Accepted Manuscript Explicit and implicit attitude toward an emerging food technology: The case of cultured meat Gerben A. Bekker, Hilde Arnout R.H. Fischer, Hilde Tobi, Hans C.M. van Trijp PII:
S0195-6663(16)30503-7
DOI:
10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.002
Reference:
APPET 3175
To appear in:
Appetite
Received Date: 3 June 2016 Revised Date:
20 September 2016
Accepted Date: 2 October 2016
Please cite this article as: Bekker G.A., Fischer H.A.R.H., Tobi H. & van Trijp H.C.M., Explicit and implicit attitude toward an emerging food technology: The case of cultured meat, Appetite (2016), doi: 10.1016/ j.appet.2016.10.002. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1
Explicit and implicit attitude toward an emerging food technology: The case of cultured meat.
2
Gerben A. Bekker a, b, Hilde Arnout R. H. Fischer a, *, Hilde Tobi b, Hans C. M. van Trijp a
3 4 a
Marketing and Consumer Behaviour Group, Social Sciences, Wageningen University & Research
RI PT
5 6
Address: Hollandseweg 1, 6706KN Wageningen, The Netherlands
7
Postal address: Messenger number 87, PO Box 8130, 6700EW Wageningen, The Netherlands
8
E-mail addresses:
[email protected];
[email protected];
[email protected]
10
b
SC
9
Biometris, Wageningen University & Research
Address: Hollandseweg 1, 6706KN Wageningen, The Netherlands
12
Postal address: Messenger number 68, PO Box 8130, 6700EW Wageningen, The Netherlands
13
E-mail address:
[email protected]
14
TE D
17
E-mail address:
[email protected] (A. R. H. Fischer)
EP
16
* Corresponding author
AC C
15
M AN U
11
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Abstract
18 19
Cultured meat is an unfamiliar emerging food technology that could provide a near endless supply of high quality protein with a relatively small ecological footprint. To
21
understand consumer acceptance of cultured meat, this study investigated the influence of
22
information provision on the explicit and implicit attitude toward cultured meat. Three
23
experiments were conducted using a Solomon four-group design to rule out pretest
24
sensitization effects. The first experiment (N = 190) showed that positive or negative
25
information about cultured meat changed the explicit attitude in the direction of the
26
information. This effect was smaller for participants who were more familiar with cultured
27
meat. In the second experiment (N = 194) positive information was provided about solar
28
panels, an attitude object belonging to the same sustainable product category as sustainable
29
food products such as cultured meat. Positive information about solar panels was found to
30
change the explicit attitude in the direction of the information. Using mood induction, the third
31
experiment (N = 192) ruled out the alternative explanation that explicit attitude change in
32
experiment 1 and 2 was caused by content free affect rather than category based inferences.
33
The implicit attitude appeared insensitive to both information or mood state in all three
34
experiments. These findings show that the explicit attitude toward cultured meat can be
35
influenced by information about the sustainability of cultured meat and information about a
36
positively perceived sustainable product. This effect was shown to be content based rather
37
than merely affect based. Content based information in a relevant context could therefore
38
contribute to the commercial success of cultured meat.
40
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
39
RI PT
20
Keywords: Cultured meat, Explicit attitude, Implicit attitude, Attitude change,
Information provision, Mood.
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Introduction
42
The basic idea of cultured meat is that animal meat is grown using a bioreactor
43
instead of an animal (Edelman, McFarland, Mironov, & Matheny, 2005; Tuomisto & de
44
Mattos, 2011; van der Weele & Tramper, 2014). Cultured meat is an emerging technology
45
that can contribute considerably to the growing need for more high quality protein at lower
46
environmental costs than conventional meat (see for example, Boland et al., 2013). Provided
47
the technology becomes successful, near endless supplies of cultured meat may be
48
produced with a relatively small ecological footprint (Mattick, Landis, Allenby, & Genovese,
49
2015; Tuomisto & de Mattos, 2011). Cultured meat, then, could (partly) replace the
50
conventional meat production with its large ecological footprint (Fiala, 2008; Steinfeld et al.,
51
2006). The success of cultured meat will depend to a large extent on consumer attitudes
52
toward the product (Datar & Betti, 2010), because consumers’ attitudes influence their
53
product choices (Armitage & Conner, 2001).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
41
Attitudes are psychological constructs that in a broad sense consist of the evaluation
54
of an object (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). A person can either retrieve a stored evaluation, or
56
construct an evaluation through cognitive elaboration of relevant information (Fazio, 2007;
57
Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). When a stored evaluation is retrieved this is often
58
automatic and without reasoning. Automatically retrieved evaluations without reasoning are
59
called implicit attitudes (Fazio, 2007; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). An evaluation
60
constructed through cognitive elaboration of available information, including that provided by
61
implicit associations, are expressed as an explicit attitude (Gawronski & Bodenhausen,
62
2006).
EP
AC C
63
TE D
55
Explicit attitudes have primarily been measured using self-report scales (Hendrick,
64
Fischer, Tobi, & Frewer, 2013). Self-report scales typically report explicit attitudes, because
65
filling out the scales requires cognitive elaboration (Bohner & Dickel, 2011; Eagly & Chaiken,
66
2007; Gawronski, 2007; Greenwald & Nosek, 2008). Implicit attitudes on the other hand, are
67
measured with response time based measurement methods (Gawronski, 2007). The time
68
required to connect an attitude object to an evaluation, indicates how closely the attitude 3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 69
object and the evaluation are implicitly associated (Hofmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le,
70
& Schmitt, 2005; Wittenbrink & Schwarz, 2007).
71
Implicit and explicit attitudes play different roles in decision making (Ayres, Conner, Prestwich, & Smith, 2012; Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009; Perugini, 2005;
73
Richetin, Perugini, Adjali, & Hurling, 2007). Implicit attitudes are more predictive for
74
spontaneous behavior, whereas explicit attitudes are more predictive for deliberate behavior
75
(Perugini, 2005). For well-known objects that are unambiguously positive (or negative), the
76
automatically activated implicit attitudes and the more elaborately reasoned explicit attitudes
77
are likely to be similar (Greenwald et al., 2009). For objects that have both positive and
78
negative associations, implicit and explicit attitudes may differ. For example, racial or gender
79
stereotyping research consistently finds that implicit attitude measures show a stereotypical
80
response, while explicit attitudes do not (Cunningham, Nezlek, & Banaji, 2004; Kawakami &
81
Dovidio, 2001; Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 1997).
SC
M AN U
82
RI PT
72
In contrast with well-known objects, unfamiliar attitude objects, such as cultured meat, are not related to well-developed stored attitudes (Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes,
84
1986; Fazio, 2007). The absence of a well-developed stored attitude toward an unfamiliar
85
attitude object requires people to construct an explicit attitude on the spot, based on
86
whatever information is provided. Provided information together with knowledge people have
87
about the unfamiliar object allows them to create an attitude based on cognitive elaboration
88
(Achterberg, 2014; Lusk et al., 2004; McComas, Besley, & Steinhardt, 2014).
EP
AC C
89
TE D
83
The implicit attitude toward an unfamiliar attitude object is likely based on the most
90
accessible associations, which are activated in response to the unfamiliar attitude object
91
(Wyer, 2008). If the unfamiliar attitude object involves unnatural, or immoral interventions, an
92
automatic disgust response, or a so-called ‘yuck’ response, is to be expected (Haidt, 2001;
93
Schnall, Haidt, Clore, & Jordan, 2008). Previous research has argued for the existence of
94
such a ‘yuck’ factor for cultured meat (Pluhar, 2010) and has found this response in interview
95
studies (van der Weele & Driessen, 2013; Verbeke, Marcu, et al., 2015). In spite of the claim
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 96
that the ‘yuck’ factor is largely automatically activated, no study on cultured meat used
97
measurement methods aimed at measuring automatically activated responses.
