Exploring non-native English-speaking teachers' cognitions about corrective feedback in teaching English oral communication

Exploring non-native English-speaking teachers' cognitions about corrective feedback in teaching English oral communication

System 55 (2015) 111e122 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect System journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/system Exploring non-native Eng...

314KB Sizes 130 Downloads 328 Views

System 55 (2015) 111e122

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

System journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/system

Exploring non-native English-speaking teachers' cognitions about corrective feedback in teaching English oral communication Muhammad Rahimi, Lawrence Jun Zhang* Faculty of Education and Social Work, University of Auckland, New Zealand

a r t i c l e i n f o

a b s t r a c t

Article history: Received 4 November 2014 Received in revised form 3 September 2015 Accepted 24 September 2015 Available online xxx

Exploring teachers' cognitions is a significant endeavour, as teachers' cognitions inform their classroom practice, and, in turn, their cognitions are influenced by their teaching experiences. This study explored the differences between novice and experienced nonnative English-speaking teachers' cognitions about corrective feedback (CF) in teaching English oral communication. Data were collected from 20 novice and 20 experienced teachers through a questionnaire and follow-up interviews. Results show statistically significant differences between the two groups. Teachers' personal experiences influenced their cognitions about the necessity of CF and the effectiveness of different CF types and timing. Interview data show teachers' teaching experiences raise their awareness of the role of mediating factors, namely learner factors, error frequency, types, and severity, target from difficulty, instructional focus, and task types in their cognitions about the necessity, timing, and types of CF. In contrast, novice teachers, partially due to their insufficient teaching experiences, had rigid cognitions about CF. Novice teachers attributed their cognitions to their personal language learning experiences. As a pedagogical implication, we recommend that student teachers be provided with opportunities for acquiring theoretical understanding about CF and translating it into classroom practice. © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Teacher cognition Corrective feedback Oral communication Non-native English-speaking teachers Iran

1. Introduction Teaching is a dynamic, complex, and situated activity which requires teachers' online decision-making (Kumaravadivelu, 2014). One such online decision making is providing corrective feedback (CF), which is an incidental rather than designed aspect of language teaching in most cases. When learners make errors, teachers usually decide spontaneously about whether to provide or withhold CF, about CF strategies, and also about the timing of providing CF. Evidently, these decisions are influenced by teachers' cognitions, an amalgam of “what they know, believe, and think” about teaching (Borg, 2003, p. 81; see also Borg, 2011). However, research on teacher cognition about CF and specifically how teachers CF cognition is (trans)formed is scant. In her recent review, Basturkmen (2012) has proposed that research on teacher cognition should move beyond designed aspects of teaching to incidental aspects (e.g., CF) and from case studies to comparative studies (e.g., novice and experienced teachers). She has argued that most research has employed case studies, which have yielded contradictory findings, and that * Corresponding author. Faculty of Education and Social Work, School of Curriculum and Pedagogy, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92601, Symonds Street, Auckland 1150, New Zealand. Tel.: þ64 9 6238899x48750 (office), þ64 221633268 (mobile). E-mail address: [email protected] (L.J. Zhang). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2015.09.006 0346-251X/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

112

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

there is not enough research on teachers' cognitions about the incidental aspects of teaching (e.g., CF). More importantly, in the general teacher education and language teacher education literature, it has been documented that experienced and novice teachers are different in classroom decision-making (see e.g., Akbari & Tajik, 2010; Pajares, 1992). Nevertheless, it is unclear whether such a pattern would be typically embodied in teachers' cognition about CF. This disciplinary knowledge gap and also the need for a new methodological approach motivated this research project. This study endeavours to fill this lacuna by exploring the differences between experienced and novice teachers' cognitions about three fundamental aspects of CF, namely necessity, types, and timing of CF (Ellis, 2009) to contribute to our understanding of subtleties of teacher cognition trans(formation) about CF in order to provide potential insights into language teacher education. 2. Literature review 2.1. Research findings on the effectiveness of CF and teachers' CF beliefs Studies have been undertaken on ESL/EFL teachers' cognitions about the diverse aspects of their practice (Goh, Zhang, Koh, & Ng, 2005), including teaching grammar (Borg & Burns, 2008), reading (Atai & Fatahi-Majd, 2014), writing (Ferris, 2014), listening (Graham, Santos, & Francis-Brophy, 2014), speaking (Baleghizadeh & Nasrollahi Shahri, 2014), and pronunciation (e.g., Baker, 2014). However, a few studies have explored teachers' cognitions about CF and how such cognitions are (trans) formed (e.g., Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Kamiya, 2014; Roothooft, 2014). Earlier research (e.g., Lyster & Ranta, 1997) have developed taxonomies of CF types that teachers used in their practice and explored: 1) teachers' rationale for CF (Fanselow, 1977), 2) attitude towards CF (Nystrom, 1983), 3) awareness, 4) beliefs, and 5) perception about CF (Long, 1977), and 6) their reasoning for providing different types of CF (Chaudron, 1986; Nystrom, 1983). This line of inquiry into CF has been extended by experimental studies that examined the differential impact of various CF types on learners' CF uptake, production, and learning (e.g., Rahimi & Zhang, 2013; Nassaji, 2013; Shegar, Zhang, & Low, 2013; Rassaei, 2013). Taken together, meta-analytic reviews of research on the effectiveness of CF (e.g. Li, 2010) show that CF is effective and the effectiveness of CF is mediated by instructional setting variables, student variables, target form variables, and teacher variables, among others. Additionally, classroom CF research (see Brown, 2014, for a meta-analytic review) demonstrates that recasts comprise 57% of all CF, whereas prompts account for 30% (Brown, 2014). Brown also identified moderators (e.g., student variables, teacher variables, classroom setting variables) that may affect teachers' CF provision. Echoing prior research results (e.g., Mackey, Polio, & McDonough, 2004), Brown's synthesis revealed that target forms (i.e., phonological, grammatical, lexical, or pragmatic errors that teachers' correct) influenced teachers' provision of CF, and that syntactic errors obtained the highest percentage of CF (43%). Moreover, Basturkmen's (2012) review of research on the relationships between teachers' beliefs and practices yielded contradictory findings. The review reveals the mediating role of context in, and constraints on, the relationship between teachers' beliefs and practices; correspondences were found mainly between experienced teachers' stated beliefs and practices and also between teachers' beliefs about and practices of planned aspects of teaching. There was not enough research to reach conclusions on the correspondence or lack of correspondence between teachers' beliefs and practices of unplanned aspects of teaching (e.g., CF). Basturkmen (2012) argued that this contradictory finding can be partially attributed to case study methodology used in research to date. She proposed that research move beyond case studies. For instance, she recommended further research on the beliefs of experienced versus inexperienced teachers and on the teachers' beliefs in relation to planned (e.g., task design) vs. incidental (e.g., CF) aspects of their teaching (see Rahimi & Zhang, 2013). 2.2. Research on novice and experienced language teachers' cognitions about language teaching Research into teacher cognitions is significant, as what teachers believe and practice are key to their success in teaching and student success in learning (Barnard & Burns, 2012). In fact, research into teachers' cognitions and especially on factors that influence (trans)forming such cognitions are imperative for informing teacher education, as student teachers' cognitions are unlikely to be influenced by reading research articles and assigned reading materials alone (Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Kagan, 1992; Va'squez & Harvey, 2010). One factor that is shown to influence teachers' cognitions is experience, particularly their previous teaching experience (Mok, 1994) and language learning experience (Borg, 2009; Farrell, 2009, 2014). Although studies exploring novice and experienced teachers' cognitions in the general teacher education field abound (e.g., Doganay & Ozturk, 2011; Melnick & Meister, 2008; Peterson & Comeaux, 1987; Tschannen-Moran & Hoy, 2007), and there are a growing number of studies on language teachers' cognitions (e.g., Akbari & Tajik, 2012; Akyel, 1997; Gao & Ma, 2011; Gatbonton, 2008; Mok, 1994; Rahmani Doqaruni, 2015), studies comparing novice and experienced teachers' cognition about CF are scan in the literature. In the field of language teacher education, studies comparing experienced and novice teachers have shown differences in teachers' pedagogical practices. For example, Akyel's (1997) comparative study of experienced and novice ESL teachers revealed that while novice teachers were concerned about sustaining the smooth flow of their instructional activities and appropriateness of their instructional strategies and considered learners' incomplete responses and initiations as barriers, experienced teachers showed flexibility in their instructional strategies and considered a wider range of instructional options in their practices. Richards, Li, and Tang' (1998) study of L2 reading teachers showed that novice teachers had a limited

