Behavioural Processes, 23 (1991) 59-74 0 1991 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. 0376-6357/91/$03.50 BEPROC
59
00331
Food deprivation
enhances both autoshaping
and autoshaping
impairment
inhibition Sheldon Department
B. Sparber,
of Pharmacology,
George
by a latent
procedure L. Bollweg
and Rita B. Messing
Medical School, University Minnesota 55455, USA
of Minnesota,
Minneapolis,
(Accepted 28 August 1990)
Abstract The
influence
of food deprivation
on acquisition
of autoshaped
operant behavior
was measured. In one study separate groups of young, male rats that were deprived to 75%, 80%, 85%, 90%, and 95% of ad lib weight were subjected to an autoshaping procedure
in which
a 6 s delay was
interposed
between
lever
retraction
(which
occurred when rats made a lever touch, or automatically after 15 s) and food pellet delivery. In a second study, groups of rats were deprived to 80% or 90% of ad lib weight prior to testing in a latent inhibition variation of the same autoshaping procedure. This learning which,
was done to determine if greater food deprivation would enhance because of the latent inhibition manipulation, is manifest as less
lever-directed behavior. Greater food deprivation was associated both with fast acquisition of autoshaped lever responding and with more reliable failure to increase lever responding in the latent inhibition paradigm. Thus, increasing food deprivation was associated with enhanced acquisition regardless of whether the required performance was an increase or a failure to increase the same behavior, indicating effect on learning.
Key words:
Autoshaping;
Food deprivation;
a specific
Latent inhibition
Introduction Food intake by experimental subjects in a laboratory setting is usually restricted when studying behavior which is reinforced with food, and behavioral consequences
60 associated with changes in levels of deprivation are often thought of as the result of changes in motivation. For example, Hovancik et al. (1984) found that increasing deprivation positively influenced the speed of responding, but not the level of discrimination performance during acquisition of a straight-alley runway task. But much evidence exists which suggests that deprivation interpreted. Peterson and McHose (1980) used a runway
effects are not so easily to a goal to show that the
speed of running (interpreted as a measure of reinforcer incentive value) is positively related to deprivation state for the first exposure to the reinforcer, but does not change in the expected way if deprivation state is changed, replicating much earlier work of Butter and Campbell (1960). Other work has shown that behaviors which are maintained by reinforcers other than food can be dramatically
affected by the level of food deprivation,
questioning
the utility of a simple explanation relying upon motivation or incentive for the deprived stimulus, since there is clear generalization to other reinforcing stimuli. Thus, Carroll (1985) found that food deprivation nearly doubled the response rate of monkeys on a fixed interval (FI) schedule of reinforcement for oral delivery of the abused hallucinogen
phencyclidine;
Gaiardi et al. (1987) similarly
concluded
that the
reinforcing and discriminative stimulus properties of morphine are greater in food deprived than in food satiated rats, using a place conditioning task. In tests of unconditioned
locomotor
activity
(i.e.
behavioral
arousal),
Campbell
and
Fibiger
(1971) found that food deprivation markedly potentiated the stimulant effect of amphetamine, and vice versa, suggesting that food deprivation may increase the responsiveness of an organism to stimuli other than food, although the role of drug disposition between nontarget tissue (e.g., adipose) and target (e.g., brain), under various degrees of deprivation Coveney and Sparber, 1990).
has not been adequately studied
(Sparber et al., 1977;
Autoshaping (Brown and Jenkins, 1968) combines aspects of classical conditioning, where the event sequence in an experiment occurs no matter what the subject does, and operant conditioning, where emission of a specific behavior brings reward or punishment.
We have previously
observed that autoshaped
amenable to parametric manipulations
lever-directed
of the delay of reinforcement,
behavior is
a consequential
variable (Messing et al., 1986), and herein systematically manipulate the antecedent variable, degree of food deprivation. Further, we wished to determine if another treatment (administration of the vasopressin vasopressin; DGA VP) that enhances acquisition optimally,
because of age or a CNS lesion,
analog desglycinamide-8-arginine in rats that may not be learning
may do so by amplifying
or mimicking
the
positive influence of increased food deprivation upon learning and performance. We have reported that acquisition of forward autoshaped behavior, with a 6 second delay of reward following retraction of a lever can be enhanced in older rats by administration of DGA VP (Messing and Sparber, 1985). Furthermore, this peptide, which is devoid of significant pressor and endocrine properties (Kovacs and DeWied, 1983), reversed a deficit in autoshaped learning caused by treatment of young, mature rats with the neurotoxin trimethyltin (Sparber et al., 1988). In one study, the rate of learning (acquisition) was examined in separate groups of male rats deprived and maintained at 75%, 80%, 85%, 90%, and 95% of ad lib weight. In a second study, rats were deprived to 80% or 90% of their ad lib weight and injected with either DCA VP or saline and tested in a latent inhibition variation of the autoshaping
task (Lubow
and Moore,
1959).
In this
procedure,
subjects
repeatedly
61
exposed stimulus
to the to-be-conditioned stimulus show retardation of conditioning
not yet paired with an unconditioned upon subsequent introduction of the
unconditioned stimulus (reinforcer). This study was carried out to determine if greater food deprivation and/or peptide injection increased the likelihood that animals would fail to autoshape after reinforcement was instated. By using a latent inhibition manipulation, we hoped to further control for the possibility that enhanced learning by more food deprived rats was nothing more than a reflection of a generalized arousal effect, thereby increasing the probability that lever-directed behavior would be emitted
during
the conditioning
process.
