SCHIZOPHRENIA RESEARCH SERIES: NEURODEVELOPMENT AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGY Elementary Phenotypes in the Neurobiological and Genetic Study of Schizophrenia Lawrence E. Adler, Robert Freedman, Randal G. Ross, Ann Olincy, and Merilyne C. Waldo This review describes the strategy of using elementary phenotypes for neurobiological and genetic linkage studies of schizophrenia. The review concentrates on practical aspects of selecting the phenotype and then understanding the confounds in its measurement and interpretation. Examples from the authors’ studies of deficits in P50 inhibition and smooth pursuit eye movement dysfunction are presented. These two phenotypes share considerable similarity in their neurobiology, including a similar response to nicotine. They also appear to co-segregate with the genetic risk for schizophrenia as autosomal co-dominant phenotypes. Although most schizophrenic patients inherit these abnormalities unilinealy, i.e., from one parent, apparent bilineal inheritance produces a more severe illness, observed clinically as childhood-onset schizophrenia. The initial study showing linkage of the P50 deficit to the chromosome 15q14 locus of the ␣7-nicotinic acetylcholine receptor is an example of the potential usefulness of these phenotypes for combined genetic and neurobiological study of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1999;46: 8 –18 © 1999 Society of Biological Psychiatry Key Words: Neuronal inhibition, schizophrenia physiology, eye movements, auditory evoked potentials, genetic segregation, childhood-onset schizophrenia
Introduction
T
he strategic decision to use elementary phenotypes for the genetic analysis of complex diseases such as schizophrenia involves several considerations. First, one must assume that the major genes for the illness are not readily resolved by linkage analysis or by direct sequencing of candidate genes. Second, one must decide that one or more specific biological phenomena are more likely to represent the inheritance of a major gene effect than the presence of the illness itself. For schizophrenia, neither of these issues have straightforward answers. Schizophrenia
From the Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center and Denver VA Medical Center, Denver, CO (all authors). Address reprint requests to Lawrence E. Adler, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Campus Box C268-16, UCHSC, 4200 E. 9th Ave., Denver, CO 80262. Received October 16, 1998; revised March 29, 1999; accepted March 30, 1999.
© 1999 Society of Biological Psychiatry
does not have Mendelian inheritance, so that the clinical illness is unlikely to represent the phenotype of a single major schizophrenia gene (Gottesman and Shields 1972; Tsuang et al 1990), although there is clearly a familial form of the illness (Kendler et al 1985). However, the power to detect smaller gene effects is enhanced by increasing the numbers of families under study and an increasing the precision of the human genome map. Both of these tactics are currently being employed for the study of schizophrenia. Thus, the ability to find major gene effects by linkage is not yet fully known. Candidate gene analysis has been less successful, but it is still a viable technique. Initial efforts to sequence genes thought to be involved in schizophrenia, such as a dopamine receptors, did not reveal any genetic abnormalities (Moldin 1997). However, these efforts were not an exhaustive examination of all the possible candidate genes and were necessarily limited by current hypotheses of the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Indeed, because the biology of schizophrenia, as a brain illness, is likely complex, any gene in the genome is a candidate gene. Thus, as for linkage, the limiting factor is the power of the analysis in terms of the number of genes that are examined and the number of people that are sequenced. The principal strength of linkage analysis is that there is no reliance on biological hypotheses about the illness because all locations in the entire genome are considered equally likely to have a pathogenic role. To retain this advantage would seem desirable, but to discard all information about the biological nature of schizophrenia seems of dubious value (Lander 1988). As an example, it was the differentiation of biological subtypes that enabled breakthroughs in the genetic analysis of coronary artery disease, a disorder which, like schizophrenia is influenced by a number of different genetic and environmental factors (Goldstein et al 1973). Focusing on families with the highest serum glucose levels as a specific phenotype led to discovery of a genetic deficit that results in Type 2 diabetes (Mahtani et al 1996). The strategy of using an alternative, elementary neurobiological phenotype for the analysis of schizophrenia assumes that one or more major pathophysiologic deficits in the illness can be identified, 0006-3223/99/$20.00 PII S0006-3223(99)00085-2
Elementary Phenotypes in Schizophrenia
and that these deficits can be related to a single underlying neuronal deficit. Thus, the use of elementary phenotypes is primarily a biological strategy. The strategy then attempts to take advantage of the power of genetic analysis by using linkage and candidate gene analysis to determine if the biological deficit is, in fact, caused by a deficit in a specific gene (Arolt et al 1996; Blackwood et al 1991; Holzman et al 1988; Kong and Cox 1997; Sham et al 1994; Shihabuddin et al 1996). To date, this approach has not succeeded in uncovering a specific genetic deficit, however, significant linkage results have been obtained using one phenotype, a deficit in the inhibition of the P50 auditory evoked response to repeated stimuli (Freedman et al 1997). The number of phenotypes that can be examined using these methods is certainly not limited to this one abnormality, so that it may be worthwhile for other biological researchers to consider this strategy. In particular, the ability to prove or disprove the heritability of a particular biological abnormality in schizophrenia provides an additional test of the validity of hypotheses about the biology of the schizophrenia, which heretofore has not been possible. Finally, a significant advantage for basing genetic linkage studies on a specific phenotype other than diagnosis, is that greater statistical power can be obtained with than might otherwise be the case (NIMH Genetics Workshop 1997). This aspect has been discussed in a previous paper (Freedman et al 1999).
