Factors influencing the success of exposure therapy for specific phobia: A systematic review

Factors influencing the success of exposure therapy for specific phobia: A systematic review

Journal Pre-proof Factors influencing the success of exposure therapy for specific phobia: A systematic review Joscha B¨ohnlein, Luisa Altegoer, Nina Kr...

6MB Sizes 0 Downloads 23 Views

Journal Pre-proof Factors influencing the success of exposure therapy for specific phobia: A systematic review Joscha B¨ohnlein, Luisa Altegoer, Nina Kristin Muck, Kati Roesmann, Ronny Redlich, Udo Dannlowski, Elisabeth J. Leehr

PII:

S0149-7634(19)30404-X

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.009

Reference:

NBR 3622

To appear in:

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

Received Date:

29 May 2019

Revised Date:

26 November 2019

Accepted Date:

6 December 2019

Please cite this article as: B¨ohnlein J, Altegoer L, Muck NK, Roesmann K, Redlich R, Dannlowski U, Leehr EJ, Factors influencing the success of exposure therapy for specific phobia: A systematic review, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews (2019), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.009

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier.

Factors influencing the success of exposure therapy for specific phobia: A systematic review. Joscha Böhnlein1,*, Luisa Altegoer1, Nina Kristin Muck1, Kati Roesmann², Ronny Redlich1, Udo Dannlowski1, & Elisabeth J. Leehr1.

1

Department of Psychiatry, University of Münster, Germany Institute for Biomagnetism and Biosignalanalysis, University of Münster, Germany

ro of

2

Running Title: Influencing factors on Exposure Therapy in Specific Phobia

Abstract Word count: 172



re Number of figures: 1

Most researched factors have not been studied enough to give concrete recommendations Factors enhancing inhibitory learning experience increase success most These are having a high self-efficacy, low trait anxiety, addressing disgust in the therapy, focusing on changing cognitions, changing contexts between ET sessions, focusing on heart rate changes and administering D-Cycloserine

Jo

 

Article word count: 8436

ur

Highlights

na

Number of tables: 3

lP

Joscha Böhnlein Department of Psychiatry University of Münster Albert-Schweitzer-Campus 1, Building A9 48149 Münster; Germany, Phone: +49251-83-51861 Email: [email protected]

-p

*Corresponding author:

ABSTRACT Although Exposure Therapy (ET) is the first-line treatment of Specific Phobia (SP), there is no clear consensus on which factors influence its success, and thus on how to conduct it

most efficiently. This review summarizes the current state of research regarding this topic. N=111 studies were in accordance with our eligibility criteria: participants had at least symptoms of SP, the intervention was ET and the study investigated a factor influencing its success. Best evidence for positive effects was found for low trait anxiety, high motivation and high self-efficacy before the ET, high cortisol levels and heart rate variation, evoking disgust additionally to anxiety, avoiding relaxation, focusing on cognitive changes, context variation, sleep, and memory-enhancing drugs. These factors may be conceptualized as modulating different aspects of learning as suggested in current models of ET that focus on

ro of

inhibitory learning mechanisms. Limitations lie in the great heterogeneity concerning operationalization of factors and success. Based on these findings, we make suggestions for

-p

improvements in ET conduction and which factors should be researched in the future.

Jo

ur

na

lP

re

Keywords: Specific Phobia, Exposure Therapy, Treatment outcome, Psychotherapy

1. INTRODUCTION Specific Phobias (SP) are some of the most prevalent psychiatric disorders, with adverse effects on patients’ everyday lives. Gustavsson (2011) numbers the total cost of anxiety disorders with over €70 billion. While anxiety disorders are the largest group of psychiatric disorders with a 12-month prevalence of 14.0%, SP is the most prevalent subgroup of these. With a 12-month prevalence of 6,4%, 22.7 million people are affected in Europe (Wittchen et al., 2011). Additionally, SP represents a risk factor for other psychiatric disorders: Patients with SP show a twofold risk of developing any anxiety disorder, depression, and any

ro of

somatoform disorder (Trumpf et al., 2010). Subclinical anxiety is even more prevalent: Curtis, Magee, Eaton, Wittchen and Kessler (1998) show that 49,5% of a large US-American sample report a “lifetime occurrence of an unreasonably strong fear” of a specific stimulus.

-p

Current models of SP explain its development through different processes of conditioning and learning like classical and operant conditioning and model learning. This has been most

re

prominently described by Rachmann (1977), and is still relevant to this day (Duits et al.,

lP

2015; Hamm, 2009; Lissek et al., 2005; Mineka and Zinbarg, 2006; Schweckendiek et al., 2011). Thus, a fear memory is formed (Foa and Kozak, 1986). In this fear structure, the feared (but harmless) stimulus is connected to the concepts of danger and corresponding

na

reactions like panic, flight and physiological reactions like activity of the sympathetic nervous system. Extinction based-treatments such as Exposure Therapy (ET) were developed to

ur

lessen the effect of the fear structure by either replacing (Foa and Kozak, 1986), competing with (Foa and McNally, 1996) or inhibiting (Craske et al., 2014) it.

Jo

Exposure Therapy (ET) is the latest of a group of therapy forms that evolved from Wolpe’s Systematic Desensitization (1954) in the early fifties of the last century. It has become the first-choice therapy form not only for specific phobia but for many different anxiety disorders (Bandelow et al., 2014) and, in slightly adjusted form, for other diseases like obsessivecompulsive disorder (Öst et al., 2015). Investigating the success of ET in patients with different anxiety disorders, Bandelow and colleagues (2015) find considerable pre-post effect sizes of d = 1.3. Modern variations of ET include Virtual Reality ET (VRET), which shows

comparable effect sizes in meta analyses and systematic reviews (Botella et al., 2017; Opriş et al., 2012). It is recommended as the first-choice treatment of specific phobias (Bandelow et al., 2014). But even though ET is seen as the most effective way to treat SP, it is not without its problems: In a systematic review for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents (excluding SP), James and colleagues (2015) call ET as the central intervention of a CognitiveBehavioral Therapy (CBT) “an effective treatment for childhood and adolescent anxiety

ro of

disorders”. However, they could not show superiority to other treatments such as medication or other forms of psychotherapy, especially at follow-up. Additionally, about a fifth of anxiety disorders patients dropped out prematurely (James et al., 2015). Furthermore, a third of treatment completers have to be classified as non-responders, irrespective of the specific

-p

type of therapy (Craske and Mystkowski, 2006; Fernandez et al., 2015; Scholten et al., 2013; Taylor et al., 2012). Additionally, the underlying mechanisms of ET are not yet well

re

understood and are subject of discussion (e.g. Craske et al., 2014; Rupp et al., 2017). Even

lP

seemingly basic aspects like the necessary length of a successful ET are not yet clear (Bandelow et al., 2014). Overall, factors discriminating a successful from an unsuccessful ET-outcome are controversially discussed (Lueken et al., 2016; Taylor et al., 2012), and

na

there is a high degree of heterogeneity in proposed and executed minutiae. Some reviews have tackled the need for an overview of factors that contribute to the success (or the lack

ur

thereof) of ET, both within the patient as well as in the conduction of the ET: For example, Lueken and colleagues (2016) presented potential neurobiological markers predicting the

Jo

success of ET in all subtypes of anxiety disorders. They show that there is evidence for the 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 variant as a predictive genetic marker, as well as anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activity as a functional neuroimaging marker and cardiovascular flexibility as a third marker. Additionally, they emphasize the high methodological heterogeneity of included studies and call for more rigorous, standardized studies. To sum up, ET is the first-choice treatment for SP, but it is neither as effective nor as well understood as one would hope. In the following review, we want to help both practitioners as

well as scientists to improve the effectiveness and our understanding of ET. We will expand on these existing reviews by giving an overview over possible influencing factors on the success (or lack thereof) of ET in SP. We take the perspective that a successful ET can be seen in changed behavior (e.g. in a Behavioral Avoidance Test, BAT), changed cognitions (e.g. in a questionnaire), changed subjective fear (e.g. in lower Subjective Units of Distress, SUDs) or changed physiology (e.g. different heart rates when confronted with the feared stimulus). Ideally, a successful ET leads to changes in every one of these categories. Additionally, modern cognitive-behavioral models of exposure therapy like the “inhibitory

ro of

learning” model by Craske and colleagues (Craske et al., 2008) state that ET works by enabling the patient to learn to inhibit their initial reaction to the feared stimulus. We therefore see these changes as a result of continuous learning processes that may take place before,

-p

during and after the ET itself. Hence, we divided potential influencing factors by their time of influence, i.e. if they play a part before, during or after the ET itself.

re

The aim of this review is twofold: Firstly, we try to show concisely which of these factors or

lP

these categories are highly relevant when planning and conducting an ET for patients with specific phobia, which factors have a lower relevance and for which factors the verdict is still out. This would help optimize the treatment and thus response rates. Secondly, we aim to

na

provide a current state of research in order to identify factors that should be investigated in

Jo

ur

more

detail.

2. METHODS This systematic review was conducted according to the PRISMA guidelines (Liberatî et al., 2009). To be eligible for inclusion in this review, articles had to adhere to the following inclusion PICOS (Participants, Intervention, Comparator, Outcome, Study Design) criteria (Liberatî et al., 2009):

ro of

(a) Participants had to be humans affected with specific phobia or significant symptoms of specific phobia. Included studies had (1) a sample of patients with specific phobia according to objective and standardized measures or interviews according to DSM-III, DSM-III-R, DSMIV or ICD-10 criteria (spider phobia, SP). The criteria changed very little between the different

-p

versions of the DSM and ICD, especially concerning the core criteria of anxiety and avoidance of a specific object, distress, and realization by the individual that the fear is

re

excessive. Hence, we did not make a difference between these classification systems. Furthermore, included studies used (2) a sample of participants with specific phobia

lP

according to self-report measures (e.g. Spider Phobia Questionnaire or Acrophobia Questionnaire), or (3) with symptoms of a specific phobia that did not fulfill the full criteria of a

na

clinical diagnosis (e.g. significant fear and avoidance of a feared stimulus in a BAT) (subclinical specific phobia, sub-SP). Studies with less than 10 participants per experimental

ur

condition were excluded.

(b) The Intervention had to be any form of exposure therapy (in vivo or in virtual reality; those

Jo

two forms will only be differentiated when it is the factor of interest). “Exposure therapy” is defined as any form of presenting the feared stimulus to the patient with the specific intent of reducing the emotional, cognitive, and somatic reaction to said stimulus. To ensure relevance of the presented results for ET as it is conducted today, studies conducting exposure with the instructed use of coping skills were excluded (e.g. systematic desensitization, see Rupp et al., 2017). An exception is made for studies that examine the effect of said coping skills and compare a therapy with versus a therapy without such skills.

Additionally, studies that presented the stimulus only in sensu were discarded due to the missing ability to standardize presentation. (c) Regarding the used comparator, only studies were included that measured within subject variables concerning their potential as a predictor of success or that had two groups that both underwent some form of ET that differed in one key aspect. (d) The outcome had to be a direct measure of treatment success (measured by questionnaires, behavioral outcome measures or standardized interviews), pre and post

ro of

intervention, and the influence of the investigated factor. (e) Concerning study design, included studies are controlled experiments in a laboratory or a naturalistic environment as long as the design is experimental in nature (i.e. a ET is part of

-p

the study and not of another treatment). Excluded are case studies and reviews. As independent variable, we required a potential influencing factor or factors; the dependent

re

variable was treatment success through exposure therapy.

We searched the databases PubMed and PsychInfo for studies published in peer-reviewed

lP

journals up to May 13th 2019 and performed hand searches in the reference lists of included studies. We used MeSH-Terms to search for studies with human subjects concerning

na

specific phobia and exposure. Included studies had to be written in English, German, French or Spanish. We did not limit the age of the subjects.

ur

The PubMed Search Term was as follows:

Jo

("phobic disorders"[MeSH Terms] NOT ("phobia, social"[MeSH Terms] OR (social[All Fields] AND ("anxiety"[MeSH Terms] OR "anxiety"[All Fields])))) AND ("exposure"[All Fields] AND "therapy"[All Fields] OR "exposure therapy"[All Fields] OR "Behavior Therapy"[Mesh] OR "exposure treatment"[All Fields] OR "exposure session"[All Fields] OR "fear modification"[All Fields] OR flooding OR "in vivo exposure"[All Fields] OR "imaginal exposure"[All Fields]) AND humans [Filter]

The search in PsychInfo was conducted in a way that mirrored the search in PubMed as closely as possible. JB screened studies found by the search terms for eligibility regarding title and abstract and, if deemed potentially relevant, subsequently assessed for eligibility according to the predefined PICOS criteria by JB and EJL in a full-text review. The interrater reliability was very high with a Pearson’s r = .927 (p <.001). A coding system (“Quality Score”, QS) on study characteristics was developed by JB and

ro of

EJL based on the one used by Lueken and colleagues (2016) as well as the MERSQI criteria (Reed et al., 2007) to assess quality and risk of bias of included studies (see Table 1). Both JB and EJL rated all included studies based on this system. Values of five and lower can be

-p

seen as low, values of ten and above as high. Insert Table 1 here.

re

We cannot eliminate the risk of bias in our results. We intentionally limited our search for

lP

relevant articles on well-known and respected databases with PubMed and PsychInfo. This, however, means that biases such as a publication bias are not only possible, but probable, all

included

studies

had

Jo

ur

na

since

to

be

accepted

by

a

peer-reviewed

journal.

3. RESULTS We included 111 studies in our review. The flowchart that shows numbers for studies screened, assessed for eligibility and subsequently included in the review as well as the reasons for exclusions can be found in Figure 1.

ro of

Insert Figure 1 here. A list of all the studies as well as relevant characteristics can be found in Table 2.

-p

Insert Table 2 here.

The following sections will present the included studies and their results that are most

re

important for our research question. We will present our results divided into the aforementioned categories: before the ET, during ET, and after ET. Each of these categories

lP

is then subdivided into the subcategories “emotional”, “cognitions”, “behavioral”, and “physiological”.

na

3.1 Before the ET

There were no researched factors in the behavioral category before the ET. In the emotional

ur

category, we did find three potential factors (trait anxiety with n=2 studies, motivation with n=2 studies and fear reactivation with n=2 studies), four potential cognitional factors (coping

Jo

style with n=3 studies, self-efficacy with n=4 studies, perceived automaticity and irrationality of threatening cognitions with n=1 study and memory with n=1 study) and finally three potential factors on the physiological level (eyeblink startle response with n=1 study, cortisol levels with n=2 studies and comorbid disorders with n=1 study). 3.1.1. Emotional factors

Trait anxiety and motivation. Two studies (Muris et al., 1998; Rodriguez et al., 1999) found that higher trait anxiety scores lead to less success of ET. Furthermore, St-Jacques and colleagues (2010a) showed that motivation was crucial for success, especially “extrinsic integrated motivation” as described in the Self-Determination Theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985). O’Sullivan and colleagues (1976) corroborated this finding. Fear reactivation. Two studies tried improving ET by reactivating fear just before ET: While Hodgson and Rachman (1970) found an improvement by showing a video with horrific

ro of

scenes that were however unrelated to the phobic objects just before implosion therapy, Shiban, Brutting, Pauli, & Mühlberger (2015a) could not replicate this finding in a much more recent study when reactivating the fear with the specific stimulus (spiders).

-p

3.1.2. Cognititive factors

Coping style. Two studies (Antony et al., 2001; Muris et al., 1993) did not find an impact of

re

coping styles on the success of ET, differentiating between “blunters” (people that tend to avoid threat-related information) and “monitors” (people that actively seek out threat-related

lP

information). However, the follow-up study to one of these (Muris et al., 1995) did find that “blunting” lead to better results. The coping style did not change because of the treatment in

na

the latter study.

Self-efficacy. Three studies found self-efficacy (both its level at the beginning and its change

ur

during the treatment) to be an important influencing factor concerning the success of ET (Bandura et al., 1977; Cote and Bouchard, 2009; Zoellner et al., 2000). Additionally, Williams

Jo

and colleagues (1984) found that a therapy focused on improving self-efficacy was as successful as one with exposition only. Cote and Bouchard (2009) additionally found changes in dysfunctional beliefs to be significantly associated with treatment success. Automaticity and irrationality of cognitions. Mayer and colleagues (2000) found that while perceived automaticity of cognitions was not related to treatment success, perceived irrationality was: For participants who rated their own cognitions as more irrational, the treatment was more successful.