98 99
Even without any information, people seem able to make sense of unfamiliar attitude objects. In order to make sense of an unfamiliar attitude object people must access some existing knowledge in their memory. This existing knowledge likely originates from an
101
existing category of objects similar to the unfamiliar attitude object. The provision of
102
information can facilitate the connection between the unfamiliar object and the existing
103
category, enabling people to make sense of the unfamiliar object (Gentner, 1988; Gregan-
104
Paxton & Moreau, 2003). As the provided information adds on existing knowledge, the effect
105
of new information will be smaller when more information is already stored in memory.
SC
RI PT
100
Once the unfamiliar object is related to an existing category, implicit and explicit
107
attitudes toward the unfamiliar object are inferred from the attitude toward the category
108
prototype (Kardes, Posavac, & Cronley, 2004; Ranganath & Nosek, 2008; Ratliff, Swinkels,
109
Klerx, & Nosek, 2012). These inferences can be based on content or content free affect
110
(Fiske & Pavelchak, 1986).
TE D
111
M AN U
106
Depending on the information the explicit and implicit attitude toward an unfamiliar attitude object can relate in different ways. Information about an unfamiliar attitude object can
113
provide new knowledge that adds propositions about the unfamiliar object, which become
114
apparent in the explicit attitude toward that object (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). This
115
results in the following hypotheses:
117 118 119 120
AC C
116
EP
112
H1a: Information related to the unfamiliar attitude object will change the explicit attitude in the direction of the valence of the information. H1b: The effect of information provision on explicit attitude change is smaller for people who are more familiar with the unfamiliar attitude object.
121 122 123
Information may also support categorization of the unfamiliar attitude object into an existing category of similar objects. This categorization can activate category based implicit 5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 124
associations. These implicit associations may inform the explicit attitude toward the
125
unfamiliar attitude object and thereby increase the relation between the explicit and implicit
126
attitude (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). This will be explored in the current study.
127
To investigate the effect of information provision on the explicit and implicit attitude toward an unfamiliar attitude object, the attitudes before and after information provision need
129
to be compared. The pretest measurement may however, influence participants’ sensitivity to
130
experimental stimuli and thereby influence the outcome of the posttest measurement
131
(Campbell & Stanley, 1966). This pretest sensitization effect has been illustrated in various
132
fields within the social sciences (Willson & Putnam, 1982). Awareness of the pretest
133
sensitization effect may be especially relevant when measuring attitudes toward attitude
134
objects that are unfamiliar to participants, because participants in the pretest are made
135
aware of their limited knowledge about the attitude object making them extra motivated to
136
pay attention to new information. In order to rule out pretest sensitization effects, a Solomon
137
four-group design can be used, which makes it possible to compare the effect of information
138
between participants that did or did not have a pretest (Solomon, 1949).
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
128
139
Experiment 1
140
In experiment 1, we explored the effect that positive or negative information about
EP
141
cultured meat had on the explicit attitude toward cultured meat, by providing new knowledge
143
that should add to the existing limited knowledge structure of cultured meat. Most
144
participants were unfamiliar with cultured meat because at the time of the experiment no
145
products had become commercially available on the consumer market and because cultured
146
meat had only limited press coverage. This made cultured meat an unfamiliar attitude object
147
with a limited knowledge structure.
AC C
142
148
Material and methods
149 150
Participants and Design
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 151
A Solomon four-group design, with an experimental 2 (positive versus negative information about cultured meat) x 2 (pretest versus no-pretest) between subjects design
153
was conducted. For each individual, the implicit and explicit attitudes toward cultured meat
154
were measured. Participants were Wageningen University students who spoke fluent Dutch.
155
They received a two euro university cafeteria voucher. Data of 203 participants were
156
collected in the fall of 2012. Thirteen participants were excluded from the sample, because
157
they did not meet the response time requirements for the implicit attitude measurement
158
method. The final sample consisted of 190 participants (54 male and 136 female), age
159
between 17 and 28 years with a median of 21.
162
Materials and manipulations
SC
161
M AN U
160
RI PT
152
The experiment was conducted in individual cubicles. Displays were 17.3 inch with a resolution of 1600 x 900 pixels and a screen refresh rate of 60 Hz from 2.4 GHz dual core
164
laptops with external keyboard and mouse. The experiment was programmed in Inquisit
165
3.0.6. (Inquisit, 2011).
TE D
163
Fictitious scenarios with positive or negative information about cultured meat were
167
created. A pilot (N = 20) showed that the positive and negative scenario were equally difficult
168
to understand and equally credible. After minor textual adjustments the information was:
169
EP
166
“Cultured meat is meat that is grown from stem cells and produced in a laboratory. It
171
has certain important [dis]advantages in comparison to regular meat.
172
Cultured meat is good [bad] for the environment because greenhouse gas emissions
173
and energy use is low [high]. It is [un]pleasant to know that by eating cultured meat you
174
are doing something good [bad] for the environment. In addition, a test panel has found
175
the taste of cultured meat to be better [worse] than that of regular meat. It is nice
176
[gross] to eat cultured meat instead of regular meat. Finally, for the production of
177
cultured meat less [more] antibiotics and growth enhancing substances are required
178
compared to regular meat. This fortunately [unfortunately] results in the consumption of
AC C
170
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 179
less [more] artificial substances.” (Positive information condition, with the negative
180
alternatives in square brackets. Translated from Dutch).
181
183
Measures Explicit attitude measures. The explicit attitude was measured using 19 items on a
RI PT
182
7-point semantic differential scale (adapted from Crites, Fabrigar, & Petty, 1994). Items were
185
presented in a random order. The explicit attitude score (Cronbach’s α = .95) was calculated
186
by averaging three subscales with respectively eight affective (Cronbach’s α = .92), seven
187
cognitive (Cronbach’s α = .92) and four general items (Cronbach’s α = .93). Split-half
188
reliability, the average of five different splits using the Spearman-Brown formula (Walker &
189
Lev, 1953), was .96 in both the pretest and posttest.
M AN U
190
SC
184
Implicit attitude measures. The Single Target Implicit Association Test (ST-IAT; see de Liver, van der Pligt, & Wigboldus, 2007; Friese, Bluemke, & Wanke, 2007), a response
192
time based measurement method, was used to measure implicit attitude. The ST-IAT has
193
been shown to be valid for measuring implicit attitudes toward single attitude objects (Bar-
194
Anan & Nosek, 2014; Bluemke & Friese, 2008).
195
TE D
191
The ST-IAT consisted of four blocks. Block 1 and 3 were practice blocks, block 2 and 4 were test blocks. In each block positive or negative words had to be categorized with the ‘a’
197
key and words with the opposite valence with the “5” key on the numeric pad. Words
198
representing cultured meat had to be categorized with the same key as the positive or
199
negative words in block 1 and 2, and with the same key as words of the opposite valence in
200
block 3 and 4. Cultured meat words assigned to the same key as positive words formed a
201
positive practice and test block and cultured meat words assigned to the same key as
202
negative words formed a negative practice and test block. Key assignment of the positive
203
and negative words, and cultured meat words were counterbalanced. A pilot study with
204
participants from the same population (N = 23) showed that the words selected to represent
205
cultured meat were associated with cultured meat (see Appendix A for the words).
AC C
EP
196
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The practice blocks consisted of 24 words (7 cultured meat, 7 positive or negative
207
words assigned to one key, and 10 oppositely valenced words assigned to the other key).