Attentional Dysfunction in Schizophrenia The multitude of theories about the biology of schizophrenia is a testimony to the fundamental uncertainty about the nature of the illness. Diagnostic criteria combine subjective and objective reports with an assessment of a prolonged clinical course, none of which points to a pathognomonic functional difficulty. Yet, there are profound and readily recognizable differences between schizophrenic patients and any other medical diagnosis (Kendler et al 1985). Several authorities have pointed to attentional dysfunction as an important pathophysiologic entity that could explain much of the clinical presentation of schizophrenia (Venables 1964). Schizophrenic subjects describe a specific difficulty in keeping their attention focused on a particular object and complain about the intrusion of unwanted sensory information (McGhie and Chapman 1961; Park et al 1995). Such difficulties are highly correlated with the psychosocial decline that is critical to the diagnosis and clinical outcome of the illness (Cornblatt and Keilp 1994; De Amicis et al 1986; Green et al 1996, 1997). Despite this literature and the clinical experience, distractibility itself is listed as a diagnostic criterion for both mania and attention deficit disorder, but not for schizophrenia, in DSM-IV (APA 1994). Indeed, bipolar
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
9
patients, when manic, also manifest such distractibility. Thus even at the clinical level, it is difficult to develop a biological representation of schizophrenia that is sufficiently specific to support genetic analysis. A critical decision with the P50 auditory evoked response was to recognize the similarities and differences in attentional disturbance in mania and schizophrenia and to attempt to develop a biological explanation that would help us increase the specificity of the P50 measure for each illness. The attentional dysfunction associated with psychosis is consistent with a loss of inhibitory function, which is the neuronal substrate normally associated with discrimination between stimuli. To characterize inhibition, neurophysiologists frequently use a conditioning-testing paradigm, in which paired stimuli are presented (Eccles 1969). The first stimulus activates excitatory inputs that cause a neuronal response, such as the P50 response, which is a positive wave occurring 50 msec after an auditory stimulus. The first stimulus also activates or conditions inhibitory pathways. These inhibitory pathways are activated too late to affect the amplitude of the response to the conditioning stimulus, but if a second stimulus is presented soon thereafter, then that the second response is suppressed by the inhibitory mechanisms still active from the conditioning response. The length of time for which the inhibition remains active varies with the type of inhibitory neuronal mechanism. For example, monosynaptic GABAA-mediated inhibition rarely lasts longer than 50 msec, whereas GABAB-mediated inhibition can last up to 300 msec (Hershman et al 1995). Because loss of inhibition at the 500 msec inter-stimulus interval maximally distinguished schizophrenic patients from normal control subjects (Adler et al 1982; Nagamoto et al 1989), multisynaptic pathways are likely to be involved. A number of studies have examined the effect of the stage or character of illness on P50 suppression in schizophrenia. Acutely, there appears to be no effect of typical neuroleptic medication on P50 ratio (Adler et al 1990a). Thus, the deficit is unlikely to reflect an abnormality in catecholaminergic neuronal transmission. However, individuals treated with atypical neuroleptics exhibit normalization of their P50 ratio coincident with improvement in their clinical symptoms (Nagamoto et al 1996, 1999). Yee and co-workers (1998) also noted that treatment with atypical neuroleptics improves P50 suppression in recent onset patients. Erwin and colleagues, on the other hand, noted that deficits in P50 suppression in their paradigm was related to a history of exposure to neuroleptic drugs (Erwin et al 1994). Yee and co-workers (1998), Erwin and co-workers (1998), Vinogradov and colleagues (1996), and Cullum and co-workers (1993) found that impaired P50 suppression was most pronounced in subjects with clinical or neurophysiologic evidence for abnormal atten-
10
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
tional and linguistic function. Jin and colleagues (1998), however, found that patients’ self-report of attentional dysfunction was not related to the presence of a deficit in P50 suppression. Finally, P50 gating deficits in families of schizophrenic patients occur in a manner consistent with possible autosomal dominance (Siegel et al 1984; Waldo et al 1991). Deficits are found not only in schizophrenic patients, but also in many of their first-degree relatives, including apparent obligate carriers (Waldo et al 1995; Young et al 1996), and confirmed by other groups (Clementz et al 1998b). Thus, the deficit likely reflects a genetically transmitted abnormality.
The Measurement of P50 Sensory Gating Abnormalities The suppression of the P50 response to repeated auditory stimuli had been observed in normal subjects prior to our finding of an abnormality of this function in schizophrenia (Chapman et al 1981; Davis et al 1996; Erwin and Buchwald 1986; Finkenzeller and Keidel 1975; Fruhstorfer et al 1970; Jerger et al 1992; Papanicolau et al 1985; Roth and Kopell 1969). Diminished suppression of P50 in schizophrenia, compared to normal control subjects, has been observed by Erwin and co-workers (1991), Boutros and co-workers (1991), Braff and Geyer (1990), Judd and co-workers (1992), Clementz and colleagues (1997), Jin and colleagues (1997), Lambert and colleagues (1993), and Yee and co-workers (1998). Kathmann and Engel (1990) found no suppression in both the normal control and the schizophrenic subjects. Despite the general convergence of findings, there are significant differences between laboratories in both the stimulation paradigm and the methods for the analysis of the evoked response. One difference is the use of stimulus trains, as opposed to paired stimuli, which have shown diminished suppression of P50 by schizophrenic subjects in some (Erwin et al 1991) but not all studies (Grillon et al 1991). A second difference is the use of multiple recording sites, instead of a single vertex electrode (Judd et al 1992; Clementz et al 1998a; Cardenas et al 1993). Although the wave is maximally recorded at the vertex, the use of these other sites to compute a single vector improves the reliability of the measure (Cardenas et al 1993). Problems in test-retest reliability have been noted by several investigators (Smith et al 1994). In addition to the use of multiple recording sites, assessment of confounding state factors such as increased catecholaminergic activity due to stress has been helpful in reducing inter-trial variance, as described further in this article. The selection of the ratio of the amplitudes the two responses (amplitude of the test response to amplitude of the conditioning response) as the primary variable has also
L.E. Adler et al
received considerable discussion. Ratios have inherent problems in reproducibility because of their mathematical properties (Cardenas et al 1993; Smith et al 1994). For example, all cerebral evoked potential measurements include both a signal and a noise component. A ratio measurement that includes noise in both the numerator and denominator will approach an assymptote of 1, as the noise increases. Smith and colleagues compared analysis of the individual amplitudes in various combinations to ratios and found that although the ratio has the lowest test-retest reliability in normal subjects, nevertheless the ratio measurement has the greatest power for distinguishing normal subjects from schizophrenic subjects (Smith et al 1994). Many investigators have been concerned about the lower amplitude of the P50 response to the first stimulus in schizophrenic subjects, compared to normal subjects. Straumanis and co-workers noted that the P50 wave increased in amplitude during neuroleptic treatment (Straumanis et al 1982). This finding was replicated in Freedman and co-workers (1983) and Adler and colleagues (1990a) studies, both of which found that the abnormality in the ratio of the two amplitudes is unchanged when schizophrenic patients are treated with typical neuroleptic drugs, despite the increase in the initial P50 response amplitude to normal levels. Because the latency of the wave is also reduced in unmedicated schizophrenic patients, the smaller wave was hypothesized to reflect a hyperexcitable neuronal substrate, which results in less synchronization of the response to stimuli and hence, a smaller evoked potential. A similar hypothesis led several groups to attempt single trial analyses, rather than relying on the averaged evoked response to a set of stimuli, to detect the relatively greater amount of desynchronization. Freedman and co-workers (1996) pointed out the difficulty of separating signal from noise using an automated single trial analysis technique. However, Zouridakis and co-workers (1997) and Jin and colleagues (1997), using other single trial analysis techniques, concluded that the diminished response to the first response, as well as the defect in gating, is heavily influenced by the loss of synchronization in the response to stimuli. Because all evoked potential techniques include significant amounts of noise in their signal, inherent in the indirect nature of recording neuronal activity at the scalp surface, assessment of the noise component is critical before a firm conclusion about the nature of single-trial responses can be made (Arpaia et al 1989).
