurophysiologic assessment of brain maturation after an 8-week trial of skinto-skin contact on preterm infants. Clin Neurophysiol 2009;120:1812–8.
684
M.G. Welch et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 675–684
Scher MS, Steppe DA, Banks DL. Prediction of lower developmental performances of healthy neonates by neonatal EEG-sleep measures. Pediatr Neurol 1996;14:137–44. Shackman AJ, McMenamin BW, Slagter HA, Maxwell JS, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ. Electromyogenic artifacts and electroencephalographic inferences. Brain Topogr 2009;22:7–12. Stefanski M, Schulze K, Bateman D, Kairam R, Pedley TA, Masterson J, et al. A scoring system for states of sleep and wakefulness in term and preterm infants. Pediatr Res 1984;18:58–62. Sullivan RM, Brake SC, Hofer MA, Williams CL. Huddling and independent feeding of neonatal rats can be facilitated by a conditioned change in behavioral state. Dev Psychobiol 1986a;19:625–35. Sullivan RM, Hofer MA, Brake SC. Olfactory-guided orientation in neonatal rats is enhanced by a conditioned change in behavioral state. Dev Psychobiol. 1986b;19:615–23. Symington A, Pinelli J. Developmental care for promoting development and preventing morbidity in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2006. CD001814. Tarullo AR, Lee J, Gera S, Condon C, Tamura EK, Grieve PG, et al. Language development linked to newborn frontal gamma power. Dev Psychobiol 2012;54:769.
Vickers A, Ohlsson A, Lacy JB, Horsley A. Massage for promoting growth and development of preterm and/or low birth-weight infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2004. CD000390. Welch MG, Hofer MA, Brunelli SA, Stark RI, Andrews HF, Austin J, et al. Family nurture intervention (FNI): methods and treatment protocol of a randomized controlled trial in the NICU. BMC Pediatr 2012;12:14. Welch MG, Hofer MA, Stark RI, Andrews HF, Austin J, Glickstein SB, et al. Randomized controlled trial of Family Nurture Intervention in the NICU: Assessments of length of stay, feasibility and safety. BMC Pediatrics 2013;13:148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2431-13-148. Welch MG, Northrup RS, Welch-Horan TB, Ludwig RJ, Austin CL, Jacobson JS. Outcomes of Prolonged Parent-Child Embrace Therapy among 102 children with behavioral disorders. Complement Ther Clin Pract 2006;12:3–12. Williams IA, Tarullo AR, Grieve PG, Wilpers A, Vignola EF, Myers MM, et al. Fetal cerebrovascular resistance and neonatal EEG predict 18-month neurodevelopmental outcome in infants with congenital heart disease. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 2012;40:304–9. Zhang TY, Labonte B, Wen XL, Turecki G, Meaney MJ. Epigenetic mechanisms for the early environmental regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression in rodents and humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013;38:111–23.