/OURNAL
Of FLUENCY
DISORDERS
8 (1983)
183-205
Foreperiod and Stuttering Severity Effects on Acoustic Laryngeal Reaction Time Ben C. Watson University
of Connecticut,
Storrs
Peter j. Alfonso University
of Connecticut,
Storfs,
and Haskins Laboratories,
New Haven, Connecticut
An earlier paper presented a model of the laryngeal reaction time (LRT) paradigm that included several factors that appeared to affect LRT values. The present study assesses the effects of two of these factors: foreperiod former was assessed by the use of 13 foreperiod by classifying
experimental
factors significantly
subjects
affected LRT
and stuttering
durations.
as either
mild
LRT
values approached normal
stutterers’
LRT
foreperiods. vibration
values Results
initiation
are discussed
underlying
in
deficits underlying
stutterers’
of differential
values
posturing
severe at all and/or
delayed LRT values. We caution
alone are insufficient
to specify fully the nature of the
deficits.
A number
of
Starkweather
experiments et al.,
(most
1976;
notably
Cross
and
Adams
Luper,
that stutterers
as a group
are significantly
initiating
phonation
in response
to reaction
reaction time paradigm
that allowed
response, we unexpectedly
experiments significant
(Watson
Hayden,
Cross
slower
1976;
et al.,
1979)
than normals
signals.
Using
in
a simple
subjects 1 to 3 set to prepare for a failed to replicate the results of these
and Alfonso,
group difference
and
1979;
reported
known
mild stutterers’
increased, whereas
greater than normal
terms
Both
these factors demon-
Specifically,
values as foreperiod
remained significantly
that acoustic measurements
or severe stutterers.
values. More importantly,
strated a composite effect on group LRT differences.
severity. The
The latter was assessed
1982).
in laryngeal
That is, we failed reaction
time
to find a
(LRT) between
stutterers and nonstutterers, a difference we will refer to as the LRT effect. However, auditory vowel
we did find significant and visual
reaction
and phase-initial
vowel
within-group
signal conditions response
LRT differences and
conditions.
between The
between isolated
latter results
Address correspondence to Peter J. Alfonso, Department of Communication Sciences, Box U-85, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06228. “Elsevier Science Publishing Co., Inc., 1983 52 Vanderbilt Ave., New York, NY 10017
0094-730X/83/$03.00
Ben C. Watson and Peter ). Alfonso
184
suggested to us that our
LRT
measurements
were indeed sufficiently
sensitive to detect an LRT effect if one existed. Other recent investigations have also failed to demonstrate a significant and adult subjects (Cullinan ner, 1981; Venkatagiri, by our original
and Springer,
1981, 1982).
experiment,
We are interested significant
as other recent experiments
those factors that form the basis for
between stutterers
end, we have conducted experiments paradigm developed
that
LRT effect.
in isolating
LRT differences
effect in both children Murphy and Baumgart-
The study reported here is motivated
as well
failed to demonstrate a significant
LRT
1980;
in our original
and their controls.
To this
based on the model of the LRT experiment.
The
model
includes
factors related to the perception of the reaction signal, production of the response, subjects
and factors specifically that influence
model “reaction sponse
type,”
significant function
LRT
signal
LRT
values.
For example,
modality,”
a production differences
of reaction signal
condition
related to characteristics
we included
a perceptual component,
component,
based on our
for both nonstutterers modality
(isolated vs phrase-initial
of stuttering
(visual
in the
and “re-
findings
and stutterers
vs auditory)
of as a
and response
vowel). The first purpose of the study
reported here is to investigate further the effects of two other factors on stutterers’
LRT
values as well
as on the LRT
included in the model as foreperiod
effect. These
and stuttering
severity.
that our failure to find a significant
LRT effect in our original
was related to our use of relatively
long foreperiods
moderate severity rating of our experimental
factors are We argued experiment
and to the mild-to-
group.
The foreperiod factor is included in the “perceptual component” of the model,
although
production
interval. In our experiments,
events
may also
occur during
this
foreperiod is defined as the interval between
the presentation of the warning cue and presentation of the phonate cue. Sufficiently known
long foreperiods provide the subject with time to prepare for a
response (Niemi
and Naatanen, 1981).