Clinical Determinants of P50 Suppression The P50 gating deficit is also found in bipolar subjects. Although inhibition is normal in both euthymic and depressed bipolar patients, acutely manic bipolar patients have a failure of inhibition in the manic state that resem-
Elementary Phenotypes in Schizophrenia
bles that seen in schizophrenia (Adler et al 1990a; Franks et al 1983). However, there are important clinical differences. The P50 gating deficit is found in both predominantly positive symptom and predominantly negative symptom schizophrenic patients (Adler et al 1990b). Similar P50 gating deficits are found in acutely ill schizophrenic inpatients (Adler et al 1982, 1990a; Ward et al 1996) and more stable schizophrenic outpatients with less psychosocial impairment (Freedman et al 1983; Ward et al 1996). Manic patients, on the other hand, lose their inhibitory deficit in concert with their successful treatment (Adler et al 1990a). Because manic patients are known to have elevated catecholamine metabolism as part of the pathophysiology of their illness, we measured their plasma levels of the norepinephrine metabolite 3-methoxy,4-hydroxy phenylethylene glycol (MHPG) prior to and after treatment with neuroleptics and lithium carbonate. Prior to treatment, their plasma-free MHPG levels were increased compared to matched normal subjects, which also coincided with impaired P50 inhibition. P50 gating improved as plasma-free MHPG decreased toward normal with medication treatment (Adler et al 1990a). Mania is not the only disorder that can result in a transient P50 auditory gating deficit. Almost half of psychiatric inpatients have diminished P50 auditory gating acutely, but most, except the schizophrenic patients, return to more normal P50 inhibitory gating during the course of hospitalization (Baker et al 1987). Post-traumatic stress disorder can also result in impaired P50 auditory gating as well (Adler et al 1991; Gillette et al 1997). Cocaine abuse also causes a transient P50 auditory gating deficit (Adler et al, unpublished data 1999; Fein et al 1996). Because P50 auditory gating in normal subjects can also change with moderately severe stress or with catecholaminergic manipulation (see below), impaired P50 auditory gating is not diagnostic of schizophrenia. It is useful as a phenotypic marker in family members of schizophrenic patients, only with the assurance that they are otherwise clinically normal. There is one report which suggests that females have higher T/C ratios than males, but the p value (p ⫽ .08) was not significant (Hetrick et al 1996). In contrast, we found that even female patients with premenstrual syndrome have unimpaired P50 auditory gating, although they have rated themselves as being distressed at the time of recording (Waldo et al 1987). P50 auditory gating is normalized in most subjects by late adolescence (Freedman et al 1987; MylesWorsley et al 1996) and remains unimpaired through age 65. After that age, loss of inhibition has been reported in normal aging adults (Papanicolau et al 1984).
Noradrenergic Mechanisms in Sensory Inhibition The studies in mania, cocaine addiction, and normal subjects under stress, suggested that increased noradren-
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
11
ergic neuronal transmission is one of the principal confounds for use of P50 inhibition as a trait marker for studies of schizophrenia. Therefore, we performed a series of animal experiments to determine if increased noradrenergic neuronal transmission disrupts sensory gating. Normal rats inhibit the auditory evoked response to repeated stimuli with parameters highly similar to those that characterize the human inhibition (Adler et al 1986, 1988; Bickford-Wimer et al 1990). The skull surface wave in rats has a major source in the CA3 region of the hippocampus (Bickford-Wimer et al 1990). Human P50 can be recorded in the hippocampus (Freedman et al 1994a), and single neurons recorded in both human and rat hippocampus show a decremented response to repeated sounds (Miller and Freedman 1995; Wilson et al 1984). Recordings at both the skull surface and in the hippocampus in rats were performed while noradrenergic neurotransmission was manipulated pharmacologically. Manipulations that increased noradrenergic neuronal transmission resulted in loss of inhibition of the auditory evoked response (Adler et al 1986, 1988; Bickford-Wimer et al 1990). One such treatment was the administration of phencyclidine, which releases norepinephrine via its effects on sigma opiate receptors and causes loss of auditory evoked potential inhibition. Because phencyclidine has other effects, such as antagonism of NMDA-glutamate receptors, the study was repeated in animals in which the noradrenergic terminals had been previously lesioned with a specific neurotoxin, DSP-4 (Adler et al 1988; Miller et al 1992). In these lesioned animals, phencyclidine had no effect on auditory evoked potential inhibition. Similar results occurred with dextroamphetamine treatment, i.e., dextroamphetamine impaired auditory evoked potential inhibition, and its effect was also blocked by pretreatment with DSP-4 (Adler et al 1986, 1988; Bickford-Wimer et al 1990). Another, more specific, treatment was the administration of yohimbine, which causes the release of norepinephrine by the blockade of the alpha-2 presynaptic noradrenergic receptors that normally inhibit norepinephrine release. Yohimbine administration caused loss of auditory evoked potential gating in rats (Stevens et al 1993). These pharmacologic effects are consistent with other neurophysiologic evidence that suggests that the primary effect of norepinephrine in the hippocampus is loss of inhibition (Madison and Nicoll 1988). This effect interacts with the polysynaptic mechanism responsible for inhibition at the 500 msec interval between stimuli. The yohimbine experiment was repeated in normal human subjects. As was found in the rats, yohimbine decreased the inhibition of the P50 evoked potential, thus validating the results of the animal model and the hypothesis of a causal relationship between loss of inhibition of the P50 potential and increased noradrenergic neurotrans-
12
L.E. Adler et al
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
mission in mania (Adler et al 1994). Elevated noradrenergic neurotransmission can also occur in normal subjects when they are under stress. The stress can be purposely introduced into the experimental paradigm (Johnson and Adler 1993) or it can be an unintended by-product of an unfamiliar experimental situation involving complicated equipment in a hospital environment (Waldo et al 1994). We found that normal subjects frequently have decreased P50 inhibition during initial recordings in these circumstances. This improvement in inhibition occurs as they gain familiarity with the recording environment. Their gain in inhibition also correlates with a decrease in plasma free MHPG levels (Waldo et al 1994). Finally, schizophrenic subjects themselves can have a similar phenomenon. Although MHPG levels are not a major determinant of their abnormality in inhibition, nonetheless there is some correlation with plasma MHPG levels (Kang et al 1997). This mechanism may be one part of the effects of clozapine on the inhibition of the auditory evoked potential. Unlike typical neuroleptics, clozapine normalizes auditory evoked potential inhibition in some schizophrenic patients (Nagamoto et al 1996), in parallel with a positive clinical response and a decrease in MHPG levels (Nagamoto et al 1999). An essay on the effects of norepinephrine on the P50 auditory evoked potential seems only tangentially related to the search for genes related to schizophrenia. In fact, there are few phenotypes that are strictly determined from meiosis, without any effect of moment-to-moment changes in the environment, influences from other genes, or both. The P50 inhibition phenotype is no exception. This paper has described only one of many influences on its measurement. Effects of alcohol (Freedman et al 1986), nicotine (Adler et al 1992, 1993), sleep (Griffith et al 1993, 1995a), stimulus intensity (Griffith et al 1995b), EEG filter settings (Freedman et al 1998), development (Benes 1997; Eslinger et al 1997; Freedman et al 1994; Myles-Worsley et al 1996), medications (Adler et al 1990a,b; Freedman et al 1983; Nagamoto et al 1996; Straumanis et al 1982; Ward et al 1996), brain lesions (Bickford and Wear 1995), traumatic brain injury (Arciniegas et al in press; Scheibel and Levin 1997), and aging (Papanicolau et al 1984) must all be considered by investigators intending to use this or any other phenotype for the study of schizophrenia. Similar considerations have been noted to be of importance in another well-studied ERP phenotype, the P300 (Pfefferbaum et al 1989).