Memory. The aforementioned study of Zoellner and colleagues (2000) also suggests that better memory for the anxious response, but not the phobic stimulus itself, is correlated with better treatment success. 3.1.3. Physiological factors Eyeblink startle response. Results by de Jong and colleagues (1991) suggest that larger eyeblink startle response in response to white noise while being confronted with a live spider (compared to while confronted with a household object) before an ET was associated with

ro of

higher success of ET. Cortisol levels. Lass-Hennemann and Michael (2014) find that, in their study, different cortisol levels in the morning compared to the evening explain differences in the success of ET

-p

conducted at these times due to enhanced extinction memory in the morning when cortisol levels are higher. Soravia and colleagues (2014) successfully used this finding by

re

administrating cortisol prior to ET and thereby improving reduction of fear.

lP

Comorbidity. One study (Ollendick et al., 2010) found that, in adolescent patients with comorbid specific phobias and anxiety disorders, ET targeting one specific phobia had a positive influence on the severity of the other comorbid phobia or anxiety disorder.

na

Furthermore, the success considering the targeted specific phobia was not affected by the existence of a comorbid anxiety disorder.

ur

3.2 During the ET

Jo

Most of the researched potential factors take effect during the ET. We found five potential factors in the emotional category (within and between session habituation with n=4 studies, disgust with n=5 studies, disgust evocation with n=3 studies, relaxation as an addition with n=3 studies, Positive Valence Training with n=1 study), four potential factors in the cognitional category (presence with n=1 study, coping self-statements with n=1 study, adding cognitive parts to ET with n=7 studies and attentional focus guiding with n= 6 studies), nine potential factors in the behavioral category (spontaneously shown safety behavior with n=1

study, evoked safety behavior with n=6 studies, , sex of the therapist with n=1 study, context variation with n=7 studies, stimulus variation with n=4 studies, length with n=11 studies computer assistance with n=14 studies, motion simulation with n=1 study, and additional manipulation with n=3 studies) 3.2.1 Emotional factors Within- and between-session anxiety decrease. Two studies found a correlation between a measurable between session habituation (BSH) and within session habituation (WSH) on the

ro of

one hand and overall success of the therapy on the other hand (Mühlberger et al., 2005a; Rachman and Levitt, 1988). Two more recent studies (with higher Quality Scores) did not find such a correlation (Baker et al., 2010; Busscher et al., 2015), especially when

-p

investigating long-term success.

Disgust. Regarding the relation between fear, disgust, and the success of ET, Smits and

re

colleagues (2002) found that anxiety and disgust declined independently from each other (anxiety declined more than disgust), but they did not find that global disgust levels were

lP

changed nor that disgust levels prior to the ET had an impact on anxiety level reduction (or vice versa). Similarly, Olatunji and colleagues (2007) found that in their study participants

na

with blood-injection—injury phobia (BII) found that anxiety declined more than disgust. However, in their study disgust and fear decline were not independent from each other. In

ur

two following studies, Olatunji et al. (2012, 2009) show fear and disgust and their respective decline to be independent from each other, with fear, but not disgust, being a significant

Jo

predictor of treatment success (measured by a BAT). Contrary to this, Matthews, Naran & Kirkby (2015) found that in their study, both anxiety and disgust reduction predicted treatment success (behavioral avoidance). Disgust evocation. Another line of research did not only measure both disgust and anxiety, but directly evoke disgust in participants with specific phobias: Both Choplin and Carter (2011) and Olatunji and colleagues (2009) were able to evoke disgust and reach a significant reduction of both anxiety and disgust by only showing disgusting pictures. They found no

differences in treatment success (both behavioral and subjective) between a group exposed to a disgusting stimulus compared to a group exposed to an anxiety-inducing stimulus, although Choplin and Carter (2011) found the anxiety-condition to have more long-lasting effects on behavior. De Jong and colleagues (2000) tried to countercondition the disgust elicited by an exposure to a spider in spider phobic individuals by presenting explicitly nondisgusting stimuli after the ET. They found no added effect on treatment success. Relaxation. Mühlberger and colleagues (2001a) find that relaxation alone is not as successful

ro of

in reducing anxiety as VRET alone is. Rimm and Medeiros (1970) find no enhancement by adding relaxation to ET, while McGlynn and colleagues (1999) even found a suppression of anxiety by a history of relaxation training.

Additional Positive Valence Training. Dour and colleagues (2016) suggest that Positive

-p

Valence Training, a training that tries to increase the valence of the feared stimulus, in this

Cognitive factors

lP

3.2.2

re

case spiders, by pointing out their usefulness, can enhance the effect of ET.

Presence and coping self-statements. Krijn and colleagues (2004) could not find an impact of presence, i.e. the feeling of actually being in a real environment while being in a Virtual

na

Reality, on the success of VRET. In another study, Krijn and colleagues (2007) showed that coping self-statements did not enhance the success of VRET.

ur

Adding Cognitive parts. One potential factor that drew much attention is the question whether ET with parts specifically addressing cognitions like cognitive preparation or cognitive tasks

Jo

in the ET is more successful than ET without such parts. Raes and colleagues (2011) did not find an advantageous effect of explicitly addressing cognitions in the ET, but did find that success was mediated by cognitive changes. Similarly, Tardif (2019) and colleagues showed that in their study, changes in cognitive beliefs and self-efficacy were predictive of ET success. Kircanski and colleagues (2012) found only marginally better results of ET augmented by affect labeling but not by guided threat-reappraisal, with Tabibnia (2008) corroborating this result. Dee’s (1972) study suggests that instructions seem to help – albeit

not significantly. However, Bandura and colleagues (1974) argue that “induction aids”, such as a gradual exposure and company, help in exposure and lead to better results. Furthermore, Sloan and Telch (2002) as well as a study by Kamphuis and Telch (2000) suggests that guided threat-reappraisal helped. Additionally one study found that a treatment without a cognitive part is effective, but less so than a treatment with cognitive contents (Van Gerwen et al., 2006). Finally, Koch and colleagues (2004) found a treatment without cognitive part as effective, but it was perceived as more intrusive.

ro of

Attentional Focus Guiding. Concerning attentional focus guiding: while one study finds that distraction is beneficial (Oliver and Page, 2008), three others find that distraction hinders extinction and lowers the success of ET (Dethier et al., 2015; Kamphuis and Telch, 2000; Telch et al., 2004). A third pair of studies find no effect of attentional focus guiding on

Behavioral factors

re

3.2.3

-p

treatment success (Antony et al., 2001; Waters et al., 2014).

Safety Behavior. Rentz and colleagues (Rentz et al., 2003) found that the (not instructed,

lP

spontaneously shown) use of safety behavior was associated with less improvement. In contrast to this, four studies find either no drawback or even small advantages of explicitly

na

instructing participants to use safety behavior (Deacon et al., 2010; Hood et al., 2010; Milosevic and Radomsky, 2008; Sy et al., 2011), and only one study found the expected

ur

disadvantage of an ET with instructed safety behavior (Sloan and Telch, 2002). Additionally, Wolitzky and Telch (2009) show that utilizing “fear antagonistic behavior”, which can be seen

Jo

as a form of safety behavior or distraction from the feared stimulus, leads to better behavioral success and more generalization. Sex of therapist. O’Sullivan and Gilner (1976) found that the sex of the therapist is a factor; female therapists seem to be beneficial regardless of the sex of the patient. Context variation. Mystkowski and colleagues (2002) have shown that in their study, exposition to the feared stimulus in a context different to the one used in the ET leads to return of fear, which is not equivalent to fear relapse, but is a partial fear recovery after ET

(Rachman, 1989). Five studies suggest that using several contexts in the ET leads to less return of fear and generally better treatment success (Bandarian-Balooch et al., 2015; Shiban et al., 2015a, 2013; Vansteenwegen et al., 2007b, 2007a), but two studies (Rodriguez et al., 1999; Shiban et al., 2015b) could not find such an effect. Stimulus variation. Both last-mentioned studies also looked at the effect of varying the stimulus itself within the ET. This question was addressed by a total of three studies: One of these (Shiban et al., 2015b) suggest that varying the stimulus is beneficial, while the other

ro of

two (Koch et al., 2004; Rodriguez et al., 1999) did not find a benefit of different stimuli. Length. When looking at the potential influence of length, one can further subdivide this

factor into three subcategories: number of sessions, length of the individual session and

-p

massed vs. expanding-spaced ET, which will be discussed in the following paragraphs.

In three different studies, Öst and his colleagues did not find any differences between a one-

re

session and a five-session ET (Hellström et al., 1996; Öst et al., 2001, 1997), although their latest study suggests a slight advantage of the five-session treatment one year after the ET

lP

(Vika et al., 2009). When comparing higher numbers of sessions, Fraser and colleagues (2001) found no difference between participants getting three versus six sessions of

na

vicarious exposure. Bornas and colleagues (2011) tackled the question differently by counting the numbers of sessions needed until they reached a successful reduction of

ur

subjective level of anxiety. Participants needed between one and seven sessions; the faster anxiety was reduced within the sessions, the fewer sessions were needed.

Jo

Length of individual sessions has been researched in three studies: Miller and Levis (1971) found a superiority of 30- and 45-minute exposure sessions over 15-minute sessions. Rachmann and Lopatka (1988) showed that in their study, extending the ET over a point of acceptably reduced anxiety did not have an added effect on the success, and neither did stopping the ET before the point of acceptably reduced anxiety (SUDs at 0). Finally, Rodriguez et al. (1999) found that, in their study longer treatment duration predicted greater return of fear.

Lastly, Rowe and Craske (1998) tried to find differences between a massed ET and an ET with sessions spaced with more times between sessions. Although both forms of ET worked comparably well at first (the massed ET worked even slightly better), they found a return of fear in the massed ET group directly after the ET and at a 1-month follow-up. Computer assistance. A modern way of presenting the feared stimulus is by using computer assistance. But respective techniques seem to be either as good as or even inferior to in vivo ET: This was shown for video exposure (Öst et al., 1998), imagery exposure (Wiederhold et

ro of

al., 2002, 2001), computer-aided ET (Tortella-Feliu et al., 2011) and internet-based therapies (Andersson et al., 2013, 2009). Additionally, Virtual Reality (VR) or Augmented Reality (AR), were either as effective (Botella et al., 2016; Emmelkamp et al., 2001; St-Jacques et al., 2010) or slightly inferior(Heading et al., 2001; Michaliszyn et al., 2010) than in vivo ET.

-p

However, the authors frequently name a lesser aversiveness (Botella et al., 2016) and lower costs, especially in the case of flight phobia (Emmelkamp et al., 2001), as advantages for VR

re

or AR. When it comes to differences between specific ways of projecting the virtual reality,

lP

Krijn and colleagues (2004) do not find a difference between an ET with a head-mounted display (HMD) and a computer automatic virtual environment (CAVE). Mühlberger and

na

colleagues (2005b) found no additional effect of motion simulation in VRET. Additional manipulation. Finally, three studies manipulated the stimulus in additional ways: Smith and colleagues (Smith et al., 1997) show that vicarious exposure to an irrelevant, not

ur

anxiety-inducing stimulus (i.e. training of the ET technique without the feared stimulus) does

Jo

improve anxiety. Craske and colleagues (1991) varied the participant’s control over the length of an exposure and found no influence on the success of treatment. Shiban and colleagues (2016) showed that specific perceptual cues (i.e. presenting pictures of a closed door to claustrophobic participants) lead to better success of ET (measured by physiological parameters) than fear-related conceptual information (i.e. telling the participants that the door was closed). 3.2.4

Physiological factors

Heart rate. When investigating heart rate, systolic blood pressure and diastolic blood pressure, Hellström and Öst (1996) could not find any stable predictors for short- or longterm success of ET. Several other studies researched the connection between heart rate and ET success: While Bornas and colleagues (2007) could predict better success when combining heart rate variability with heart rate entropy, Alpers and Sell (2008) even showed that in their participants, heart rate at the start of ET alone correlated more with its success than measures of subjective anxiety. Bornas and colleagues (2011) added to this showing that “flexible patients” (i.e. patients with greater heart rate variability) improved more. In line

ro of

with this, Busscher and colleagues (2013) showed that heart rate was a predictor for participants with high anxiety sensitivity, but less so for patients with low anxiety sensitivity. In a follow-up study (Bornas et al., 2012) that further discriminated between tonic and phasic

-p

heart rate, this finding was corroborated. However, two studies (Benoit Allen et al., 2015; Busscher et al., 2015) could not find correlations between long-term treatment success and

re

heart rate reactivity or changes.

lP

Feedback of body reactions. Craske and colleagues (1995) gave participants corrective information on their bodily reactions (like explaining that shortness of breath did not lead to suffocation). While this lead to higher fear reduction in claustrophobics, the effect did not

na

generalize and was not found in spider or snake phobic participants. However, two studies (Telch et al., 2000; Wiederhold et al., 2002) found that feedback of the heart rate did

ur

enhance the effect of ET, while one study could not find an influence of (visual or tactile) feedback (Tardif et al., 2019).

Jo

Caffeine State. Mystkowski and colleagues (2003) showed that the “caffeine state”, i.e. how much coffee a participant drank, has an effect on post-ET anxiety in that a “caffeine state” differing to the one in the ET lead to greater return of fear. Anxiety-differing medication. A number of different medication was administered in studies to examine its effect on the success of ET. A study conducted by Meyerbroeker and colleagues (2012) could not find enhancing effects for Yohimbine Hydrochloride. For Quetiapine, an

effect was limited to physiological anxiety measurements (Diemer et al., 2013) but did not show in behavioral measurements or subjective ratings by the participants. For Benzodiazepines, a study even found a hindering effect for flight phobic participants (Wilhelm and Roth, 1997). Similarly, Oxytocin showed diminishing effects on success in one study (Acheson et al., 2015). Lastly, Naltrexone was not administered as a potential enhancement but rather to test the role of the opioid system: Both Merluzzi, Taylor, Boltwood, and Gotestam (1991) and Arntz, Merckelbach, & de Jong (1993) find that the administration of Naltrexone hinders reduction of anxiety in an ET, thus suggesting a role of the opioid system

ro of

in its success. 3.3 After the ET

Only a few factors in this category have been researched, with one emotional (conditioned

-p

odor with n=1 study) and one cognitive factor (return of phobic cognitions with n=2 studies)

re

and 3 physiological factors (neural activation with n=3 studies, sleep with n=2 studies and memory-enhancing medication with n=5 studies). Emotional factors

lP

3.3.1

Conditioned odor. Rihm et al. (2016) could not enhance the effect of an ET by re-

participants.

Cognitive factors

ur

3.3.2

na

administrating an odor that was previously conditioned to a successful ET to their sleeping

Jo

Return of phobic cognitions. Rodriguez and colleagues (1999) found evidence that “recovery of phobic cognitions”, i.e. an increase in phobic cognitions about the feared stimulus assessed with the Spider Beliefs Questionnaire (Arnoud Arntz et al., 1993), leads to less long-term success of ET. 3.3.3

Physiological factors

Neural activation. Hauner and colleagues (2012) show that the magnitude of the activation of a part of the visual cortex (a lingual cortex subregion) to specific fearful stimuli measured by

fMRI shortly after ET predicted long-term treatment success. Schienle and colleagues (2007) showed that successful ET correlated with a reduction in amygdala and insula reactivity to relevant pictures when success was measured with somatic panic symptoms and correlated with only amygdala changes when measured with arousal ratings in a BAT. Finally, Landowska and colleagues (2018) found no correlation between brain activity measured with functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNRIS) and subjective anxiety ratings in a group of acrophobic participants during a VRET.

ro of

Sleep. Both Kleim and colleagues (2014) and Pace-Schott and colleagues (2012) showed that respectively 90 minutes or 12 hours of sleep lead to better post-therapy anxiety reductions.

