Evaluating treatment efficacy for adults: Assessment of stuttering disability

Evaluating treatment efficacy for adults: Assessment of stuttering disability

EVALUATING TREATMENT EFFICACY FOR ADULTS: ASSESSMENT OF STUTTERING DISABILITY The propod strategy for evaluating the efficacy of’ treatment for adult...

817KB Sizes 44 Downloads 114 Views

EVALUATING TREATMENT EFFICACY FOR ADULTS: ASSESSMENT OF STUTTERING DISABILITY

The propod strategy for evaluating the efficacy of’ treatment for adult\ ti ho stutter involves repeated ahse\smcnt\ ofst~ttcring and speech naturalness i’-om conversation ample\ recorded in and away from the clinic. This stl-:lteg)’ conceptualircs chronic stuttering among adult\ as ;I di\or-dcr that is hc\t vicwd and measured at three level\: impairment. disability. and handicap. .A common goal of current treatments for adult stuttering i\ the reduction or climinatlon of the disability of stuttering. Therctbre. the evaluation of treatment efi’icac~ is logically and most directly related to mea\urc\ of stuttering disability. Both basic and optional mca\ures of the diahilith, of stuttering arc propod and discussed.

INTRODUCTION In 1973 a review tering

is

Andrews, about

of behavior

a comparatively 1973.

p. 408).

the reliability

can be found

1973: mcnt.

(Coyle it was

quency tering

1975). widely

counts

There

counts

of these until

events

are now

1981

findings

measures

of stuttering

been apparent

; MacDonald that

sufficiently

(Kully

and

concerns for

and Martin.

on event-by-cvcnt

the effect\

data indicating

be misplaced

“\tuf-

on the loci ofstutteriny

recently, were

that

(Ingham

increasing

it has

low agreement

for quantifying

may

however.

example,

1979: Curlee.

stated

to measure”

of perceptual For

believed,

of stuttering

measure

clients.

frequency

that time.

evidence

In spite

and stuttering

disorder

and validity

and Mallard.

Young.

as 21primary

Since

in the literature.

some time that listeners events

therapy

simple

agrecmcnt high for

of treatment that

and

such Bohcrg.

ago-eeon fretheir with

confidence Ic)XX),

USC \tutin and

320

R. F. CURLEE

subsequent efforts to develop procedures that yield measures of satisfactory reliability have achieved only modest success to date (Cordes et al., 1992). It should be noted also that persuasive arguments have been advanced in recent years that challenge the value of listener judgments of stuttering even if those judgments were reliable. Perkins (1990) claims, for example, that a speaker’s subjective experience of the loss of control of the speech production process is the only valid indicator of the occurrence of stuttering events. Others, such as Adams and Runyan (IYSI), Freeman (l984), and Smith (l990), have argued that the events that listeners perceive as stuttering events are only a subset of the abnormal speech production events that characterize the speech of those who stutter. Such arguments have selected neuropsychological and/or neurophysiological variables, rather disruptions in speech, as the most appropriate level at which to study and describe stuttering. Stuttering has been studied at various levels of observation, from central nervous system activity to perceptions of the acoustic product of the speech production process. All levels have provided useful information, and it is not yet clear which level(s) will be most valuable to our understanding of the nature of stuttering and of how best to manage it. Different levels of observation rely on different definitions and different measures of stuttering phenomena. A useful perspective for conceptualizing and evaluating the clinical management of a chronic, behavioral disorder, which Prins (1991) applied to stuttering, is that of the World Health Organization (WHO). This model views disorders of stuttering as having three levels: I. An impairment, which would include all of the neuropsychological and neurophysiological events that immediately precede and accompany the audible and visible events of stuttering. 2. A disahili~y, which would comprise the audible and visible events that are the behavioral manifestations of stuttering. 3. A handicap, which would consist of the disadvantages that result from reactions to the audible and visible events of a person’s stuttering and that would include those of the person who stutters. This view of stuttering appears to be particularly useful for differentiating the goals of different clinical management approaches and for organizing the different measures of treatment efficacy that are pertinent to those goals. There is much more information about the speech disabilities of those who stutter than there is about the nature of their speech production disruptions or about the kinds of handicaps that may result. Indeed, the nature of the disruptions in the planning and execution of speech