jects, 95% had ratios of the amplitude of the test response to the amplitude of the conditioning response less than .50. For schizophrenic patients, 95% had ratios greater than .40. Ratios less than or equal to .40 were therefore considered normal, and ratios greater than or equal to .50 were considered abnormal. Ratios in between (.41 to .49) were considered unknown (Freedman et al 1997). Because ratios have difficult mathematical properties, in terms of skewness and, in this case, a number of 0 values (normals), these qualitative values were used for segregation analysis. The segregation analysis, performed by the SAGE programs (Elston 1996), was consistent with the autosomal dominant effect of a single gene. The disease allele frequency was assumed to be .05, with penetrance .80 in either the homozygous or heterozygous state, consistent with autosomal dominant inheritance. The rate of phenocopies, abnormal P50 ratios occurring in individuals with normal genotypes, was assumed to be .01. These assumptions yield a prevalence for elevated P50 ratio of 8.7% of the population, with 10.4% of the elevated P50 ratios being phenocopies. The prevalence is consistent with our previous samples of normal populations (Waldo et al 1994). This model was significantly more likely than the assumption of no genetic transmission (p ⬍ 6.53 ⫻ 10 ⫺10 ) or a recessive model (p ⬍ 1.51 ⫻ 10 ⫺5 ), or for a simple additive model, which might have been expected if the trait were multigenic (Freedman et al 1997; Kong and Cox 1997). An initial linkage analysis with this model showed some evidence for linkage of impaired P50 inhibitory gating with markers in the chromosome 15q14 area (Coon et al 1993). Later, this area was also shown to be the locus of the ␣7-nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit gene, which animal models had suggested was involved in the cholinergic activation of interneurons in the hippocampus (Chini et al 1994). Subsequent linkage analysis showed linkage to D15S1360, a polymorphic dinucleotide repeat marker in the chromosome 15q14 region, identified in a yeast artificial chromosome that contains the ␣7nicotinic cholinergic receptor (Freedman et al 1997). Linkage to this region of 15q14 has been further supported by analysis of the inheritance of schizophrenia in the NIMH Genetics Initiative families (Leonard et al 1998). At this point, because of the complexity of the gene itself (Gault et al 1998), no corresponding molecular abnormality has yet been found in the region of linkage, so that the genetic basis for inheritance of the P50 abnormality or schizophrenia has yet to be determined.
Genetic Linkage Using the P50 Phenotype P50 ratios from schizophrenic patients, their relatives, and normal subjects, were entered into a segregation analysis. The 95% confidence levels for both normal subjects and schizophrenic patients were determined. For normal sub-
Familial Transmission of Other Abnormalities in Schizophrenia Although the search for a molecular abnormality in the 15q14 region is still in progress, the results with the P50
Elementary Phenotypes in Schizophrenia
phenotype appear positive enough to suggest that the strategy of identifying specific phenotypes in the schizophrenia spectrum may have advantages for both biological and genetic research. Therefore, we have begun a more systematic search for other elementary neuronal phenotypes. Some of them may simply be improved P50 inhibition measurements, which will allow even more powerful genetic analysis, but will continue to identify the chromosome 15q14 locus. Some may identify other loci. For example, various eye-tracking dysfunctions have been shown to co-segregate with a previously identified linkage on chromosome 6 (Arolt et al 1996), and cerebral atrophy in one family has been shown to co-segregate with a schizophrenia-associated locus on chromosome 5 (Shihabuddin et al 1996). Radant and Hommer (1992) reported that schizophrenic patients have intrusions of small anticipatory saccades into their smooth-pursuit tracking and that this phenomenon is a more robust discriminator between schizophrenic and normal subjects than other measures of dysfunction. This measure, often quantify as either the frequency of saccades or the percentage of eye movement due to the saccades, is relatively stable during middle childhood development (ages 8 to 13 years) and thus suitable for phenotypic studies before the age of onset of schizophrenia (Ross et al 1993). Furthermore, the preliminary indication is that the measure is distributed in families in a pattern consistent with an autosomal dominant trait. It is present in half the offspring of schizophrenic parents, and it is found in parents who have been identified as obligate carriers as described previously (Ross et al 1998a,b; 1999a,b,c). Although the intrusion of small anticipatory saccades into smooth pursuit is not the only defect in smoothpursuit eye-movement function in schizophrenia, this deficit shares three characteristics with the P50 inhibitory deficit. First, both deficits can be conceptualized as inhibitory deficits. Second, both deficits are expressed in parents who are apparent obligate carriers of the genes that convey a risk for schizophrenia (Ross et al 1998a,b; 1999a,b,c). Third, both deficits are normalized in schizophrenic patients by high doses of nicotine, such as those that occur due to the patients’ own smoking (Olincy et al 1998). The anticipatory saccades and smooth-pursuit eye movements themselves both move the eye in the direction of the target. The smaller anticipatory saccades, in particular, seem to be responsive to the general instruction to track the target. Although there is some failure of the smooth-pursuit system, as measured by the increased percentage of eye movement accounted for by catch-up saccades in schizophrenic subjects, the intrusion of anticipatory saccades is a more prominent feature in schizophrenia, as judged by the differences in effect sizes (Ross et al 1999c). These findings suggest that the most signif-
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
13
icant portion of the global failure of eye-movement function is failure to inhibit the intrusion of these saccades. Basic neurophysiologic studies point to the cerebellar cortex as an area in which signals for both types of eye movements appear and perhaps arrive at the site where the decision about which type to use is made (Pierrot-Deseilligny 1994).