Memory-enhancing mediation. The most-researched enhancing drug is D-Cycloserine, a

-p

partial agonist to the glutamatergic NMDA receptor (Hofmann, 2014) with heterogeneous

re

results: While Smits and colleagues (2013) find that application of D-Cycloserine after a “successful” session, i.e. a session with within-session reduction of anxiety according to

lP

“Subjective Units of Distress (SUDs, but not after an “unsuccessful” one) seems to enhance the effect of the treatment in a group of height phobic individuals and Byrne et al. (2015)

na

corroborate this finding in a group of children with varying specific phobias, Tart et al. (2013) do not find an enhancing effect of D-Cycloserine.

ur

Methylene Blue seems to have an effect that is comparable to that of D-Cycloserine, i.e. being effective after a successful, but counterproductive after an unsuccessful exposure

Jo

session (Telch et al., 2014).

4. DISCUSSION The present review aimed to give an overview over factors spanning the time before, during and after the ET itself and their respective empiric corroboration. Before discussing the limitations and the conclusion of this review and giving concrete advice for clinicians and researchers on how to use the results in their respective work, we will summarize our results

ro of

for different factors.

4.1. Summary of evidence

-p

4.1.1. Before the ET

At this point in time, one can only recommend considering two emotional factors before ET

re

as factors influencing the success of ET: A low trait anxiety and a high motivation, both researched by two studies with acceptable QS. Coping stiles (3 studies with acceptable to

lP

high QS and mixed results) has not been shown to be relevant in an adequate number of studies.

na

The fact that four studies with acceptable to high QSs point to self-efficacy being an important factor fits well with the results of the aforementioned study by Williams and

ur

colleagues (1984). No other cognitive factors emerged as good candidates for influencing the success of ET.

Jo

For potential physiological influencing factors before ET, it must be said again that replication of findings is needed (especially for comorbidity, that has only been researched by one study with acceptable QS), albeit Cortisol levels seem to be the most promising candidate, with two promising studies with very high QSs finding and using a relationship between Cortisol levels and ET success. 4.1.2.

During the ET

When it comes to emotional factors during the ET, the literature summarized here calls into question if WSH and BSH are necessary, with two studies with lower QS finding a relationship, while to studies with higher QSs fail to do so. This concurs with the work by Craske and colleagues (Craske et al., 2008b), and with the results of a recent meta-analysis by Rupp and colleagues (Rupp et al., 2017). It remains entirely unclear whether disgust and its decline through an ET is independent from anxiety and whether it is a relevant predictor of ET success. Five studies, all with lower to medium QSs, come to very contradicting results. However, the three included studies with medium QSs (Choplin and Carter, 2011; de Jong et

ro of

al., 2000; Olatunji et al., 2009) suggest that exposing to disgusting stimuli does also help reduce anxiety. Additionally, avoiding relaxation shows promise as a relevant factor in three studies with QSs ranging from low to high (McGlynn et al., 1999; Mühlberger et al., 2001b;

-p

Rimm and Medeiros, 1970).

Regarding cognitive factors during the ET, it seems clear that focusing on changes in fear-

re

relevant cognitions during the ET improves the success rate of ET with a high number of

lP

studies, all with medium to high QSs, finding a relationship. However, we cannot specify the best kind of cognitive addition more precisely, as the different studies used very

na

heterogeneous cognitive additions.

Going on to look at potential behavioral factors during the ET change of context - but not change of the stimulus itself - has emerged as a factor of success in ET, especially regarding

ur

long-term effects. Regarding length three studies with high QS found five sessions to be

Jo

sufficient, with evidence from one high-quality study that five sessions is superior over one session when it comes to long-term effectiveness. Extending the time of individual sessions does not seem to improve the success. Virtual Reality Exposure Therapy (VRET) seems to be a viable option for the treatment of specific phobias: In most of the reviewed studies (that all have good QSs), it has shown superiority or at least no inferiority to other forms of treatment that have been used frequently when in vivo exposure was not possible or too aversive and seems to be a viable alternative

to in vivo ET. This finding is in line with newer reviews (Botella et al., 2017; Maples-Keller et al., 2017). Regarding physiological factors, there are several different measurements of heart rate activity that show potential as factors predicting treatment success, none of which has been studied in a satisfactory amount, even though all studies show medium to high QSs. Nonetheless, it seems clear that heart rate changes and differences are an important factor for the success of ET.

ro of

4.1.3. After the ET Overall, factors influencing the success that take effect after ET play a comparably small role. Except for the return of phobic cognitions (with only one, medium to high quality study),

-p

no potential factors emerged in the emotional or cognition category. When it comes to additions concerning physiological aspects, best evidence exists for sleep as well as

re

administering D-Cycloserine as being helpful (albeit only if it is administered with a successful ET; 3 studies with high QS). In their review concerning the use of cognitive

lP

enhancers in exposure-based therapies for several diseases, Singewald and colleagues (2015) come to a similar conclusion. See Hofmann (2014) for a guidelines for clinical practice

na

of using D-Cycloserine as an enhancer of ET.

For an overview over the status of every factor included in this review, see table 3.

ur

Insert Table 3 here.

Jo

4.2. Embedding our results into the theoretical background Our findings show only a handful of factors that definitely play a part in the success of ET. However, these factors can be integrated very well into the aforementioned Inhibitory Learning Approach by Craske (Craske et al., 2014, 2008). This approach states that patients develop secondary inhibitory associations to the feared stimuli; ET is thus successful when it provides patients with possibilities to build and strengthen these new associations (Craske et al., 2014). This theory differentiates itself from older models of the underlying mechanisms of

ET like the Pavlovian conditioning model in that it does not postulate an overwriting of the old associations but a building of separate, new associations. Thus, factors that enable maximum new learning and create an environment in which learning is possible help the most. In our review, these are: A high self-efficacy before the ET (Bandura et al., 1977; Cote and Bouchard, 2009; Williams et al., 1984; Zoellner et al., 2000), a low trait anxiety (Muris et al., 1998; Rodriguez et al., 1999), addressing disgust as well as anxiety when choosing the stimulus (Choplin and Carter, 2011; de Jong et al., 2000; Olatunji

ro of

et al., 2009), focusing on changing phobia-specific cognitions (Bandura et al., 1974; Dee, 1972; Kamphuis and Telch, 2000; Kircanski et al., 2012; Koch et al., 2004; Raes et al., 2011; Sloan and Telch, 2002; Tabibnia et al., 2008; Tardif et al., 2019; Van Gerwen et al., 2006), changing contexts between ET sessions (Bandarian-Balooch et al., 2015; Mystkowski et al.,

-p

2002; Shiban et al., 2015a, 2013; Vansteenwegen et al., 2007b), focusing on heart rate changes (Alpers and Sell, 2008; Bornas et al., 2012, 2011, 2007; Busscher et al., 2013;

re

Hellström and Öst, 1996), administering D-Cycloserine after a successful ET (Byrne et al.,

lP

2015; Smits et al., 2013; Tart et al., 2013) and sleeping after one (Kleim et al., 2014; Pace-

na

Schott et al., 2012).

All of these factors help the patient build new associations with the feared stimulus: Either by

ur

enabling them to be more open to new experiences that violate existing expectancies concerning the feared stimulus (high self-efficacy and low trait anxiety), by “deepen the

Jo

extinction” (Rescorla, 2006) in the case of addressing disgust additionally to anxiety and focusing on cognitions and heart rate change or by enabling reconsolidation (changing contexts, D-Cycloserine, and sleep). Nevertheless, some of the strategies to optimize ET proposed by Craske and colleagues are not corroborated by our findings, namely the call for variability of stimulus. Especially since most of the assumptions of the Inhibitory Learning Approach are met, it seems surprising that this is not the case for changing stimuli. One possible reason might be the focus of this

review on a very specific anxiety disorder, namely SP. SP is characterized by the fear of one specific stimulus. Thus, there might not be a need to generalize as much to other forms of the same feared stimulus. This also explains why the length and number of the ET sessions seem to play a smaller role. It is easier to learn new cognitions in the context of a specific phobia than in the context of other phobias or anxiety disorders. When an ET is successful in providing these new learning experiences, it does not need as much time or many repetitions for this learning experience to consolidate.

ro of

Furthermore, our findings show that some factors only influence the success measured on one dimension (behavioral, subjective, physiological, or in clinicians’ ratings). This relationship between different levels of measurement of the success and different factors is

4.2.1

Perspective for clinical research

-p

not yet clear (Lambert and Ogles, 2009; Moustgaard et al., 2014).

re

There is only a small number of sufficiently researched and corroborated factors influencing the success of ET in patients with specific phobia. Studies that aim to replicate previous

lP

results, with sufficiently sized and well-screened samples, are needed to further the understanding of what makes ET successful.

na

Based on our findings, researchers wanting to select factors worthy of future research and to find good operationalizations should start at the underlying theoretical mechanisms. As

ur

already mentioned, factors that could help improve the learning experience in ET (e.g. by directing the patient’s attention on the new experience, limiting distractions and unnecessary

Jo

parts of the ET) are more likely to be relevant factors of success than factors that stand in no connection to the learning experience. Agreeing on shared operationalizations of factors as well as shared measures of success, with a special focus of measuring long-term success, is of utmost importance for the advancement of the research field. Furthermore, studies show a huge difference in regard to the form of ET they use (OST, VRET and multi-session ET are used similarly often) and their inclusion and exclusion criteria. We presented studies that show that these different forms of

ET are comparable, as they are very similar to each other in terms of success and underlying mechanism. Nevertheless, an effort should be made in trying to mirror everyday practice in future studies and to use more comparable forms of ET to ensure generalizability. Lastly, we see the inclusion of participants with only mild anxiety as a big problem: This calls into question if the respective results are transferrable to clinical practice, where patients have comparably severe disorders and symptomatology. Studies with participants with clinical diagnoses sometimes come to different conclusions, not least

ro of

because ET seems to be more effective with participants with more severe anxiety. Therefore, we argue that studies should try to include only participants with clinically significant specific phobia.

This is especially the case since once the question of who profits from ET most is answered,

-p

the question what to do with the ones who do not becomes even more pressing. This group

re

of hard-to-treat patients might need a more optimized and individualized treatment. However, based on the current literature, it is hard to say how such optimizations should look like; more

lP

research is needed in this field. Some studies that try to fill this need: Studies like the one from Schwarzmeier and colleagues (Schwarzmeier et al., in press) try to identify these

na

patients before a treatment begins. Future studies will have to show how to individualize therapy.

Perspective for clinical practice

ur

4.2.2

A conclusion for how to conduct ET with patients is very hard to draw. Nevertheless, some

Jo

advice can be given.

All things considered, the literature shown here underlines that ET is a very effective form of treatment; even if both the mechanisms and the influencing factors still need to be put under scrutiny, a majority of the studies presented here shows an improvement in anxiety (subjectively, behaviorally and physiologically). It is thus superior to other forms of treatment for patients with SP.

ET should have the goal to expose the patient with the feared stimulus as fast and as direct as possible (while still taking the commitment of the patient into account) to ensure the best learning experience. Factors that have been shown to improve the learning experience are addressing disgust as well as anxiety (i.e.when building an anxiety hierarchy, therapists should also ask for especially disgust-inducing stimuli and take respective stimuli into consideration when choosing exposure stimuli), focusing onchanging cognitions in the ET (by concretely exploring cognitions before and after the ET to stress changes), changing context for different sessions, monitoring and addressing heart rate changes during the ET and sleep

ro of

and/or D-Cycloserine after a (successful) ET. Lastly, ET should be conducted as close as possible to the current state of the art (Bandelow et al., 2014), since this is the form of ET that is best examined and the influence of all deviances from it are not yet clear.

-p

Some factors that we found to have an impact on the success of ET cannot be controlled by the clinician directly, mainly the motivation, self-efficacy and trait anxiety of their patients.

re

However, we would argue that there is nevertheless merit in accounting for their influence:

lP

As we have said, ET is the therapy of choice even for patients with less chance of success. Clinicians should just be aware that success might take longer for patients with unfavorable attitudes or traits, and might want to address motivation and self-efficacy before starting with

na

ET (e.g. by exploring and discussing potential dysfunctional cognitions that lead to these, or using techniques like Motivational Interviewing (Miller, 1983) to improve them. Moreover,

ur

specific adjustments and individual optimization might be necessary for this group of patients. As discussed, the nature of these optimizations is not yet clear, making this group

Jo

of patients a particular challenge. 4.3 Limitations

A large number of factors have been researched in the literature, but most of them by only a very small number of studies. Additionally, a lot of potential factors have been operationalized very heterogeneously in different studies, leading to problems in comparing these studies even if the same factor was the object of interest. This leads to problems

especially when different potential factors (e.g. distraction and cognitive additions) influence each other. Finally, both the characterization of participants and the definition of success of ET differed greatly, further complicating comparison between studies. Additionally, a lot of studies do not take satisfactory follow-up measurements: Studies show that there is a considerable change in the reaction to an ET even 6 and 12 months after treatment, most widely studied in respect to the aforementioned concept of return of fear (Haaker et al., 2014), which often is disregarded in the presented literature.

ro of

Many studies that looked at potential predictors of success of ET did not meet our inclusion criteria and were thus excluded from this review. Hence, it could be argued that with less rigid inclusion criteria, more predictors could have been unveiled and the picture for the predictors discussed here could have looked differently. Similarly, one could argue that we

-p

limited ourselves too much by only including studies regarding SP, and excluding other

re

studies that, while conducting research on ET, did so with participants having (symptoms of) another phobia or anxiety disorder. But we think that, especially in the face of the number of

lP

different predictors discussed in the literature and the huge number of articles published in this area, it is of importance to adhere to strict quality standards to be able to get trustworthy

na

results.

Furthermore, there is still considerable heterogeneity in our results. This heterogeneity raises

ur

the question whether our inclusion criteria were not strict enough: We included a number of articles that have a sample of participants with only light symptomatology or that measure the

Jo

success (or lack thereof) of the ET in an unsatisfactory amount. However, we decided to include these studies in order to give a picture of the existing research that is as exhaustive as possible – as long as they fulfilled certain minimal criteria outlined in our PICOS criteria (see 2. Methods). Since all the included studies had to satisfy these inclusion criteria, we feel that reporting their results is not without merit even if their methodology leaves things to be desired. Additionally, we tried to make the reader aware of quality differences between included studies by reporting their respective QS.

Moreover, studies measured outcome in different ways, and some studies included several kinds of measuring outcome like subjective (e.g. via questionnaires), behavioral (e.g. with Behavioral Avoidance Tests), physiological (e.g. via measuring electrodermal activity) or professional (e.g. a rating by therapists). There is merit to every one of these kinds of measures, since they show different aspects of change that tend to be intercorrelated, but not equal to each other (Ogles et al., 1990). Hence, a combination is desirable to get a complete pictures as much as possible (Lonsdorf et al., 2017). This is why the QS is higher the more different kinds of measuring outcome were applied. In our review, eight included

ro of

studies did only measure success in one of the aforementioned ways; all the other studies employed two or more ways.

Another problem in interpreting our results lies in the fact that there is a risk of bias across

-p

the included studies (see 2.Methods). This is especially disconcerting because oftentimes, one potential factor has only been researched by one team of researchers. For example,

re

OST has been researched most actively by Öst and colleagues. This makes it hard to

lP

compare two factors with each other, given that the quality of the underlying research may differ. We again tried to tackle this problem with our Quality Score, which should give an

4.4 Conclusion

na

insight into the quality of every study (albeit not being able to fully eliminate risk of bias).

ur

This review gives an overview over potential factors influencing the success of ET in patients with SP. Best evidence is shown for the following factors: low trait anxiety, high motivation

Jo

and high self-efficacy before the ET, high cortisol levels and heart rate variation, evoking disgust additionally to anxiety, avoiding relaxation, focusing on cognitive changes, context variation, sleep, and memory-enhancing drugs. This review serves two purposes: Firstly, it helps practitioners in planning and conducting an ET with a high chance to succeed. Although conclusive evidence for many aspects is still missing, we nevertheless show that enabling to learn new associations with the feared stimulus should be the focus. Secondly, this review presents the challenges and the groundwork for future research: It shows the

need for unified operationalizations of both potential factors and the measurement of the outcome. It presents many potential factors that should be replicated in future studies and provides a theoretical background for other factors that can be researched in the future.