208
The test blocks consisted of 48 words (14 cultured meat, 14 valenced words, and 20
209
oppositely valenced words) (following the procedure of Karpinski & Steinman, 2006). Within
210
a block, words were presented in random order (see Fig. 1 for schematic example).
RI PT
206
211
“A” “5” 7 Positive 10 Negative 7 Cultured meat
M AN U
Positive test block
SC
Positive practice block
“A” “5” 14 Positive 20 Negative 14 Cultured meat
Negative practice block
Negative test block “A” 20 Positive
“5” 14 Negative 14 Cultured meat
AC C
212
“5” 7 Negative 7 Cultured meat
EP
time
TE D
“A” 10 positive
213
Fig. 1. Schematic example of an ST-IAT consisting of four consecutive blocks, with key
214
assignment and number of words that need to be categorized.
215 216
The implicit attitude was operationalized as the D score and calculated by dividing the
217
difference of the mean response latencies between the positive and negative test block by
218
the standard deviation of all test block response latencies. Response latency was measured
219
as the time it took to correctly categorize a word from the first moment it was presented.
220
Individual trials with response latencies greater than 10,000 milliseconds were removed 9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 221
(following Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003). Participants who had a response latency
222
smaller than 300 milliseconds in more than 10% of their trials were excluded from the sample
223
(following Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003). Split-half reliability, using the Spearman-
224
Brown formula, was .57 in the pretest and .72 in the posttest. Demographic and background measures. Self-reported familiarity with cultured
RI PT
225 226
meat was measured by the item “prior to this study, to what extent were you familiar with
227
cultured meat” with the options unfamiliar, a little bit familiar and familiar.
228
230
Procedure
SC
229
Before the start of the experiment, participants were told they would participate in a study testing their speed and accuracy to categorize a variety of words and that it did not
232
matter whether they knew about cultured meat. After electronic confirmation of consent
233
participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions using block randomization.
234
M AN U
231
Participants in the pretest condition started with the ST-IAT followed by the explicit attitude measurement method. Afterwards they received positive or negative information
236
about cultured meat. Participants assigned to the no-pretest condition started the experiment
237
with the information. All participants were instructed to read the information carefully. To
238
ensure minimum reading time it was only possible to continue with the ST-IAT after 30
239
seconds. Following the ST-IAT, the explicit attitude was measured.
241 242 243 244
EP
Next, demographic and background information was collected, participants were
AC C
240
TE D
235
debriefed, received their reward and were thanked for participation.
Statistical analyses
Effects of information provision, presence of a pretest, and familiarity with cultured
245
meat on posttest explicit and implicit attitude scores were analyzed in two ways. The first
246
analysis, using a general linear model (GLM), included posttest explicit and implicit attitude
247
scores as dependent variables, information condition and pretest condition as factor and
248
familiarity as a covariate. All main effects, and the two-way interactions between information 10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT condition and pretest condition, and between information condition and familiarity were
250
included. This analysis provided the possibility to observe a possible pretest sensitization
251
effect. The second analysis, using a repeated measures GLM, focused on explicit and
252
implicit attitude change between the pretest and posttest for those participants who were
253
assigned to the pretest condition.
254
Results
255 256
RI PT
249
Prior to information, participants had a negative implicit attitude score toward cultured meat, M = -0.18, SD 0.32, t(96) = -5.58, p <.001, and an explicit attitude score around the
258
central scale point, M = 3.99, SD = 1.10 (see Table 1, for mean implicit and explicit attitude
259
scores by level of familiarity with cultured meat). Pretest and posttest explicit attitude scores
260
were correlated, r (95) = .543, p < .001, as were pretest and posttest implicit attitude scores,
261
r (95) = .205, p = .044. Pretest explicit and implicit attitude scores were not correlated in the
262
positive information condition, r(46) = -.050, p = .738, or the negative information condition,
263
r(47) = .200, p = .168. The correlation between the posttest explicit and implicit attitude
264
scores was found to be significant in the positive information condition, r (92) = .343, p =
265
.001, but not in the negative information condition, r (94) = .190, p = .064.
TE D
M AN U
SC
257
Hypothesis testing using posttest data of all participants. Positive and negative
267
information had an effect on posttest explicit attitude score, F(1, 184) = 27.03, p <.001, η2 =
268
.128, in the direction of the information (see Fig. 2), which supports hypothesis 1a (mean
269
positive information = 4.45 and mean negative information = 3.46). This was not found for
270
posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 184) = 3.73, p = .055, η2 = .020 (see Fig. 2) (mean
271
positive = -.11 and mean negative = -.20). Familiarity had an effect on posttest explicit
272
attitude score, F(1, 184) = 11.10, p = .001, η2 = .057, participants who reported to be more
273
familiar with cultured meat had a more positive posttest explicit attitude score, r (188) = .177,
274
p = .014 (see Table 1). Familiarity had no effect on posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 184)
275
= 2.47, p = .118, η2 = .013. The interaction between familiarity and information on posttest
276
explicit attitude score, F(1, 184) = 9.16, p = .003, η2 = .047, indicated that information had
AC C
EP
266
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT less effect on posttest explicit attitude score when participants were more familiar with
278
cultured meat, which is in line with hypothesis 1b. As a consequence, information had a
279
significant effect for participants who reported to be unfamiliar or a little bit familiar, but not for
280
participants who reported to be familiar with cultured meat (see Table 1). A similar pattern
281
was not found for posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 184) = 0.47, p = .492, η2 = .003. No
282
differences were found between participants with and without pretest on posttest explicit
283
attitude score, F(1, 184) = 0.16, p = .690, η2 = .001, or posttest implicit attitude score, F(1,
284
184) = 0.13, p = .715, η2 = .001, nor was the interaction between information and pretest
285
significant for the posttest explicit attitude score, F(1, 184) = 1.54, p = .217, η2 = .008, or
286
posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 184) = 0.85, p = .358, η2 = .005.
SC
Hypothesis testing using data of participants with both a pretest and a posttest.
M AN U
287
RI PT
277
No difference was found between pretest and posttest explicit attitude scores, F(1, 93) =
289
0.01, p = .970, η2 < .001, or between pretest and posttest implicit attitude scores, F(1, 93) =
290
1.60, p = .209, η2 = .017. The significant interaction between pretest-posttest difference and
291
information condition, F(1, 93) = 51.31, p < .001, η2 = .356, indicated that explicit attitude
292
score changed in the direction of the information (see Fig. 2) (mean difference positive
293
information = 0.76 and mean difference negative information = - 0.72), which provides further
294
support for hypothesis 1a. No interaction between pretest-posttest difference and information
295
condition was found for the implicit attitude score, F(1, 93) = 0.24, p = .625, η2 = .003 (see
296
Fig. 2). There was no interaction between pretest-posttest difference and familiarity for
297
explicit attitude score, F(1, 93) = 0.23, p = .635, η2 = .002, or implicit attitude score, F(1, 93) =
298
2.61, p = .110, η2 = .027. The three-way interaction between pretest-posttest difference,
299
information condition and familiarity on explicit attitude score change, F(1, 93) = 13.39, p <
300
.001, η2 = .126, indicated that attitude change in the direction of the information was smaller
301
for participants who were more familiar with cultured meat (see Table 1), which provides
302
further support for hypothesis 1b. This effect was not found for implicit attitude score, F(1, 93)
303
= 0.02, p = .881, η2 < .001.