Possible Co-Dominant Inheritance of the Physiologic Abnormalities in ChildhoodOnset Schizophrenia The distribution of the elementary neurophysiologic phenotypes, and even of neuropsychological measures of attention, in schizophrenic patients and their parents is consistent with autosomal dominant inheritance. In other words, one parent, generally the one with an ancestral family history of schizophrenia, is the carrier of this abnormality, which is then also found in the schizophrenic offspring (Harris et al 1996; Ross et al 1998a,b; Siegel et al 1984). Even in families without an identifiable ancestral history, generally one, and only one, parent carries the neurophysiologic or neuropsychological abnormality (Waldo et al 1995). True genetic dominance means that the phenotypic abnormality is identical, regardless of whether an individual inherits only one or both abnormal alleles. Both the linkage and segregation studies, as well as the family studies, we have performed to date suggest that the inheritance of the P50 abnormality and of the anticipatory saccade abnormality is dominant (Freedman et al 1997). Inheritance of two abnormal alleles can only be rigorously established once molecular deficits have been identified. However, most geneticists who are looking for such homozygous inheritance would examine individuals with an early onset of illness, as such individuals generally inherit a more severe form of illness as well. Such a generic description of homozygous inheritance actually fits the clinical description of childhood-onset schizophrenia well (Alaghband-Rad et al 1995; McKenna et al 1994). The illness is rare, which is to be expected if two genetic abnormalities are required, and it has the requisite early onset and greater severity, while still resembling the putative more common heterozygous version of the illness, i.e., adult onset schizophrenia. We examined 10 cases of childhood-onset schizophrenia and their two parents. While we did not assess genetic inheritance, we could compare the frequency of bilineal appearance of the two physiologic phenotypes, the P50 auditory gating deficit and the increased anticipatory saccades in the parents of the proband to the usual unilineal appearance in adult-onset schizophrenia. We found bilineal appearance of elevated anticipatory sac-
14
L.E. Adler et al
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
cades in 8 of 10 pairs of parents, and bilineal appearance of P50 inhibitory abnormalities in 6 of 10. A corollary is that the neurophysiologic phenotypes should be more severely abnormal in the homozygous individuals, in this case, in the childhood-onset schizophrenic patients. We observed this effect for the eye-tracking abnormality. The percent of eye movement due to anticipatory saccades in these childhood-onset schizophrenic patients was approximately twice as great as that observed in adult-onset schizophrenic subjects (Ross et al 1998c). Thus, there is preliminary evidence that this abnormality is inherited in a co-dominant, rather than in a strict dominant, fashion. Furthermore, these results suggest a pathogenic mechanism, bilineal inheritance, that was not previously obvious in this puzzling variant of schizophrenia.
Conclusion This paper attempted to develop some of the practical issues in choosing phenotypes for the genetic and neurobiological study of schizophrenia. The initial clinical observation of an attention deficit in schizophrenia and its operational redefinition in neuronal terms, using the conditioning-testing paradigm was a critical step. Difficulties in specificity were resolved by the combination of clinical and animal model studies that provided critical data confirming a clinical effect of nicotine on P50 auditory gating and the mediating role of the 2-7 nicotine receptor (Adler et al 1992, 1993, 1998; Bickford and Wear 1995; Breese et al 1997a,b; Frazier et al 1997; Freedman et al 1995; Luntz-Leybman and Freedman 1992; Griffith et al 1998; Olincy et al 1997, 1999; Stevens et al 1996; Stevens and Wear 1997; Stevens et al 1998). Similar difficulties arise with other phenotypes, as demonstrated by the difficulty in determining the exact nature of dysfunction in eye-movement function (Ross et al 1998a,b). Convergence in genetic and neurobiological findings between the two phenotypes, in terms of their pattern of segregation and their response to nicotine, is encouraging. The different segregation pattern of both phenotypes in childhood-onset schizophrenia may be especially revealing. This paper did not attempt to review the technical problems of linkage analysis itself, as it applies to the use of physiologic phenotypes. That discussion occurs in another review article that appears earlier in Biological Psychiatry (Freedman et al 1999). This paper, similarly, did not attempt to review the vast body of work on physiologic dysfunction in schizophrenia from other authors, many of whom have made seminal contributions to this field. That discussion occurs in a chapter in the CD ROM version of Psychopharmacology: A Fourth Generation of Progress (Freedman et al 1999).
This work was presented at the conference, “Schizophrenia: From Molecule to Public Policy,” held in Santa Fe, New Mexico in October 1998. The conference was sponsored by the Society of Biological Psychiatry through an unrestricted educational grant provided by Eli Lilly and Company.
References Adler LE, Gerhardt G, Franks R, et al (1990a): Sensory physiology and catecholamines in schizophrenia and mania. Psychiatry Res 31:297–309. Adler LE, Hoffer LD, Nagamoto HT, Waldo MC, Kisley MA, Griffith JM (1994): Yohimbine impairs P50 auditory sensory gating in normal subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 10: 249 –257. Adler LE, Hoffer LD, Wiser A, Freedman R (1993): Normalization of auditory physiology by cigarette smoking in schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry 150:1856 –1861. Adler LE, Hoffer LD, Griffith JM, Waldo M, Freedman R (1992): Normalization by nicotine of deficient auditory sensory gating in the relatives of schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry 32:607– 616. Adler LE, Nagamoto N, Drebing C, Bronson J (1991): Impaired auditory sensory gating in patients with 20 years of combat post-traumatic stress disorder and depression. American Psychiatric Association, New Orleans, May, 1991. Adler LE, Olincy A, Waldo M, et al (1998): Schizophrenia, sensory gating, and nicotinic receptors. Schizophr Bull 24: 189 –202. Adler LE, Pachtman E, Franks R, Pecevich M, Waldo MC, Freedman R (1982): Neurophysiological evidence for a deficit in inhibitor mechanisms involved in sensory gating in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 17:639 – 654. Adler LE, Pang K, Gerhardt G, Rose GM (1988): Modulation of the gating of auditory evoked potentials by norepinephrine: pharmacological evidence obtained using a selective neurotoxin. Biol Psychiatry 24:179 –190. Adler LE, Rose GM, Freedman R (1986): Neurophysiologic studies of sensory gating in rats: Effects of amphetamine, phecyclidine and haloperidol. Biol Psychiatry 21:787–798. Adler LE, Waldo M, Tatcher A, Cawthra E, Baker N (1990b): Lack of relationship of auditory sensory gating defects to negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 3:131– 138. Alaghband-Rad J, McKenna K, Gordon CT, et al (1995): Childhood-onset schizophrenia: The severity of premorbid course. J Amer Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 34:1273– 1283. American Psychiatric Association (1994): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition. Washington, DC: APA Press, Inc. Arciniegas DB, Adler LE, Topkoff JL, Cawthra E, Filley CM, Riete ML (1999): Attention and memory dysfunction after traumatic brain injury: Cholinergic mechanisms, sensory gating, and a hypothesis for further investigation. Brain Injury, in press. Arolt V, Lencer R, Nolte A, et al (1996): Eye tracking dysfunction is a putative phenotypic susceptibility marker of schizo-
Elementary Phenotypes in Schizophrenia