ASSESSMENT

that

OF

results

in the audible

is not well-understood. of those often

who

result

handicaps 1992).

have

ber

of other

assessing

handicaps

Stuttering

Inventory

Attitudes

(Watson.

arc needed of those

posed

from 1967).

was

stutter.

derived

from

Stuttering rarely

3.

A primary

Measures of

suited Thus,

this

stuttering

current

article

of reliable

signs

of stuttering

uating The

situations

by Costello stuttering from

the clinic.

conversational several

years

even

these

few

and mcaand handito be pro-

and inferences: event

until

at other

childhood

it has

levels

of

into

adult

life

is to eliminate

OI

remissions.

procedures

events

and

that comprise

most are

are well

systematic

valid

based

focus are.

comparisons

best

who stutter.

measures

on the

of an adult

perceptual

acquainted

;I practical.

on a primary

therefore.

of adults

of treatment

the

that

who

and that

of stuttering

procedures

and time provide

with of such

appropriate

judg-

the clinical measures

way for eval-

of treatment.

assessment

and lngham adults’

from

events

that

are those observers

the efficacy proposed

10X4),

workers

strategy

behavioral

the efficacy

proposes

disability

of

stuttering.

treatment

for assessing

to

and control.

spontancou\

of the behavioral

ments across

of adult

et al.,

disubilitics.

of events

treatment

prove

scales for

of study.

as a behavioral

and visible

et

the Perception

from

assumptions

persis;ts

goal of current the audible

the disability goal

that

extended.

useful

stuttering

efficacy

prediction

well

Ingham.

and a num-

1 could

(Craig of

units

descriptions

better

behavior

decrease 4.

that

evidences

scales

treatment

less

of Communication

be apparent

the following

provide

I991

the impairments.

which

(Onslow

1980).

include

of observation.

and rate

et al..

perceptions

is best conceptualized

observation

(Gow

Inventory

of control

The

been demonstrated 2.

the

It should

levels

lo quantify

who

Stuttering

locus

and arc even

Potentially

stuttering

of employers’ 1991).

(Wood

reactions

them.

and personal/social

(Alphonse.

(Woolf,

that different

study

impairment.

that result 198X),

with

and articulatory

flow

measures

stuttering

and Calver.

sures

I.

ofa

assessment

examples

blood

interact

intervals

durations

physiological

indices

caps

systematic

disability

and emotional

vocational.

phonation

cerebral

be useful

the

littlc

of vowel

regional

of a stuttering

who

educational,

received

variability

events

the cognitive

as those

Nevertheless,

IYE),

(Craig

as well

in significant

al..

and

and visible

Likewise.

stutter,

understood.

121

DISAHII~I’I’Y

speech Samples partners.

posttreatment,

strategy (1984) samples

is consistent

and involves

that are recorded

should

involve

extend

from

with

repeated

different pretreatment

and be supplemented

that advocated assessments

both

of

in and away

speaking samples by reports

tasks

and

through of family

322

R. F.CURLEE

members and occasional covert samples. Prior to the initiation ment, the following measures should be obtained to document ture and severity of a stuttering disability.

BASIC MEASURES

OF STUTTERING

of treatthe na-

DISABILITY

These measures are considered to be necessary components of any assessment of stuttering. They will be sufficient, however, only if no stuttering events are observed in a speech sample and if the sample’s speech naturalness ratings fall within the range of those expected of nonstuttering speakers. It should also be noted that elevated scores on the S-24 scale could indicate the need to obtain recordings of additional speech samples. If, however, the S-24 score is not elevated, no stuttering events are observed in the speech samples assessed and speech naturalness ratings are comparable to those of nonstuttering speakers, there should be no need to obtain further quantification unless there is reason to report a severity rating derived from standardized procedures. The basic, obligatory measures needed to assess a stuttering disability include: 1. frequency of stuttering (in percentage of syllables [% SS] or percent words [% WS] stuttered); 2. ratings of speech naturalness; 3. administration of the S-24 Scale (Andrews and Cutler, 1974).