Funding and conflicts of interest The study was supported by grants of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; German Research Foundation; Collaborative Research Centre “Fear, Anxiety, Anxiety Disorders”

Jo

ur

na

lP

re

-p

ro of

SFB-TRR-58) projects Z2 and C09 to UD. There was no conflict of interest.

5

REFERENCES

Acheson, D.T., Feifel, D., Kamenski, M., Mckinney, R., Risbrough, V.B., 2015. Intranasal oxytocin administration prior to exposure therapy for arachnophobia impedes treatment response. Depress. Anxiety 32, 400–407. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22362

ro of

Alpers, G.W., Sell, R., 2008. And yet they correlate: Psychophysiological activation predicts self-report outcomes of exposure therapy in claustrophobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 1101–1109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.11.009

-p

Andersson, G., Waara, J., Jonsson, U., Malmaeus, F., Carlbring, P., Ost, L.-G., 2009.

Internet-based self-help versus one-session exposure in the treatment of spider phobia:

re

a randomized controlled trial. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 38, 114–120. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506070902931326

lP

Andersson, G., Waara, J., Jonsson, U., Malmaeus, F., Carlbring, P., Öst, L.-G., 2013. Internet-based exposure treatment versus one-session exposure treatment of snake

na

phobia: a randomized controlled trial. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 42, 284–291. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2013.844202

ur

Antony, M.M., McCabe, R.E., Leeuw, I., Sano, N., Swinson, R.P., 2001. Effect of distraction and coping style on in vivo exposure for specific phobia of spiders. Behav. Res. Ther.

Jo

39, 1137–1150.

Arntz, Arnoud, Lavy, E., van den Berg, G., van Rijsoort, S., 1993. Negative beliefs of spider phobics: A psychometric evaluation of the spider phobia beliefs questionnaire. Adv. Behav. Res. Ther. 15, 257–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/0146-6402(93)90012-Q Arntz, A, Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P., 1993. Opioid antagonist affects behavioral effects of exposure in vivo. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 61, 865–870.

Baker, A.S., Mystkowski, J.L., Culver, N.C., Yi, R., Mortazavi, A., Craske, M.G., 2010. Does habituation matter? Emotional processing theory and exposure therapy for acrophobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1139–1143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.009 Bandarian-Balooch, S., Neumann, D.L., Boschen, M.J., 2015. Exposure treatment in multiple contexts attenuates return of fear via renewal in high spider fearful individuals. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 47, 138–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.12.006 Bandelow, B., Reitt, M., Rover, C., Michaelis, S., Gorlich, Y., Wedekind, D., 2015. Efficacy of

192. https://doi.org/10.1097/YIC.0000000000000078

ro of

treatments for anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 30, 183–

Bandelow, B., Wiltink, J., Alpers, G.W., Benecke, C., Deckert, J., Eckhardt-Henn, A., Ehrig,

-p

C., Engel, E., Falkai, P., Geiser, F., Gerlach, A.L., Harfst, T., Hau, S., Joraschky, P.,

Kellner, M., Köllner, V., Kopp, I., Langs, G., Lichte, T., Liebeck, H., Matzat, J., Reitt, M.,

re

Rüddel, H.P., Rudolf, S., Schick, G., Schweiger, U., Simon, R., Springer, A., Staats, H., Ströhle, A., Ströhm, W., Waldherr, B., Watzke, B., Wedekind, D., Zottl, C., Zwanzger,

lP

P., M.E, B., 2014. Deutsche S3-Leitlinie Behandlung von Angststörungen. SpringerVerlag.

na

Bandura, A., Adams, N.E., Beyer, J., 1977. Cognitive processes mediating behavioral change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 35, 125–139.

ur

Bandura, A., Jeffery, R.W., Wright, C.L., 1974. Efficacy of participant modeling as a function of response induction aids. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 83, 56–64.

Jo

https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036258

Benoit Allen, K., Allen, B., Austin, K.E., Waldron, J.C., Ollendick, T.H., 2015. Synchronydesynchrony in the tripartite model of fear: Predicting treatment outcome in clinically phobic children. Behav. Res. Ther. 71, 54–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.05.009 Bornas, X., Gelabert, J.M., Llabrés, J., Balle, M., Tortella-Feliu, M., 2011. Slope of change throughout exposure treatment for flight phobia: the role of autonomic flexibility. J. Clin.

Psychol. 67, 550–560. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20780 Bornas, X., Llabrés, J., Tortella-Feliu, M., Fullana, M.A., Montoya, P., Lopez, A., Noguera, M., Gelabert, J.M., 2007. Vagally mediated heart rate variability and heart rate entropy as predictors of treatment outcome in flight phobia. Biol. Psychol. 76, 188–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.007 Bornas, X., Riera del Amo, A., Tortella-Feliu, M., Llabrés, J., 2012. Heart rate variability profiles and exposure therapy treatment outcome in flight phobia. Appl. Psychophysiol.

ro of

Biofeedback 37, 53–62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-011-9179-5 Botella, C., Fernández-Álvarez, J., Guillén, V., García-Palacios, A., Baños, R., 2017. Recent Progress in Virtual Reality Exposure Therapy for Phobias: A Systematic Review. Curr.

-p

Psychiatry Rep. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-017-0788-4

Botella, C., Perez-Ara, M.A., Breton-Lopez, J., Quero, S., Garcia-Palacios, A., Banos, R.M.,

re

2016. In Vivo versus Augmented Reality Exposure in the Treatment of Small Animal

lP

Phobia: A Randomized Controlled Trial. PLoS One 11, e0148237. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148237

na

Busscher, B., Spinhoven, P., de Geus, E.J.C., 2015. Psychological Distress and Physiological Reactivity during in Vivo Exposure in People with Aviophobia. Psychosom.

ur

Med. 77, 762–774. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000209 Busscher, B., Spinhoven, P., van Gerwen, L.J., de Geus, E.J.C., 2013. Anxiety sensitivity

Jo

moderates the relationship of changes in physiological arousal with flight anxiety during in vivo exposure therapy. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 98–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.10.009

Byrne, S.P., Rapee, R.M., Richardson, R., Malhi, G.S., Jones, M., Hudson, J.L., 2015. Dcycloserine enhances generalization of fear extinction in children. Depress. Anxiety 32, 408–414. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22356 Choplin, C.E., Carter, M.M., 2011. A comparison of repeated exposure to fearful and

disgusting stimuli among spider phobics. Behav. Interv. 26, 23–40. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.319 Cote, S., Bouchard, S., 2009. Cognitive mechanisms underlying virtual reality exposure. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 121–129. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0008 Craske, M.G., Bunt, R., Rapee, R.M., Barlow, D.H., 1991. Perceived control and controllability during in vivo exposure: Spider phobics. J. Anxiety Disord. 5, 285–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/0887-6185(91)90029-S

ro of

Craske, M.G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J.L., Chowdhury, N., Baker, A.S.,

2008. Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 5– 27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003

-p

Craske, M.G., Mohlman, J., Yi, J., Glover, D., Valeri, S., 1995. Treatment of claustrophobias and snake/spider phobias: fear of arousal and fear of context. Behav. Res. Ther. 33,

re

197–203.

lP

Craske, M.G., Mystkowski, J.L., 2006. Exposure Therapy and Extinction: Clinical Studies., in: Craske, M.G., Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., Craske (Ed), M.G., Hermans (Ed),

na

D., Vansteenwegen (Ed), D. (Eds.), Fear and Learning: From Basic Processes to Clinical Implications. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, US, pp.

ur

217–233. https://doi.org/10.1037/11474-011 Craske, M.G., Treanor, M., Conway, C.C., Zbozinek, T., Vervliet, B., 2014. Maximizing

Jo

exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behav. Res. Ther. 58, 10–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006

Curtis, G., Magee, W.J., Eaton, W.W., Wittchen, H.-U., Kessler, R.C., 1998. Specific fears and phobias. Br. J. Psychiatry 173, 212–217. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.173.3.212 de Jong, P.J., Merckelbach, H., Arntz, A., 1991. Eyeblink startle responses in spider phobics before and after treatment: A pilot study. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 13, 213–223. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00960785

de Jong, P.J., Vorage, I., van den Hout, M.A., 2000. Counterconditioning in the treatment of spider phobia: effects on disgust, fear and valence. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 1055–1069. Deacon, B.J., Sy, J.T., Lickel, J.J., Nelson, E.A., 2010. Does the judicious use of safety behaviors improve the efficacy and acceptability of exposure therapy for claustrophobic fear? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 41, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.10.004 Deci, E.L., Ryan, R.M., 1985. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior.

ro of

Contemp. Sociol. https://doi.org/10.2307/2070638 Dee, C.K., 1972. Instructions and the extinction of a learned fear in the content of taped implosive therapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 39, 123–132.

-p

Dethier, V., Bruneau, N., Philippot, P., 2015. Attentional focus during exposure in spider

phobia: the role of schematic versus non-schematic imagery. Behav. Res. Ther. 65,

re

86–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.12.016

lP

Diemer, J., Domschke, K., Mühlberger, A., Winter, B., Zavorotnyy, M., Notzon, S., Silling, K., Arolt, V., Zwanzger, P., 2013. Acute anxiolytic effects of quetiapine during virtual reality

na

exposure-A double-blind placebo-controlled trial in patients with specific phobia. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 23, 1551–1560.

ur

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.01.001 Dour, H.J., Brown, L.A., Craske, M.G., 2016. Positive valence reduces susceptibility to return

Jo

of fear and enhances approach behavior. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 50, 277–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.09.010

Duits, P., Cath, D.C., Lissek, S., Hox, J.J., Hamm, A.O., Engelhard, I.M., Van Den Hout, M.A., Baas, J.M.P., 2015. Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depress. Anxiety. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22353 Emmelkamp, P.M., Bruynzeel, M., Drost, L., van der Mast, C.A., 2001. Virtual reality treatment in acrophobia: a comparison with exposure in vivo. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 4,

335–339. https://doi.org/10.1089/109493101300210222 Fernandez, E., Salem, D., Swift, J.K., Ramtahal, N., 2015. Meta-analysis of dropout from cognitive behavioral therapy: Magnitude, timing, and moderators. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 83, 1108–1122. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000044 Foa, E.B., Kozak, M.J., 1986. Emotional Processing of Fear. Exposure to Corrective Information. Psychol. Bull. 99, 20–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20 Foa, E.B., McNally, R.J., 1996. Mechanisms of change in exposure therapy, in: Current

ro of

Controversies in the Anxiety Disorders.

Fraser, J., Kirkby, K.C., Daniels, B.A., Gilroy, L.J., Montgomery, I.M., 2001. Three versus six sessions of computer-aided vicarious exposure treatment for spider phobia. Behav.

-p

Chang. 18, 213–223. https://doi.org/10.1375/bech.18.4.213

re

Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Jacobi, F., Allgulander, C., Alonso, J., Beghi, E., Dodel, R., Ekman, M., Faravelli, C., Fratiglioni, L., Gannon, B., Jones, D.H., Jennum, P.,

lP

Jordanova, A., Jönsson, L., Karampampa, K., Knapp, M., Kobelt, G., Kurth, T., Lieb, R., Linde, M., Ljungcrantz, C., Maercker, A., Melin, B., Moscarelli, M., Musayev, A.,

na

Norwood, F., Preisig, M., Pugliatti, M., Rehm, J., Salvador-Carulla, L., Schlehofer, B., Simon, R., Steinhausen, H.-C., Stovner, L.J., Vallat, J.M., den Bergh, P. Van, van Os, J., Vos, P., Xu, W., Wittchen, H.-U., Jönsson, B., Olesen, J., 2011. Cost of disorders of

ur

the brain in Europe 2010. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 718–779.

Jo

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.08.008 Haaker, J., Golkar, A., Hermans, D., Lonsdorf, T.B., 2014. A review on human reinstatement studies: An overview and methodological challenges. Learn. Mem. 21, 424–440. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.036053.114 Hamm, A.O., 2009. Specific phobias. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 32, 577–591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2009.05.008 Hauner, K.K., Mineka, S., Voss, J.L., Paller, K.A., 2012. Exposure therapy triggers lasting

reorganization of neural fear processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 9203– 9208. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1205242109 Heading, K., Kirkby, K.C., Martin, F., Daniels, B.A., Gilroy, L.J., Menzies, R.G., 2001. Controlled comparison of single-session treatments for spider phobia: Live graded exposure alone versus computer-aided vicarious exposure. Behav. Chang. 18, 103– 113. https://doi.org/10.1375/bech.18.2.103 Hellström, K., Fellenius, J., Öst, L.G., 1996. One versus five sessions of applied tension in

ro of

the treatment of blood phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 101–112. Hellström, K., Öst, L.G., 1996. Prediction of outcome in the treatment of specific phobia. A cross validation study. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 403–411.

-p

Hodgson, R.J., Rachman, S.J., 1970. An experimental investigation of the implosion

re

technique. Behav. Res. Ther. 8, 21–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(70)90030-6 Hofmann, S.G., 2014. D-cycloserine for treating anxiety disorders: making good exposures

lP

better and bad exposures worse. Depress. Anxiety 31, 175–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22257

na

Hood, H.K., Antony, M.M., Koerner, N., Monson, C.M., 2010. Effects of safety behaviors on fear reduction during exposure. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1161–1169.

ur

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.006 James, A.C., James, G., Cowdrey, F.A., Soler, A., Choke, A., 2015. Cognitive behavioural

Jo

therapy for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD004690.pub4

Kamphuis, J.H., Telch, M.J., 2000. Effects of distraction and guided threat reappraisal on fear reduction during exposure-based treatments for specific fears. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 1163–1181. Kircanski, K., Lieberman, M.D., Craske, M.G., 2012. Feelings into words: contributions of

language to exposure therapy. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1086–1091. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443830 Kleim, B., Wilhelm, F.H., Temp, L., Margraf, J., Wiederhold, B.K., Rasch, B., 2014. Sleep enhances exposure therapy. Psychol. Med. 44, 1511–1519. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713001748 Koch, E.I., Spates, C.R., Himle, J.A., 2004. Comparison of behavioral and cognitivebehavioral one-session exposure treatments for small animal phobias. Behav. Res.

ro of

Ther. 42, 1483–1504. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2003.10.005 Krijn, M., Emmelkamp, P.M.G., Biemond, R., de Wilde de Ligny, C., Schuemie, M.J., van der Mast, C.A.P.G., 2004. Treatment of acrophobia in virtual reality: the role of immersion

-p

and presence. Behav. Res. Ther. 42, 229–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/S00057967(03)00139-6

re

Krijn, M., Emmelkamp, P.M.G., Olafsson, R.P., Schuemie, M.J., van der Mast, C.A.P.G.,

lP

2007. Do self-statements enhance the effectiveness of virtual reality exposure therapy? A comparative evaluation in acrophobia. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 10, 362–370.

na

https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9943

Lambert, M.J., Ogles, B.M., 2009. Using clinical significance in psychotherapy outcome research: The need for a common procedure and validity data. Psychother. Res. 19,

ur

493–501. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503300902849483

Jo

Landowska, A., Roberts, D., Eachus, P., Barrett, A., 2018. Within- and Between-Session Prefrontal Cortex Response to Virtual Reality Exposure Therapy for Acrophobia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12, 362. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00362

Lass-Hennemann, J., Michael, T., 2014. Endogenous cortisol levels influence exposure therapy in spider phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 60, 39–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.06.009 Liberatî, A., Altman, D.G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P.C., Ioannidis, J.P.A., Clarke,

M., Devereaux, P.J., Kleijnen, J., Moher, D., 2009. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration, in: Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006 Lissek, S., Powers, A.S., McClure, E.B., Phelps, E.A., Woldehawariat, G., Grillon, C., Pine, D.S., 2005. Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 1391–1424. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAT.2004.10.007

ro of

Lonsdorf, T.B., Menz, M.M., Andreatta, M., Fullana, M.A., Golkar, A., Haaker, J., Heitland, I., Hermann, A., Kuhn, M., Kruse, O., Meir Drexler, S., Meulders, A., Nees, F., Pittig, A., Richter, J., Römer, S., Shiban, Y., Schmitz, A., Straube, B., Vervliet, B., Wendt, J., Baas, J.M.P., Merz, C.J., 2017. Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological

-p

considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition,

re

extinction, and return of fear. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 77, 247–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2017.02.026

lP

Lueken, U., Zierhut, K.C., Hahn, T., Straube, B., Kircher, T., Reif, A., Richter, J., Hamm, A., Wittchen, H.U., Domschke, K., 2016. Neurobiological markers predicting treatment

na

response in anxiety disorders: A systematic review and implications for clinical application. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.005

ur

Maples-Keller, J.L., Yasinski, C., Manjin, N., Rothbaum, B.O., 2017. Virtual RealityEnhanced Extinction of Phobias and Post-Traumatic Stress. Neurotherapeutics.