Lest the reader think that a 75% deprivation level is unduly harsh or cruel, such regimens, or even more severe regimens, have been found to produce robust decreases
in
experiments
age related
changes
and increases
in
life
span
conducted over the past 50 years (cf. Goodrick
of
rats
in
numerous
et al., 1982).
Methods
Subjects Autoshaping. Thirty five male Long Evans rats, nine weeks of age (Blue Spruce, Altamont, NY) were individually housed and maintained in a temperature (22 + 1°C) and humidity (40-50%) controlled environment on a 12:12 hour light/dark schedule, with
lights on at 0700. Food (Purina
rat chow) and tap water were available ad lib for
one week. After this period of acclimation the rats weighed 302 f 10.6 g (mean k s.d.). They were then divided into 5 groups of 7 rats/group and food deprived over one week and maintained within +2% of 95%, 90%, 85%, 80% and 75% of their ad lib weights.
Before
behavioral
testing
began, a dish
containing
twenty
45
mg rein-
forcement pellets (#0021, Bioserv Inc., Frenchtown, NJ) was given to each animal in its home cage prior to its normal feeding. Each animal consumed all of the pellets within two
hours.
reinforcer
This
was
done
to assure
the
acceptability
and effectiveness
of
the
and to obviate neophobia.
Forty male Long Evans rats were obtained and Latent inhibition manipulation. treated as above. Weights after ad lib feeding (330 + 16.5 g), and maintenance at weights during behavioral testing (80 or 90% of ad lib weight, means for individual rats over
5 preexposure
sessions
and 10 autoshaping
sessions
+2%)
were
similar
to
Experiment 1. Subjects were maintained at the same deprivation level during the behavioral sessions in four treatment groups of 10 rats per group: 90% DGA VP, 90% saline, 80% DGA VP, and 80% saline. However, one rat in the 90% saline group had to be excluded from data analyses because of illness during reinforced autoshaping sessions. DGA VP (kindly supplied by Organon International, Oss, The Netherlands) was dissolved in 0.9% NaCl solution (saline). DGA VP (7.5 pg/kg body weight) or saline (1 ml/kg)
was injected S.C. 60 minutes
prior to the preexposure
sessions.
Apparatus Laurel, MD) Rats were tested in Skinner boxes (Model 143-22, BRS/LVE, isolation chambers with closed circuit television specially constructed
placed in cameras
62 (Sparber, 1980). Skinner boxes were 31 cm wide X 25 cm deep X 25 cm high, with grid floors consisting of 0.5 cm diameter stainless steel bars spaced 2 cm apart, center to center. Each box had standard house and cue lamps and a speaker that delivered a white masking noise. Boxes contained a metal strip 7.5 cm wide on two walls, the bottom edge of which was 15 cm above the grid floor. Contacts with the strip (strip touches)
indicated exploratory
rearing behavior, and were monitored
eter-type touch circuit standardized with a retractable lever (BRS/LVE
against a 2M D resistance. Model RRL-005,
with a drinkom-
Each box was equipped
Laurel, MD),
and contacts with the
lever, extended or retracted, were monitored with a touch circuit similarly calibrated. The boxes were controlled and data were collected and data were collected by TRS80 Color Computers
(Tandy Corp., Fort Worth,
each treatment group were distributed potential source of variation.
TX) and custom
equally
built interfaces,
Rats from
among the boxes to control
for that
Procedure Autoshaping. Nine autoshaping sessions consisting of 24 trials per session were conducted on Tuesdays, Wednesdays and Thursdays for three weeks. Sessions began with illumination of the house lamp and a white cue lamp above the lever; both remained
lit
throughout
each 24 trial
session.
A trial
consisted
of
a computer
generated random interval with an average of 45 s and a range of 22-68 s (the intertrial interval, or ITI), followed by a lever extension into the Skinner box. The lever retracted when the rat touched the extended lever (extended lever touch, ELT). If 15 s elapsed with no ELT, the lever also retracted. A food pellet was delivered 6 s after lever retraction; thus, 6 s constituted the reinforcement delay (RFD). Food pellet delivery ended the trial. While older rats can be autoshaped with a 6 s delay of reinforcement, their rate of acquisition with a longer delay is much slower (Messing et al., 1986). Moreover,
delays shorter
in acquisition
than 6 s may not be optimal
rates caused by low doses of a neurotoxin
for studying
subtle
differences
(Cohen et al., 1987). Since we
have used a 6 s delay of reinforcement for such studies, keeping deprivation levels constant, we chose to characterize the effects of various levels of deprivation, using a 6 s delay. Latent inhibition manipulation. Sessions (12 trials per session) were identical to those for autoshaping, but the pellet dispenser was disconnected for the first 5 sessions ml/kg)
and the animals were injected S.C. 60 min prior to testing.
with either DGA VP (7.5 pg/kg) or saline (1 These preexposure sessions, without rein-
forcement, were conducted Monday-Friday, and were followed by 10 autoshaping sessions (12 trials/session) with reinforcer delivery and no injections, which were conducted Monday-Friday over the following 2 weeks (no sessions were run over weekends). Deprivation levels were maintained all I5 behavioral sessions.
Data collection
at 80% or 90% of ad lib weight during
and statistics
Autoshaping. Data consisted of ELT, interim lever touches (paw and nose poking behavior toward the retracted lever during the ITI and RFD), latencies to respond to the extended
lever, and strip
touches
(measuring
unconditioned
exploratory
rearing
63
behavior).