OPTIONAL SEVERITY MEASURES STUTTERING DISABILITY

OF

Some of these measures should be selected to supplement the basic measures listed above whenever a sample contains stuttering events or receives elevated speech naturalness ratings. The specific measures selected should be determined by the characteristics of clients’ speech and stuttering events. For example, if a client’s longest sound prolongation or tense pause was less than a second, efforts to quantify their typical duration would likely have little clinical significance. In contrast, careful assessments of the duration of long stuttering events may provide early indications during treatment that a client’s severity of stuttering is decreasing. Even if a sample is free of stuttering events but receives elevated speech naturalness ratings, such measures as speech rate may provide insight into the reasons for the elevated rating. Many times, all of these measures may be needed. Finally, it should also be noted that additional, supplementary measures may also be

ASSESSMENT

OF DISABlI,ITY

needed in order to better assess the idiosyncratic stuttering events and patterns of speech of some clients. For example, the eye contact of many stuttering clients during conversations is frequently inappropriate, such behavior may be targeted for change in therapy. If so. it would be important to devise optional measure(s) of eye contact during video recordings of a client’s speech samples. Optional measures that will be needed most often to further quantify the severity of a stuttering disability include: I. duration of both typical and longest stuttering events (in seconds and/or number of repeated units); 2. length of typical and longest stutter-free segments of speech (in syllables or words): 3. rate of speech in syllables (SPM) or words (WPM) per minute: 4. administration of a standardized scale such as the Stuttering Severity Index (Riley. 1974).

MEASUREMENT

PROCEDURES

Descriptions of the optional severity measures listed above can be found in a number of refercnccs tc.g., Costello and Ingham. 1984). all of these measures will be used to assess the speech Ordinarily. samples of adults who stutter that were recorded prior to treatment in order to quantify a client’s stuttering disability in adequate detail. Samples obtained at termination of treatment and final posttreatment samples would utilize only those measures needed to further quantify any remaining stuttering events or unnatural speech patterns. Several of these measure are also intended to provide more sensitive indicators of changes in stuttering. both increases and decreases, during the intervention process. which may not be readily apparent in frequency of stuttering measures. Such measures (e.g., rate of speech) may also provide additional insight into the reasons for a client receiving elevated speech naturalness ratings. In contrast. the basic measures of stuttering disability are intended to indicate whether or not someone has stuttered during a recorded sample and, if so, how much. In addition. speech naturalness ratings should provide evidence of a client’s use of any compensatory speaking behaviors in the sample that sound unnatural or that draw adverse attention from listeners. Finally, the S-24 Scale is intended to suggest communication interactions away from the clinic that warrant observation and assessment. Guidelines for obtaining and interpreting these measures will be summarized briefly in the paragraphs that follow. There are no standard procedures in current use for counting stut-