Jo

https://doi.org/10.1007/s13311-017-0534-y

Matthews, A.J., Naran, N., Kirkby, K.C., 2015. Symbolic online exposure for spider fear: habituation of fear, disgust and physiological arousal and predictors of symptom improvement. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 47, 129–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.12.003 Mayer, B., Merckelbach, H., Muris, P., 2000. Self-reported automaticity and irrationality in

spider phobia. Psychol. Rep. 87, 395–405. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2000.87.2.395 McGlynn, F.D., Moore, P.M., Lawyer, S.R., Karg, R., 1999. Relaxation training inhibits fear and arousal during in vivo exposure to phobia-cue stimuli. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 30, 155–168. Merluzzi, T. V, Taylor, C.B., Boltwood, M., Gotestam, K.G., 1991. Opioid antagonist impedes exposure. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 59, 425–430. Meyerbröker, K., Powers, M.B., van Stegeren, A., Emmelkamp, P.M.G., 2012. Does

ro of

yohimbine hydrochloride facilitate fear extinction in virtual reality treatment of fear of flying? A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychother. Psychosom. 81, 29–37. https://doi.org/10.1159/000329454

-p

Michaliszyn, D., Marchand, A., Bouchard, S., Martel, M.-O., Poirier-Bisson, J., 2010. A

randomized, controlled clinical trial of in virtuo and in vivo exposure for spider phobia.

re

Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 13, 689–695. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0277

lP

Miller, B. V., Levis, D.J., 1971. The effects of varying short visual exposure times to a phobic test stimulus on subsequent avoidance behavior. Behav. Res. Ther. 9, 17–21.

na

https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(71)90031-3 Miller, W.R., 1983. Motivational Interviewing with Problem Drinkers. Behav. Psychother. 11,

ur

147–172. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0141347300006583 Milosevic, I., Radomsky, A.S., 2008. Safety behaviour does not necessarily interfere with

Jo

exposure therapy. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 1111–1118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.011

Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R., 2006. A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. Am. Psychol. 61, 10–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.1.10 Moustgaard, H., Bello, S., Miller, F.G., Hróbjartsson, A., 2014. Subjective and objective

outcomes in randomized clinical trials: definitions differed in methods publications and were often absent from trial reports. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 67, 1327–1334. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JCLINEPI.2014.06.020 Mühlberger, A., Herrmann, M.J., Wiedemann, G., Ellgring, H., Pauli, P., 2001a. Repeated exposure of flight phobics to flights in virtual reality. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 1033–1050. Mühlberger, A., Herrmann, M.J., Wiedemann, G., Ellgring, H., Pauli, P., 2001b. Repeated exposure of flight phobics to flights in virtual reality. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 1033–1050.

ro of

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00076-0 Mühlberger, A., Petrusek, S., Herrmann, M.J., Pauli, P., 2005a. Biocyberpsychologie:

Subjektive und physiologische Reaktionen von Flugphobikern und Gesunden bei

-p

Exposition mit virtuellen Flügen. = Biocyber psychology: Subjective and physiological reactions in flight phobics and normal subjects during flight simulation. Zeitschrift für

re

Klin. Psychol. und Psychother. Forsch. und Prax. 34, 133–143.

lP

https://doi.org/10.1026/1616-3443.34.2.133

Mühlberger, A., Wiedemann, G., Pauli, P., 2005b. Subjective and physiologic reactions of flight phobics during VR exposure and treatment outcome: What adds motion

na

simulation? Annu. Rev. CyberTherapy Telemed. 3, 185–192. Muris, P., de Jong, P.J., Merckelbach, H., van Zuuren, F.J., 1993. Monitoring coping style

ur

and exposure outcome in spider phobics. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 21, 329–333.

Jo

https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465800011656 Muris, P., Mayer, B., Merckelbach, H., 1998. Trait anxiety as a predictor of behaviour therapy outcome in spider phobia. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 26, 87–91. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465898000095 Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P.J., 1995. Exposure therapy outcome in spider phobics: effects of monitoring and blunting coping styles. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 461– 464.

Mystkowski, J.L., Craske, M.G., Echiverri, A.M., 2002. Treatment context and return of fear in spider phobia. Behav. Ther. 33, 399–416. https://doi.org/10.1016/S00057894(02)80035-1 Mystkowski, J.L., Mineka, S., Vernon, L.L., Zinbarg, R.E., 2003. Changes in caffeine states enhance return of fear in spider phobia. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 71, 243–250. O’Sullivan, M.J., Gilner, F.H., 1976. Sex of Model and Motivation To Change Behavior As Factors in Modeling. Psychol. Rep. 38, 595–601.

ro of

https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1976.38.2.595 Ogles, B.M., Lambert, M.J., Weight, D.G., Payne, I.R., 1990. Agoraphobia Outcome Measurement: A Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Assess. 2, 317–325.

-p

https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.2.3.317

Olatunji, B.O., Ciesielski, B.G., Wolitzky-Taylor, K.B., Wentworth, B.J., Viar, M.A., 2012.

re

Effects of Experienced Disgust on Habituation During Repeated Exposure to Threat-

lP

Relevant Stimuli in Blood-Injection-Injury Phobia. Behav. Ther. 43, 132–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.04.002

na

Olatunji, B.O., Smits, J.A.J., Connolly, K., Willems, J., Lohr, J.M., 2007. Examination of the decline in fear and disgust during exposure to threat-relevant stimuli in blood-injectioninjury phobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 21, 445–455.

ur

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.05.001

Jo

Olatunji, B.O., Wolitzky-Taylor, K.B., Ciesielski, B.G., Armstrong, T., Etzel, E.N., David, B., 2009. Fear and disgust processing during repeated exposure to threat-relevant stimuli in spider phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 671–679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.04.012 Oliver, N.S., Page, A.C., 2008. Effects of internal and external distraction and focus during exposure to blood-injury-injection stimuli. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 283–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.01.006

Ollendick, T.H., Öst, L.G., Reuterskiöld, L., Costa, N., 2010. Comorbidity in youth with specific phobias: Impact of comorbidity on treatment outcome and the impact of treatment on comorbid disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 827–831. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.024 Opriş, D., Pintea, S., García-Palacios, A., Botella-Arbona, C., Szamosközi, Ş., David, D.O., 2012. Virtual reality exposure therapy in anxiety disorders: a quantitative meta-analysis. Depress. Anxiety 29, 85–93. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20910

ro of

Öst, L.-G., Havnen, A., Hansen, B., Kvale, G., 2015. Cognitive behavioral treatments of obsessive–compulsive disorder. A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies published 1993–2014. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 40, 156–169.

-p

https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2015.06.003

Öst, L.-G., Stridh, B.M., Wolf, M., 1998. A clinical study of spider phobia: prediction of

re

outcome after self-help and therapist-directed treatments. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, 17–35.

lP

Öst, L.G., Alm, T., Brandberg, M., Breitholtz, E., 2001. One vs five sessions of exposure and five sessions of cognitive therapy in the treatment of claustrophobia. Behav. Res. Ther.

na

39, 167–183.

Öst, L.G., Ferebee, I., Furmark, T., 1997. One-session group therapy of spider phobia: direct

ur

versus indirect treatments. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 721–732. Pace-Schott, E.F., Verga, P.W., Bennett, T.S., Spencer, R.M.C., 2012. Sleep promotes

Jo

consolidation and generalization of extinction learning in simulated exposure therapy for spider fear. J. Psychiatr. Res. 46, 1036–1044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.04.015

Rachman, S., 1989. The return of fear: Review and prospect. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 9, 147– 168. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(89)90025-1 Rachman, S., 1977. The conditioning theory of fearacquisition: A critical examination. Behav. Res. Ther. 15, 375–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(77)90041-9

Rachman, S.J., Levitt, K., 1988. Panic, fear reduction and habituation. Behav. Res. Ther. 26, 199–206. Rachman, S.J., Lopatka, C., 1988. Return of fear: Underlearning and overlearning. Behav. Res. Ther. 26, 99–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(88)90108-8 Raes, A.K., Koster, E.H.W., Loeys, T., De Raedt, R., 2011. Pathways to change in onesession exposure with and without cognitive intervention: an exploratory study in spider phobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 964–971. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.06.003

ro of

Reed, D.A., Cook, D.A., Beckman, T.J., Levine, R.B., Kern, D.E., Wright, S.M., 2007.

Association Between Funding and Quality of Published Medical Education Research. JAMA 298, 1002. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.298.9.1002

-p

Rentz, T.O., Powers, M.B., Smits, J.A.J., Cougle, J.R., Telch, M.J., 2003. Active-imaginal

Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 1337–1353.

re

exposure: examination of a new behavioral treatment for cynophobia (dog phobia).

lP

Rescorla, R.A., 2006. Deepened extinction from compound stimulus presentation. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 32, 135–144. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-

na

7403.32.2.135

Rihm, J.S., Sollberger, S.B., Soravia, L.M., Rasch, B., 2016. Re-presentation of Olfactory

ur

Exposure Therapy Success Cues during Non-Rapid Eye Movement Sleep did not Increase Therapy Outcome but Increased Sleep Spindles. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10,

Jo

340. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00340 Rimm, D.C., Medeiros, D.C., 1970. The role of muscle relaxation in participant modeling. Behav. Res. Ther. 8, 127–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(70)90082-3

Rodriguez, B.I., Craske, M.G., Mineka, S., Hladek, D., 1999. Context-specificity of relapse: effects of therapist and environmental context on return of fear. Behav. Res. Ther. 37, 845–862.

Rowe, M.K., Craske, M.G., 1998. Effects of an expanding-spaced vs massed exposure schedule on fear reduction and return of fear. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, 701–717. Rupp, C., Doebler, P., Ehring, T., Vossbeck-Elsebusch, A.N., 2017. Emotional Processing Theory Put to Test: A Meta-Analysis on the Association Between Process and Outcome Measures in Exposure Therapy. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 24, 697–711. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2039 Schienle, A., Schafer, A., Hermann, A., Rohrmann, S., Vaitl, D., 2007. Symptom provocation

ro of

and reduction in patients suffering from spider phobia: an fMRI study on exposure therapy. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 257, 486–493. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-007-0754-y

-p

Scholten, W.D., Batelaan, N.M., Van Balkom, A.J., Wjh. Penninx, B., Smit, J.H., Van Oppen, P., 2013. Recurrence of anxiety disorders and its predictors. J. Affect. Disord. 147, 180–

re

185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.10.031

lP

Schwarzmeier, H., Leehr, E.J., Böhnlein, J., Seeger, F., Roesmann, K., Gathmann, B., Herrmann, M.J., Siminski, N., Junghöfer, M., Straube, T., Grotegerd, D., Dannlowski, U., Lueken, U., n.d. Theranostic markers for personalized therapy of spider phobia:

na

Methods of a bicentric external cross-validation machine learning approach. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res.

ur

Schweckendiek, J., Klucken, T., Merz, C.J., Tabbert, K., Walter, B., Ambach, W., Vaitl, D., Stark, R., 2011. Weaving the (neuronal) web: Fear learning in spider phobia.

Jo

Neuroimage 54, 681–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.049

Shiban, Y., Brutting, J., Pauli, P., Mühlberger, A., 2015a. Fear reactivation prior to exposure therapy: does it facilitate the effects of VR exposure in a randomized clinical sample? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 46, 133–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.09.009 Shiban, Y., Pauli, P., Mühlberger, A., 2013. Effect of multiple context exposure on renewal in spider phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 68–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.10.007

Shiban, Y., Peperkorn, H.M., Alpers, G.W., Pauli, P., Mühlberger, A., 2016. Influence of perceptual cues and conceptual information on the activation and reduction of claustrophobic fear. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 51, 19–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.11.002 Shiban, Y., Schelhorn, I., Pauli, P., Mühlberger, A., 2015b. Effect of combined multiple contexts and multiple stimuli exposure in spider phobia: A randomized clinical trial in virtual reality. Behav. Res. Ther. 71, 45–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.05.014

ro of

Sloan, T., Telch, M.J., 2002. The effects of safety-seeking behavior and guided threat reappraisal on fear reduction during exposure: an experimental investigation. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 235–251.

-p

Smith, K.L., Kirkby, K.C., Montgomery, I.M., Daniels, B.A., 1997. Computer-delivered

modeling of exposure for spider phobia: relevant versus irrelevant exposure. J. Anxiety

re

Disord. 11, 489–497.

Smits, J.A.J., Hofmann, S.G., Rosenfield, D., DeBoer, L.B., Costa, P.T., Simon, N.M.,

lP

O’Cleirigh, C., Meuret, A.E., Marques, L., Otto, M.W., Pollack, M.H., 2013. Dcycloserine augmentation of cognitive behavioral group therapy of social anxiety

na

disorder: prognostic and prescriptive variables. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 81, 1100– 1112. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034120

ur

Smits, J.A.J., Telch, M.J., Randall, P.K., 2002. An examination of the decline in fear and

Jo

disgust during exposure-based treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 1243–1253. Soravia, L.M., Heinrichs, M., Winzeler, L., Fisler, M., Schmitt, W., Horn, H., Dierks, T., Strik, W., Hofmann, S.G., de Quervain, D.J.-F., 2014. Glucocorticoids enhance in vivo exposure-based therapy of spider phobia. Depress. Anxiety 31, 429–435. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22219 St-Jacques, J., Bouchard, S., Belanger, C., 2010a. Is virtual reality effective to motivate and raise interest in phobic children toward therapy? A clinical trial study of in vivo with in

virtuo versus in vivo only treatment exposure. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71, 924–931. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.08m04822blu St-Jacques, J., Bouchard, S., Belanger, C., 2010b. Is virtual reality effective to motivate and raise interest in phobic children toward therapy? A clinical trial study of in vivo with in virtuo versus in vivo only treatment exposure. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71, 924–931. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.08m04822blu Sy, J.T., Dixon, L.J., Lickel, J.J., Nelson, E.A., Deacon, B.J., 2011. Failure to replicate the

ro of

deleterious effects of safety behaviors in exposure therapy. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 305– 314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.02.005

Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M.D., Craske, M.G., 2008. The Lasting Effect of Words on

-p

Feelings: Words May Facilitate Exposure Effects to Threatening Images. Emotion 8, 307–317. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.307

re

Tardif, N., Therrien, C.-É., Bouchard, S., 2019. Re-Examining Psychological Mechanisms

lP

Underlying Virtual Reality-Based Exposure for Spider Phobia. Cyberpsychology, Behav. Soc. Netw. 22, 39–45. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0711

na

Tart, C.D., Handelsman, P.R., DeBoer, L.B., Rosenfield, D., Pollack, M.H., Hofmann, S.G., Powers, M.B., Otto, M.W., Smits, J.A.J., 2013. Augmentation of exposure therapy with post-session administration of d-cycloserine. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47, 168–174.

ur

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.09.024

Jo

Taylor, S., Abramowitz, J.S., McKay, D., 2012. Non-adherence and non-response in the treatment of anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 26, 583–589. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.02.010

Telch, M.J., Bruchey, A.K., Rosenfield, D., Cobb, A.R., Smits, J., Pahl, S., Gonzalez-Lima, F., 2014. Effects of post-session administration of methylene blue on fear extinction and contextual memory in adults with claustrophobia. Am. J. Psychiatry 171, 1091–1098. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101407

Telch, M.J., Valentiner, D.P., Ilai, D., Petruzzi, D., Hehmsoth, M., 2000. The facilitative effects of heart-rate feedback in the emotional processing of claustrophobic fear. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 373–387. Telch, M.J., Valentiner, D.P., Ilai, D., Young, P.R., Powers, M.B., Smits, J.A.J., 2004. Fear activation and distraction during the emotional processing of claustrophobic fear. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 35, 219–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.03.004 Tortella-Feliu, M., Botella, C., Llabres, J., Breton-Lopez, J.M., del Amo, A.R., Banos, R.M.,

ro of

Gelabert, J.M., 2011. Virtual reality versus computer-aided exposure treatments for fear of flying. Behav. Modif. 35, 3–30. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445510390801

Trumpf, J., Margraf, J., Vriends, N., Meyer, A.H., Becker, E.S., 2010. Specific phobia predicts

-p

psychopathology in young women. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 45, 1161– 1166. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-009-0159-5

re

Van Gerwen, L.J., Spinhoven, P., Van Dyck, R., 2006. Behavioral and cognitive group

lP

treatment for fear of flying: a randomized controlled trial. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 37, 358–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.05.002

na

Vansteenwegen, D., Vervliet, B., Hermans, D., Thewissen, R., Eelen, P., 2007a. Verbal, behavioural and physiological assessment of the generalization of exposure-based fear reduction in a spider-anxious population. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 291–300.

ur

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.008

Jo

Vansteenwegen, D., Vervliet, B., Iberico, C., Baeyens, F., Van den Bergh, O., Hermans, D., 2007b. The repeated confrontation with videotapes of spiders in multiple contexts attenuates renewal of fear in spider-anxious students. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1169– 1179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.08.023 Vika, M., Skaret, E., Raadal, M., Öst, L.G., Kvale, G., 2009. One- vs. five-session treatment of intra-oral injection phobia: a randomized clinical study. Eur. J. Oral Sci. 117, 279– 285. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0722.2009.00628.x

Waters, A.M., Farrell, L.J., Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J., Milliner, E., Tiralongo, E., Donovan, C.L., McConnell, H., Bradley, B.P., Mogg, K., Ollendick, T.H., 2014. Augmenting one-session treatment of children’s specific phobias with attention training to positive stimuli. Behav. Res. Ther. 62, 107–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.07.020 Wiederhold, B.K., Gevirtz, R.N., Spira, J.L., 2001. Virtual reality exposure therapy vs. imagery desensitization therapy in the treatment of flying phobia., in: Riva, G., Galimberti, C., Riva (Ed), G., Galimberti (Ed), C. (Eds.), Towards Cyberpsychology:

ro of

Mind, Cognition and Society in the Internet Age. IOS Press, Amsterdam, Netherlands, pp. 253–272.