Preparatory activity that
may occur during the foreperiod includes perception of the warning cue, formulation
and transmission
of appropriate motor commands to posture
the speech mechanism for the required response, and movements of the various
components
prephonatory
of the speech mechanism
posture.
occurs is a function
The
to achieve the required
extent of preparatory
of foreperiod
duration. Thus,
activity
that actually
short foreperiods
may
ForeperiodlSeverity
restrict
Effects on Acoustic LRT
preparatory
perhaps,
activity
formulation
foreperiods
185
to perception
and transmission
may permit
mands and posturing
formulation
of the warning of
motor
commands;
and transmission
of the speech mechanism
cue and, long
of motor com-
prior to presentation of
the phonate cue. The
notion of a foreperiod
supported by Izdebski’s
effect on nonstutterers’
LRT
values is
(1980) observation of a U-shaped function when
LRT values are plotted across a range of increasing foreperiods. That is, he found that LRT values decrease to a minimum
as foreperiod increases to
about 1500 msec and then increase as foreperiod increases beyond 1500 msec. These results suggest that LRT values occurring at foreperiods
less
than 1500 msec may reflect the subject’s inability
to complete preparat-
ory activity.
msec may reflect the
increasing
subject’s inattention
LRT
values beyond 1500
to the task or failure to maintain the prephonatory
posture. We have argued previously particularly
dependent
hypothesized
on
that stutterers’
foreperiod
that when certain stutterers
posture the speech mechanism,
LRT
duration.
values may be
Specifically,
are given sufficient
We concluded that the long foreperiods
original
(1 to 3 set) provided stutterers
used in our
with ample time to
achieve the appropriate posture prior to the initiation contributed to our finding of a nonsignificant
of phonation and
LRT effect.
The studies referred to earlier that reported a significant (and used isolated vowels response
conditions
as the response, a task similar
in our original
warning cues in their experimental Cross and Luper, 1979;
experiment)
posture
experiments warning
Cross et al., 1979).
before the presentation
that report significant
cue may reflect stutterers’
did not incorporate
Consequently,
LRT
LRT
the response.
it cannot be
achieved the appropriate
of the phonate cue. Thus, effects but do not include
difficulty
with posturing
mechanism prior to phonation onset as well as difficulties initiating
LRT effect
to one of the
designs (Adams and Hayden, 1976;
determined if the stutterers in these experiments response
time to
they will demonstarte LRT values similar
to those of normals. experiment
we
a
the speech
associated with
It seems possible that certain stutterers’
delayed
values may be related to posturing, that is, prephonatory events (as
suggested
by Freeman
delayed LRT
values
and Ushijima,
1978),
may be more directly
while
other
stutterers’
related to initiation
of the
response or perhaps a combination of posturing and initiation activities.
If
186
Ben C. Watson
and Peter 1. Alfonso
this is the case, one may suspect that certain stutterers’ approach normal values as foreperiod ers’ LRT
values could remain
throughout in this
LRT
increases. However,
significantly
the entire range of foreperiods.
values will other stutter-
greater than normal values The first hypothesis
study states that there is a foreperiod
under test
effect on stutterers’
LRT
values. To test this notion, we extended the range of the foreperiods from 100 to 3000
msec. Specifically,
those stutterers
with
deficits
only
in
posturing the speech mechanism will demonstrate LRT values approaching normal values as foreperiod deficits in initiating
increases, whereas those stutterers
with
the response, or in both posturing and initiation,
will
demonstrate LRT values significantly
greater than normal values through-
out the range of short to long foreperiods. The second factor that may affect stutterers’ severity,
included
in the model
results of several studies (Hayden, 1975; Alfonso,
LRT values is stuttering
under “subject
characteristics.”
Lewis et al., 1979;
1982) suggest that mild stutterers
may exhibit
The
Watson and
LRT values more
similar to normals than would severe stutterers.
Additional support for this
notion
obtained
is found
experiment
in a comparison
rating of our experimental et al. (19811,
for
differences
yet the
results
groups. Finally,
are clearly
in the stuttering
hypothesis stutterers’
significantly
and nonstutterers
under test
procedures were very
different.
of similar We
duration,
suggest
that
ratings of the experimental
severity effect on timing
Specifically,
is found
she observed that severe
longer vocal and manual “execution”
time values than did nonstutterers mild stutterers
severity
support for a stuttering
displayed
Reich
of these studies may, in part, be attribut-
in data reported by Borden (1982). stutterers
However,
as moderate-to-severe,
Both included foreperiods
between the results
able to differences
classified
LRT effect. The experimental
in the two studies.
example,
subjects
in our original
The average severity
group was mild-to-moderate.
using stuttering
obtained a significant similar
of results
and in a study by Reich et al. (1981).
while none of the differences between reached significance.
is that there
is a stuttering
Thus,
the second
severity
effect on
LRT values. That is, we expect that a group of severe stutterers
will demonstrate greater LRT values than will a group of mild stutterers. The two hypotheses just described assess the independent effects of foreperiod
and stuttering
pared to nonstutterers.
severity
Of interest,
on stutterers’
LRT
values when com-
however, is the relationship
between
ForeperiodlSeverity
foreperiod
Effects on Acoustic
and stuttering
LRT
severity.