324

R.F.CURLEE

tering events that are widely accepted. For example, some clinicians restrict counts to specific disfluency topologies, others count any speech disfluency that is perceived as unusual, abnormal, or effortful as stuttering. The development and adoption of a standardized set of such procedures would likely reduce at least some of the variability in stuttering counts often reported among different clinics and clinicians. Cordes et al. (1992) found that listeners who are well acquainted with the clinical signs of stuttering and who evidence high intraobserver agreement are also likely to evidence higher interobserver agreement. Consequently, the assessment procedures that will be described are best conducted by individuals with a number of years experience working with stuttering adults and who also evidence self-agreement levels consistently in excess of 0.80 when counting stuttering events. The following guidelines, which could also be used with children who stutter, illustrate one way of counting stuttering events of adults. Count only dysfluencies that are perceived as stuttered events rather than all disfluencies. Each syllable or word should be counted as either stuttered or not stuttered and should be counted only once regardless of how it was disrupted. If sounds. syllables, words, or phrases are repeated as if to facilitate initiation of the following word, only the syllable or word that follows is counted as stuttered. Count the number of syllables or words stuttered twice. If these counts agree, use that number to calculate % SS or % WS. If they do not agree, count stuttering events again and use the median or modal count for calculations. These guidelines view stuttering events to be a perceptual “threshold phenomenon” (Martin and Haroldson, 1981) and all speech events perceived to be stuttered are counted, regardless of the type of disfluency involved. This approach differs from those that define stuttering as a group of specific behaviors, typically the kernel characteristics of stuttering that were proposed by Wingate (19641, and that count only specific types of disfluencies as stuttering events. Empirical comparisons of these two approaches have found “perceptual threshold” counts to result in similar (Curlee, 1981) or higher listener agreement (Martin and Haroldson, 1981). As was noted above, there are no standardized procedures for training judges to identify stuttering events reliably or for monitoring or for recalibrating such judges’ performance following training. Consequently, there is an urgent need to develop such training and monitoring procedures or to establish special facilities that would assess recorded speech samples much as medical laboratories now assess fluid and tissue samples. In the absence of such standardization, one way to increase the precision and reliability of measurements is to rely on the

ASSESSMENT

OF DISABILITY

325

central tendency of repeated measures as was proposed in the above guidelines. Ratings of speech naturalness should utilize the procedures described by Martin and colleagues (1984). Those portions of recorded speech samples that are assessed for frequency of stuttering events should also be rated in terms of speech naturalness every 30 set by at least three to five independent raters. The median rating of each segment should be used to determine both the median and range of all ratings during a sample and should be compared to the ratings obtained from nonstuttering speakers. It should be noted that speech naturalness ratings need to be obtained from a much larger number of nonstuttering speakers so that the resulting data base will likely encompass the range of fluent and nonfluent speakers in the nonstuttering population. Such data would also permit the reporting of stuttering adults’ speech naturalness ratings as standard z-scores, which would also facilitate comparison of a client’s ratings to those of the nonstuttering population. Administration of the S-24 Scale (Andrews and Cutler, 1974) is included as a basic measure because of the need to select extra-clinic communication settings that may be troublesome for adults who stutter. Thus, this scale is not used to evaluate clients’ attitudes about speaking but rather as an indicator of the types of speaking tasks and situations that may be causing a client current stuttering difficulties. Scale items that are scored “1” can be utilized to identify the type and number of extra-clinic samples that need to be obtained, especially during transfer and follow-up assessments, so that arising or persisting difficulties with stuttering can be monitored and evaluated appropriately.

SAMPLES Prior to the initiation of treatment, at least two in-clinic conversations should be videotaped, one face-to-face, the other via telephone. Each should include at least 500 words, or 7.50 syllables, of the client’s speech. Neither conversational partner should be familiar to the client, and each sample should include segments intended to be stressful. Oral reading samples should also be recorded if it is reported that this task poses special difficulties or is of special importance to the client. At least three samples of similar length should be recorded away from the clinic. One should involve a conversational partner who is familiar to the client and who will be able to continue as a conversational partner through posttreatment samples. Remaining samples should be selected from those reported to be especially difficult for the client or those that elicited “ 1” responses from the client on the S-24 Scale. Large