Interim
lever touches
represent
adjunctive
and/or
superstitious
and rate increases in this variable provide a measure of learning
behavior,
not as constrained
by
ceiling effects as ELT data (Messing et al., 1986). Strip touches occur to a greater extent shortly after introducing the subject to the novel chamber and tend to diminish as habituation occurs and/or attention is focused on the lever and food delivery through. However, we have found strip touches to be a convenient measure of drug induced arousal or locomotor stimulatory effects (Sparber, 1980), which may contribute to, or be responsible 1987).
Failure
measures
for, altered acquisition
to respond
to the extended
of total latency. Additionally,
of autoshaped behavior (Huang et al.,
lever was treated
latencies
as a I5
for actual touches
s latency
for
of the extended
lever (ELT latency) were analyzed to determine if deprivation affected response speed after learning occurred. To control for the effect of “practice” in subjects that acquired the lever-food association in early, rather than in later sessions, speed of responding was thus analyzed on the day each rat achieved a learning criterion: the number of ELT exceeding the 95% confidence interval for basal ELT established for all rats on the first day of autoshaping, during 2 of 3 consecutive sessions. It is probable that some rats begin to form an association between the manipulandum and food pellet delivery during the first session. Therefore, using the first session statistic for baseline (unconditioned) behavior yields a conservative measure of learning. Square root transformations
were used for analyses of interim
lever touches and strip touches
to deal with heterogeneity of variance for these measures. Error terms from ANOVAs were used to compute Duncan’s t-statistics on overall means (mean of nine sessions) of the behavioral
measures
(Winer,
1971).
Evidence of learning
by individual
groups
was obtained by performing serial repeated measures ANOVAs across sessions (l-2, l-3, l-4, etc.) until a significant difference was found between the first and last session
analyzed (in the appropriate direction)
by Fisher’s
LSD test. ELT were analyzed
with a criterion for purposes of direct comparison with data from experiment 2 (see below). This criterion was defined as the number of ELT greater than the upper limit of a 95% confidence interval established on the first autoshaping session (computed as 18.54 of 24 possible ELT). For a 3 session (weekly) block, learning was therefore defined as > 56 ELT per 3 session block. Rats were divided into a low deprivation (90% and 95% of ad lib weight)
and a high deprivation
(75% and 80% of ad lib weight)
group and x’analysis was then carried out, comparing totals of learners and nonlearners in each 3 session block. Latent inhibition manipulation. Data were collected as in autoshaping. Due to the bimodal distribution of data in the reinforced sessions, x2-comparisons of ELT were performed, using a criterion value to divide rats into two categories; those achieving higher levels of performance than observed in preexposure sessions, and those failing to achieve such levels (i.e., showing latent inhibition). The criterion for failure to increase autoshaped lever responding was defined as the upper limit of a 95% confidence interval for ELT for all animals during the preexposure sessions, or 23 or fewer ELT (5 sessions
Xl2
possible
ELT/session
= 60 possible
ELT).
Results The results indicate autoshaping acquisition tion.
that, within the range of deprivation levels utilized, both and latent inhibition are enhanced by greater food depriva-
64
u 360 -
95%
-
90%
-
85%
-
80%
300 z z
240 -
z + 5
180 -
ti
120-
60 -
0 : 0
I
I
12
I
I
I
I
I
I
,
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
SESSION Fig. 1. Relationship between deprivation state and total latencies to touch the extended lever (24 trials X 15 s maximum/session). Each point represents the mean for a group of 7 rats.
Autoshaping. Figure 1 total latency to touch the extended lever. As is apparent, more food deprivation was associated with a shorter latency. No Group differences were seen in Session 1 (one factor ANOVA, F,,,o= 0.434, p = 0.783). Repeated measures ANOVA over the nine autoshaping sessions showed significant effects of Group (F4.30 = 3.229, p = 0.026), Session (F8,zW = 27.39, p < O.OOl), and a Group by Session interaction (F32,240= 1.831, p = 0.006). There were significant differences between 95% and 75% groups (t = 4.13, p < 0.05) and 90% and 75% groups (t = 3.88, p < 0.05) by Duncan’s test on the combined 9 session means. The stepwise nature of the group differences
over the last four sessions
evidence of the lever-pellet
is also apparent. The 95% group showed
little
associated even after nine sessions, and did not attain a (F,,, = 0.83, p = 0.580). The 75%, 80%, 85% and 90% groups
significant effect of Session showed the effect of Session
after 4 (Fj,,8 = 8.3, p = O.OOl), 4 (F3,18 = 4.20, p = 0.020), 5 = 3.85, p = 0.015), and 7 sessions (F,,,, = 4.161, p = 0.003), respectively. (F‘W As did latency analyses, similar analyses of ELT behavior (not shown) indicated that greater deprivation was generally associated with more extended lever touching. There
was
F4,3,,= 0.604,
no evidence p = 0.662).
of Group
differences
in
session
1 (one
factor
ANOVA,
Analysis
by repeated measures ANOVA over all 9 sessions showed significant effects of Group (F,,,, = 3.028, p = 0.033) and Session (F8,zm = 11.86, p c 0.001). As was the case with latency, significant differences by Duncan’s test on 9 session means emerged between 95% and 75% groups (t = 4.11, p c 0.05) and 90% and 75% groups (t = 3.79, p i 0.05). Because of the bimodal distribution of the data derived from the latent inhibition procedure (see below), ELT data from experiment 1 were also analyzed by x2 to facilitate comparison between experiments. Thus, ELT responding of individual animals in high and low deprivation groups was plotted in 3 session blocks (sessions l-3, 4-6, and 7-9)
in Figure 2, omitting
the intermediate
deprivation
group (85% ad lib weight)
65
Deprlvztion
low
low
high
Sess. 1-3 Fig. 2. Relationship shaping.