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

113

understanding of L2 reading as compared with the experienced teachers. Tsui (2009) found three critical differences between expert and non-expert teachers: ability in integrating their knowledge into instructional practices, consideration of the contextual factors in their practices, and engagement in conscious reflection and deliberation. Rahmani Doqaruni's (2015) study of novice and experienced teachers' use of communication strategies (CS) in dealing with the difficulty of communicating their intended meaning in the classroom show that the novice teachers used relatively the same pattern of CS in their oral communication, while their experienced counterpart used the least number of CSs. Rahmani interpreted this finding as a sign of transformation in these teachers' cognitions as a result of their teaching experience over time. However, comparative studies on language teachers' cognitions have yielded contradictory findings partially due to the case study methodology (see Basturkmen, 2012; for comments). Gatbonton (2008), for example, found both similarities and difference in the novice and experiences teachers' pedagogical knowledge base (PK). The novice teachers were concerned about their relationship with their students and the students' reaction to their instruction rather than instructional procedures and learning outcomes with which experienced teachers were preoccupied. Gatbonton posited that similarities between two groups' PK indicate that certain aspects of PK can be acquired by “apprenticeship of observation”, but the differences suggest that intervention is imperative for teachers to acquire other aspects. In contrast, Watzke (2007) found that regardless of teaching experience, teachers were concerned with students' learning outcomes and well-being. These findings contradict those of Berliner's (1986) and Nunan's (1992) studies, which revealed that teachers started their career mainly preoccupied with self-image and classroom management issues, while experienced teachers concentrated more on instructional procedures and student learning outcomes. Akbari and Tajik (2010) also found a critical difference between novice and experience teachers with regard to their PK priorities; novice teachers ranked “language management” as their top pedagogical thought category, whereas “self-reflection” was the experienced teachers' number one preoccupation. These contradictory findings with regard to the role of experience in the (trans)formation of teachers' cognitions calls for further research to identify 1) the similarities and differences between novice and experienced teachers and 2) aspects of language teaching expertise, which can only be acquired through intervention. Once these findings are available, they can be used for providing potential insights into language teacher education.

2.3. Research on novice and experienced language teachers' cognitions about CF With regard to the novice and experienced teachers' cognition about CF, two studies investigated novice and experienced ESL teachers' CF beliefs and practices (Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Mackey et al., 2004). Junqueira and Kim (2013) collected data through observations, stimulated recalls, and interviews. Their findings show that the amount of CF that teachers provided and learners' uptake and repair in both teachers' classes were comparable. However, the experienced teacher provided more CF types, with more balanced proportions across target forms. Teachers' teaching experience and training did not seem to affect teachers' beliefs about the inefficacy of CF, whereas teachers' language learning experience appeared to have a greater role in affecting both teachers' beliefs about the inefficacy of CF. Mackey et al. (2004) explored the impact of ESL teachers' teaching experience and education on their awareness and use of incidental focus-on-form episodes (FoFE), which is also referred to as incidental CF. Their results show that the experienced teachers exploited FoFE more frequently than their inexperienced counterparts to draw the learners' attention to the ungrammaticality of the learners' utterances. In contrast, the novice teacher commented on the semantic content of the learners' errors and ignored the ungrammaticality of their utterances. These authors conducted a follow-up short-term workshop to investigate its impact on the novice teachers' awareness and use of FoFE. The short-term workshop raised teachers' awareness of FoFE, but teachers were not able to integrate their FoFE awareness into practice. The authors suggested these teachers might gain such ability with experience. As Mackey et al. (2004) and Junqueira and Kim (2013) have convincingly observed, classroom teaching experience has an important impact on teacher' cognitions about CF, and further research is warranted to investigate the role of experience in (trans)forming teachers' cognition. Additionally, from a methodological perspective, adopting mixed-methods approaches is imperative to better understand the nature and sources of teachers' cognitions about CF (Basturkmen, 2012). Evidently, studies on the effectiveness of CF, classroom-based observational research on CF, language teachers' cognitions, and novice and experienced teachers' cognitions abound. However, there is a paucity of comparative studies on novice and experienced EFL teachers' cognitions and sources of their cognitions about CF (Junqueira & Kim, 2013). This study endeavours to fill this void and provide potential insights into language teacher education. Motivated by Basturkmen's (2012, p. 282) call for “research agenda beyond case studies”, we used a mixed methods design to explore the differences between experienced and novice teachers' cognitions about three fundamental aspects of CF (see Ellis, 2009) and their reasoning for their cognitions. Comparing teachers' cognitions about the other aspects of CF and the relationships between teachers' cognitions about, and practices in, CF are beyond the scope of this study. Specifically, this study was designed to seek answers to the following research questions: 1) 2) 3) 4)

Are there any significant differences between the novice and experienced teachers' cognitions about the necessity of CF? Are there any significant differences between the novice and experienced teachers' cognitions about the timing of CF? Are there any significant differences between the novice and experienced teachers' cognitions about the types of CF? What do the novice and the experienced teachers attribute their cognitions about CF to?