phrenia and maps to a locus on chromosome 6p in families with multiple occurrence of the disease. Am J Med Genet 67:564 –579. Arpaia JP, Isenhart R, Sandman CA (1989): A characterization of a single-trial adaptive filter and its implementation in the frequency domain. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 73:362–368. Baker N, Adler LE, Franks RD, et al (1987): Neurophysiological assessment of sensory gating in psychiatric inpatients: comparison between schizophrenia and other diagnoses. Biol Psychiatry 22:603– 617. Benes FM (1997): Corticolimbic Circuitry and the development of psychopathology during childhood and adolescence. In: Krasnegor NA, Lyon GR and Goldman-Rakic PS, editors. Development of the Prefrontal Cortex: Evolution, Neurobiology, and Behavior. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co, pp 211–239. Bickford PC, Wear KD (1995): Restoration of sensory gating of auditory evoked response by nicotine in fimbria-fornix lesioned rats. Brain Res 705(1–2):235–240. Bickford-Wimer PC, Nagamoto H, Johnson R, et al (1990): Sensory gating in hippocampal neurons: A model system in the rat. Biol Psychiatry 27:183–192. Blackwood DHR, St. Clair DM, Muir WJ, Duffy JC (1991): Auditory P300 and eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenic pedigrees. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48:899 –909. Boutros NN, Zouridakis G, Overall J (1991): Replication and extension of P50 findings in schizophrenia. Clin Electroencephalogr 22:40 – 45. Braff DL, Geyer MA (1990): Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47:181–188. Breese CR, Adams C, Logel J, et al (1997a): Comparison of the regional expression of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor ␣7 mRNA and [125]-␣-bungarotoxin binding in the human postmortem brain. J Comp Neurol 387:385–398. Breese CR, Marks MJ, Logel J, et al (1997b): Effect of smoking history on [3H] nicotine binding in human postmortem brain. J Pharmacol Exper Ther 282(1):7–13. Cardenas VA, Gerson J, Fein G (1993): The reliability of P50 suppression as measured by the conditioning/testing ratio is vastly improved by dipole modeling. Biol Psychiatry 33:335– 344. Chapman CR, Colpitts YH, Mayenko JK, Gaglioardi GJ (1981): Rate of stimulus repetition changes evoked potential amplitude: Dental and auditory modalities compared. Exp Brain Res 43:246 –252. Chini B, Raimond E, Elgoyhen AB, Moralli D, Balzaretti M, Heinemann S (1994): Molecular cloning and chromosomal localization of human ␣7-nicotinic receptor subunit gene (CHRNA7). Genomics 19:379 –381. Clementz BA, Geyer MA, Braff DL (1997): P50 suppression among schizophrenia and normal comparison subjects: A methodological analysis. Biol Psychiatry 41:1035–1044. Clementz BA, Geyer MA, Braff DL (1998a): Multiple site evaluation of P50 suppression among schizophrenia and normal comparison subjects. Schizophr Res 30:71– 80. Clementz BA, Geyer MA, Braff DL (1998b): Poor P50 suppression among schizophrenia patients and their first-degree biological relatives. Am J Psychiatry 155:1691–1694.
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
15
Coon H, Plaetke R, Holik J, et al (1993): Use of a neurophysiological trait in linkage analysis of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 34:277–289. Cornblatt BA, Keilp JG (1994): Impaired attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 20:31– 46. Cullum CM, Harris JG, Waldo MC, et al (1993): Neurophysiological and neuropsychological evidence for attentional dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 10:131–141. Davis H, Mast T, Yoshie N, Zerlin S (1966): The slow response of the human cortex to auditory stimuli. Recovery process. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 21:105–113. De Amicis LA, Wagstaff DA, Cromwell R (1986): Reaction time crossover as a marker of schizophrenia and of higher functioning. J Nerv Ment Dis 174:177–179. Eccles JC (1969): The Inhibitory Pathways of the Central Nervous System. Liverpool: University Press. Elston RC (1996): SIBPAL, Statistical Analysis for Genetic Epidemiology (Case Western Reserve, Cleveland, Ohio), version 2.7. Erwin RJ, Buchwald JS (1986): Midlatency auditory evoked responses: Differential recovery cycle characteristics. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 64:417– 423. Erwin RJ, Buchwald JS (1987): Midlatency auditory evoked responses in the human and the cat model. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl 40:461– 467. Erwin RJ, Mawhinney-Hee M, Gur RC, Gur RE (1991): Midlatency auditory evoked responses in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 30:430 – 442. Erwin RJ, Stasel D, Gur RE (1994): Effects of medication history on midlatency auditory evoked responses in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 11:251–258. Erwin RJ, Turetsky BI, Moberg P, Gur RC, Gur RE (1998): P50 abnormalities in schizophrenia: Relationship to clinical and neuropsychological indices of attention. Schizophr Res 33: 157–168. Eslinger PJ, Biddle KR, Grattan LM (1997): Cognitive and social development in children with prefrontal cortex lesions. In: Krasnegor NA, Lyon GR, and Goldman-Rakic PS, Development of the Prefrontal Cortex: Evolution, Neurobiology, and Behavior. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., pp 295–336. Fein G, Biggins C, MacKay S (1996): Cocaine abusers have reduced auditory P50 amplitude and suppression compared to both normal controls and alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry 39:955– 965. Finkenzeller P, Keidel WD (1975): Correlates of auditory perception in averaged prestimulatory EEG-DC recordings. In: Quantitative Analysis of the EEG: Methods and Applications. AEG-Telefunken, 1975; reprinted in Keidel W, Kallert S, Korth M, editors. The Physiological Basis of Hearing. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag, p 198. Franks R, Adler LE, Waldo M, Alpert J, Freedman R (1983): Neurophysiological studies of sensory gating in mania: Comparison with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 18:989 –1005. Frazier CJ, Rollins YD, Breese CR, Leonard S, Freedman R, Dunwiddie TV (1997): Acetylcholine activates an ␣-bungarotoxin sensitive nicotinic current in rat hippocampal interneurons, but not pyramidal cells. J Neurosci 18:1187–1195.
16
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
Freedman R, Adler LE, Bickford P, et al (1994a): Schizophrenia and nicotinic receptors. Harvard Rev Psychiatry 2(4):179 – 192. Freedman R, Adler LE, Leonard S (1999): Alternative phenotypes for the complex genetics of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 45:551–558. Freedman R, Adler, LE, Nagamoto HT, Waldo M (1998): Selection of digital filtering parameters and P50 amplitude. Biol Psychiatry 43:921–922. Freedman R, Adler LE, Waldo M (1987): Gating of the auditory evoked potential in children and adults. Psychophysiology 24:223–227. Freedman R, Adler LE, Waldo M, et al (1996): Inhibitory gating of an evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects: Human recordings, computer stimulation, and an animal model. Arch Gen Psychiat 53: 1114 –1121. Freedman R, Adler LE, Waldo MC, Pachtman E, Franks RD (1983): Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in inhibitory pathways in schizophrenia: Comparison of medicated and drug-free patients. Biol Psychiatry 18:537–551. Freedman R, Coon H, Myles-Worsley M, et al (1997): Linkage of a neurophysiological deficit in schizophrenia to a chromosome 15 locus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:587–592. Freedman R, Hall M, Adler LE, Leonard S (1995): Evidence in postmortem brain tissue for decreased numbers of hippocampal nicotinic receptors in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 38:22–23. Freedman R, Ross RG, Adler LE (1999): Physiological indicators of the schizophrenia phenotype. Psychopharmacology: Fourth Generation of Progress—CD/ROM, New York: Raven Press. Freedman R, Stromberg I, Nordstrom AL, et al (1994b): Neuronal development in embryonic brain tissue derived from schizophrenic women and grafted to animal hosts. Schizophr Res 13:259 –270. Freedman R, Waldo M, Adler L, Baker N, Levin D, Deitrich R (1986): Electrophysiological effects of low-dose alcohol on human subjects at high altitude. Alcohol Drug Res 6:289 – 297. Fruhstorfer H, Soveri P, Jarvilehto T (1970): Short-term habituation of the auditory evoked response in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 28:153–159. Gault J, Robinson M, Berger R, et al (1998): Genomic organization and partial duplication of the human ␣ 7 neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptor gene (CHRNA7). Genomics 52:173–185. Gillette GM, Skinner RD, Rasco LM, et al (1997): Combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder exhibit decreased habituation of the P1 midlatency auditory evoked potential. Life Sci 61(14):1421–1434. Goldstein JL, Hazzard WR, Schrott HG, Bierman EL, Motulsky AG (1973): Hyperlipidemia in coronary heart disease. II. Genetic analysis of lipid levels in 176 families and delineation of a new inherited disorder. J Clin Invest 51:1544. Gottesman II, Shields J (1972): Schizophrenia and Genetics: A Twin Study Vantage Point. New York: Academic Press. Green MF (1996): What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 153:321–330.