Sessions
deprivation: omitting
between were
“high”
combined
session)
95% confidence
defined
learning.
and
combining
groups (“low
represents interval
In sessions
p =0.105).
learned
into
(85%) group.
The dashed line at 56 ELT
subjects
state
Sess. 7-9 lever
blocks
of
extended
3 and groups
l-3, 4-6,
established
4-6
and 7-9,
(ELT)
touches were
x2 = 7.146, df =I, p = 0.008; p = 0.020).
to
autoreflect
(90% and 95% of ad lib weight), by a “+I’
in session
significantly
during
combined (high)
greater than the upper
high and low deprivation
In sessions
(sessions
or “low”
Each rat is represented ELT
high
Sess. 4-6
a value 3 times
for
low
and
(75% and 80% of ad lib weight)
the intermediate
2.625, df =I,
deprivation
hfgh
or a “0” (low).
limit
1 by all subjects;
did not differ
of a (single 2 56 ELT
significantly
(x2 =
more high than low deprivation sessions
7-9,
x2 = 5.39,
df =I,
the 75% and 80% groups (“high” deprivation) and the 90% and 95% deprivation). Analysis of block 1 indicated nonsignificant differences in
the number of subjects reaching the criterion, when comparing high (4 of 14) and low (0 of 14) deprivation groups. Significant differences did emerge in block 2, with the number of subjects reaching criterion in the high deprivation group (10 of 14) greater than the number in the low deprivation group (2 of 14). This difference continued into the third block; the number of high deprivation group (5-14). Analysis of ELT by both x2 and ANOVA thus reported the latency results, showing a significant positive influence of greater food deprivation on delayed reinforcement autoshaped learning.
66 g
35
B
1
Ill 30u iTi 0 25St!. Y
20Y
5 c
15”
E >
W A
loY
z = ii
z
5-
02 0123456789
SESSION Fig. 3. Relationship
between deprivation
interval plus reinforcement
state and square root of interim
delay) lever touching.
Each point represents
(the sum of intertrial
the mean for a group of
7 rats.
Interim There
lever
were
touching
no session
data
p = 0.138), but significant measure)
( Fa,240= 25.569,
p = 0.037 emerged
(square
1 responding effects
for Group
p -C 0.001)
over 9 sessions,
on the means for all 9 sessions
root
transformed)
differences (F4,,,
are
depicted
in
Figure
3.
ANOVA (F,,a, = 1.89, = 3.303, p = 0.023), Session (repeated by one
factor
by Session interaction (F32,240= 1.546, as well as significant differences by Duncan’s test
and a Group
between the 95% and 80% groups (t = 4.24, p < O.Ol),
the 95% and 75% groups (t = 3.49, p -C0.05), and the 90% and 80% groups (t = 3.64, p < 0.05). A significant effect of Session was reached by the 95% group on session 9 (F&W3= 2.45, p = 0.026). This effect occurred on session 8 in the 90% (F,,d2 = 4.66, p = 0.001) and 85% (F7,42 = 3.05, p =O.Oll) groups, and on session 4 in the 80% (Fa.18= 7.14, p = 0.002) and 75% (F3,,8 = 4.91, p = 0.002) groups. The 9 session mean of the 80% group was higher than (but not significantly different from) the 75% group for
interim level touches. To better characterize the learning enhancement associated with greater food deprivation, total ELT latency per session, counting only those trials in which a touch actually occurred, and deprivation were regressed and are displayed in Figures 4A and 4B. Figure 4A shows a significant
positive
correlation
between the number of sessions
to attain a learning criterion (19 ELT, rounded upward from 18.54 [the upper limit of the 95% confidence interval for all groups on session I], on 2 of 3 consecutive sessions), and the average session 9 ELT latency for individual animals (y = 0.439 X 0.178; r = 0.65, p = 0.001). Thus, rats which reached the criterion sooner when more “practiced” by session 9 and took less time to respond to the lever when it was extended. In contrast, Figure 4B shows no significant association between deprivation status and average ELT latency on the session this criterion was reached. Thus, when practice effects are eliminated, deprivation level was not related to speed of responding during the acquisition
phase.
67 3
' 1
y =
- 0.178 + 0.439x
R=0.65, P=.OOl
Sessions Fig. 4A. Correlation
between first session
ELT, 2 of 3 consecutive
sessions)
to
on which
Criterion
a learning criterion
and mean ELT latency (s) on session
animal that reached the criterion,
7
regardless
P y = 1
CV ._
1, 70
I
Fig. 46. Lack of correlation
q
I
1
I
Strip
between
on which a learning criterion
the
touching
data,
as the session
groups
did
not
I 95
deprivation
an
differ
p < 0.001) was (FWO = 13.01, p = 0.071) was not significant.
state and average ELT
latency (s) on the first sessions).
an animal that reached the criterion.
indication
progressed.
VVEght)
was reached (19 or more ELT, 2 of 3 consecutive
Each point represents
creased
an
status.
R = ,237, ~~300
Grbsup (% sFOree FeEling session
was reached (19 or more
9. Each point represents
of deprivation
7.3717 - .0434x
i;j6
Q
of
As with
(F4,so = 1.81, observed
exploratory the other
rearing
behavior,
measures,
session
p =0.153). over
9 sessions;
A
significant the
Group
generally 1 levels
effect effect
of
de-
among Session
(F4,s0 = 2.41,
68 The autoshaping data thus support the idea that within the deprivation range we tested, more deprivation is associated with more rapid acquisition of the lever-pellet association, manifest as more behaviour directed toward the extended and retracted lever. Furthermore, greater deprivation is not associated with a systematic, general increase in activity level in the operant chamber during acquisition sessions. This suggests
that enhanced learning
of the lever-pellet
animals is not secondary to greater probabilities
association
of touching
in the more deprived
the extended lever during
early sessions. Repeated measures ANOVA of ELT, interim lever Latent inhibition manipulation. touching, total latency, and strip touches for preexposure sessions l-5, during which
00 0
* -c +++ *
: 0
+
90
80
L
% Ad Ilb S.W.