114

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

3. Method 3.1. Context of the study Language education in Iran is provided by mainstream formal schooling system and private language schools. The main objective of teaching English as a foreign language (EFL) at public schools is to assist students to achieve survival level communicative competence in order to be able to communicate with others (Secretariat of the Higher Council of Education, 2006, cited in Farhady & Hedayati, 2009). Despite the attempts made to implement communicative language teaching approaches in Iran's mainstream schooling system by modifying English textbooks and promoting formative assessment rather than summative testing, the outcome is unsatisfactory (Farhady & Hedayati, 2009). This unsatisfactory outcome is partially due to the washback effect of summative discrete-point language tests. Because of the discrete-point nature of University Entrance Exam and language tests at all grades, students are concerned with high performance on the tests rather than achieving communicative ability. Consequently, teachers are not able to implement the communicative approach curriculum which is aimed at enabling students to achieve communicative competence (Safarnavadeh, 2004; Zhang & Ben Said, 2014). Private schools in Iran provide both intensive and extensive English language courses based on the principles of communicative approaches (Rahimi & Zhang, 2013; Zhang & Rahimi, 2014). Typically, English courses are taught by nearnative English-speaking teachers who hold university degrees (e.g., BA) in teaching English a foreign language (TEFL). The main objective of these courses is to enable students to communicate with others in both oral and written English. Preparatory courses for international tests such as the International English Language Testing System (IELTS) and the Test of English as a Foreign Language (TOEFL) Internet-based Test (iBT) are also offered. The language schools where this study was conducted offer English conversation courses for all proficiency levels. 3.2. Participants The participants, 40 non-native English-speaking teachers, were recruited from among teachers teaching oral communication classes in private language schools due to a popular demand in Iran for improving English oral communication skills rather than public schools or universities, as in these two institutions language teaching is reading-based and oral communication is not emphasized (Zhang & Rahimi, 2014). The teacher participants' experience varied from less than one year to 15 years, and they were teaching low-intermediate to advanced level oral communication classes. They were male and female teachers whose age ranged between 24 and 45 years. All teachers had a Bachelor Degree in teaching EFL, and had attended pre-service teacher training courses at the private schools. In the literature (e.g., Gatbonton, 2008), teachers with more than four to five years of teaching experience and those with less than two years of experience are considered experienced and novice, respectively. Twenty novice teachers with less than one year experience and 20 experienced teachers with more than five years’ experience from among 180 teachers working at private language schools in central Iran were recruited via an invitation email message to participate in this study on a voluntary basis. Teachers were advised that they might be invited to participate in an interview and member checking sessions on a voluntary basis. They were informed that they could withdraw from the study at any stage without any consequences. The participating teachers had received similar teacher education and were teaching in a similar context. The most important difference between the novice and experienced teachers was their classroom teaching experience. 3.3. Instruments and procedures 3.3.1. Teacher questionnaire For this study, the first author distributed a questionnaire, adapted from Fukuda (2004), to teachers to investigate their cognitions about CF. The questionnaire has two sections. The first section was designed to collect background information about teachers, including their gender, university degree, and the experience of teaching EFL. The second section includes twelve questions investigating teachers' cognitions about the necessity of error correction, timing of error correction, and types of corrective feedback (Ellis, 2009). As this study aimed at exploring teachers' cognitions about the effectiveness of timing of CF, rather than teachers' agreement or disagreement with different timing of CF, the Likert-type scales measuring agreement (1 ¼ strongly disagree to 5 ¼ strongly disagree) were changed to scales measuring effectiveness (1 ¼ Very ineffective to 5 ¼ Very effective). To identify the teachers' reasoning for, and sources of, their cognitions about CF, follow-up semi-structured interviews (Fontana & Frey, 1994), designed based on the teachers' responses to the questionnaire items, were conducted with randomly selected five novice and five experienced teachers from among 40 teachers who completed the CF cognitions questionnaire. 3.4. Data collection and analysis procedure In the first phase of the data collection, the first author distributed the questionnaire to the novice and experienced EFL teachers in the schools' staffroom. The teachers were requested to return the completed questionnaire in two weeks to the

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

115

secretary of the school. The items of the questionnaire were divided into three categories: The necessity of CF, the timing of CF, and the methods of CF. The teachers were asked to rate the items on a 5-point Likert-type scale. In the second phase of the study, 10 teachers (five teachers from each group) were randomly selected and invited to participate in the interview, which was conducted in Farsi and English. The teachers did not know the interview questions. The interviews were recorded with the consent of the participants and transcribed by the first author. The interviews were conducted in private language schools in central Iran and each interview took 27 min. After ensuring that the collected data met the assumptions of the t-test and that the questionnaire data, the Likert-type scale section, enjoyed acceptable level of reliability, (a ¼ 0.78), we ran t-tests to analyse quantitative data to examine if there were statistically significant differences between the two groups' cognitions about CF. Further analysis was also conducted to identify the percentage of teachers' ratings for questionnaire items. A deductive approach (Patton, 2002) guided by research question 4 was used to analyse the interview data. The analysis was conducted by the iterative reading of the transcripts to look for typical patterns of teachers' reasoning for, and sources of, their CF cognitions. Two coders, who were well versed in the CF literature, coded the qualitative data. Their coding agreement reached 87%; their disagreements in coding were resolved through discussion. The coders' interpretations of teachers' interview data were submitted to the teachers in a separate meeting held with each teacher (member-checking) for their confirmation. Teachers agreed with the coders' interpretation of their data. The findings of the qualitative data corroborated those of the quantitative data. Triangulation of the quantitative and qualitative data, one of the merits of mixed methods design, further ensured the validity of the collected data.

4. Results and discussion 4.1. Novice vs. experienced teachers' cognitions 4.1.1. Necessity of error correction For Question 1 on the questionnaire, the teachers were requested to rate the statement, “Students' spoken errors should be corrected.” As Table 1 shows, the mean score of the experienced teachers (M ¼ 4.50) was higher than that of the novice teachers (M ¼ 3.75). The result indicated that the experienced teachers favoured error correction more than their novice colleagues. Ninety percent of the experienced teachers and 75% of the novice teachers responded with “strongly agree” or “agree” to Question 1. It is interesting that none of the teachers strongly disagreed with the statement. As Table 2 demonstrates, the findings indicate that both groups of teachers agreed with correcting students' errors in oral communication classes; the experienced teachers, however, believed in the importance of providing CF to a much greater extent than the novice teachers did. The discrepancies between the two groups' responses were statistically significant t(38) ¼ 3.21, p ¼ 0.003, with a large effect size, d ¼ 1.01. Both the novice and experienced teachers believed that providing CF is the teachers' responsibility; however, they attributed their cognitions to different sources. The novice teachers believed that teachers have to correct learners' errors; they believed that students need to be assured that they are using correct forms. They asserted that not giving CF can confuse the students. They highlighted their own language learning experience as a source of their cognitions, as shown in the extract of the interview with Siamak. As students, we expected our teachers to give us the correct form of our errors, so that we could improve our language proficiency. My classmates and I did not like lenient teachers who did not give us any CF. We did not like to keep talking during language classes without learning. Actually, it is the teachers' responsibility to give corrective feedback. (Siamak, Novice teacher, Interview: 11/12/2013) The experienced teachers also emphasized on drawing learners' attention to their errors and helping them develop their interlanguage; however, they attributed their cognitions to their teaching experience. Such a pattern is typically represented in the interview with Mohsen. By teaching different students with different proficiency levels, emotional and psychological status, and language learning aptitudes, I have come to the conclusion that students like to be corrected. Students like to be corrected in different ways, though. I do believe that CF works; students typically do not repeat the same errors in their following