AC C
EP
TE D
288
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
RI PT
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of explicit attitude score (1 = negative; 7 = positive) and implicit attitudes score (0 = neutral) by familiarity with cultured meat in experiment 1. Pretest Posttest A little bit A little bit Unfamiliar familiar Familiar Unfamiliar familiar Familiar Condition N M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
3.69 0.93
49 47
3.87 1.01
4.05 1.12
48 -0.21 0.33 46
-0.17 0.34
TE D -0.28 0.32
EP
49 -0.04 0.29 47
4.74 1.50
4.93 1.28 4.05 1.07
4.33 0.95 4.57 0.90
4.85 1.52 4.21 0.35
2.52 1.03 2.90 0.83 Implicit attitude score
3.61 1.05 3.76 0.87
4.48 1.57 3.41 1.90
M AN U
3.67 0.89
AC C
Positive information cultured meat Pretest No-pretest Negative information cultured meat Pretest No-pretest
48 46
SC
Explicit attitude score Positive information cultured meat Pretest No-pretest Negative information cultured meat Pretest No-pretest
13
5.14 1.15
-0.14 0.13
-0.23 0.41 -0.21 0.32
-0.08 0.44 0.05 0.36
0.05 0.39 -0.54 0.30
-0.05 0.36
-0.23 0.41 -0.23 0.39
-0.16 0.33 -0.23 0.30
-0.06 0.52 -0.31 0.32
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 304
Positive information cultured meat (pretest condition) Negative information cultured meat (pretest condition)
6.0
Positive information cultured meat (no-pretest condition) Negative information cultured meat (no-pretest condition)
5.0
RI PT
4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 Pretest
Posttest
Measurement
Positive information cultured meat (pretest condition)
0.4
Negative information cultured meat (pretest condition)
0.3
Positive information cultured meat (no-pretest condition)
0.2
Negative information cultured meat (no-pretest condition)
0.0 -0.1 -0.2 -0.3 -0.4 -0.5
TE D
0.1
EP
Implicit attitude score (95% CI)
0.5
M AN U
a)
Pretest
Posttest Measurement
AC C
b)
SC
Explicit attitude score (95% CI)
7.0
Fig. 2. Mean explicit (panel a) and implicit (panel b) attitude score in the pretest and posttest of experiment 1. Error bars indicate the 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) 305 306
Discussion
307
In support of hypothesis 1a, positive or negative information provided about cultured
308
meat influenced the explicit attitude toward cultured meat in the direction of the information.
309
Participants who were more familiar with cultured meat reported a more positive explicit 14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT attitude toward cultured meat. The effect of information provision on the explicit attitude
311
toward cultured meat was smaller for people who were more familiar with cultured meat,
312
which confirms hypothesis 1b. As the observed average explicit attitude score toward
313
cultured meat was based on a sample of university students, the average explicit attitude
314
score should not be generalized to the general population. No difference was found in
315
posttest attitude scores between participants in the pretest and no-pretest condition. The
316
observed effect of information and familiarity on explicit attitude was not found for implicit
317
attitude. The increased correlation between explicit and implicit attitude after information
318
provision in the positive information condition suggests that the explicit and implicit attitude
319
converged after receiving positive information. It appears that information about the
320
unfamiliar attitude object cultured meat added knowledge to the existing knowledge structure
321
and that it also activated implicit associations. The current experiment cannot distinguish
322
whether it was provided information about cultured meat or activated associations that
323
informed the explicit attitude toward cultured meat. This will be investigated in experiment 2.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
310
325 326
TE D
324
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 partly replicated and extended experiment 1. The effect of positive information from experiment 1 was replicated. In addition, it was investigated whether the
328
explicit attitude toward cultured meat could be influenced by providing positive information
329
about an object remotely related to culture meat. We investigated whether information about
330
solar panels, an attitude object belonging to the sustainable product category (Hobman &
331
Ashworth, 2013), a category that also includes sustainable food products such as cultured
332
meat (Verbeke, Sans, & Van Loo, 2015), influences the attitude toward cultured meat. The
333
activation of the sustainable product category is expected to facilitate a connection between
334
the sustainable food product cultured meat and the sustainable product category. This
335
connection would enable category based inferences for the unfamiliar attitude object cultured
336
meat. We replicated the positive information condition as positive information about solar
337
panels was expected to be more credible than negative information.
AC C
EP
327
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 338
Material and methods
339 340 341
Participants and design A Solomon four group design, with an experimental 2 (positive information about cultured meat versus positive information about solar panels) x 2 (pretest versus no-pretest)
343
between subjects design was conducted in the fall of 2012. Participants were 204 Dutch
344
speaking Wageningen University students. They received two euro as reward. Seven
345
participants were excluded because they did not meet the response time requirements for
346
the implicit attitude measurement method and three participants were excluded because they
347
did not finish the experiment, leaving 194 participants (71 males and 123 females). Age
348
ranged from 17 to 28 years with a median of 20.
350
Materials, measures and procedure
M AN U
349
SC
RI PT
342
Materials, measures and procedures were equal to experiment 1 with the exception of
352
the information. Positive information about cultured meat and positive information about solar
353
energy were developed and piloted (N = 17). After some minor improvements a second pilot
354
(N = 5) showed that both information scenarios were equally positive, easy to understand
355
and trustworthy. After some final adjustments the information was:
EP
TE D
351
356
358 359 360
“Cultured meat produced in laboratories [electricity from solar panels on rooftops] has
AC C
357
important advantages in comparison to meat from cattle [electricity from coal power plants].
Recent scientific research (1) estimates that the whole production process of cultured
361
meat produced in laboratories [electricity from solar panels on rooftops], in
362
comparison with the production of meat from cattle [electricity from coal power plants],
363
leads to more than 90% reduction in greenhouse gas emissions. In addition, land and
364
water usage is estimated to be more than 95% lower.
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT (1) Smith. M. J., & Moore, R. W. (2010). Environmental influences of cultured meat
366
[solar energy] production. Journal of Environment and Technology, 51(13), 4219-
367
4227.”
368
(Cultured meat information condition, with the solar panel alternatives between
369
square brackets. Translated from Dutch. The reference is fictitious).
370
Results
371 372
RI PT
365
In the pretest, participants showed a negative implicit attitude toward cultured meat, M = -0.20, SD = 0.33, t(93) = -5.73, p < .001, and an explicit attitude score around the central
374
scale point, M = 4.18, SD = 1.30 (see Table 2, for mean implicit and explicit attitude scores
375
by level of familiarity with cultured meat). Pretest and posttest measures of explicit attitude
376
were correlated, r(92) = .834, p < .001, as were the pretest and posttest implicit attitude
377
measures, r (92) = .412, p < .001. Pretest explicit and implicit attitude scores were not
378
significantly correlated in the cultured meat information condition, r(45) = .051, p = .734, but
379
were significantly correlated in the solar panel information condition, r(46) = .292, p = .044.
380
Comparable to the pretest, the posttest explicit and implicit attitude scores were not
381
significantly correlated in the cultured meat information condition, r (95) = .066, p = .521, but
382
were in the solar panel information condition, r (97) = .335, p = .001.