L.E. Adler et al
Green MF, Marshall BD Jr, Wirshing WC, et al (1997): Does risperidone improve verbal working memory in treatmentresistant schizophrenia? Am J of Psychiatry 154(6):799 – 804. Griffith J, Freedman R (1995a): Normalization of the auditory P50 gating deficit of schizophrenic patients after NREM, but not REM sleep. Psychiatry Res 56:271–278. Griffith J, Hoffer LD, Adler LE, Zerbe GO, Freedman R (1995b): Effects of sound intensity on a midlatency evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects. Psychophysiology 32:460 – 466. Griffith J, O’Neill JE, Petty F, Garver D, Young D, Freedman R (1998): Nicotine receptor desensitization and sensory gating deficits in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 44:98 –106. Griffith JM, Waldo M, Adler LE, Freedman R (1993): Normalization of auditory sensory gating in schizophrenic patients after a brief period for sleep. Psychiatry Res 49:29 –39. Grillon C, Ameli R, Braff DL (1991): Middle latency auditory evoked potentials (MAEPs) in chronic schizophrenics. Schizophr Res 5:61– 66. Harris JG, Adler LE, Young DA, et al (1996): Neuropsychological dysfunction in parents of schizophrenics. Schizophr Res 20:253–260. Hershman KM, Freedman R, Bickford PC (1995): GABA-B antagonists diminish the inhibitory gating of auditory response in the rat hippocampus. Neurosci Lett 190:133–136. Hetrick WP, Sandman CA, Bunney WE Jr., Jin Y, Potkin SG, White MH (1996): Gender differences in gating of the auditory evoked potential in normal subjects. Biol Psychiatry 39:51–58. Holzman PS, Kringlen E, Matthysse S, et al (1988): A single dominant gene can account for eye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia in offspring of discordant twins. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45:641– 647. Jerger K, Biggins C, Fein G (1992): P50 suppression is not affected by attentional manipulation. Biol Psychiatry 31:365– 377. Jin Y, Bunney WE, Sandman CA, et al (1998): Is P50 suppression a measure of sensory gating in schizophrenia? Biol Psychiatry 43(12):873– 878. Jin Y, Potkin SG, Patterson JV, Sandman CA, Hetrick WP, Bunney WE Jr (1997): Effects of P50 temporal variability on sensory gating in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 70(2):71– 81. Johnson M, Adler LE (1993): Transient impairment of P50 auditory sensory gating by a cold-pressor test. Biol Psychiatry 33:380 –387. Judd LL, McAdams L, Budnick B, Braff DL (1992): Sensory gating deficit in schizophrenia: New results. Am J Psychiatry 149:488 – 493. Kang D-Y, Poole J, McCallin K, Fein G, Vinogradov S (1997): Sensory gating deficit in schizophrenia: Relation to catecholamine metabolites. Schizophr Res 24:234. Kathmann N, Engel RR (1990): Sensory gating in normals and schizophrenics: A failure to find strong P50 suppression in normals. Biol Psychiatry 27:1216 –1226. Kendler KS, Gruenberg AM, Tsuang MT (1985): Psychiatric illness in the first-degree relatives of schizophrenic and surgical control patients: A family study using DSM-III criteria. Arch Gen Psychiatry 42:770 –779. Kong A, Cox NJ (1997): Allele-sharing models: LOD scores and accurate linkage tests. Am J Hum Genet 61(11):1179 –1188.
Elementary Phenotypes in Schizophrenia
Lamberti JS, Schwarzkopf SB, Smith DA, Rilly J, Martin R (1993): Diminished P50 auditory evoked suppression in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 33:117A. Lander ES (1988): Splitting schizophrenia. Nature (London) 336:105–106. Leonard S, Gault J, Moore T, et al (1998): Further investigation of a chromosome 15 locus in schizophrenia: Analysis of affected sibpairs from the NIMH genetics initiative. Neuropsychiatrict Genet 81:308 –312. Luntz-Leybman V, Bickford PC, Freedman R (1992): Cholinergic gating of response to auditory stimuli in rat hippocampus. Brain Res 587:130 –136. Madison DV, Nicoll RA (1988): Norepinephrine decreases synaptic inhibition in rat hippocampus. Brain Res 442:131– 138. Mahtani MM, Widen E, Lehto M, et al (1996): Mapping of a gene for type 2 diabetes associated with an insulin secretion defect by a genome scan in Finnish families. Nat Genet Sep14:90 –94. McGhie A, Chapman JS (1961): Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. Br J Med Psychol 34:103– 116. McKenna K, Gordon CT, Rapoport JL (1994): Childhood onset schizophrenia: Timely neurobiological research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 33:771–781. Miller CL, Bickford-Wimer PC, Leybman V, Adler LE, Gerhardt G, Freedman R (1992): Phencyclidine induces a physiological feature of psychosis. Neuropharmacology 31:1041–1048. Miller CL, Freedman R (1995): The activity of hippocampal interneurons and pyramidal cells during the response of the hippocampus to repeated auditory stimuli. Neuroscience 69: 371–381. Moldin SO (1997): The maddening hunt for madness genes. Nat Genet 7:127–129. Myles-Worsley M, Coon H, Byerley W, Waldo M, Young D, Freedman R (1996): Developmental and genetic influences on the P50 sensory gating phenotype. Biol Psychiatry 39: 289 –295. Nagamoto HT, Adler LE, Hea RA, Griffith JM, McRae K, Freedman R (1996): Gating of auditory P50 in schizophrenics: Unique effects of clozapine. Biol Psychiatry 40:181–188. Nagamoto HT, Adler LE, McRae KA, et al (1999): Auditory P50 in schizophrenics on clozapine: Improved gating parallels clinical improvement and changes in pMHPG. Neuropsychobiology 39:10 –17. Nagamoto HT, Adler LE, Waldo MC, Freedman R (1989): Sensory gating in schizophrenics and normal controls: Effects of changing stimulation. Biol Psychiatry 25:549 –561. National Institute of Mental Health Genetics Workgroup (1997): Genetics and Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: National Institutes of Health, pp 3–23, 47–50. Olincy A, Leonard S, Young DA, Sullivan B, Freedman R (1999): Decreased bombesin peptide response in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 20:52–59. Olincy A, Ross RG, Young DA, Freedman R (1997): Age diminishes performance on an antisaccade eye movement task. Neurobiol Aging (18)5:483– 489. Olincy A, Ross RG, Young DA, Roath M, Freedman R (1998):
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
17