Table 1 Novice vs. experienced teachers' responses on the necessity of error correction. Necessity of CF

Disagree Undecided Agree Strongly agree Total

Groups

Total

Novice

Experienced

3 2 13 2 20

1 1 15 3 20

4 3 28 5 40

116

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

Table 2 Comparison of responses on the necessity of error correction: novice vs. experienced teachers.

Novice Experienced

N

Mean

SD

t

df

Sig. (2-tailed)

d

20 20

3.75 4.50

0.910 0.512

3.21

38

0.003

1.01

Note. d ¼ effect size.

productions, but I take many factors into account in providing CF to increase the effectiveness of CF. (Mohsen, Experienced teacher Interview: 13/12/2013) Understandably, novice and experienced teachers' to their learning and teaching experience, respectively, as evidence for their cognitions about CF can be explained, to some extent, by the recency of their experiences. In line with prior research (Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Kagan, 1992; Kennedy, 1997; Va'squez & Harvey, 2010), teachers stated their experience, not their teacher training courses, as a source of their cognitions. In addition, our triangulation of the qualitative data and the quantitative data shows that the findings are in line with previous research (Borg, 2006; Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Mori, 2011); namely, teachers' cognitions are influenced by their experience as language learners and teachers. Also, our study shows that teachers consider providing CF as their responsibility. This finding is also reported in prior studies (e.g., Chaudron, 1988). However, the findings regarding EFL teachers' cognitions about the necessity of providing CF contradict ESL teachers' stated beliefs (e.g., Junqueira & Kim, 2013), which suggests that teaching contexts influence teachers' cognitions. 4.2. Methods of corrective feedback The second category of items on the questionnaire targeted the teachers' cognitions about the effectiveness of CF types. The category comprised seven methods of CF types, including, recasts, repetition, clarification request, elicitation, explicit feedback, metalinguistic feedback, and no CF (Ellis, 2009). Table 3 reflects the novice and experienced teachers' cognitions about the effectiveness of each CF types. Recast (M ¼ 3.75) and Clarification request (M ¼ 3.55) had the highest mean among the novice teachers, whereas explicit feedback (M ¼ 3.90) and Recast (M ¼ 3.80) had the highest mean among the experienced teachers. No CF had the lowest mean for the experienced teachers, while elicitation request and explicit CF had the lowest mean for the novice teachers. These findings show the differences between the novice and experienced teachers' cognitions about the effectiveness of CF types. Table 3 shows that the differences between the novice teachers and experienced teachers' cognitions about explicit CF, t(38) ¼ 4.932, p < 0.001, d ¼ 1.56 and metalinguistic CF, t(38) ¼ 3.395, p ¼ 0.002, d ¼ 1.08 were statistically significant, with a large effect size for each. In other words, the experienced teachers favoured explicit CF and metalinguistic CF as effective methods for error treatment, while the novice teachers considered them ineffective. The highest statistically significant difference between the two groups' cognitions about CF types was the effectiveness of the explicit CF. Seventy-five percent of the experienced teachers believed that explicit CF was effective, whereas only 15% of the novice teachers regarded it as effective. The finding suggests that the experienced teachers believed in the value of explicit CF for their students' L2 acquisition. On the other hand, novice teachers did not consider explicit CF as an effective CF type for enhancing L2 learners' oral communication skills. There were statistically significant differences between the two groups of teachers with regard to the effectiveness of metalinguistic CF. Sixty percent of the experienced teachers believed that metalinguistic feedback is effective, whereas only 15% of the novice teachers did so. Table 4 shows a percentage compilation of the novice and experienced teachers' cognitions. Recast (70%) and clarification request (60%) were the most favoured methods of CF among the novice teachers. Repetition and elicitation with 50% each were the third most favoured error treatment method, while explicit CF, metalinguistic feedback, and No CF were recognized as the least effective methods (15% each) by the novice teachers. The findings indicate that the novice teachers favoured implicit CF to the explicit CF types. Follow-up interview data reflected these teachers' language learning experience as a

Table 3 Responses on the methods of corrective feedback: novice vs. experienced teachers. Types of feedback

Clarification request Repetition Explicit feedback Elicitation No corrective feedback Metalinguistic feedback Recasts Note. d ¼ effect size.

Experienced teachers

Novice teachers

M

SD

M

SD

3.75 3.60 3.90 3.55 1.95 3.55 3.80

1.11803 1.23117 0.91191 1.09904 0.82558 0.88704 0.95145

3.55 3.25 2.30 2.30 2.35 2.60 3.75

1.14593 1.25132 1.12858 0.97872 1.08942 0.88258 0.71635

t

df

Sig. (2-Tailed)

d

0.559 0.892 4.932 0.760 1.309 3.395 0.188

38 38 38 38 38 38 38

0.580 0.378 0.000 0.452 0.198 0.002 0.852

0.22 0.28 1.56 1.21 0.41 1.08 0.06

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

117

Table 4 Comparison of teachers’ responses on the effectiveness of methods of corrective feedback (%): novice vs. experienced teachers. Types of feedback