90
90
80
between
deprivation
state and ELT
The dashed line at 23 ELT is the upper limit preexposure by a ‘I+”
sessions, (“hi”,
reinforcement) were
and defines
80%
no significant
p > 0.25); in sessions (x2 = 5.72, df =l,
with
differences 6-10,
p <0.02),
for the latent
of a 95% confidence
inhibition
interval
manipulation.
established
during
the
failure
ad lib weight)
is contrasted
Sess.6-lo
Sess. i-5
Pre-exposure Fig. 5. Relationship
a0
to increase lever responding. Each rat is represented Preexposure (no or a “0” (“lo”, 90% ad lib weight).
sessions during
1-5
and sessions
preexposure,
the 80% group responded demonstrating
6-10
or during significantly
greater likelihood sponding.
(reinforcer
session
l-5
delivered). (x2=0.27,
There df =I,
less than did the 90% group
of failure
to increase
lever
re-
69
time pellets were not delivered (data not shown), showed only an effect of Session (except for ELT, see below), as follows: ELT, F,,,,, = 2.959, p = 0.022 (ELT decreasing over sessions); interim lever touching: F,,,,, = 10.863, p -C0.001 (decreasing over sesover sessions); and strip sions); total latency: F,,,,, = 2.746, p = 0.031 (increasing touching: F,,,,, = 3.877, p = 0.005 (decreasing over sessions). After the preexposure sessions, rats were autoshaped for IO sessions with delivery of the reinforcer 6 s after each lever retraction, as in experiment 1. ELT responding during preexposure increase
and autoshaping
lever responding
below). The saline-DGAVP
was less
comparison
is depicted in Figure 5. The criterion than or equal to 23 responses/5 did
not
reach significance
for
for failure to sessions
either
(see
block
of
reinforced sessions (x2 = 0.03, df = 1, p > 0.75; x2 = 0.94, df = 1, p > 0.25, respectively). The 80%-90% comparison (collapsed across DCAVP or saline treatment) for the first 5 session
block of autoshaping
was not different
(x2 = 0.27, df = 1, p > 0.25); behavior
toward the lever appeared to be depressed in all groups as a result of the latent inhibition manipulation. In the second block, sessions 6-10, a difference emerged. The greater deprivation of the 80% group was thus associated with more failures to increase responding, with 9 of 20 subjects remaining below the criterion, against only 2 of 19 in the less deprived group. The possibility that exploratory strip touches were affected by peptide treatment or food deprivation was also investigated. Repeated measures ANOVA of the preexposure sessions (where reinforcement was not delivered) showed no effect of Treatment (F,,,, = 0.408, p = 0.563), Deprivation (F1,36 = 0.0003, p = 0.985), nor Deprivation by Treatment interaction (F,,,, = 0.037, p = 0.849). There was an effect of Session by Deprivation (F,,,, = 0.982, p = 0.419), nor Session by Deprivation by Treatment (F,,,,, = 0.928, p = 0.449). These results indicate a lack of difference among the groups in strip touching during the preexposure sessions. Repeated measures ANOVA of strip touches for the subsequent 10 autoshaping sessions showed no effect of peptide the 90% Treatment (F,,35 = 2.276, p = 0.140), but there was an effect of Deprivation: group touched the strip more (a ten session average of 5.5) than the 80% group (a ten session average of 4.7; both values, square root strip touches) (F,,35 = 7.081, p = 0.012). There was no overall Treatment by Deprivation interaction (F1,35 = 0.060, p = 0.809), but there was a Session
effect (F9,31s = 10.873,
p < 0.001). There
was no interaction
of
Session by Treatment (F9,3,5 = 1.055, p = 0.396), Session by Deprivation (F9,3,5 = 0.567, p = 0.824), nor Deprivation by Treatment by Session interaction (F9,3,5 = 0.829, p = 0.590).
Discussion We found significant enhancement of acquisition of autoshaped behavior with increasing deprivation, as well as greater likelihood that more deprived rats would fail to increase the same behavior after a latent inhibition manipulation. Thus, learning was enhanced by food deprivation
when food was used as a reinforcer
regardless
of
whether the same behavior was increased or decreased. There was no effect of food deprivation on session 1 measures for autoshaping, or on lever-directed behaviors during preexposure sessions for latent inhibition. This argues against the possibility that food deprivation generally increases reactivity or unconditioned behavior toward
70
the lever, which may have thereby enhanced autoshaped acquisition by virtue of a greater probability of the emitted behaviors being reinforced. When we analyzed the speed of responding conservative criterion
(latency to touch the extended lever) on the day when a for learning was achieved in the autoshaping experiment, we
found
between
no correlation
deprivation
lever
support the notion that learning, not response food deprivation. Moreover, since lever-directed
and response
latency.