Improvement in smooth pursuit eye movements after cigarette smoking in schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 18:174 –185. Olincy A, Young DA, Freedman R (1997): Increased levels of nicotine metabolite cotinine in schizophrenic smokers compared to other smokers. Biol Psychiatry 42:1–5. Papanicolau AC, Loring DW, Eisenberg HM (1984): Age-related differences in recovery cycle of auditory evoked potentials. Neurobiol Aging 5:291–295. Papanicolau AC, Loring DW, Eisenberg HM (1985): Evoked potential attenuation in the two-tone paradigm. Int J Psychobiol 3:23–28. Park S, Holzman PS, Goldman-Rakic PS (1995): Spatial working memory deficits in the relatives of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:821– 828. Pfefferbaum A, Ford JM, White PM, Roth WT (1989): P3 in schizophrenia is affected by stimulus modality, response requirements, medication status, and negative symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry 46:1035–1044. Pierrot-Deseilligny C (1994): Saccade and smooth-pursuit impairment after cerebral hemispheric lesions. Eur Neurol 34:121–134. Radant AD, Hommer DW (1992): A quantitative analysis of saccades and smooth pursuit during visual pursuit tracking: A comparison of schizophrenics with normals and substance abusing controls. Schizophr Res 6:225–235. Ross RG, Harris JG, Olincy A, Radant A, Adler LE, Freedman R (1998a): Familial transmission of two independent saccadic abnormalities in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 30:59 –70. Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, et al (1999a): Evidence for bilineal inheritance of physiological indicators of risk in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet: Neuropsychiatric Genet 88:188 –99. Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, et al (1999b): The effects of age on a smooth pursuit tracking task in adults with schizophrenia and normals. Biol Psychiatry, in press. Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, Radant A, Adler LE, Freedman R (1998b): Anticipatory saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements and familial transmission of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 44:690 – 697. Ross RG, Olincy A, Radant A (1999c): Amplitude criteria and anticipatory saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology, in press. Ross RG, Radant AD, Hommer DW (1993): A developmental study of smooth pursuit eye movements in normal children from 7 to 15 years of age. J Child Adolesc Psychiatry 32:783–791. Roth WT, Kopell BS (1969): The auditory evoked response to repeated stimuli during a vigilance task. Psychophysiology 6:301–309. Schiebel RS, Levin HS (1997): Cognitive and social development in children with prefrontal cortex lesions. In: Krasnegor NA, Lyon GR, Goldman-Rakic PS, editors. Development of the Prefrontal Cortex: Evolution, Neurobiology, and Behavior. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co, pp 283–294. Sham PC, Morton NE, Muir WJ, et al (1994): Segregation analysis of complex phenotypes: An application to schizophrenia and auditory P300 latency. Psychiatric Genet 4:29 – 38.
18
BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1999;46:8 –18
Shihabuddin L, Silverman JM, Buchsbaum MS, et al (1996): Ventricular enlargement associated with linkage marker for schizophrenia-related disorders in one pedigree. Mol Psychiatry 1(3):215–222. Siegel C, Waldo M, Mizner G, Adler LE, Freedman R (1984): Deficits in sensory gating in schizophrenic patients and their relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 41:607– 612. Smith DA, Boutros NN, Schwarzkopf SB (1994): Reliability of P50 auditory event-related potential indices of sensory gating. Psychophysiology 31:495–502. Stevens KE, Freedman R, Collins AC, et al (1996): Genetic correlation of hippocampal auditory evoked response and alpha-bungarotoxin binding in inbred mouse strains. Neuropsychopharmacol 15:152–162. Stevens KE, Kem WR, Mahnir VM, Freedman R (1998): Selective ␣7-nicotinic agonists normalize inhibition of auditory response in DBA mice. Psychopharmacology 136:320 – 327. Stevens KE, Meltzer J, Stryker SL, Rose GM (1993): Disruption of sensory gating by the alpha-2 selective noradrenergic antagonist yohimbine. Biol Psychiatry 33:130 –132. Stevens KE, Wear KD (1997): Normalizing effects of nicotine and a novel nicotinic agonist on hippocampal auditory gating in two animal models. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 57:869 – 874. Straumanis JJ, Shagass C, Roemer RA (1982): Influence of antipsychotic and antidepressant drugs on evoked potential correlates of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 17:1101. Tsuang MT, Lyons MJ, Faraone SV (1990): Heterogeneity of schizophrenia: Conceptual models and analytic strategies. Br J Psychiatry 156:17–26. Venables P (1964): Input dysfunction in schizophrenia. In: Maher BA, editor. Progress in Experimental Personality Research. Orlando, FL: Academic Press, pp 1– 47.
L.E. Adler et al
Vinogradov S, Solomon S, Ober BA, Biggins CA, Shenaut GK, Fein G (1996): Do semantic priming effects correlate with sensory gating in schizophrenia? Biol Psychiatry 39:821– 824. Waldo M, Carey G, Myles-Worsley M, et al (1991): Codistribution of sensory gating deficit and schizophrenia in multi-affected families. Psychiatry Res 39:257–268. Waldo MC, Cawthra E, Adler LE, et al (1994): Auditory sensory gating, hippocampal volume, and catecholamine metabolism in schizophrenics and their siblings. Schizophr Res 12:93– 106. Waldo MC, Graze K, de Graff Bender S, Adler LE, Freedman R (1987): Premenstrual mood changes and gating of the auditory evoked potential. Psychoneuroendocrinology 12:35– 40. Waldo M, Myles-Worsley M, Madison A, Byerley W, Freedman R (1995): Sensory gating deficits in parents of schizophrenics. Am J Med Genet 60:506 –511. Ward PB, Hoffer LD, Liebert BJ, Catts SV, O’Donnell M, Adler LE (1996): Replication of a P50 auditory gating deficit in Australian patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 64: 121–135. Wilson CL, Babb TL, Halgren E, Wang ML, Crandall PH (1984): Habituation of human neuronal limbic response to sensory stimulation. Exp Neurol 84:74 –97. Yee CM, Nuechterlein KH, Morris SE, White PM (1998): P50 suppression in recent-onset schizophrenia: Clinical correlates and risperidone effects. J Abnorm Psychol 107:691– 698. Young D, Waldo M, Rutledge JH, Freedman R (1996): Heritability of inhibitory gating of the P50 auditory evoked potential in monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Neuropsychobiology 33:113–117. Zouridakis G, Boutros NN, Jansen BH (1997): A fuzzy clustering approach to study the auditory P50 component in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 69:169 –181.