Wiederhold, B.K., Jang, D.P., Gevirtz, R.G., Kim, S.I., Kim, I.Y., Wiederhold, M.D., 2002. The treatment of fear of flying: a controlled study of imaginal and virtual reality graded

-p

exposure therapy. IEEE Trans. Inf. Technol. Biomed. 6, 218–223.

re

Wilhelm, F.H., Roth, W.T., 1997. Acute and delayed effects of alprazolam on flight phobics

lP

during exposure. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 831–841.

Williams, S.L., Dooseman, G., Kleinfield, E., 1984. Comparative effectiveness of guided

505–518.

na

mastery and exposure treatments for intractable phobias. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 52,

Wittchen, H.-U., Jacobi, F., Rehm, J., Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Jönsson, B., Olesen,

ur

J., Allgulander, C., Alonso, J., Faravelli, C., Fratiglioni, L., Jennum, P., Lieb, R., Maercker, A., van Os, J., Preisig, M., Salvador-Carulla, L., Simon, R., Steinhausen, H.-

Jo

C., 2011. The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 655–679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018

Wolitzky-Taylor, K.B., Telch, M.J., 2009. Augmenting in vivo exposure with fear antagonistic actions: a preliminary test. Behav. Ther. 40, 57–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2007.12.006

Wolpe, J., 1954. Reciprocal Inhibition as the main basis of psychotherapeutic effects. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 72, 205. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurpsyc.1954.02330020073007 Zoellner, L.A., Echiverri, A.M., Craske, M.G., 2000. Processing of phobic stimuli and its

Jo

ur

na

lP

re

-p

ro of

relationship to outcome. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 921–931.

Identification

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart for study inclusion.

Records identified through database searching: n=2619 (PubMed: n=2045; PsycInfo: n=573)

Screening

Records after duplicates removed: n=1938

Records excluded:

ro of

Records screened: n=1938

n=1386

Full-text articles excluded, with

lP

n=552

re

Eligibility

Full-text articles assessed for eligibility:

-p

reasons (n=441):

Included

Studies included in qualitative synthesis:

Jo

ur

na

n=111

 wrong sample: n=142  Intervention does not meet criteria of exposition: n=115  n<10: n=63  no predictors as independent variable: n=43  short report: n=14  not an empirical study (i.e. review or case study): n=28  success of intervention is not measured: n=6  No or wrong control group (e.g. no ET in the control group): n=29  language: n=1

ro of

-p

re

lP

na

ur

Jo

Table 1 Quality and risk of bias assessment for studies researching factors influencing the success of Exposure Therapy in Specific Phobia 1 point

0.5 points

Independent sample

Yes

-

Sample size

≥20/condition

≥15/condition

Diagnosis secured

SCID or comparable

Questionnaire with clear cutoff AND BAT

Exclusion criteria

Reported

-

Concomitant Medication reported

No

-

Statistical control of possible confounders

Yes (or matched groups/ exclusion of confounders)

-

Standardized

Length or specific content not standardized, but manualized

Sample (0-3 points) No

re

Standardization

Behavioral measure

Yes

Physiological measure

Yes

Rating by professional

Randomization Blinding

Jo

RCT methodology (0-2 points)

-

Yes

-

Yes, exclusively

Yes, plus non-evidence based addition (medication etc.)

ur

Evidence-based treatment? (0-1 point)

na

Yes

lP

Outcome measures (0-4 points) Self-report

-p

Exposure Therapy (0-1 point)

ro of

Confounder Control (0-3 points)

Yes

-

Yes

-

Sum score study quality (0-14 points)

SCID: Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV; BAT= Behavioral Avoidance Test; RCT = Randomized Controlled Trial

Table 2 Included studies, their quality score and results ordered by the researched factors Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

Before the ET – emotional factors 1998

OST

36 women

Spider Phobia

The higher the trait anxiety scores, the less patients profited from behavior therapy.

7

-

-

Rodriguez, Craske, Mineka, & Hladek

1999

In vivo exposure

65 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

Higher trait anxiety scores are associated with less successful therapy (increased return of fear)

9,5

-

-

2010

Exposure, in vivo only vs. VR plus in vivo

Spider Phobia

No increase in motivation through the use of VRET; significant impact of motivation (especially extrinsic integrated motivation) on subjective treatment success

9,25

+

 see Computer assistance

Fear reactivation

Jo

 see Sex of therapist

1976

31 children (8-15 years) in 2 groups

OST (15 minutes)

40 women in 4 groups

Snake Phobia

Significant impact of the motivation to change; interaction of motivation with modeling behavior

4,5

+

+

ur

O’Sullivan, & Gilner

lP

St-Jacques, Bouchard, & Bélanger

na

Motivation

re

 see Context and stimulus variation, Length of the session, Recovery of phobic cognitions

ro of

Muris, Mayer, & Merckelbach

-p

Trait Anxiety

Hodgson, & Rachman

1970

Implosive therapy

50 in 5 groups

Snake Phobia

Improved effect of implosive therapy by showing horrifying images immediately but not delayed before implosion by imaging phobic images

5,5

Shiban, Brutting, Pauli, & Mühlberger

2015

VRET + In Vivo ET (one week later) with vs. without fear

32 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

No group differences between people with fear reactivation prior to VR and in vivo exposure and

10,25

O

O

PH

PR

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

PH

10

O

O

O

8

O

O

-p

Topic

No change of coping styles over the time (i. e. two years period), but better treatment results for blunting vs. monitoring

7,5

+

+

Snake Phobia

Self-efficacy as a uniformly accurate predictor of performance on tasks

7

+

+

+

O

activation

those without

Before the ET – cognitive factors 2001

60 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

2 hours without distraction vs. 1 hour with and 1 hour without distraction

 see Attentional focus guiding

No main effect of coping style, distraction nor their interaction on the short-term outcome

1993

OST

33

Spider Phobia

Muris, Merckelbach, & de Jong

1995

OST

36 women

Spider Phobia

Bandura, Adams, & Beyer

1977

Observation only vs. observation and imitation (=performance mastery experiences)

33 in 3 groups

Cote & Bouchard

2009

VRET

28

Spider Phobia

Best predictors of change in general outcomes and cardiac response: changes in perceived self-efficacy and dysfunctional beliefs; best predictor of change in behavioral avoidance: change in dysfunctional beliefs

10

lP

re

Muris, De Jong, Merckelbach, & van Zuuren

ur Jo

Self-efficacy

OST

ro of

Antony, McCabe, Leeuw, Sano, & Swinson

na

Coping Style

No substantial contribution of the coping style to short-term or long-term outcomes

Zoellner, Echiverry, & Craske

2000

2 sessions of in vivo exposure

46

Spider Phobia

Positive relationship of selfefficacy to better behavioral performance at post-treatment

7

Williams, Dooseman, & Kleinfield

1984

exposition vs. guided mastery exposition

32 in 3 groups

Driving Phobia, Acrophobia

Same effect of exposition and mastery/self-efficacy-oriented treatment regarding the duration of exposure and degree of inducement to confront threats

6,75

+

+

+

+

PR

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

O/+

O/+

PH

PR

Mayer, Merckelbach, & Muris

2000

OST

39

Spider Phobia

Memory

Zoellner, Echiverry, & Craske

2000

2 sessions of in vivo exposure

46

Spider Phobia

41

Spider Phobia

Cortisol levels

Lass-Hennemann, & Michael

2014

OST in the morning vs. evening

lP

OST

7

+

Eyeblink startle response has a modest predictive effect, explaining 16% of the treatment effect

7

-

60 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Higher effectiveness of exposure therapy in the morning vs. evening; suggested underlying effect: higher endogenous cortisol levels in the morning group, enhancement of extinction memory by cortisol

12

(+)

2014

Jo

Soravia, Heinrichs, Winzeler, Fisler, Schmitt, Horn., …, & de Quervain

ur

na

1991

Better memory for the anxious response (but not the phobic stimulus) leads to better treatment success

re

Before the ET: physiological factors de Jong, Merckelbach, & Arntz

7

ro of

Perceived automaticity and irrationality of cognitions

Eyeblink startle response

No influence of self-received automaticity on treatment outcome; irrationality as predictor for treatment outcome (the more the fear is experienced as irrational, the higher the profit from the exposure therapy)

-p

rapidly; better effect of mastery (self-efficacy) model regarding restoring behavioral functioning and diminishing anxiety

Two sessions of in vivo exposurebased group therapy with vs. without Cortisol 1hr. before session

22 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Increased effectiveness by administrating Cortisol; significantly greater reduction in fear of spiders at follow-up, but not immediately after treatment

11

+

OST

100 youths

diverse

The presence of comorbid phobias or anxiety disorders did not affect treatment outcomes;

9,5

O

+

Before the ET: comorbid disorders Comorbid disorders

Ollendick, Öst, Reuterskiold, & Costa

2010

O

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

PH

significant reductions in the clinical severity of other cooccurring specific phobias and related anxiety disorders through the treatment of the targeted specific phobias During the ET: emotional factors 2005

VRET

42 in 2 groups

Aviophobia

Rachman, & Levitt

1988

Diverse

74 in 4 groups

Claustrophobia

Baker, Mystkowski, Culver, Yi, Mortazavi, & Craske

2010

Two sessions of in vivo exposition

44

Acrophobia

 see Heart rate

Disgust

Smits, Telch, & Randall

2002

8,75

+

Suggested relation between fear reduction and “habituation” (but not between initial levels of fear and “habituation”)

3,5

+

No consistent prediction of treatment outcome through the variables of the emotional processing theory; no support was found for initial fear activation (IFA) or within session habituation (WSH, fear subsides within sessions); measures of between session habituation (BSH, fear subsides between sessions) were predictive of short-term change, but these effects were attenuated at followup

7,75

?

O

?

-p

re

na ur 2015

Jo

Busscher, Spinhoven, & de Geus

Reductions in subjective fear ratings and HR reactions during VRET serve as significant predictors of therapy outcome

ro of

Mühlberger, Petrusek, Herrmann, & Pauli

lP

Within- and between session anxiety decrease

Two days of Cognitivebehavioural group treatment (CBGT) with VRET and in vivo exposure

79

Aviophobia

Association of within-flight habituation of HR reactivity and respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity and lower flight anxiety directly after the flight (shortterm), but not on long-term; low self-reported anxiety during in vivo flight exposure as the best predictor of successful long-term therapy outcome

10,5

+

30 min selfguided in vivo

27 women

Spider Phobia

Significant declines in spider fear and spider-specific disgust but not in global disgust sensitivity

7

O

+

O

O

PR

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

exposure

30min. selfguided in vivo exposition

22

Blood-InjuryInjection Phobia

Olatunji, Ciesielski, Wolitzky-Taylor, Wentworth, & Viar

2012

Video exposition with or without disgust activation by showing clips of vomiting people

44 in 2 groups

Blood Injection and Injury Phobia

Olatunji, WolitzkyTaylor, Ciesielski, Armstrong, Etzel, & David

2009

Video exposition with or without disgust activation by showing disgusting pictures

Disgust evocation

Choplin, & Carter

Decline in fear and disgust across trials; fear as an independent predictor for behavior in the behavioral avoidance test, contrary to disgust (dependent on fear); initial fear during exposure as a predictor for fear and disgust in the behavioral avoidance test afterwards

8

O

O

-p

re

lP

?

Spider Phobia

Significant declines in fear and disgust through repeated exposure; reduction in disgust during exposure in the disgust activation condition remained significant after controlling for change in fear

8,75

O

O

Internet-delivered exposure (images); real vs. Hyper-real images

28 in two groups

Spider Phobia

Significant reductions in behavioral avoidance and selfreported phobic symptoms but no difference for real vs. hyperreal images, but both disgust and anxiety reduction are predictors for success

9

+

+

OST (40-90min)

30 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Significant decrease of fear, avoidance and disgust through exposure to disgust or fear stimuli; greater decrease in

7,25

?