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
373
Hypothesis testing using posttest data of all participants. No significant
384
difference was found between posttest explicit attitude scores for the directly and indirectly
385
related information condition, F(1, 188) = 0.29, p = .592, η2 = .002 (see Fig. 3), or for posttest
386
implicit attitude scores, F(1, 188) = 0.43, p = .511, η2 = .002 (see Fig. 3). Familiarity had an
387
effect on the posttest explicit attitude score, F(1, 188) = 32.11, p < .001, η2 = .146,
388
participants who were more familiar with cultured meat had a more positive explicit attitude
389
toward cultured meat (see Table 2). This was not found for posttest implicit attitude score,
390
F(1, 188) = 0.14, p = .709, η2 = .001. There was no significant interaction between familiarity
391
and information condition on posttest explicit attitude score, F(1, 188) = 0.42, p = .517, η2 =
392
.002, or posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 188) = 2.42, p = .121, η2 = .013. No difference
AC C
383
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT was found between participants with and without a pretest for posttest explicit attitude, F(1,
394
188) = 1.61, p = .207, η2 = .008, or posttest implicit attitude, F(1, 188) = 0.02, p = .898, η2 <
395
.001. There was no significant interaction between information condition and the presence or
396
absence of a pretest on posttest explicit attitude score, F(1, 188) = 1.25, p = .264, η2 = .007,
397
or posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 188) = 0.07, p = .792, η2 < .001.
398
RI PT
393
Hypothesis testing using data of participants with both a pretest and a posttest. We continued by comparing posttest attitude scores with pretest attitude scores and found
400
that the posttest explicit attitude was different from the pretest explicit attitude score, F(1, 90)
401
= 19.30, p < .001, η2 = .177 (see Fig. 3), (mean pretest = 4.18, mean posttest = 4.61). This is
402
in line with hypothesis 1a. No difference between pretest and posttest implicit attitude scores
403
was found F(1, 90) = 0.67, p = .416, η2 = .007 (see Fig. 3). The interaction between pretest-
404
posttest difference and information condition was not significant for explicit attitude score,
405
F(1, 90) = 2.48, p = .119, η2 = .027, or implicit attitude score, F(1, 90) = 0.07, p = .932, η2 <
406
.001. The interaction between pretest-posttest difference and familiarity for explicit attitude
407
score, F(1, 90) = 11.13, p = .001, η2 = .110, indicated that attitude change was smaller for
408
participants who were more familiar with cultured meat. This was not found for implicit
409
attitude score, F(1, 90) = 0.10, p = .750, η2 = .001. The three-way interaction effect between
410
pretest-posttest difference, information condition and familiarity on explicit attitude score, F(1,
411
90) = 6.14, p = .015, η2 = .064, indicated that explicit attitude change in the direction of the
412
information was smaller for participants who were more familiar with cultured meat (see
413
Table 2), which supports hypothesis 1b. This was not found for implicit attitude score, F(1,
414
90) = 0.11, p = .745, η2 = .001.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
399
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Table 2. Means and standard deviations of explicit attitude score (1 = negative; 7 = positive) and implicit attitudes score (0 = neutral) by familiarity with cultured meat in experiment 2. Pretest Posttest A little bit A little bit Unfamiliar familiar Familiar Unfamiliar familiar Familiar Condition N M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD Explicit attitude score Positive information cultured meat Pretest 48 3.54 1.22 4.41 1.39 5.23 0.91 4.62 0.87 4.77 1.27 5.26 1.22 No-pretest 46 3.72 1.10 4.76 0.96 5.27 1.06 Positive information solar panels 4.46 1.14 4.76 1.94 4.11 1.09 4.67 1.04 4.88 1.59 Pretest 49 3.81 1.08 No-pretest 47 3.65 0.94 4.91 0.89 5.36 0.75 Implicit attitude score Positive information cultured meat Pretest 48 -0.15 0.24 -0.15 0.31 -0.46 0.18 -0.07 0.32 -0.20 0.33 -0.24 0.29 No-pretest 46 -0.17 0.31 -0.16 0.42 -0.11 0.26 Positive information solar panels Pretest 49 -0.26 0.31 -0.21 0.43 0.03 0.26 -0.21 0.26 -0.15 0.32 -0.04 0.23 No-pretest 47 -0.15 0.29 -0.18 0.31 -0.01 0.35
19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 415
Positive information cultured meat (pretest condition) Positive information solar panels (pretest condition)
6.0
Positive information cultured meat (no-pretest condition) Positive information solar panels (no-pretest condition)
5.0
RI PT
4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 Pretest
Posttest
Measurement
Positive information cultured meat (pretest condition)
0.4
Positive information solar panels (pretest condition)
0.3
Positive information cultured meat (no-pretest condition)
0.2
Positive information solar panels (no-pretest condition)
0.0 -0.1 -0.2 -0.3 -0.4 -0.5
TE D
0.1
EP
Implicit attitude score (95% CI)
0.5
M AN U
a)
Pretest
Posttest Measurement
AC C
b)
SC
Explicit attitude score (95% CI)
7.0
Fig. 3. Mean explicit (panel a) and implicit (panel b) attitude score in the pretest and posttest of experiment 2. Error bars indicate the 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) 416 417 418
Discussion Experiment 2 replicated the finding for the positive information condition in experiment
419
1. Moreover, it extended experiment 1 by showing that positive information about solar
420
panels, a sustainable product belonging to the same sustainable product category as 20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT cultured meat, resulted in a more positive explicit attitude toward cultured meat. The effect of
422
information was smaller for people who were more familiar with cultured meat. The effect
423
information provision about solar panels had on the explicit attitude toward cultured meat
424
may indicate that participants were able to map cultured meat onto the broader sustainable
425
product category and derive their evaluation from this category. When no information is
426
available about an unfamiliar attitude object, activated implicit associations belonging to a
427
category of objects similar to the unfamiliar object, may inform the explicit attitude toward the
428
unfamiliar attitude object. However, an alternative explanation could be that since both
429
cultured meat and solar panels were presented as positive, cultured meat was related to
430
generally positive affective associations instead of a product based category. To investigate
431
this alternative explanation, content free affective associations were induced in a third
432
experiment.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
421
433
Experiment 3
434
To distinguish category based inferences from affect based inferences, experiment 3
TE D
435
investigated the influence of content free affect on the attitude toward cultured meat. If
437
content free affect informs the attitude toward cultured meat, a content free induced positive
438
or negative mood state should influence the attitude toward cultured meat in the direction of
439
the information (Clore et al., 2001; Schwarz & Clore, 1983).
441 442 443
AC C
440
EP
436
Material and methods
Participants and design
A Solomon four group design, with an experimental 2 (positive mood versus negative
444
mood) x 2 (pretest versus no-pretest) between subjects design was conducted in the winter
445
of 2014. Participants were 202 Wageningen University students that spoke fluent Dutch.
446
They received a two euro university canteen voucher for participation. Seven participants
447
were excluded because they did not meet the response time requirements for the implicit
448
attitude measurement method, one participant was excluded for not finishing the experiment 21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 449
and two participants were excluded because they did not follow the instructions. The final
450
sample consisted of 192 participants (68 male and 124 female) aged 17 to 55 years with a
451
median of 21.
452
Materials, measures and procedure
RI PT
453
All materials, measures and procedures were equal to experiment 1 and 2, with the
455
addition of the mood manipulation. Positive or negative mood state were induced by asking
456
participants to re-imagine a positive or negative event that happened in the previous year
457
that evoked strong feelings (see for example, Akbari & Hommel, 2012; Arnold & Reynolds,
458
2009; McFarland, Beuhler, von Rüti, Nguyen, & Alvaro, 2007; Strack, Schwarz, &
459
Gschneidinger, 1985). Participants were asked to report the event in at least five sentences,
460
followed by three items on a 9-point scale: “I experience the imagined live event as”
461
extremely unpleasant to extremely pleasant, not at all vivid to extremely vivid; and “the
462
feelings evoked by this event are” extremely negative to extremely positive. A pilot (N = 16)
463
confirmed the effectiveness of the mood manipulation. Participants were told they
464
participated in a number of unrelated studies and that this part was included to develop a
465
questionnaire to assess important life events in a systematic and reliable way. The mood
466
manipulation method received ethical clearance from the Social Sciences Ethics Committee
467
of Wageningen University.