Clarification Repetition Explicit Elicitation No CF Metalinguistic Recasts

Very effective

Effective

Ineffective

Very ineffective

Novice

Experienced

Novice

Experienced

Novice

Neutral Experienced

Novice

Experienced

Novice

Experienced

20 15 5 5 5 0 10

25 20 25 15 0 10 20

40 35 10 45 10 15 60

45 50 50 50 5 50 55

20 20 20 30 20 40 25

15 10 15 15 15 25 10

15 20 40 15 45 35 5

10 10 10 15 50 15 15

5 10 25 5 20 10 0

5 10 0 5 30 0 0

source of their rigid cognitions about CF. They believed that what worked for them as language learners will work for others as well, and they were concerned with saving their students' face. Elham's comments are most compelling. Telling students directly that their sentences are incorrect can make them anxious and discourage them from speaking in front of their classmates. I remember when my friend and I were learning English; we didn't like to be mocked by our classmates, when our teachers corrected our errors by explaining that what we said was incorrect, and why it was incorrect. (Elham, Novice teacher, Interview: 11/12/2013) Another novice teacher, Siamak, stated: I believe teachers should not point out students' errors explicitly. As a language learner, whenever I received explicit feedback, I got anxious and lost track of my thought because I had to think about what I was going to say and also listen to the teachers' feedback. But when the teacher did not interrupt me by just repeating correct form of my errors, I would continue speaking without becoming very anxious and losing track of what I wanted to say. (Siamak, Novice teacher, Interview: 11/12/2013) However, the experienced teachers considered both implicit and explicit CF types almost equally effective. They rated recast and explicit CF as the most effective (75% each), clarification and repetition as the second most effective (70% each), elicitation (65%) and metalinguistic (60%) as the third and fourth most effective, and no CF (5%) as the least effective CF types. These findings indicate that, although the experienced teachers provided explicit CF on their students' utterances that deviated from the target forms, they also provided opportunities for their students to do self-correction instead of entirely relying on their teachers' correction. Explicit CF assists learners in acquiring target forms by drawing their attention to the error, whereas indirect CF can help learners acquire self-editorial skills (Rahimi & Zhang, 2013). Our findings suggest that the experienced teachers believed in the equal effectiveness of various CF types. Further, followup interviews with the experienced teachers revealed that these teachers considered the significant role of contextual and individual factors in providing CF (e.g., students' proficiency level, level of anxiety, and types of error). The experienced teachers commented that “The effectiveness of CF types depends on the type of error, the type of learning task, and the student who makes the error” (Nasim, Experienced Teacher, Interview: 13/12/2013). They also stated that their classroom practices have helped them realize the importance of different types of CF, as is shown in an experienced teacher, Nasim's comments: My teaching experience tells me that I should monitor what works and how it works for each individual learner's different errors in different learning activities; there is no one single CF type that works for all individuals and all error types in all learning activities” (Nasim, Experienced teacher, Interview: 13/12/2013). The experienced teachers direct students' attention to the ill-formed utterances and allow students to self-correct errors that they can self-correct (Ellis, 2009). These teachers believed that providing explicit feedback in such cases was inappropriate because the teachers' explicit feedback in such cases may deny learners opportunities to produce “pushed output” (Swain & Lapkin, 2002), which is believed to be conducive to L2 acquisition. In light of the interview data, on the other hand, if the experienced teacher believed that the students cannot self-correct errors (e.g., after providing opportunities for self- and peer correction), they give explicit CF. When my students make errors, I alert them to do self-correction. I do not want to interfere with the flow of their communication. If it is a serious error, which makes their production incomprehensible, I give them the correct form of their errors. If they do not understand my correction and repeat the error, I explain the underlying rules because it shows that they have not learned the rules yet. Even sometimes I ask them to repeat the correct form several times, for example, in the case of pronunciation errors (Mohsen, Experienced teacher, Interview: 13/12/2013). The experienced teachers also believed in providing explicit CF as they considered metalinguistic knowledge significant in their students' learning journey (Rahim & Zhang, 2013; Zhang, 2010). Overall, the experienced teachers' cognitions about CF (their being in favour of both CF types), which might appear contradictory to an outsider, indicate their flexible cognitions about CF. These teachers attributed their flexibility in their cognition to their considering the role of individual learner factors,

118

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

types and severity of errors, instructional focus, and task types in providing CF. Although classroom teaching experience was the most important difference between the two groups and both groups had received similar education and teacher training program, the differences between the two groups ’cognitions about CF can partially be attributed to their teaching experience. Both the quantitative and follow-up qualitative data indicate the flexibility of the experienced teachers' cognitions about CF and the novice teachers’ rigidity. While the experienced teachers believed that their teaching experiences influenced their cognitions about CF (e.g., Kang & Cheng, 2014), novice teachers highlighted the role of their own language learning experience in affecting their cognitions. It is unexpected that neither group mentioned their teacher education programs as a factor influencing their cognitions about CF, which corroborate some recent findings along this line of research (e.g., Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Va'squez & Harvey, 2010). 4.3. Timing of error correction Questions 2 to 5 in the questionnaire were designed to elicit teachers' cognitions about the effectiveness of the four different times to correct learners' oral production errors. The category comprised four items: (3) immediately after errors occur even it interrupts oral production, (4) right at the end of students' oral production, (5) after the activities end, and (6) at the conclusion of class. Table 5 shows the teachers’ cognitions about CF timing. Among the four options, “at the conclusion of class” received the lowest mean from novice and experienced teachers (M ¼ 2.30 and M ¼ 2.55, respectively). “As soon as errors are made” received the highest mean score from the experienced teachers (M ¼ 4.05), whereas “after the student finishes speaking” received the highest mean (M ¼ 3.80) from the novice teachers. Table 5 also shows that the differences between the novice teachers and experienced teachers’ cognitions about the effectiveness of immediate CF, t(38) ¼ 5.522, p < 0.001, d ¼ 1.75, and CF after the activity, t(38) ¼ 3.688, p ¼ 0.001, d ¼ 1.16 were statistically significant; the effect sizes for both were large. In other words, the experienced teachers believed that immediate CF and CF after the activity were appropriate times for error treatment, while novice teachers did not believe in the effectives of CF provision in these two times. CF after the student finishes speaking was considered appropriate by both groups, while CF at the end of class was conceived by both groups inappropriate. The survey results are illustrated in Table 6. Eighty percent of the experienced teachers rated immediate CF very effective or effective. On the contrary, a mere 15% of the novice teachers considered correcting their students' erroneous utterances as soon as they occurred. For “CF after students finish talking”, there was no statistically significant difference between the novice teachers (70%) and experienced teachers (75%). The difference was negligible; almost the same percentage of both groups regarded “CF after students finish talking” as the suitable time to correct students' erroneous forms. There were statistically significant discrepancies between the two groups of teachers with regard to correcting errors “after the activities”. Seventy percent of the experienced teachers rated “after the activities” very effective or effective. They agreed that learners' errors should be treated “after the activities”, whereas only 20% of the novice teachers regarded correction “after the activities” effective. With regard to treating students' errors at the conclusion of class, 70% of the novice and 60% of the experienced teachers considered correcting students' errors at this time very ineffective or ineffective. They believed that the students' erroneous utterances should be corrected right after they occur even if CF hampers the flow of students' oral production. They were focused more on drawing learners' attention to accuracy in their spoken English; the experienced teachers regarded accuracy and fluency as crucial factors for their students' interlanguage development. Both groups believed that treating students' errors after students complete their talk can augment the accuracy and fluency of their oral production since it provides opportunities to direct students’ attention to their erroneous utterance without interrupting the flow of their oral production. Consistent with their cognitions about the effectiveness of CF types, the experienced teachers' flexibility and novice teachers’ rigidity in their cognitions about the effectiveness of four different timings for giving CF emerged from the interview data. The experienced teachers rejected the either-implicit-or-explicit approach to CF, and attributed their cognitions about CF to their classroom teaching experience, as explicitly stressed by Mohsen: For timing of CF, I think, it depends on the significance of the error. If it makes the students utterance incomprehensible, I will immediately correct the error to help her make her point clear. Also, if it is a repeated error, I will try to draw the students' attention to the point immediately; otherwise, I will wait for the students to finish their talk. Also, it depends on the type of activities. If the focus of the lesson is grammar, I will correct immediately. If we are working on fluency, I Table 5 Comparison of responses on the timing of error correction: novice vs. experienced teachers. Timing of feedback