Such
data
speed or reactivity, is enhanced by behaviors during the 5 preexposure
sessions of the latent inhibition manipulation were also not different when comparing the more and less food deprived groups, the greater likelihood that the subjects in the more deprived group would fail to increase responding cannot be accounted for by their experiencing a greater number of unreinforced lever touches. Further
evidence that food deprivation
performance
are data showing
effects on learning are not due to effects on
that rats which acquired the autoshaped lever response
under food deprivation (80% of ad lib. weight) continued to respond at high levels three weeks later, when they were reintroduced to the apparatus after being allowed to return to 100% of their former ad lib body weights (Kim and Sparber, unpublished observations). These results complement work showing that the establishment and maintenance of associations between events are enhanced by food deprivation even if food is not used as a reinforcing stimulus: reinforcing effects of drugs are increased as a result of greater food deprivation (reviewed by Carroll and Meisch, 1984). Thus, the enhancing consequences of food deprivation simply explained by generalized
up cognitive processes, broadly defined, cannot be arousal, specific incentive, or other motivational
constructs which have been used to interpret which the subject is more deprived. Exploratory
rearing
behavior
behavior
was not systematically
directed
toward
stimuli
affected by treatment
to
in the
autoshaping study, but was affected by deprivation level after the latent inhibition manipulation. Exploratory rearing behavior was significantly greater in the less deprived, compared to the more deprived group, which also showed more lever-directed behaviors during autoshaping after the latent inhibition manipulation. We interpret this as evidence for less latent inhibition in the less deprived group. It is most likely that strip touch behavior under these special conditions is a measure of early adventitiously reinforced behaviors, and correlates with other behaviors directed by the less deprived group toward its environment, upon which it is operating during the establishment of the association between retraction of the lever and food pellet delivery. Like the more deprived group, this group experienced 5 sessions during which lever extension and retraction were unrelated to any food pellets. When the food pellets were later introduced, the fact that the less deprived group was also less affected by the latent inhibition manipulation most likely was responsible for the emergence
of multiple
types of behaviors
until
they autoshaped
to the lever by a
process of successive approximations. Observation of several rats during acquisition of autoshaped behavior (through closed circuit television systems in the sound attenuating environment) also supports this interpretation. In contrast to the present data, prior work in this laboratory showed that DCAVP enhances acquisition of autoshaped behavior (Messing and Sparber, 1983; 1985); however, another laboratory did not replicate these results (Mundy and Iwamoto, 1987). These conflicting results are an indication of the general controversy surrounding the study of vasopressin and its analogs in learning and memory. Strupp and
71
Levitsky “ceiling
(1985) argue that vasopressin and its analogs do affect cognition, but that effects” may militate against observations of positive effects. Mundy and
lwamoto used 265-345 g male Sprague Dawley rats, which learned the autoshaping association very quickly. Our earlier studies (in contrast to the present one) utilized much older, heavier rats (up to 625 g), which learned more slowly, perhaps because of an aging process, or because maintenance of rats at similar percentages of ad lib body weights
might produce very different
levels of effective deprivation
when ad lib body
weights vary by a factor of two. Thus, it may be that the subjects used by Mundy and lwamoto were already learning and performing optimally, and therefore were not susceptible
to the peptide’s enhancing property.
In fact, we too have failed to observe
enhancement of autoshaped behavior acquisition when younger, with fast acquisition rates have been given DGAVP (unpublished the present
study,
when
DGAVP
was given during
preexposure
lighter, healthy rats observations), or in sessions
for latent
inhibition, which also utilized younger, lighter, healthy (i.e., nonlesioned) rats, presumably also capable of learning optimally. In contrast, we did find that DGAVP reversed
the learning
deficit
of rats compromised
by administration
of the
limbic
forebrain neurotoxin trimethyltin (TMT) (Sparber et al., 1988). The consequences of varying degrees of deprivation of food and/or water under laboratory conditions upon learning or performance of learned, stabilized behavior probably involve multiple systems which can also be activated by other experimental manipulations (e.g., drug administration) which induces physiological changes associated with inefficient
life sustaining natural needs. From an evolutionary viewpoint, it would be to activate, via separate and distinct chemical signals, diverse organ sys-
tems, each of which
produces separate and distinct
receptor mediated responses
for
altering function in an uncoordinated manner. It is more parsimonius to posit the evolution of hierarchical systems, with each level activated by a neurohumoral factor functioning for maintenance of homeostasis in diverse organ systems. By having relatively few biochemical signals (e.g., steroids, peptides, biogenic amines), each with diverse physical chemical properties and time courses of action, and diverse receptor types and subtypes for these signals, great economy can be achieved. This systems analysis perspective appears to be useful for describing many biological phenomena, including what may be the biological bases for our observations. For example, the ability to conserve water, focus attention, and otherwise cognitive function are of obvious utility under conditions of life threatening or food shortage,
which
necessitate
exploration
of new territory
sharpen drought
and coping