?

na 2011

6,25

46 in 2 groups

ur

Jo

2015

Significant declines in disgust and especially fear across trials; decline in fear across trials; no significant effect of trial on disgust after control for change in fear; no moderation of the fear activation or reduction during exposure through global disgust sensitivity prior to exposure

ro of

2007

Matthews, Naran, & Kirkby

PH

through exposure; stronger decay for fear vs. disgust; no moderation of the level of fear activation or fear reduction during treatment through disgust levels at pretreatment

Olatunji, Smits, Connolly, Willems, & Lohr

 see Disgust evocation

B

O

PR

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

PH

+

avoidance behavior through exposure to fear stimuli Video exposition with or without disgust activation by showing disgusting pictures

46 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Exposure to a disgusting stimulus reduces disgust additionally to and more than fear; exposure to a feared stimulus shows the opposite pattern

8,75

+

+

de Jong, Vorage, & van den Hout

2000

OST with vs. without counterconditioning

34 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

No additional effect of adding counterconditioning aspects to the exposure

8,5

O

O

Mühlberger, Herrmann, Wiedemann, Ellgring, & Pauli

2001

Four sessions of VRET vs. lengthy relaxation training session

40 in 2 groups

Aviophobia

Greater fear reduction through repeated VRET vs. relaxation;

10

-

Rimm, & Medeiros

1970

One session: relaxation only vs. participant modeling only vs. relaxation followed by participant modeling vs. control;

40 in 4 groups

Snake Phobia

Reduction of fear in the modeling conditions; no enhancement of the participant modeling exposure through relaxation

6

O

O

McGlynn, Moore, Lawyer, & Karg

1999

-p

-

na

lP

Relaxation

re

 see Disgust

ro of

2009

Olatunji, WolitzkyTaylor, Ciesielski, Armstrong, Etzel, & David

20 in 2 groups

Snake Phobia

Reduced arousal and fear during in vivo exposures through relaxation training; relaxation working by lowering arousal throughout the course of exposure

8

-

-

20x38sec. exposition trials on 2 days (10/10) with vs. without positive valence training (at the end of day 1)

61 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Significant less subjective fear and less behavioral avoidance through positive valence training

6

+

+

Additional Positive Valence Training

Jo

ur

6 x 4min. in vivo exposition with vs. without relaxation training before

Dour, Brown, & Craske

During the ET: cognitive factors

2016

-

PR

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B O

Krijn, Emmelkamp, Biemond, De Wilde De Ligny, Schuemie, & Van Der Mast

2004

VRET using a head-mounted display (HMD – low presence) vs. a computer automatic virtual environment (CAVE – high presence)

37 in 3 groups

Acrophobia

Less experienced presence and anxiety during the first VRET session for early drop-outs; no relationship of presence and anxiety in treatment completers

9

O

Coping selfstatements

Krijn, Emmelkamp, Olafsson, Schuemie, & van der Mast

2007

VRET with vs. without production / activation of coping selfstatements

26 in 2 groups

Acrophobia

Reduction of anxiety of heights, avoidance of height situations and improvement of attitudes towards heights through VRET; no influence of adding selfstatements

8,5

O

CBT vs. Exposure only

Raes, Koster, Loeys, & De Raedt

2011

Behavioral vs. cognitivebehavioral OST

31 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Strong treatment effects and cognitive mediation of these effects in both conditions; mediation of exposure effects through cognition even without explicitly targeting those

9,5

O

O

Tardif, Therrien, & Bouchard

2019

30-minute VRET with visual vs. visual and tactical vs. visual, tactical, and haptic feedback

59 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

Changes in self-reported selfefficacy and cognitive beliefs predicted ET success

9

+

+

OST (10 exposures, each lasting 38 seconds) focussing on affect-labelling vs. reappraisal vs. distraction

88 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

Reduced skin conductance response in the post-test for the affect-labelling group; no differences in self-reported fear; greater reductions in fear responding by greater use of anxiety and fear words

8

O

O

ur

2012

Jo

Kircanski, Lieberman, & Craske

na

 see Feedback to body sensations

lP

re

ro of

Presence

-p

Topic

Tabibnia, Liebermann, & Craske

2008

Exposure to picturs with no vs. negative vs. neutral labels

48 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

Negative labels improved success when measured with skin conduction, but not with HR acceleration

9,5

Dee

1972

Taped implosive therapy with

58 in 5

Snake Phobia

Weak facilitating effect of instructions in reducing fear;

5,75

PH

+

?

?

?

PR

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

different instructions vs. no instructions vs. control

groups

Type of Specific Phobia

Better improvement through guided threat reappraisal

7,5

+

+

Claustrophobia

Facilitating effect of guided threat reappraisal and detrimental effect of cognitive load task on fear reduction; highest fear reduction and lowest return of fear in the guided threat reappraisal condition

7,5

+

2002

6x5min. exposure only vs. with guided threat focus and reappraisal vs. with safetybehaviorutilization

46 in 3 groups

Claustrophobia

2000

6x5min. in vivo

Koch, Spates, &

lP

re

-p

ro of

Sloan, & Telch

58 in 4 groups

ur

na

Exposure only vs. with guided threat reappraisal vs. with a cognitive load distracter task vs. with both

2006

2004

B

+

Snake Phobia

Van Gerwen, Spinhoven, & Van Dyck

SR

+

36 in 3 groups

 see Attentional focus guiding

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor

6

Participant Modeling

Kamphuis, & Telch

Quality Score

Significant higher improvement of behavior and attitudes through medium or high number of performance/ induction aids (company, gradual exposure); highest generalization effect on some measures for moderate induction aids; no effect of number of facilitations on therapeutic efficacy

1974

 see Safety Behavior

Main findings

retardation of fear reduction through the absence of any instructions; uncertainty about the efficacy of standardized, taped therapy

Bandura, Jeffery, & Wright

Jo

Topic

1-day behavioral (BGT) vs. 2-days cognitivebehavioral group treatment (CBGT); both conditions with information about flights, airplanes, etc.

150 in 3 groups

Aviophobia

Success of both treatment conditions; higher effectiveness of CBGT on subjective measures of fear and self-efficacy

10

+

?

Behavioral vs.

40 in 4

Phobia of small

No significant difference in the

9,5

?

O

PH

PR

Author

Year

Himle  see Stimulus variation

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

6,5

+

+

Return of distress through the activation of concepts unrelated to spiders (negative effect of distraction); decrease in emotional and avoidance responses between sessions through activation of concepts related to spiders

10

+

+

Claustrophobia

Facilitating effect of guided threat reappraisal and detrimental effect of cognitive load task on fear reduction; highest fear reduction and lowest return of fear in the guided threat reappraisal condition

7,5

+

2015

Image exposure only vs. with activation of concepts related vs. unrelated to spiders

66 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

Kamphuis, & Telch

2000

6x5min. in vivo

58 in 4 groups

ur

re

na

Exposure only vs. with guided threat reappraisal vs. with a cognitive load distracter task vs. with both 30-minute selfguided exposure (1) with threatening vs. neutral words and (2) images only vs. with a cognitive load task

60 in 4 groups

Claustrophobia

Slower between-trial habituation and less fear reduction posttreatment when distracted during exposure; no effect of threat manipulation

8

-

-

OST

60 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

No effect of distraction nor its interaction with coping style on the short-term outcome

10

O

O

2 hours without distraction vs. 1

PH

?

-p

ro of

Dethier, Bruneau, & Philippot

2001

B

Highest fear reduction in the (external) distraction condition; greatest decrease in the behavioral avoidance task and greatest increase in perceived control after treatment in the (external) distraction condition

Blood-InjuryInjection Phobia

Antony, McCabe, Leeuw, Sano, & Swinson

SR effectiveness of behavioral vs. cognitive-behavioral OST; behavioral OST perceived as more intrusive

50 in 5 groups

2004

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor

animals and insects (spider, snake, rat/mouse, crawling insects)

Exposure only vs. with internal vs. external focus vs. external vs. internal distraction

Telch, Valentiner, Ilai, Young, Powers, & Smits

Quality Score

groups

2008

 see CBT vs. Exposure only

Main findings

cognitivebehavioral OST with vs. without programmed generalization

Oliver, & Page

Jo

Attentional Focus Guiding

Treatment

lP

Topic

O

PR

Author

Year

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

11

+

?

improvement of fear and avoidance in all conditions; less improvement through safety behavior utilization during treatment

7,5

-

-

2014

Attention Training towards positive stimuli (ATP) vs. control (ATC), both followed by OST

37 children in 2 groups

diverse

Rentz, Powers, Smits, Cougle, & Telch

2003

exposure in vivo (IV) vs. imaginal (IE) vs. active imaginal (AI)

82 in 3 groups

Cynophobia (Dog Phobia)

Deacon, Sy, Lickel, & Nelson

2010

6x5min. in vivo exposure with vs. without access to safety/coping aids during the first four exposure trials

33 in 2 groups

Claustrophobia

no reliable advantages or disadvantages of the availability of safety behavior

-p

7

O

Hood, Antony, Koerner, & Monson

2010

Exposure with vs. without safety behavior use (SBU vs. NSB)

43 in 2 groups

na

Spider Phobia

Faster approach in the SBU condition; small but significant decrease in approach distance at follow-up in the SBU condition

7,5

+

+

OST (45min.) with vs. without the use of safety gear

62 in 2 groups

Snake Phobia

Better approach during the exposure using safety behavior/gear; no differences in the follow-up behavioral avoidance test, significant improvement in fearful cognitions, subjective anxiety and distance of approach

8,5

+

O

6x max. 5min. in vivo exposure only vs. with safety behavior availability (SBA)

58 in 3 groups

Claustrophobia

Equal fear reduction in all conditions, but significantly greater improvement in selfefficacy and claustrophobic cognitions through SBU vs.

7

?

Sy, Dixon, Lickel, Nelson, & Deacon

lP

ur 2008

Jo

Milosevic, & Radomsky

2011

significantly greater reductions in danger expectancies and improved attention bias towards positive stimuli, but no differences in the clinician, parent or child-rated clinical outcomes

re

During the ET: behavioral factors Safety Behavior

Main findings

PH

PR

hour with and 1 hour without distraction

 see Coping style

Waters, Farrell, Zimmer-Gembeck, Milliner, Tiralongo, Donovan, McConnell, Bradley, Mogg, & Ollendick

Treatment

ro of

Topic

?

-

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

vs. utilization (SBU) 46 in 3 groups

Claustrophobia

Wolitzky, & Telch

2009

6x6min. in vivo exposure with vs. without oppositional actions vs. placebo (pulsed audio-photic stimulation) vs. control

88 in 4 groups

Acrophobia

O’Sullivan & Gilner

1976

OST: 2 hours without distraction vs. 1 hour with and 1 hour without distraction

40 women in 4 groups

2002

Bandarian-Balooch, Neumann, & Boschen

2015

Shiban, Brütting, Pauli, & Mühlberger

2015a

B

7,5

-

-

10

+

PH

?

-p

Better fear extinction and generalization through the use of oppositional actions during exposure

Significant contribution of the perceived role consonance between the subject and the model to behavior change

4,5

+

46 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

Return of fear in a new context in the follow up (1 week later); return of fear independent from perceptions of safety, danger, control and predictability

9

+

+

OST in one vs. three contexts

46 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

One-context participants showed renewal of fear (behavioral, physiological and subjective), while this was attenuated in three contexts

8,5

+

+

+

VRET after vs. without fear reactivation with a live spider

32 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Long-term reduction of spider fear in both groups regardless of the fear reactivation

10,25

O

O

O

OST with different treatment contexts

ur

Mystkowski, Craske, & Echiverri

Jo

Context variation

SR

Snake Phobia

lP

 see Motivation

Weakened effect of between-trial habituation through safetybehavior utilization; significantly more fear post-treatment and follow-up for the utilized safetybehavior condition

ro of

6x5min. exposure only vs. with guided threat focus and reappraisal vs. with safetybehaviorutilization

re

Sex of therapist

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor

exposure only

2002

 see CBT vs. Exposure only

Quality Score

na

Sloan, & Telch

Main findings

PR

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B +

PH

Shiban, Pauli, & Mühlberger

2013

4x 5 minuten VRET

40 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Extinction in multiple contexts was able to significantly reduce renewal compared to extinction in a single context

9,5

+

Vansteenwegen, Vervliet, Iberico, Baeyens, Van den Bergh, & Hermans

2007

Video exposition in one vs. three contexts

52 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

Significant reduction of fear through extinction; generalization of extinction in the multiple but not in the single extinction group

6,5

+

Vansteenwegen, Vervliet, Hermans, Thewissen, & Eelen

2007

Three sessions of two hours of group in vivo exposure

26 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Enhancement of efficiency of exposure treatment in the long term through conducting treatment and tests in different contexts

7,5

+

+

+

Rodriguez, Craske, Mineka, & Hladek

1999

In vivo exposure

65 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

No influence of context or stimulus on return of fear

9,5

O

O

O

2015

4x 5 minuten VRET

Attenuation of return of fear by multiple context exposure in VR short-term but not long-term;

11

O/+

O/+

9,5

O

O

lP 38 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

na

Shiban, Schelhorn, Pauli, & Mühlberger

long-term effect of fear attenuation only in the multi stimuli single context group; higher benefit in the multiple stimuli condition in the behavioral avoidance test short- and longterm; no superiority of the combined multiple stimuli and contexts condition found

ur Jo

Koch, Spates, & Himle  see CBT vs. Exposure only

+

re

see Trait anxiety, Length of the session, Recovery of phobic cognitions

Stimulus variation

Main findings

ro of

Context and stimulus variation

Author

-p

Topic

2004

Behavioral vs. cognitivebehavioral OST with vs. without programmed generalization

40 in 4 groups

Phobia of small animals and insects (spider, snake, rat/mouse, crawling insects)

No added effect through programmed generalization

PR

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

One vs. five session(s) applied tension vs. one session tension only

30 in 3 groups

Blood Phobia

Significant improvement in all treatment conditions for shortand long-term; treatment of choice: one-session applied tension or tension only

11,5

O

O

Öst, L.-G., Brandberg, M., & Alm, T.

1997

One vs. five session(s) of exposure and cognitive restructuring (3hrs. massed vs. 6hrs. gradual treatment)

28 in 2 groups

Aviophobia

No differences between the conditions

9,5

O

O

Öst, Alm, Brandberg, & Breitholtz

2001

One vs. five session(s) of exposure vs. five sessions of cognitive therapy

46 in 4 groups

Claustrophobia

10

O

O

Vika, Skaret, Raadal, Öst, & Kvale

2009

One vs. five sessions of CBT by dentists (sensu Öst)

55 in 2 groups

Fraser, Kirkby, Daniels, Gilroy, & Montgomery

2001

ro of

1996

Significant treatment effect of one and five sessions of CBT performed by specially trained dentists; less anxiety for five sessions at the one-year follow up

10,25

?

?

30 in 2 groups

Significant treatment effect in both conditions; no significant differences between the threeand six-session groups; competence on the program already reached after three sessions (also in the six-sessions group)

10

O

O

na

2011

Computerassisted fear of flying treatment (CAFFT)

37

Aviophobia

The fewer the number of attended sessions, the faster the rate of change; significant correlation of HR variability and within-session rates of change (WSRC), faster improvement of people with higher HR variability

7,25

+

1971

6x 30min. in vivo

40 in 4

Snake Phobia

Same effect of the length of 0 vs. 30 vs. 45 minutes, better than 15

5,5

-

ur

3 vs. 6 sessions of CAVE Expo

PH

O

-p Intra-Oral Injection Phobia

 see Heart rate

Miller, & Levis

Treatment success through all three conditions; no differences

Spider Phobia

Jo

Bornas, Gelabert, Llabres, Balle, & Tortela-Feliu

Length of the

Main findings

Hellstrom, Fellenius, & Öst

lP

Number of sessions

Author

re

Topic

+

-

PR

Author

Year

session

Treatment

Sample Size

exposition

groups

Type of Specific Phobia

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

PH

O

O

-

-

-

+

?

+

-

-

?

+

+

?

minutes; possible explanation (speculative!): reaching the highest amount of fear without time for tackling the fear and coping strategies

Rachman, & Lopatka

1988

One-session exposition with therapeutic modelling (undervs. full- vs. overlearning)

39 in 3 goups

Snake or Spider Phobia

Rodriguez, Craske, Mineka, & Hladek

1999

In vivo exposure

65 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

Massed vs. expandingspaced ET (4 sessions in 1 day vs. 4 in 4 weeks)

31 in 2 groups

No benefit of extending the ET after reaching a level of no subjective fear

6,5

Longer treatment duration predicted higher chance of return of fear

Rowe, & Craske

1998

Computer assistance

Öst, Stridh, & Wolf

1998

Spider Phobia

Similar good effect of massed (ME) and expanding-spaced (ESE) exposure; significantly more habituation across exposure trials in the ME vs. ESE group; return of fear in the ME, but not in the ESE group

na

lP

Massed vs. expandingspaced exposure

re

-p

 see Trait anxiety, Context and stimulus variation, Recovery of phobic cognitions

Main findings

ro of

Topic

103 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

Significantly better effect through individual in vivo vs. group in vivo treatment vs. video treatment

Imaginal Exposure Therapy (IET) vs. VRET with vs. without physiological feedback

30 in 3 groups

Aviophobia

Higher effectiveness of VRGET vs. IET; possibly added efficacy through physiological feedback

Jo

ur

Self-help manual vs. video vs. group vs. individual exposure treatment

8,75

Wiederhold, Jang, Gevirtz, Kim, Kim, & Wiederhold

 see Feedback to body reactions

2002

VR-Expo

9,25

PR

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

2001

VRET with vs. without physiological feedback vs. imagery conditions

30 in 3 groups

Aviophobia

VR patients (irrespective of feedback) improved considerably more than imagery patients

9,75

+

+

Tortella-Feliu, Botella, Llabres, Breton-Lopez, del Amo, Banos, & Gelabert

2011

VRET vs. computer-aided exposure with (CAE-T) vs. without (CAE-SA) therapist’s assistance through exposure

60 in 3 groups

Aviophobia

Significant improvement in all conditions; no differences between groups

10

O

O

Andersson, G., Waara, J., Jonsson, U., Malmaeus, F., Carlbring, P., & Öst, L.-G

2013

OST vs. guided Internet-delivered self-help

30 in 2 groups

Snake phobia

?