469 470
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
468
SC
454
Results
In the pretest, participants had a negative implicit attitude toward cultured meat, M = -
471
0.18, SD 0.36, t(93) = -4.86, p <.001, and an explicit attitude around the central scale point,
472
M = 4.24, SD = 1.10 (see Table 3, for mean implicit and explicit attitude scores by level of
473
familiarity with cultured meat). Within-participant pretest and posttest explicit attitude scores
474
were significantly correlated, r (92) = .964, p < .001, while pretest and posttest implicit
475
attitude scores were not, r (92) = .195, p = .060. Pretest explicit and implicit attitude scores
476
were not significantly correlated in the positive mood condition, r(46) = -.138, p = .349, or 22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 477
negative mood condition, r(46) = .086, p = .563. The posttest explicit and implicit attitude
478
scores were not significantly correlated in the positive mood condition, r(96) = .164, p = .107,
479
but were in the negative mood condition, r(94) = .317, p = .002. Hypothesis testing using posttest data of all participants. The mood condition did
481
neither influence the posttest explicit attitude score, F(1, 186) = 0.69, p = .408, η2 = .004 (see
482
Fig. 4), nor the posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 186) = 0.07, p = .799, η2 < .001 (see Fig.
483
4). Familiarity had an effect on posttest explicit attitude score, F(1, 186) = 10.86, p = .001, η2
484
= .055, participants who were more familiar with cultured meat reported a more positive
485
posttest explicit attitude score, r (190) = .241, p = .001. This was not found for posttest
486
implicit attitude score, F (1, 186) = 0.84, p = .361, η2 = .004). There was no significant
487
interaction between familiarity and mood condition on posttest explicit attitude score, F(1,
488
186) = 0.07, p = .792, η2 < .001, or posttest implicit attitude score, F(1, 186) = 0.26, p = .608,
489
η2 = .001. Posttest attitude scores were not different for participants with or without pretest,
490
neither for explicit, F(1, 186) = 2.60, p = .108, η2 = .014, nor implicit attitude scores, F(1, 186)
491
= 1.32, p = .252, η2 = .007. There was no interaction between mood condition and presence
492
of a pretest for posttest explicit attitude score F(1, 186) = 1.05, p = .308, η2 = .006. There was
493
however, an interaction between mood condition and the presence of a pretest for posttest
494
implicit attitude score F(1, 186) = 4.21, p = .042, η2 = .022, where a positive induced mood
495
for participants in the pretest condition resulted in a more positive posttest implicit attitude
496
score. This interaction effect indicates a pretest sensitization effect. The difference between
497
pretest and posttest scores were therefore not investigated.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
480
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Table 3. Means and standard deviations of explicit attitude score (1 = negative; 7 = positive) and implicit attitudes score (0 = neutral) by familiarity with cultured meat in experiment 3. Pretest Posttest A little bit A little bit Unfamiliar familiar Familiar Unfamiliar familiar Familiar Condition N M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD Explicit attitude score Positive mood Pretest 48 3.66 1.15 4.22 1.12 5.22 0.31 3.75 1.33 4.32 1.17 5.25 0.36 No-pretest 46 3.64 1.06 3.79 1.05 4.75 0.88 Negative mood 4.34 1.15 4.73 0.96 4.00 0.81 4.29 1.34 4.65 1.15 Pretest 49 4.02 0.78 No-pretest 47 3.80 0.88 4.12 1.16 4.95 1.40 Implicit attitude score Positive mood Pretest 48 -0.25 0.32 -0.16 0.32 -0.31 0.57 -0.25 0.37 -0.04 0.36 -0.16 0.52 No-pretest 46 -0.28 0.42 -0.26 0.29 -0.31 0.19 Negative mood Pretest 49 0.02 0.35 -0.25 0.42 -0.15 0.32 -0.22 0.28 -0.28 0.31 0.00 0.33 No-pretest 47 -0.08 0.45 -0.16 0.39 -0.25 0.39
24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Positive mood (pretest condition) Negative mood (pretest condition)
6.0
Positive mood (no-pretest condition) Negative mood (no-pretest condition)
5.0
RI PT
4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 Pretest
Posttest Measurement
Positive mood (pretest condition)
0.4
Negative mood (pretest condition)
0.3
Positive mood (no-pretest condition)
0.2
Negative mood (no-pretest condition)
0.1 0.0 -0.1 -0.2 -0.3 -0.4 -0.5
TE D
Implicit attitude score (95% CI)
0.5
M AN U
a)
Posttest
Measurement
AC C
EP
Pretest
b)
SC
Explicit attitude score (95% CI)
7.0
Fig. 4. Mean explicit (panel a) and implicit (panel b) attitude score in the pretest and posttest of experiment 3. Error bars indicate the 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) 498 499 500
Discussion The mood condition did not show an effect on explicit nor implicit attitudes for the
501
whole sample. Only for those with a pretest, the induced positive mood had an effect on
502
implicit attitude. This finding suggests a pretest sensitization effect for mood induction on the
503
implicit attitude toward cultured meat. Only for the positive mood condition, a correlation was 25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 504
found between the explicit and implicit attitude. In conclusion, this experiment does not
505
support the alternative explanation suggested in experiment 2 that content free affect, rather
506
than category based inferences, had a relevant influence on the explicit attitude toward
507
cultured meat.
General Discussion
509 510
RI PT
508
As expected, information changed the explicit attitude of the unfamiliar object cultured meat into the direction of the valence of the information. Consistent with literature on attitude
512
change (Gregg, Seibt, & Banaji, 2006; Ratliff et al., 2012), attitude change was larger when
513
participants were less familiar with cultured meat.
SC
511
Providing participants with positive information about cultured meat or solar panels
515
resulted in positive attitude change toward cultured meat. Mood on the other hand, did not
516
influence the attitude toward cultured meat. Both cultured meat and solar panels are
517
sustainable products and as such indirectly related. It appears that providing content based
518
information about another sustainable product influenced participants’ attitudes toward
519
cultured meat in a similar way as content based information about cultured meat itself.
520
Information about solar panels may have activated a broader sustainable product category,
521
which allowed the attitude expression toward cultured meat. The pre-activated associations
522
with sustainability in turn may have facilitated making sense of the unfamiliar attitude object
523
(Gentner, 1988; Gregan-Paxton & Moreau, 2003), in this case cultured meat. That mood did
524
not influence the attitude toward cultured meat rules out the alternative explanation that the
525
effect of solar panel information on the explicit attitude toward cultured meat was merely
526
based on the positive affect induced by the benefits of solar panels (as suggested by for
527
example, Clore et al., 2001; Schwarz & Clore, 1983). This suggests that the explicit attitude
528
was influenced by content based arguments and not content free affect.