unknown predators. It is possible that there is a critical level of deprivation, laboratory conditions, for activation of physiological and behavioral processes
with under which
are comparable to those activated by such naturally occurring emergencies. The finding by Berry and Swain (1989) that nictitating membrane conditioning improved in rabbits that were water deprived for 22 hrs, compared to controls with ad lib access to water, supports this idea. Dorsa and Bottemiller (1983) found that vasopressin-like immunoreactivity (VP-LI) is differentially affected by water deprivation: simultaneously reduced in the neurointermediate pituitary and increased in plasma. They also found reduced VP-LI in septum and concurrent increases in amygdala, demonstrating responsiveness to water deprivation in brain regions involved with functions other than fluid regulation. Thus, it is tempting to speculate that some of the effects of food deprivation upon learning and performance may be produced via similar endocrinological and humoral changes. Recent work supports the notion that physiological
72
modulation of hormones and neurotransmitters may be involved in the influence of food deprivation on learning. For example, food deprivation or fasting engenders increased plasma corticosterone in rats (Suemaru et al., 1986) and increased plasma cortisol in humans (Fichter and Pirke, 1984). The hippocampus, a structure very much involved
in learning
and memory,
shows
the highest
concentration
of corticosterone
and its receptors in rat brain (McEwen, 1982), and there is evidence that autoshaping and operant behavioral adaptation in general are dependent upon normal functioning of this brain region and its adrenal steroid receptors. We have found that deficits in acquisition
of autoshaped
lever responding
and in operant response
progressive fixed ratio challenge (in which the ratio requirement in rats given TMT are correlated with decreases in corticosterone campus (Gerbec et al., 1988; Messing et al., 1988). Further, depriving rats to 75% of ad lib weight attenuated deficits
levels
during
a
is double each day) receptors in hippo-
like DGAVP administration, in autoshaping seen in rats
treated with TMT (Bollweg and Sparber, unpublished observations). Other work indicates interactions among endogenous glucocorticoids,
vasopressin,
and catecholamines that may be related to the food deprivation associated learning enhancement that we observed. For example, Stone et al. (1987) reported interactions between
catecholaminergic
receptor
stimulation
and the
presence
or absence of
corticosterone. When fi-adrenergic receptors in rat cortex slices were stimulated, there was an increase in second messenger cyclic AMP. This was further augmented by concurrent stimulation of a-adrenergic receptors. The augmentation was suppressed by chronic ACTH or corticosterone treatment or enhanced by adrenalectomy. Moreover, in rat hippocampal slices, AVP potentiated norepinephrine (NE) induced padrenergic-mediated cyclic AMP accumulation (Brinton and McEwen, 1989). Food deprivation is associated with a decrease in fi-adrenergic receptors (Stone, 1983), while TMT induces increased P-receptor density in forebrain (Messing and Sparber, 1986). Since food deprivation can affect corticosterone levels (e.g., Suemaru et al., 1986) and hypothalamic and/or other brain region vasopressin concentrations (Jhanwar-Uniyal et al., 1988), it may influence transduction processes of monoaminergic the positive
influence
the modulation of the signal reception and receptor types and subtypes. Thus, some of
of food deprivation
observed
in the present
experiments
may
have been mediated by influence upon glucocorticoids, vasopressin, catecholamines and their respective receptors. Experiments that directly determine relationships between food deprivation,
these endogenous
substances,
and learning
are necessary
to test this hypothesis.
Acknowledgements This
research was partially
supported
by USPHS
grants DA 01880
and HD 20111.
References Berry,
SD.
facilitates Brinton,
R.E.
and Swain, nictitating
R.A.
(1989).
membrane
and McEwen,
Journal of Neuroscience,
B.S.
Water
deprivation
conditioning. Vasopressin
9 (3), 752-759.
optimizes
Behavioral
hippocampal
Neuroscience,
neuromodulation
in the
activity
and
103 (I), 71-76. hippocampus.
(1989).
73 Brown,
P.L. and Jenkins,
Experimental Butter,
H.M.
Analysis
C.M. and Campbell,
drive level. (1960). Campbell,
B.A.
starvation. Carroll,
(1985). Carroll,
B.A. Running
and Fibiger,
H.C.
Performance
speed as a function
(1971).
maintained
and fixed-interval
(1968).
of successive
and Physiological
Potentiation
M.E. and Meisch,
Vol. 4 (pp. 47-88).
by orally
schedules
Journal of Pharmacology
Journal
of
reversals
Psychology,
53 (I),
of the
in hunger
52-54.
amphetamine-induced
delivered
and Experimental
R.A. (1984).
phencyclidine
in food-satiated
Orlando,
under
and food-deprived
Therapeutics,
arousal
by
Increased drug-reinforced
second-order,
rhesus
monkeys.
232 (2), 351-359. behavior
P.B. Dews and J.E. Barrett (Eds), Advances
C.A., Messing,
reinforcement
key-peck.
233, 424-4125.
tion. In T. Thompson, Cohen,
of the pigeon’s
11, l-8.
Journal of Comparative
Nature,
M.E.
tandem
Auto-shaping
of Behavior,
due to food depriva-
in Behavioral
Pharmacology,
FL: Academic Press.
R.B. and Sparber,
autoshaped
S.B. (1987).
Selective
learning
impairment
behavior caused by low doses of trimethyltin.
of delayed
Psychopharmacol-
ogy, 93, 301-307. Coveney,
J.R. and Sparber,
Interaction
S.B.
(1990).
of food deprivation,
Delayed
stress
effects
and dose.
of amphetamine
Pharmacology
or
phencyclidine:
Biochemistry
and Behavior,
36, 443-449. Dorsa,
D.M.
and Bottemiller,
L.A. (1983).
logic and chromatographic Peptides, Fichter,
and their
peptides
response
in the rat brain:
to water
deprivation.
immunoRegulatory
6, 393-403.
M.M.
and Pirke,
subjects.
In K.M.
124-135).
Berlin:
the stimulus Behavior,
K.M.
Pirke
(1984).
Hypothalamic
and D. Ploog
(Eds.),
The
pituitary
function
Psychobiology
in starving
of Anorexia
healthy
Nervosa
(pp.
Springer-Verlag.
Gaiardi, M., Bartoletti,
Cerbec,
Vasopressin-like
behavior
M., Bacci, A., Cubellini,
properties
of morphine
C. and Babbini,
in food deprived
M. (1987).
Increased sensitivity
rats. Pharmacology
Biochemistry
to and
26, 719-723.
E.N.,
Messing,
R.B.
and Sparber,
adaptation and hippocampal
S.B.