+

Andersson, Waara, Jonsson, Malmeus, Carlbring, & Öst

2009

OST vs. guided Internet-delivered self-help

24 in 2 groups

Botella, Pérez-Ara, Bretón-López, Quero, GarcíaPalacios, & Banos

2016

one session of in vivo (IVE) vs. augmented reality exposure (ARE)

St-Jacques, Bouchard, & Bélanger  see Motivation

2010

-p

re

9

?

-

50 in 2 groups

Small Animal Phobia

Improvement in both conditions; IVE more useful than ARE (posttreatment, but not at follow-up); ARE considered as less aversive, high acceptance by patients

12

-/?

+/?

VRET vs. VRET and exposure in vivo

10

Acrophobia

Effectiveness of both conditions; VRET effective, even with relatively cheap hard- and software on stand-alone computers currently on the market

2,5

O

O

Exposure, in vivo only vs. VR plus in vivo

31 children (8-15 years) in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

No increase in motivation and success through the use of VRET

9,25

O

lP

Good improvement in both conditions; better improvement for in vivo exposure, partially significant

na 2001

Improvement in both conditions; self-report shows no group effect; sig. interaction for BAT in favor of OST

Spider Phobia

ur

Emmelkamp, Bruynzeel, Drost, & van der Mast

ro of

Wiederhold, Gevirtz, & Spira

Jo

Topic

PH

PR

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

9,5

-

-

2001

Prolonged singlesession treatment of live graded exposure (LGE) vs. computer-aided vicarious exposure (CAVE)

40 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

Michaliszyn, Marchand, Bouchard, Martel, & Poirier-Bisson

2010

VRET vs. in vivo exposure

43 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

Significant improvements in both groups; greater improvement on the Spider Beliefs Questionnaire for in vivo exposure

11

-

?

Krijn, Emmelkamp, Biemond, de Wilde de Ligny, Schuemie, & van der Mast

2004

VRET using a head-mounted display (HMD – low presence) vs. a computer automatic virtual environment (CAVE – high presence)

37 in 3 groups

Acrophobia

no relationship of presence and anxiety in treatment completers ; no effect of either using HMD or CAVE VRET

9

O

O

Motion simulation

Mühlberger, Wiedemann, & Pauli

2005

VRET with vs. without motion simulation

25 in 2 groups

Aviophobia

Stronger fear during VRET and slower habituation in the motion vs. no-motion group, but no differences in treatment outcome

8,5

O

Additional manipulation of the stimulus

Smith, Kirkby, Montgomery, & Daniels

1997

3x40minute computerdelivered treatment with relevant vs. irrelevant exposure, relevant with vs. without feedback

38 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

Significant phobic improvement in all conditions; no significant effect of either relevance of exposure or manipulation of the onscreen feedback

8

O

6 exposure trials of max. two minutes

80 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

No effect of perceived emotional control in general nor control over the duration of exposure on the fear levels, fear reduction, or fear prediction accuracy;

5

O

-p

PH

re

lP

na

ur

Jo 1991

ro of

Heading, Kirkby, Martin, Daniels, Gilroy, & Menzies

Craske, Bunt, Rapee, & Barlow

Significant improvement for the LGE group; no difference between the CAVE and waiting list control group (exception: subjective distress, little support for single-session CAVE treatment)

Quality Score

O

PR

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

9

+

No identification of stable predictors for short- or long-term success; only significant predictors in single studies, seen as chance findings

10

O

O

Aviophobia

Patients with high HR variability improve more; HR variability only in combination with HR entropy as a significant predictor

10

+

+

Claustrophobia

Higher HR at the beginning of exposure as a predictor for better treatment outcome; HR correlates more with success than subjective measures

8

+

2016

4x 5min. VRET with pretreatment fear reaction activation by specific perceptual cues (C) vs. conceptual fearrelated information (I) vs. combination of both (CI)

38 in 3 groups

Claustrophobia

Hellstrom, & Öst

1996

OST vs. fivesession therapistguided exposure vs. self-directed exposure

138 (from four other studies)

Spider Phobia, Blood Phobia, Injection Phobia

Bornas, Gelabert, Llabres, Balle, &Tortella-Feliu

2007

Computerassisted fear of flying treatment (CAFFT)

33

Alpers, & Sell

2008

6x 30min. in vivo exposition

10

re

lP

na ur 2011

Jo

Bornas, Gelabert, Llabres, Balle, & Tortella-Feliu

Computerassisted fear of flying treatment (CAFFT)

37

Aviophobia

Significant correlation of HR variability and within-session rates of change (WSRC), faster improvement of people with higher HR variability

7,25

+

Cognitivebehavioural group treatment (CBGT)

50

Aviophobia

HR and parasympathetic activity is shown as a predictor for patients with high (but not for those with low) anxiety sensitivity

9,5

+

 see Numbers of sessions

Busscher, Spinhoven, van Gerwen, & de Geus

2013

PH +

-p

During the ET: physiological factors Heart rate (HR)

Higher experienced initial selfreported fear through C; no significant enhancement through the combination (CI); higher fear habituation for C; differing physiological parameters in the groups, differing decreases in heart rate for claustrophobics throughout the conditions

ro of

Shiban, Peperkorn, Alpers, Pauli, & Mühlberger

B

PR

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

Self- vs. therapistadministered CAFFT vs. VRET

20 in 2 groups

Aviophobia

Combination of HR variability and HR entropy as a predictor for success of treatment; lower tonic HR in the successful group; stronger decrease of the HR during the phasic level in the successful group (higher HR variability)

10,5

Benoit Allen, Allen, Austin, Waldron, & Ollendick

2015

OST

98 children

diverse

No change of cardiovascular reactivity through ET; no predictive power of cardiovascular reactivity

9,5

-

Busscher, Spinhoven, & de Geus

2015

Two days of Cognitivebehavioural group treatment (CBGT)

79

Aviophobia

Association of within-flight habituation of HR reactivity and respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity and lower flight anxiety directly after the flight (shortterm), but not on long-term

10,5

?

1995

Two sessions in vivo exposition plus relaxation vs. disconfirmation of misappraisals of bodily sensations

70 in 2 groups (49 snake / spider phobics, 21 claustrophobics)

Claustrophobia, Snake/Spider Phobia

Equal effect on basic beliefs about arousal sensations; improvement of claustrophobic fear reduction through disconfirmation of misappraisals of bodily sensations; little effect on snake/spider phobia; no generalization to nontargeted phobic situations or over time

9

-

6x5min. selfguided in vivo exposure only (EOC) vs. with heart rate feedback (HRF) vs. with paced tone control (PTC)

54 in 3 groups

Claustrophobia

Significant greater fear reduction and lower fear levels posttreatment through HRF; no higher levels of initial fear activation

7,75

+

Imaginal Exposure Therapy (IET) vs. VRET with vs. without

30 in 3 groups

Aviophobia

Higher effectiveness of VRET vs. IET; possibly higher efficacy of VRET through added physiological feedback

9,25

+

Telch, Valentiner, Ilai, Petruzzi, & Hehmsoth

2000

Wiederhold, Jang, Gevirtz, Kim, Kim, & Wiederhold  see Computer

2002

-

-

+

+

PR +

-p re

lP

ur

na

Craske, Mohlman, Yi, Glover, & Valeri

ro of

2012

Jo

Feedback to body reactions

PH

+

Bornas, Riera del Amo, Tortella-Feliu, & Llabres

 see Within and between session anxiety decrease

B

?

Topic

Author

Year

assistance

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

9

-

-

+

PH

PR

physiological feedback VRET 2019

30-minute VRET with visual vs. visual and tactical vs. visual, tactical, and haptic feedback

59 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

 see CBT vs. Exposure only

No influence of the type of feedback on the success of VRET

ro of

Tardif, Therrien, & Bouchard

Mystkowski, Mineka, Vernon, & Zinbarg

2003

OST with vs. without caffeine

43 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Greater return of fear for a different caffeine state during treatment and follow-up

5,5

+

Yohimbine hycdrochloride (YOH)

Meyerbroeker, Powers, van Stegeren, & Emmelkamp

2012

VRET with vs. without YOH before exposure

45 in 2 groups

Aviophobia

No enhancing effect of the manipulation of the noradrenaline level through YOH in the used dosage (10mg)

10,5

O

Quetiapine

Diemer, Domschke, Mühlberger, Winter, Zavorotnyy, Notzon., …, & Zwanzger

2013

VRET with vs. without Quetiapine 8hrs. before exposure

58 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Quetiapine shows significant anxiolytic effects, but only on physiological ratings

11,5

O

+

Benzodiazepine

Wilhelm, & Roth

1997

OST with vs. without alprazolam before exposure

28 women in 2 groups

Aviophobia

Alprazolam increases physiological activation under acute stress conditions and hinders therapeutic effects of ET

8,25

-

-

Oxytocin

Acheson, Feifel, Kamenski, Mckinney, & Risbrough

2015

OST with vs. without pretreatment intranasal Oxytocine administration

23 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

Self-report shows impeded treatment response; no significant effect on behavioral measures; less confidence in the treatment before treatment and lower ratings of therapeutic alliance for the Oxytocine

11

-

Naltrexone

Merluzzi, Taylor, Boltwood, & Gotestam

1991

Graded exposure with vs. without Naltrexone

30 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

No differences concerning selfefficacy and cognitions, but higher anxiety in Naltrexone group

10

?

Arntz, Merckelbach,

1993

OST 2 hours

48 in 3

Spider Phobia

Hindering effect on ET through the induction of Naltrexone;

11,5

O

re

lP

na

ur

Jo

-p

Caffeine state

O

-

-

-

O

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

& de Jong

Type of Specific Phobia

groups

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

B

11

O

O

Return of phobic cognitions is associated with less successful outcome (increased return of fear)

-

-

+

PH

greater avoidance relapse through Naltrexone; no influence on emotional, cognitive and physiological measures

After the ET: emotional factors Rihm, Sollberger, Soravia, & Rasch

2016

2 in vivo expositions followed by 90min. sleep with vs. without odor vs. awake control group

54 in 3 groups

Spider Phobia

1999

in vivo exposure

65 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

Rodriguez, Craske, Mineka, & Hladek

12

Spider Phobia

Individual differences in the magnitude of visual cortex activations shortly after therapy as a predictor for long-term success; stable reorganization of neural responses to initially feared stimuli in successful therapy

7,5

+

lP 2012

OST

Jo

ur

Hauner, Mineka, Voss, & Paller

na

After the ET: physiological factors Neural activation

9,5

re

 see Trait anxiety, Context and stimulus variation, Length of the session

-p

After the ET: cognitive factors Recovery of phobic cognitions

No enhanced effect of sleep after exposition through presentation of a contextual odor

ro of

Conditioned odor

Schienle, Schafer, Hermann, Rohrmann, & Vaitl

2007

OST (ca. 4 hours)

14

Spider Phobia

Correlation between the reduction of subjective somatic panic symptoms and decrease of right amygdala activity in fMRI

9

+

Landowska, Roberts, Eachus, & Barrett

2018

VRET

14

Acrophobia

No correlation between subjective fear ratings and neural activation measured with fNRIS

6,75

-

-

PR

Topic

Author

Year

Treatment

Sample Size

Type of Specific Phobia

Main findings

Quality Score

Measure of success and evidence for use as predictor SR

Methylene blue

VRET (OST), afterwards 90min. sleep vs. awakefulness

40 in 2 groups

Spider Phobia

90 minutes of sleep lead to less anxiety in a post-therapy BAT and less catastrophic spiderrelated cognitions; the more stage 2 sleep, the better

9,25

+

Pace-Scott, Verga, Bennett, & Spencer

2012

14x1min. video exposure in the morning vs. in the evening, with vs. without sleep before delaymeasurement

66 in 4 groups

Spider Phobia

extinction augmentation and extinction generalization in the sleep-condition

6,5

+

Smits, Rosenfield, Otto, Powers, Hofmann, Telch, …, & Tart

2013

2x 30min. VRET with vs. without D-Cycloserine post session

29 in 2 groups

Acrophobia

D-Cycloserine appears to enhance the benefits of exposure treatment when applied after a successful session, but it seems to have detrimental effects when administered after inadequate/unsuccessful exposure sessions.

9,5

+

Byrne, Rapee, Richardson, Malhi, Jones, & Hudson

2015

OST with vs. without DCycloserine before the session

35 children (6-14years) in 2 groups

Dog and Spider Phobia

Less avoidance and less increase in fear through DCycloserine for a new stimulus in a new context; possible enhancement of ET and especially generalization in children and youths

8,5

Tart, Handelsman, Deboer, Rosenfield, Pollack, Hofmann, …, & Smits

2013

Telch, Bruchey, Rosenfield, Cobb, Smits, Pahl, & Gonzalez-Lima

PH

PR

+

+

-p re

na

lP

2014

ro of

2014

+

2x 30min. VRET with vs. without D-Cycloserine post session

29 in 2 groups

Acrophobia

No augmented efficacy of ET through the application of postsession D-Cycloserine

10

O

6x 5min. exposure trials with vs. without Methylene blue post session

42 in 2 groups

Claustrophobia

Methylene blue enhances memory and hinders return of fear after a successful ET, but has detrimental effects after an unsuccessful ET

11

+

ur

D-Cycloserine

Kleim, Wilhelm, Temp, Margraf, Wiederhold, & Rasch

Jo

Sleep

B

+

O

Quality Score: In case of differences between the two judges, the mean of the two scores is reported; measure of success: SR = Self report, B = behavioral measure, PH = physiological measure, PR = professional rating; Evidence for use as factor: + = good evidence for positive influence; - = good evidence for negative influence; o = evidence that it is not applicable as a predictor; ?= no conclusive evidence; OST: One Session Therapy sensu ÖST; ET: Exposure Therapy; fMRI: functional magnet resonance imaging; fNRIS: functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy.

Table 3 Every researched factor and its current state in respect to its influence on the success of ET Factor Comment Factors that seem to have an influence on the success of ET Less trait anxiety leads to better ET results

Motivation

More motivation leads to better ET results

Self-efficacy

More self-efficacy leads to better ET results

Cortisol levels

Higher cortisol levels improve learning in ET

Disgust evocation

Evoking disgust additionally to anxiety improves ET results Relaxation hinders ET effectiveness

Relaxation

Addressing cognitive changes leads to better ET results

Adding cognitive parts Context variation

-p

Using different contexts leads to better ET results

ro of

Trait anxiety

Heart rate

re

HR plays a role, but it is unclear which measure best to use (variability, entropy, tonic vs. phasic HR…)

Sleeping after a successful ET leads to better results

Memory enhancing medication

Taking memory enhancing drugs (D-Cycloserine) leads to better long-term success

lP

Sleep

Potential factors that could have an influence on the success of ET (more research needed)

Eyeblink startle response

Less startle response after specific stimulus leads to better ET results Unclear if heightened feelings of disgust are independent of anxiety and if they play a role in ET success

Jo

Disgust

Better memory leads to better ET results

ur

Memory

Higher perceived irrationality (but not automaticity) of thoughts leads to better ET results

na

Automaticity/Irrationality of thoughts

Positive Valence Training seems to improve ET success

Positive Valence Training

Unclear relationship between showing safety behavior and ET success

Safety behavior

Female therapists lead to better success Massed ET leads to less ET success

Sex of therapist

Feedbacks leads to better ET success ET success does not generalize over different caffeine states

Massed ET feedback of heart rate

Medication hinders success of ET

Caffeine state Anxiety differing medication (given during ET) Return of phobic conditions

A return of phobic cognitions leads to less success of ET Higher visual cortex activation after ET leads to better long-term success

ro of

Visual cortex activation

Factors that have no influence on the success of ET

lP

Comorbidity Within-session habituation/between-session habituation

re

Factors that probably have no influence (more research needed)

-p

Fear reactivation coping style stimulus variation number of sessions (no improvement by more sessions) length of sessions (no improvement by longer ET sessions) computer assistance (no improvement over normal ET)

Presence Coping self-statements

na

Attentional focus guiding Motion simulation

ur

Conditioned odor

Jo

ET: Exposure Therapy; HR: Heart rate