529
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
514
In contrast with the explicit attitude, the implicit attitude was not influenced by
530
information provision or content free affect. Literature on attitude change suggests that
531
implicit attitudes are not easily formed or changed after a single exposure to information 26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; Gregg et al., 2006; Ratliff et al., 2012). One possible
533
reason why no changes in implicit attitude were observed is the relatively low reliability of the
534
ST-IAT measures. Reliability for the ST-IAT reported in the current paper fits in the range of
535
reported reliabilities for similar measurement methods (see for example, Smith, Ratliff, &
536
Nosek, 2012). Nevertheless the low reliability may have resulted in a low sensitivity of the
537
implicit attitude measurement method compared to the explicit attitude measurement method
538
(Lebel & Paunonen, 2011). We should therefore be careful to interpret the absence of
539
significant results as a strong indication that implicit attitudes did not change. In the current
540
study we did find correlations between the posttest implicit and explicit attitude in several
541
experimental conditions where there was no correlation between the pretest implicit and
542
explicit attitude. The correlation between the implicit and explicit attitude suggests that
543
participants’ explicit attitude toward cultured meat may be informed by activated implicit
544
associations of cultured meat or a related category containing objects similar to cultured
545
meat (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; Ranganath & Nosek, 2008; Ranganath, Smith, &
546
Nosek, 2008; Ratliff et al., 2012). These correlation patterns between the implicit and explicit
547
attitude were not stable however, possibly due to low reliability. Therefore, future research is
548
required to investigate under what conditions these correlations occur.
SC
M AN U
TE D
Cultured meat is a non-fictitious novel food product, which is therefore likely
EP
549
RI PT
532
connected to one or more categories of similar objects. This connection can be made
551
instantaneously and without cognitive deliberation and the category in turn, can inform the
552
explicit and implicit attitude. This raises the question to which objects and which categories
553
cultured meat is connected and how this categorization influences the explicit and implicit
554
attitude toward cultured meat.
AC C
550
555
In the current paper we used a Solomon four-group design (Solomon, 1949) to rule
556
out pretest sensitizations effects. The results revealed no relevant differences between the
557
effect that information or mood induction had on the explicit attitude for participants with and
558
without a pretest. The variation in findings across domains for implicit attitude measures
559
(Greenwald et al., 2009) underlines the importance to rule out alternative explanations such 27
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 560
as pretest sensitization effects. Control mechanisms for effects caused by the experimental
561
design help ruling out alternative explanations (Kim & Willson, 2010). We therefore suggest
562
to control for pretest sensitization effects in studies using an experimental design that
563
includes a pretest. In summary, the current paper shows that the explicit attitude toward cultured meat
RI PT
564
can be influenced by content based information about cultured meat, but also by content
566
based information about an indirectly related product. Content based information provision
567
about cultured meat in a relevant context could therefore play a role in the commercial
568
success of cultured meat.
SC
565
569
571
Acknowledgements
M AN U
570
This research was funded by the Dutch Ministry of Economic Affairs. The Dutch Ministry of Economic Affairs had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis
573
and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper
574
for publication.
576 577
References
Achterberg, P. (2014). Knowing hydrogen and loving it too? Information provision, cultural
EP
575
TE D
572
predispositions, and support for hydrogen technology among the Dutch. Public
579
Understanding of Science, 23(4), 445–453. doi:10.1177/0963662512453117
580 581 582 583
AC C
578
Akbari, C. S., & Hommel, B. (2012). More creative through positive mood? Not everyone! Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1–7. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00319 Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behaviour : A metaanalytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.
584
Arnold, M. J., & Reynolds, K. E. (2009). Affect and retail shopping behavior: Understanding
585
the role of mood regulation and regulatory focus. Journal of Retailing, 85(3), 308–320.
586
doi:10.1016/j.jretai.2009.05.004
587
Ayres, K., Conner, M. T., Prestwich, A., & Smith, P. (2012). Do implicit measures of attitudes 28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 588
incrementally predict snacking behaviour over explicit affect-related measures?
589
Appetite, 58(3), 835–841. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.019
590
Bar-Anan, Y., & Nosek, B. A. (2014). A comparative investigation of seven indirect attitude measures. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 668–688. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-
592
0410-6
593
RI PT
591
Bluemke, M., & Friese, M. (2008). Reliability and validity of the Single-Target IAT (ST-IAT):
594
assessing automatic affect towards multiple attitude objects. European Journal of Social
595
Psychology, 38(6), 977–997. doi:10.1002/ejsp.487
Bohner, G., & Dickel, N. (2011). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of
SC
596
Psychology, 62(1), 391–417. doi:doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131609
598
Boland, M. J., Rae, A. N., Vereijken, J. M., Meuwissen, M. P. M., Fischer, A. R. H., van
M AN U
597
599
Boekel, M., … Hendriks, W. H. (2013). The future supply of animal-derived protein for
600
human consumption. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 29(1), 62–73.
601
doi:10.1016/j.tifs.2012.07.002
603 604
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1966). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for
TE D
602
research. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C., & Isbell, L. (2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore
606
(Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition (pp. 27–62). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence
607
Erlbaum Associates.
AC C
608
EP
605
Crites, S. L., Fabrigar, L. R., & Petty, R. E. (1994). Measuring the affective and cognitive
609
properties of attitudes: Conceptual and methodological issues. Personality and Social
610
Psychology Bulletin, 20(6), 619–634.
611
Cunningham, W. A., Nezlek, J. B., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Implicit and explicit ethnocentrism:
612
Revisiting the ideologies of prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
613
30(10), 1332–1346. doi:10.1177/0146167204264654
614 615
Datar, I., & Betti, M. (2010). Possibilities for an in vitro meat production system. Innovative Food Science & Emerging Technologies, 11(1), 13–22. 29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 616
de Liver, Y., van der Pligt, J., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2007). Positive and negative
617
associations underlying ambivalent attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social
618
Psychology, 43(2), 319–326.
622 623 624 625 626 627
RI PT
621
Cengage Learning. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (2007). The advantages of an inclusive definition of attitude. Social Cognition, 25(5), 582–602.
Edelman, P. D., McFarland, D. C., Mironov, V. A., & Matheny, J. G. (2005). In vitro-cultured meat production. Tissue Engineering, 11(5-6), 659–662.
SC
620
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth
Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25(5), 603–637.
M AN U
619
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic
628
activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229–238.
629
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229
Fiala, N. (2008). Meeting the demand: An estimation of potential future greenhouse gas
TE D
630 631
emissions from meat production. Ecological Economics, 67(3), 412–419.
632
doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2007.12.021
Fiske, S. T., & Pavelchak, M. A. (1986). Category-based versus piecemeal-based affective
634
responses: Developments in schema-triggered affect. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T.
635
Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior
636
(pp. 167–203). New York: Guilford Press.
AC C
637
EP
633
Friese, M., Bluemke, M., & Wanke, M. (2007). Predicting voting behavior with implicit attitude
638
measures: The 2002 German parliamentary election. Experimental Psychology, 54(4),
639
247–255. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.247
640 641 642 643
Gawronski, B. (2007). Editorial: Attitudes can be measured! But what is an attitude? Social Cognition, 25(5), 573–581. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.573 Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological 30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 644 645 646 647
Bulletin, 132(5), 692–731. Gentner, D. (1988). Methapor as structure mapping: The relational shift. Child Development, 59, 47–59. Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Attitudinal dissociation: What does it mean? In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit
649
measures (pp. 65–82). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
RI PT
648
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit
651
association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social
652
Psychology, 85(3), 481.
SC
650
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding
654
and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal
655
of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 17–41.
656
M AN U
653
Gregan-Paxton, J., & Moreau, P. (2003). How do consumers transfer existing knowledge? A comparison of analogy and categorization effects. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
658
13(4), 422–430.
659
TE D
657
Gregg, A. P., Seibt, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Easier done than undone: Asymmetry in the malleability of implicit preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(1),
661
1–20. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.1
663 664
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgement. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.
AC C
662
EP
660
Hendrick, T. A. M., Fischer, A. R. H., Tobi, H., & Frewer, L. J. (2013). Self-reported attitude
665
scales: Current practice in adequate assessment of reliability, validity, and
666
dimensionality. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43(7), 1538–1552.