Experienced M

SD

M

SD

Immediate CF CF after students finish talking CF after the activity CF at the conclusion of class

4.05 3.80 3.60 2.55

0.95 0.83 0.94 0.89

2.45 3.65 2.50 2.30

0.89 0.88 0.95 0.80

Note. d ¼ effect size.

Novice

t

df

Sig. (2-Tailed)

d

5.52 0.56 3.69 0.94

38 38 38 38

0.000 0.582 0.001 0.356

1.73 0.17 1.16 0.29

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

119

Table 6 Teachers’ responses on the timing of error correction (%): less Exp.ts vs. more Exp.ts. Timing of feedback

Immediate CF CF after students finish talking CF after the activity CF at the conclusion of class

Very effective

Effective

Neutral

Ineffective

Very ineffective

Less Exp.ts

Exp.ts

Less Exp.ts

Exp.ts

Less Exp.ts

Exp.ts

Less Exp.ts

Exp.ts

Less Exp.ts

15 60 20 10

45 60 60 20

25 15 20 20

10 15 10 20

50 15 50 60

10 10 20 55

10

10

35 15 10

10 10

Exp.ts

5

will do the correction after the student finishes. Through teaching over the years, I have learned the tricks of the trade, I think. (Mohsen, Experienced teacher, Interview: 13/12/2013) On the other hand, novice teachers referred to their language learning experience as a source of their cognitions about the effectiveness of providing CF after students finish talking. They were concerned about the emotional reaction of their students because of their own learning experience. …if it is a serious error, I take notes and teach the form directly to the whole class after we finish speaking tasks without pointing at the student who made the error. This way, they can learn the form without being laughed at, without becoming anxious, and without losing their self-confidence. I have learned this from one of my English language teachers whom I liked very much because of her error correction style. (Elham, Novice teacher, Interview: 11/12/2013) In line with Kang and Cheng's (2014) study, the findings of this study highlight the role of classroom teaching experience in the development of teachers' cognitions. As such, providing pre-service teachers with approaches to theorize from their teaching is imperative. This can help novice teachers understand the role of contextual and individual learner factors in the effectiveness and efficiency of their teaching approaches. Empowered with reflective teaching (Farrell, 1999, 2014) during inservice or pre-service teacher education, followed by classroom observations, working with experienced teachers, and seeking experienced teachers' arguments for their choice of approach can help novice teachers become aware of their choices and seek to justify their preference for their classroom practices by focussing on the impact of their preferences on their students' learning outcome (Guskey, 2002). With regard to the development of teachers' cognitions about CF, Ellis' (2009) guidelines for CF should be used to raise teachers' awareness of different types of CF, timing of CF, and necessity of CF, among others. Then, through reflection in, on, and for, action (Farrell, 2014), joining a community of professionals (Richards, 2008), and monitoring the impact of their classroom practice with regard to CF on their students' learning outcomes, teachers can be mentored to learn how to theorize from their informed evidence-based practices to come up with solutions to their local issues (Zhang & Ben Said, 2014; Zhang & Rahimi, 2014). They can also contribute to theory by theorizing their practice, namely, taking a cyclical approach (Kang & Cheng, 2014). That is, as discussed previously, novice teachers can be informed by recent theories through reading recent research articles and meta-analyses about the topic and discussing the relevance of the theory to their context with experienced teachers. Their reflective practice can also be used as a source of evidence and data from their teaching context, which can be used to inform theory and other teachers’ classroom practices for mutual professional growth. 5. Conclusion Li (2010) meta-analysis of the second language acquisition (SLA) literature on CF shows that overall feedback was effective and the effective sizes ranged from medium to large and such effects were maintained over time. Teacher cognition research has also identified factors influencing teachers' cognitions about CF (Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Mackey et al., 2004). Building upon the prior teacher cognition literature on CF, the findings of our study extends prior research findings by revealing that teacher education alone, without reflective teaching experiences, does not transform teachers' cognitions, and novice teachers start teaching with rigid cognitions about CF. Novice teachers extrapolate their personal language learning experiences to all other language learners without taking into account the factors that experienced teachers report considering in providing CF due to their teaching experiences, which lead to flexibility in their cognitions about CF. Our findings suggest that as non-native English speaking teachers gain more classroom teaching experience in an EFL context, they value the role of CF in learners' interlanguage development to a larger extent than the novice teachers do. This finding is in contrast with the stated CF beliefs of teachers working in English-as-a-second-language (ESL) contexts (see Junqueira & Kim, 2013), highlighting the role of context in influencing teachers' cognitions' about CF. Our findings about the experienced teachers' flexibility in their cognitions about types and timing of CF and novice teachers' rigidity corroborate Junqueira and Kim's (2013) results with regard to ESL teachers' practices in providing CF. Such findings indicate that the experienced teachers, through theorizing from their practice (e.g., Kang & Cheng, 2014), realize that there is no single panacea for correcting all kinds of errors. That is, depending on the particularities of their teaching contexts, teachers choose the approaches that their practices inform them as the most appropriate for their particular group of learners. On the contrary, the novice teachers' cognitions suggest that they have distinct tendencies in their cognitions about CF and believe that implicit CF and explicit CF at the conclusion of talk or activity is the most effective regardless of the diversity of classroom context variables (e.g., learners' attributes, error types, and task types). Furthermore, in line with previous studies