326

R. F. CURLEE

differences in frequency of stuttering across samples (e.g., >25%) may indicate the need for replicating or obtaining additional samples. If an intensive treatment schedule (e.g., >I5 hr weekly) is utilized to establish speech that is free of stuttering, initial sampling procedures should be repeated before beginning transfer training if the efficacy of transfer training is to be evaluated. If a less intensive treatment schedule is utilized, outside clinic samples with the familiar partner and of one problematic situation should be recorded on alternate weeks until the client is ready for transfer training. At that time, initial sampling procedures should be replicated if the effect of transfer training is to be assessed. During transfer training, samples of at least two extra-clinic conversations should be recorded regularly until treatment is terminated. Early in training, such samples should be recorded at least weekly and should include one with the familiar conversation partner until there is no stuttering and speech naturalness ratings are within normal limits (WNL) for four consecutive samples. Later, only monthly samples may need to be recorded so long as speech naturalness ratings remain WNL and no stuttering events are observed on any sample. Additional recordings would be used to sample those reported as troublesome by the client or those that are suspected as problematic based on monthly administrations of the S-24 Scale. With prior written permission of the client, monthly covert samples can be used to evaluate a client’s speech when he or she is presumed to be unaware of being assessed. Initial sampling procedures should be repeated at termination of transfer training and at 6-month and yearly intervals thereafter so long as contact with clients is maintained.

SUMMARY The strategy that has been proposed for evaluating treatment efficacy relies on assessments of a client’s stuttering disability from recorded speech samples obtained in different settings prior to and at regular intervals during and following treatment. It was argued that stuttering is, in essence, a behavioral event, that current treatment procedures focus on modifying or eliminating these behavioral events, and that the perceptual judgments of experienced, reliable observers provide the most valid way of measuring any changes that occur in these behavioral events during therapy. This strategy is also consistent with the WHO model of chronic behavioral disorders in that it views adult stuttering as consisting of an impairment, a disability, and a handicap. Several basic and optional measures of stuttering severity and speech naturalness were proposed for determining the extent to which a stut-

ASSESSMENT

tcring

disability

ir\ not clear should

327

OF L>ISAHIL,ITY

decreases

if changes

bc described

during

the course

occurring

of treatment.

in speaking

as “treatment

behavior

Although

during

in “speech

or as an increase

effects”

it

thcrapq

the measures proposed are intended to quantify any such skills.” “treatment effects” or changes in “speech skills” that may occur. Thus. if samples recorded in a clinical setting are compared to those recorded away from the clinic, the generality of such “treatment cffecty” or the extent to which a client can employ his or her “speech skills”

;ICI-ass settings

sample

settings

can bc assessed. These recordings do need to client encounter\ routinely during activities of daily living as well as those reported to bc ;I problem. or suggc\ted as problematic by the S-24 scale. in order to increase the representativeness of the samples assessed. I:inally, comparisons samples recorded at regular intervals over time permit assessment of the permanence of “treatment effects” or the durability ofa client’s “speech skills.” From that

;I

of

one pcr\pectivc.

increases

uralness

indicate

would

other.

a need for

about

the efficacy

in stuttering ;I relapse

further

and/or

in

“speech

skill”

of the treatment

training.

employed

in speech nat-

decreases

“treatment

effects”:

Of

from

course.

the

inferences

can be drawn

from

either

perspective. The

assessment

about lized will

not provide

fails who

speech

singly

during

for

current

[ 1992]).

when

(e.g..

Prins which

sufficient,

were

observed

treatment.

This

kind

of information.

studies

could

that

procedures.

in isolation

procedures

found

be

tcstcd

in

or even

in the client’s which

treatments

be obtained

would

for

through

evaluate

the

to be most

a series

effects

effective

is

chronic

and combination.

appropriately

or

for adults

known

that

design

OLII- undcr-

compare

not

are necessary.

uti-

strategy

it works

treatments

and it is

of current

were

this

to expand

succeeds.

of procedures

arc to be improved,

could

needed

most

if the cft’icacy

of those

studies

example.

information that

the changes

and after

individual

the generality subject

For

provides

procedures Unfortunately.