667
doi:10.1111/jasp.12147
668
Hobman, E. V, & Ashworth, P. (2013). Public support for energy sources and related
669
technologies: The impact of simple information provision. Energy Policy, 63, 862–869.
670
doi:10.1016/j.enpol.2013.09.011
671
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 672
analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report
673
measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(10), 1369–1385.
674
doi:10.1177/0146167205275613 Inquisit. (2011). Computer software. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software LLC.
676
Kardes, F. R., Posavac, S. S., & Cronley, M. L. (2004). Consumer inference: A review of
677
processes, bases, and judgment contexts. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(3),
678
230–256. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1403_6
RI PT
675
Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The Single Category Implicit Association Test as a
680
measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
681
91(1), 16–32. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.16
683
Kawakami, K., & Dovidio, J. F. (2001). The reliability of implicit stereotyping. Personality and
M AN U
682
SC
679
Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(2), 212–225. doi:10.1177/0146167201272007
684
Kim, E. S., & Willson, V. L. (2010). Evaluating pretest effects in pre-post studies. Educational
685
and Psychological Measurement, 70(5), 744–759. doi:10.1177/0013164410366687 Lebel, E. P., & Paunonen, S. V. (2011). Sexy but often unreliable: The impact of unreliability
687
on the replicability of experimental findings with implicit measures. Personality and
688
Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(4), 570–583. doi:10.1177/0146167211400619 Lusk, J. L., House, L. O., Valli, C., Jaeger, S. R., Moore, M., Morrow, J. L., & Traill, W. B.
EP
689
TE D
686
(2004). Effect of information about benefits of biotechnology on consumer acceptance of
691
genetically modified food: Evidence from experimental auctions in the United States,
692
England, and France. European Review of Agricultural Economics, 31(2), 179–204.
693
AC C
690
Mattick, C. S., Landis, A. E., Allenby, B. R., & Genovese, N. J. (2015). Anticipatory life cycle
694
analysis of in vitro biomass cultivation for cultured meat production in the United States.
695
Environmental Science & Technology, 49, 11941–11949. doi:10.1021/acs.est.5b01614
696
McComas, K. A., Besley, J. C., & Steinhardt, J. (2014). Factors influencing U.S. consumer
697
support for genetic modification to prevent crop disease. Appetite, 78C, 8–14.
698
doi:10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.006
699
McFarland, C., Beuhler, R., von Rüti, R., Nguyen, L., & Alvaro, C. (2007). The impact of 32
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 700
negative moods on self-enhancing cognitions : The role of reflective versus ruminative ,
701
mood orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 728–750.
702
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.728
704 705 706
Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(1), 29–45. doi:10.1348/014466604X23491
RI PT
703
Pluhar, E. B. (2010). Meat and morality: alternatives to factory farming. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 23, 455–468.
Ranganath, K. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Implicit attitude generalization occurs immediately;
708
Explicit attitude generalization takes time. Psychological Science, 19(3), 249–254.
709
SC
707
Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Distinguishing automatic and controlled components of attitudes from direct and indirect measurement methods.
711
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(2), 386–396.
712
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.008
713
M AN U
710
Ratliff, K. A., Swinkels, B. A. P., Klerx, K., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Does one bad apple(juice) spoil the bunch? Implicit attitudes toward one product transfer to other products by the
715
same brand. Psychology and Marketing, 28(8), 531–540.
716
TE D
714
Richetin, J., Perugini, M., Adjali, I., & Hurling, R. (2007). The moderator role of intuitive versus deliberative decision making for the predictive validity of implicit and explicit
718
measures. European Journal of Personality, 21(4), 529–546. doi:10.1002/per.625
720 721
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral
AC C
719
EP
717
judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bullitin, 34, 1096–1109. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being -
722
Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social
723
Psychology, 45(3), 513–523.
724
Smith, C. T., Ratliff, K. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Rapid assimilation: Automatically
725
integrating new information with existing beliefs. Social Cognition, 30(2), 199–219.
726
doi:10.1521/soco.2012.30.2.199
727
Solomon, R. L. (1949). An extension of control group design. Psychological Bulletin, 46(2), 33
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 728 729
137–150. Steinfeld, H., Gerber, P., Wassenaar, T., Castel, V., Rosales, M., & de Haan, C. (2006).
730
Livestock’s long shadow: Environmental issues and options. Renewable Resources
731
Journal, 24(4), 15–17. Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of
RI PT
732 733
time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social
734
Psychology, 49(6), 1460–1469. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.49.6.1460
735
Tuomisto, H. L., & de Mattos, M. J. T. (2011). Environmental impacts of cultured meat production. Environmental Science & Technology, 45(14), 6117–6123.
737
doi:10.1021/es200130u
740 741 742
M AN U
739
van der Weele, C., & Driessen, C. (2013). Emerging profiles for cultured meat; Ethics through and as design. Animals, 3, 647–662. doi:10.3390/ani3030647 van der Weele, C., & Tramper, J. (2014). Cultured meat: Every village its own factory? Trends in Biotechnology, 32(6), 294–6. doi:10.1016/j.tibtech.2014.04.009 Verbeke, W., Marcu, A., Rutsaert, P., Gaspar, R., Seibt, B., Fletcher, D., & Barnett, J. (2015).
TE D
738
SC
736
“Would you eat cultured meat?”: Consumers’ reactions and attitude formation in
744
Belgium, Portugal and the United Kingdom. Meat Science, 102, 49–58.
745
doi:10.1016/j.meatsci.2014.11.013
746
EP
743
Verbeke, W., Sans, P., & Van Loo, E. J. (2015). Challenges and prospects for consumer acceptance of cultured meat. Journal of Integrative Agriculture, 14(2), 285–294.
748
doi:10.1016/S2095-3119(14)60884-4
AC C
747
749
Walker, M. W., & Lev, J. (1953). Statistical Inference. New York: Henry Holt and Company.
750
Willson, V. L., & Putnam, R. R. (1982). A meta-analysis of pretest sensitization effects in
751
experimental design. American Educational Research Journal, 19(2), 249–258.
752
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit
753
level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social
754
Psychology, 72(2), 262–274. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.262
755
Wittenbrink, B., & Schwarz, N. (2007). Introduction. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), 34
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 756
Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 1–13). New York: The Guilford Press.
757
Wyer, R. S. (2008). The role of knowledge accessibility in cognition and behavior: Implications for consumer information processing. In C. P. Haugtvedt, P. M. Herr, & F.
759
R. Kardes (Eds.), Handbook of consumer psychology (pp. 31–76). New York, NY:
760
Taylor & Francis Group.
RI PT
758
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
761
35
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Appendix A
762 763
Words used in the ST-IAT
764 765
RI PT
Table A1 Cultured meat words (Dutch) cultured burger (kweekburger) cultured cutlet (kweekschnitzel) cultured ground meat (kweekgehakt)
SC
cultured sausage (kweekworst) cultured meat (in vitro vlees)
tissue culture (weefselkweek)
AC C
EP
TE D
766
M AN U
laboratory (laboratorium)
36
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table A2 Words with a positive or negative valence (Dutch) Valence
Words
Positive
lucky (gelukkig) love (liefde) freedom (vrijheid)
RI PT
happy (blijdschap) vacation (vakantie) party (feest) friend (vriend)
SC
paradise (paradijs) peace (vrede)
Negative
abuse (misbruik) hatred (haat) disaster (ramp) war (oorlog) disgust (walging) murder (moord)
TE D
accident (ongeluk)
M AN U
cheerful (vrolijk)
death (dood)
failure (mislukking)
AC C
767
EP
grief (verdriet)
37