120

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

(Borg, 2006; Junqueira & Kim, 2013), our follow-up interview data show that the novice teachers' cognitions about CF was mainly informed by their personal language learning experiences. Moreover, akin to the findings of Junqueira and Kim (2013), teacher education programs did not emerge as a key factor informing teachers' cognitions about CF in our study. It appears that these teachers' assigned readings in the teacher education programs did not seem to influence their cognitions about teaching, or to be more specific, about CF. This finding lends support to prior research findings that reading research articles alone might not significantly affect teachers’ cognitions and practices (Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Kagan, 1992; Kennedy, 1997; Va'squez & Harvey, 2010). Although reading articles can provide the student teachers with the theoretical necessity and raise their awareness of instructional procedures and strategies, it does not provide them with opportunities for reflecting in, on, and for, action (Farrell, 2014) and for theorizing teaching and CF from their practices. In light of our findings, and in agreement with other scholars (Farrell, 2014; Junqueira & Kim, 2013; Va'squez & Harvey, 2010), we suggest that through teacher education programs offered at teacher-education institutions and agencies, teachers-in-training be empowered with the theoretical knowledge of CF and practical skills of responsive CF provision. For instance, Ellis' (2009) guidelines on CF and other meta-analytic reviews of effectiveness of CF (e.g., Brown, 2014; Li, 2010; Lyster & Saito, 2010) can provide the student teachers with such theoretical knowledge, and Farrell (2014) proposal for “reflection in, on, and for, action in teacher learning” can help student teachers acquire theoretical knowledge of reflective practice. Follow-up reflective practice can provide opportunities for teachers' responsive provision of CF. This might result in the flexibility in teachers' cognitions about CF and lead them to consider the role of particularities of teaching contexts (e.g., classroom setting variables, learner variables, task type and target form variables) in their provision of CF. It is highly possible that such reflective practice will transfer to other aspects of their teaching and result in the effective and efficient teacher development, with positive outcomes for their students' learning. In short, in order for teachers to benefit from theoretical knowledge, teacher education programs should be designed with due consideration of not only building teachers’ theoretical knowledge but also providing them with opportunities to apply their theoretical knowledge reflectively in various teaching contexts. Admittedly, we note three limitations in our study. First, as we were interested in exploring the role of classroom experience in teachers' cognitions about three aspects of CF, we did not collect data in relation to their actual classroom practices in providing CF. Teachers might develop certain cognitions with regard to certain aspects of teaching but might not practise them in reality accordingly owing to a multitude of factors. As such, this is an obvious shortcoming of our study. Future research can further illustrate the role of experience in teachers' cognitions about, and practices in providing CF. There is also a need to find out the sources of teachers’ cognitions about CF. Second, our study focused only on the role of teachers' teaching experience in one teaching context. As such, future studies might need to compare the impact of sociocultural contexts on teachers' cognitions about and practices in providing CF. Furthermore, only three aspects of teachers’ cognitions about CF (necessity, types, and timing) were explored in our study with a limited number of participants. Future research can expand this line of inquiry. Methodologically, although we employed a mixed methods approach to collect the data, because of our small sample size, further studies are in order so that the issues we explored can be crystalized. Developing or adapting a more comprehensive questionnaire, using more robust statistical analysis, and validating the instrument with a large number of random samples of participants can boost the validity and reliability of the instruments and findings. Likewise, in-depth case studies of teachers can also shed more light on teachers' cognitions about and practices in other dimensions of CF, including teachers' perceptions of the severity of the errors and their provision of CF and the focus of the lesson (content vs. form) and teachers’ provision of CF, among other aspects. Acknowledgements We declare that this research has not been supported financially or whatsoever by any funding agency. We are also indebted to the Editors' for their patience during the revision. We are particularly grateful to Dr Andy Gao for his detailed suggestions for revising our paper. All faults remain ours. References Akbari, R., & Tajik, L. (2010). Teachers' pedagogic knowledge base: A comparison between experienced and less experienced practitioners. Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 34(6), 52e73. http://dx.doi.org/10.14221/ajte.2009v34n6.4. Akbari, R., & Tajik, L. (2012). Second-language teachers' moral knowledge base: A comparison between experienced and less experienced male and female practitioners. Journal of Moral Education, 41(1), 39e59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2011.630384. Akyel, A. (1997). Experienced and student EFL teachers' instructional thoughts and actions. Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 678e704. Atai, M. A., & Fatahi-Majd, M. (2014). Exploring the practices and cognitions of Iranian ELT instructors and subject teachers in teaching EAP reading comprehension. English for Specific Purposes, 33, 27e38. Baker, A. (2014). Exploring teachers' knowledge of second language pronunciation techniques: Teacher cognitions, observed classroom practices, and student perceptions. TESOL Quarterly, 48, 136e163. Baleghizadeh, S., & Nasrollahi Shahri, M. N. (2014). EFL teachers' conceptions of speaking competence in English. Teachers and Teaching, 20, 738e754. Barnard, R., & Burns, A. (2012). Researching language teacher cognition and practice: International case studies. Buffalo, NY: Multilingual Matters. Basturkmen, H. (2012). Review of research into the correspondence between language teachers' stated beliefs and practices. System, 40, 282e295. Berliner, D. (1986). In pursuit of expert pedagogue. Educational Researcher, 15, 5e13. Borg, S. (2003). Teacher cognition in language teaching: A review of research on what language teachers think, know, believe, and do. Language Teaching, 36, 81e109.