(1988).
corticosterone
Parallel
binding
changes
in operant
in rats treated with
behavioral
trimethyltin.
Brain
Research, 460, 346-351. Goodrick,
C.L.,
Ingram,
intermittent Hovancik, of
D.K.,
J.R., Gruning,
positive
97 (I),
in
refeeding.
Life Sciences,
Chapleur,
content
in
Society
fragment, Lubow,
M.L.,
J.R. and Cider,
N.L.
(1982).
Effects
of
28, 233-241.
Effect of deprivation
straight-alley
level on the magnitude
performance.
American
Journal
of
D. (1983).
A.J. and Leibowitz,
nuclei:
for Neuroscience
Learning enhancement
relation
Abstracts, Hormonally
to
Neuroendocrinology, stimulus.
S.F.
(1988).
deprivation
Vasopressin
and
and subsequent
active arginine-vasopressin and a behaviorally
suppresses
endo-
active vasopressin
37, 258-261.
A.U. (1959).
to the conditioned
food
59.11.
fever in rats: lack of effect of oxytocin
R.E. and Moore,
and behavior arousal
41, 1083-1088.
Burlet,
hypothalamic
Kovacs, G.L. and DeWied, toxin-induced
A. (1984).
discriminative
R.B. and Sparber, S.B. (1987).
induced by yohimbine. M.,
Freeman,
37-46.
Huang, M.S., Messing, Jhanwar-Uniyal,
M.A.,
and life span in rats. Gerontology,
R. and Intorre,
induction
Psychology,
oxytocin
Reynolds,
feeding upon growth
Latent inhibition:
Journal
the effect of non-reinforced
of Comparative
and
Physiological
preexposure
Psychology,
52,
415-419. McEwen,
B.S. (1982).
Glucocorticoids
Ganten and D. Pfaff (Eds.), (pp. l-22). Messing,
New York:
R.B., Bollweg,
tin poisoning 491-502.
Current
and hippocampus: Topics
receptors
in search of a function.
in Neuroendocrinology.
Adrenal
In D.
actions on brain
Springer-Verlag.
G., Chen, Q. and Sparber, S.B. (1988).
on learning
and hippocampal
corticosterone
Dose specific effects of trimethylbinding.
Neurotoxicology,
9 (3),
74 Messing,
R.B.,
Kleven,
MS.
and Sparber, S.B. (1986).
task generates adjunctive Messing,
R.B.
extinction Messing,
and superstitious
and Sparber,
S.B.
of autoshaped
behavior.
R.B. and Sparber,
desglycinamide
(1983).
reinforcement
Behavioral
Des-Cly-vasopressin
improves
Greater task difficulty
vasopressin
on appetitively
amplifies
motivated
in an autoshaping
Processes,
13, 327-338.
acquisition
European Journal of Pharmacology,
S.B. (1985).
arginine
Delaying
behaviors.
and slows
89, 43-51.
the facilitatory
learning.
effect of
Behavioral
Neuro-
science, 99,1114-1119. Messing,
R.B. and Sparber, S.B. (1986).
by trimethyltin. Mundy,
W.R.
Toxicology
and Iwamoto,
scopolamine
Biochemistry
D. and McHose,
as a function
Frankos
methods
P-adrenergic
ligand binding
induced
Studies
on desglycinamide
autoshaping
and Behavior,
model
arginine
vasopressin
of learning/memory
and
in rats.
27, 307-315. deprivation
effect in instrumental
level at the time of exposure
to the reinforcer.
conditioning
Animal
Learning
8 (4), 642-646.
S.B. (1980).
V.H.
(1987).
J. (1980). The residual
of deprivation
and Behavior, Sparber,
E.T.
Increased forebrain 32, 107-112.
in a modified/lever-touch
Pharmacology Peterson,
Letters,
Use of learned behavior
(Eds.),
Effects of foods
for predicting
toxicity.
in testing
for neurotoxicity.
and drugs on the development
Proceedings
of the Fifth
F.D.A.
In M.R.
Gryder
of the nervous Science
and
system:
Symposium
(pp.
49-61). Sparber, S.B., in adipose thology
Nagasawa, S. and Burklund, after daily
K.E.A.
administration
and Pharmacology,
(1977).
to rats. R.B. (1988).
deficit by desglycinamide-8-arginine
vasopressin.
E.A. (1983).
Reduction
tent food deprivation. Stone,
E.A., McEwen,
nents
B.S.,
Reversal
in Chemical
of a trimethyltin-induced
Life Sciences,
Pa-
Herrera,
Letters,
AS.
response
after chronic
intermit-
40, 33-37.
and Carr, K.D.
cyclic AMP
learning
42, 171-177.
in cortical beta adrenergic receptor density
Neuroscience
of noradrenergic
pool of d-amphetamine
Communications
18 (3), 423-431.
Sparber, S.B., Cohen, C.A. and Messing, Stone,
A mobilizable
Research
(1987).
in cortical
Regulation slices.
of a- and P-compo-
European
Journal
of Phar-
macology, 141, 347-356. Strupp,
B.J. and Levitsky,
D.A.
against. Neuroscience Suemaru,
S., Hashimoto,
(1985).
A mnemonic
and Biobehavioral K., Hattori,
role for vasopressin:
Reviews,
T., Inoue, H., Kageyama, J. and Ota, Z. (1986).
duced changes in rat brain corticotropin-releasing response. Winer,
Life Sciences,
B.). (1971).
Statistical
the evidence
factor (CRF)
Starvation-in-
and pituitary-adrenocortical
39, 1161-1166. principles
in experimental
for and
9, 399-411.
design.
New York:
McGraw-Hill.