;I treatment

or in combination.

single-subject

treatment’s

stutters.

of information why

significance

stuttering

who

described

treatment

and Andrews

responsible

of critical

was

of a variety

Neilson

procedures.

adult

not.

consist

and

partly

the kind

it does

stutter

[ IYE]

client

of the reasons

when

that

of the specific

\vith a specific

standing

of

strategy

the cf’ficacy

of ;I Then.

in singlc-

controlled

group

studies. Treatment

could

also

stuttering tering skill\

failures.

which

provide

crucial

therapies.

When

and increasing practiced

treatment

speech

in therapy

have

reccivcd

information

for

little

systematic

improving

is not effective

naturalness.

when

OCCLKS incon\istcntly.

in reducing

transfer when

study,

the cff‘icacy

of

stut-

of the speech there

are post-

328

R. F. CURLEE

treatment relapses in speaking performance, it is not known if the fault lies with the treatment procedures, with a clinician’s application of those procedures, with the level of speech skill achieved by the client, with the client’s practice habits or motivation, or with some other variable(s). Initial research in this area could employ detailed case studies to help identify variables that might be contributing to treatment failures of specific clients that would then be followed by single-subject studies that modified those variables identified in order to confirm or rule-out their significance. Alternatively, if stuttering events that are perceived by listeners are only an incomplete, perhaps unrepresentative subset of the population of speech production events of those who stutter, further insight into treatment failures might be obtained from observations that focus on the impairment level of stuttering. It is possible, for example, that the neuropsychological and neurophysiological events that precede and accompany the audible and visible speech events of stuttering may affect the extent to which speech skills acquired in treatment will be used outside of treatment, the extent to which such speech skills are performed automatically, and/or the extent to which a stuttering disability, and its handicaps, will be resolved permanently. Systematic assessments of the efficacy of the treatments utilized by a profession are essential to the maintenance of the clinical integrity of any profession. Few disorders pose greater challenges to the assessment of treatment efficacy than does stuttering. Nevertheless, it is of critical importance that a standard set of procedures be adopted for evaluating the treatment outcomes of adults who stutter so that significant variations in such outcomes, from one procedure or clinic to another, can be reliably identified. In addition, it is similarly important that systematic studies be initiated to identify the specific therapy procedures that contribute the most to successful treatment outcomes and the variables that are largely responsible for treatment failures. Otherwise, it is unlikely that our treatment of adults who stutter will improve substantially in the foreseeable future. ACKNOWLEDGMENT Preparation of this article was supported in part by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development NIH grant 2 R44 HD2620902A1.

REFERENCES Adams, M., and Runyan, C. (1981) Stuttering and fluency: Exclusive events or points on a continuum? Journal of Fluency Disorders, 6, 197-218.

ASSESSMENT

329

OF DISARIL,ITY

Alphonse, P. The effect of speech motor training on stutterers’ physiology. In: Spcrc,ll Motor Control ctnd Strrttcrin,q (Peters, Hulstijn, W.. eds.). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, 1991.

speech H. and

Cutler, J. (1974) Stuttering therapy: The relation in symptom level and attitude, JorrrttnI c?f‘Spc~c~ch B Hcctritlg Disorders. 39. 3 13-3 IY.

Andrews, between

G.. and changes

Cordes, A., Ingham, R.. Frank. P.. and Ingham. J. ( IYY2) Time-interval analysis of interjudgc and intra.judge agreement for stuttering event -judgments. Jo~trnrrl of’ Sprcch & Hctrrin,q Rcscrtrch. 35. 483-4Y4. Costello,

J.. and Ingham.

R. Assessment

strategies for stuttering. Nrrtttw rrttd Twrrttnrrtt o/’ Sfrrttering (Curlee, R., and Perkins, cds.). San Diego: College-Hill, lYX4, pp. 303-334.

In: W.,

Craig. A., and Calver, Studies of perception

P. (1941) Following up on treated stutterers: of fluency and job status. Jotrrnrrl c?f‘Spccch & Hcnritt,q Rcscrr~~~l~. 34, 27Y-284.