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

121

Borg, S. (2006). Teacher cognition and language education: Research and practice. London: Continuum. Borg, S. (2009). Language teacher education. In A. Burns, & J. C. Richards (Eds.), The Cambridge guide to second language teacher education (pp. 163e171). New York: Cambridge University Press. Borg, S. (2011). The impact of in-service teacher education on language teachers' beliefs. System, 39, 370e380. Borg, S., & Burns, A. (2008). Integrating grammar in adult TESOL classrooms. Applied Linguistics, 29, 456e482. Brown, D. (2014). The type and linguistic foci of oral corrective feedback in the L2 classroom: A meta-analysis. Language Teaching Research. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1177/1362168814563200. Chaudron, C. (1986). Teachers' priorities in correcting learners' errors in French immersion classes. In R. R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn (pp. 64e84). Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Chaudron, C. (1988). Second language classrooms. New York: Cambridge University Press. Doganay, A., & Ozturk, A. (2011). An investigation of experienced and inexperienced primary school teachers' teaching process in science and technology classes in terms of metacognitive strategies. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, 11, 1320e1325. Ellis, R. (2009). Corrective feedback and teacher development. L2 Journal, 1, 3e18. Fanselow, J. (1977). The treatment of error in oral work. Foreign Language Annals, 10, 583e593. Farhady, H., & Hedayati, H. (2009). Language assessment policy in Iran. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 29, 132e141. Farrell, T. S. C. (1999). Reflective practice in an EFL teacher development group. System, 27, 157e172. Farrell, T. S. C. (2009). The novice teacher experience. In A. Burns, & J. C. Richards (Eds.), The Cambridge guide to second language teacher education (pp. 182e189). New York: Cambridge University Press. Farrell, T. S. (2014). ‘Teacher you are stupid!’ e Cultivating a reflective disposition. TESL-EJ, 18(3). Retrieved from: http://www.tesl-ej.org/wordpress/issues/ volume18/ej71/ej71a2/. Ferris, D. (2014). Responding to student writing: Teachers' philosophies and practices. Assessing Writing, 19, 6e23. Fontana, A., & Frey, J. H. (1994). Interviewing: The art of science. In N. K. Denzin, & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 361e376). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Fukuda, Y. (2004). Treatment of spoken errors in Japanese high school oral communication classes. Unpublished Master's thesis. San Francisco, CA, USA: California State University. Gao, X., & Ma, Q. (2011). Vocabulary learning and teaching beliefs of pre-service and in-service teachers in Hong Kong and Mainland China. Language Awareness, 20(4), 327e342. Gatbonton, E. (2008). Looking beyond teachers' classroom behaviour: Novice and experienced ESL teachers' pedagogical knowledge. Language Teaching Research, 12(2), 161e182. Goh, C. C. M., Zhang, L. J., Koh, G. H., & Ng, C. H. (2005). Knowledge, beliefs and syllabus implementation: A study of English language teachers in Singapore. Singapore: National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University. Graham, S., Santos, D., & Francis-Brophy, E. (2014). Teacher beliefs about listening in a foreign language. Teaching and Teacher Education, 40, 44e60. Guskey, T. R. (2002). Professional development and teacher change. Teachers and Teaching, 8, 381e391. Junqueira, L., & Kim, Y. (2013). Exploring the relationship between training, beliefs, and teachers' corrective feedback practices: A case study of a novice and an experienced ESL teacher. Canadian Modern Language Review, 69, 181e206. Kagan, D. (1992). Implications of research on teacher belief. Educational Psychologist, 27, 65e90. Kamiya, N. (2014). The relationship between stated beliefs and classroom practices of oral corrective feedback. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching, 1e14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17501229.2014.939656 (Ahead-of-print). Kang, Y., & Cheng, X. (2014). Teacher learning in the workplace: A study of the relationship between a novice EFL teacher's classroom practices and cognition development. Language Teaching Research, 18, 169e186. Kennedy, M. (1997). The connection between research and practice. Educational Researcher, 26, 4e12. Kumaravadivelu, B. (2014). Afterword: Rethinking global perspectives and local initiatives in language teaching. In S. Ben Said (Ed.), Language teachers and teaching: Global perspectives, local initiatives (pp. 317e323). New York, NY: Routledge. Li, S. (2010). The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60, 309e365. Long, M. H. (1977). Teacher feedback on learner error: Mapping cognition. In D. H. Brown, C. C. A. Yorio, & T. R. Crymes (Eds.), vol. 77. On TESOL (pp. 278e294). Washington, DC: TESOL. Lyster, R., & Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake: Negotiation of form in communicative classrooms. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 37e66. Lyster, R., & Saito, K. (2010). Oral feedback in classroom SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 265e302. Mackey, A., Polio, C., & McDonough, K. (2004). The relationship between experience, education and teachers' use of incidental focus-on-form techniques. Language Teaching Research, 8, 301e327. Melnick, S. A., & Meister, D. G. (2008). A comparison of beginning and experienced teachers' concerns. Educational Research Quarterly, 31(3), 39e56. Mok, W. D. (1994). Reflecting on reflections: A case study of experienced and inexperienced ESL teachers. System, 22, 93e111. Mori, R. (2011). Teacher cognition in corrective feedback in Japan. System, 39, 451e467. Nassaji, H. (2013). Participation structure and incidental focus on form in adult ESL classrooms. Language Learning, 63, 835e869. Nunan, D. (1992). The teacher as decision-maker. In J. Flowerdew, M. Brock, & S. Hsia (Eds.), Perspectives on second language teacher education (pp. 135e165). Hong Kong: City Polytechnic. Nystrom, N. J. (1983). Teacher-student interaction in bilingual classrooms: Four approaches to error correction. In H. W. Seliger, & M. H. Long (Eds.), Classroom-oriented research in second language acquisition (pp. 169e189). Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Pajares, M. F. (1992). Teachers' beliefs and educational research: Cleaning up a messy construct. Review of Educational Research, 62(3), 307e332. Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Peterson, P. L., & Comeaux, M. (1987). Teachers' schemata for classroom events: The mental scaffolding of teachers' thinking during classroom instruction. Teaching and Teacher Education, 3, 319e331. Rahmani Doqaruni, V. (2015). Communication strategies in experienced vs. inexperienced teachers' talk: A sign of transformation in teacher cognition. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17501229.2015.1009071. Rahimi, M., & Zhang, L. J. (2013). The role of incidental unfocused prompts and recasts in improving EFL learners' accuracy. Language Learning Journal. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1080/09571736.2013.858368. Rassaei, E. (2013). Corrective feedback, learners' perceptions, and second language development. System, 41, 472e483. Richards, J. C. (2008). Second language teacher education today. RELC Journal, 39, 158e177. Richards, J. C., Li, B., & Tang, A. (1998). Exploring pedagogical reasoning beliefs. In J. C. Richards (Ed.), Beyond training. New York: Cambridge University Press. Roothooft, H. (2014). The relationship between adult EFL teachers' oral feedback practices and their beliefs. System, 46, 65e79. Safarnavadeh, K. (2004). A comparative analysis of the English language curriculum in Iran, Japan, and Pakistan. Unpublished Master’s thesis. Tehran, Iran: Teacher Education University. Shegar, C., Zhang, L. J., & Low, E. L. (2013). Effects of an input-output mapping practice task on EFL learners' acquisition of two grammatical structures in English. System, 44, 283e308. Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2002). Talking it through: Two French immersion students' response to reformulation. International Journal of Educational Research, 3, 285e304. Tschannen-Moran, M., & Hoy, A. W. (2007). The differential antecedents of self-efficacy beliefs of novice and experienced teachers. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23(6), 944e956.

122

M. Rahimi, L.J. Zhang / System 55 (2015) 111e122

Tsui, A. B. (2009). Distinctive qualities of expert teachers. Teachers and Teaching: Theory and practice, 15(4), 421e439. squez, C., & Harvey, J. (2010). Raising teachers' awareness about corrective feedback through research replication. Language Teaching Research, 14, Va 421e443. Watzke, J. L. (2007). Foreign language pedagogical knowledge: Toward a developmental theory of beginning teacher practice. Modern Language Journal, 91, 63e82. Zhang, L. J. (2010). A dynamic metacognitive systems account of Chinese university students' knowledge about EFL reading. TESOL Quarterly, 44, 320e353. Zhang, L. J., & Ben Said, S. (2014). Toward a global understanding of local initiatives in language teaching and teacher education: Global rules, local roles. In S. Ben Said, & L. J. Zhang (Eds.), Language teachers and teaching: Global perspectives, local initiatives (pp. xviiiexxx). New York, NY: Routledge. Zhang, L. J., & Rahimi, M. (2014). EFL learners' anxiety level and their beliefs about corrective feedback in oral communication classes. System, 42, 429e439.