Craig. A., Franklin, J.. and Andrews. G. (lYX4) A scale to measure locus of control of behaviour. British Jortrtlctl c?f’Mctiictrl Psyc~hology, 57. 173-180. Coyle, M.. and Mallard, A.R. (1979) Word-by-word analysis of observer agreement utilizing audio and audiovisual techniques. Jortrnul c?f’Flrtc~ncy Disorders,

Curlee,

R. (1981) Observer

Jottrnul

Freeman,

4. 23-28.

agreement

on disfluency

F. Laryngeal

muscle

activity of stutterers. R., and Perkins,

Trc~uttncnt of‘ Strrtfcrincq (Curlee,

Diego:

and

stuttering.

of’Spc~c~ch & Hrrrring Resctrrc~lt, 24. 595-600.

College-Hill,

In: Nrlttrrc and W., eds.). San

1984, pp. 105-l 16.

Cow. M.. and Ingham, R. ( 1992) Modifying fied intervals of phonation: The effect Spccc/~

&

Hccrring Research,

electroglottograph-idention stuttering. Jorrrnul of’ 35, 495-5 11.

lngham, R., and Andrews. G. (1973) Behavior therapy and stuttering: A review. Journal of.5pccc.h & HrarinR Disorders, 38, 405-44 I. Kully. D.. and Boberg, E. (1988) An investigation of inter-clinic agreement in the identification of fluent and stuttered syllables. Journal of' Flrrcttcy Disorders, 13, 309-3 1X.

R. F. CURLEE

330

MacDonald, reliable Hearing

J., and Martin,

R. (1973) Stuttering

and unambiguous response Research, 16, 691-699.

Martin, R., and Haroldson, definitions and moment Research, 24, 59-63. Martin, R., Haroldson, naturalness. Journul

classes.

and disfluency Journal

as two

of Speech

&

S. (198 I) Stuttering identification: Standard of stuttering. Journal of Speech & Hearing

S., and Triden, K. (1984) Stuttering and speech of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 49, 53-58.

Neilson, M., and Andrews, G. Intensive fluency stutterers. In: Stuttering and Related Disorders R., ed.). New York: Thieme Medical Publishers,

training of chronic of Fluency (Curlee, 1992, pp. 139-164.

Onslow, M., van Doorn, J., and Newman, D. (1992) Variability of acoustic segment durations after prolonged-speech treatment for stuttering. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 35, 529-536. Perkins, W. (1990) What is stuttering? Disorders, 55, 370-382.

Journal

qf Speech

& Heuring

Prins, D. Theories of stuttering as event and disorder: Implications for speech production processes. In: Speech Motor Control and Stuttering (Peters, H., Hulstijn, W., and Starkweather, C.W., eds.). Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1991, pp. 571-580. Prins, D. Management of stuttering: Treatment of adolescents and adults. In: Stuttering and Related Disorders of Fluency (Curlee, R., ed.). New York: Thieme Medical Publishers, 1992, pp. 115-138. Riley, G. (1974) A stuttering severity instrument for children Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 27, 314-322. Smith, A. (1990) Toward a comprehensive mentary. Journal of Speech 6; Hearing

and adults.

theory of stuttering: A comDisorders, 55, 398-401.

Watson, J. (1988) A comparison of stutterers’ and nonstutterers’ affective, cognitive and behavioral self-reports. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 3 1, 377-385. Wingate, Speech Woolf,

M. (1964) A standard and Hearing Disorders,

G. (1967) The assessment

definition of stuttering. 29, 484-489. of stuttering

as struggle,

Journal

avoidance

of

ASSESSMENT OF DISABILITY

Wood.

F.,

Stump.

D..

McKeehan.

( 1980) Patterns ofrcgional ing aloud by stutterer\ dence for inadequate &

LA,I,qutrgc~,

Rcccived

9.

33I

A..

S.. and Proctor.

both on and oft‘ haloperidol left frontal

activation

141-144.

and accepted

Sheldon,

cerebral blood tlow during attempted

January

1993.

medication:

during stuttering.

J.

rcadEviHrtrirl