187
Consequently,
the second purpose of
this study was to assess the combined effect of foreperiod and stuttering severity
on stutterers’
certain
stutterers’
LRT
LRT
values. values
For example,
would
foreperiod increases, in that these stutterers’ primarily tively,
related to difficulty
we hypothesized
significantly
different
foreperiods,
implying
related to difficulty
initiating
and initiation
stutterers,
classified
“posture” mild
whereas
normals’
by severity,
primary
as
Alterna-
values would
throughout
the entire
delayed LRT
remain
range of
values may be of
could be characterized according to the hypothesis.
That is, is it the case that
is posturing
primary
posturing and response initiation? Specifically,
that
We wanted to ascertain if groups of
difficulty
severe stutterers’
LRT
the response or, perhaps, a combination
difficulties.
versus the “initiation”
stutterers’
values
posturing the speech mechanism.
that these stutterers’
posturing
normal
delayed LRT values may be
that other stutterers’ from
we hypothesized
approach
difficulty
the speech mechanism is some combination
of
The third hypothesis tested this notion.
we expected that mild stutterers’
normal values, whereas severe stutterers’
LRT
values will approach
LRT values will
remain signifi-
cantly greater than normal values, as foreperiod increases. In summary,
the first
purpose of this study was to determine
the
effects of two factors included in the model of the LRT paradigm (Watson and Alfonso, second
1982) on the LRT effect and on stutterers’
purpose
was
deficits-posturing
to test
the
notion
versus initiation-underlie
LRT values. The
that qualitatively
different
mild and severe stutterers’
delayed LRT values.
Subjects Subjects participating in this study included 10 adult stutterers and 5 adult nonstutterers. stutterers’
LRT
In order to test the effect of stuttering
values,
it was necessary
subjects on this dimension. separate
analyses
pathologist during
of
subjectively
Stutterers
severity.
to classify
the experimental
First,
a certified
speech-language speech and speech
Passage of the stuttering
second certified speech-language pathologist objectively speech samples using the Stuttering the Stuttering
Interview
on
were classified on the basis of three
rated the conversational
reading of the Rainbow
severity
A
rated the same
Severity Index (SSI) (Riley,
(SI) (Ryan, 1974).
subjects.
1972) and
Ben C. Watson and Peter 1. Alfonso
188
The
results
of the stuttering
indicate that the experimental groups: five severe stutterers
severity
analysis,
shown
subjects could be classified and five mild stutterers.
Weiss,
1965;
Izdebski,
1980),
1,
Since reaction time
values may be affected by subject sex and age (Birren 1955;
in Figure
as two distinct and Botwinick,
we matched the control
against the average age and sex ratio of the two stuttering
group
groups.
Test Stimuli Figure 2 illustrates effect of foreperiod
one sequence of the stimuli
on LRT
variable interstimulus
interval
require
breathe normally
that subjects
Consequently,
used to assess the
values. Each sequence was separated by a (ISI) of 8 to 12 sec. ISIS of this duration between response
sequences.
subjects are not able to remain in a phonatory position
-
EXPERIMENTAL
SEX -
AQE
QROUP
SI
M
48;4
severe
severe
severe
severe
2
F
36;3
severe
severe
severe
severe
3
M
31;6
severe
severe
severe
severe
PUBJECT
1
4
881
SUBJECTIVE
F
30;o
severe
severe
severe
M
w 33;8
severe
severe
moderate
6
M
44:5
mild
mild
mild
7
M
41;4
mild
mild
mild
mild/ moderate mild
8
M
26;9
mild
ioderate
mild
mild
9
M
22;8
mild
mild
mild
mild
mild
mild
mild
mild
5 Mean A08
IA%" Apc
-
M -
z&s
11
M
48;7
normal
12
F
35;8
normal
13
M
26;lO
normal
M
26;0
normal
M -
%ifL
14 &
Figure 1:
severe I
normal
Results of the stuttering
A
severity
analysis.
severe
Foreperiodbeverity
Effects on Acoustic LRT
189
Phonation Interval
Reaction Signal onoe, onael
Variable I.S.I.
b .
Termination Signal
t
I
,
l
\
1
Variable Foreperiod S-12 Seconds
_
100-3000 msec.
_
2 Seconds
1 Second
-
.
Figure 2: One sequence of stimuli used to assess the effect of foreperiod on LRT. The reaction signal varied from 100 to 3000 msec. Reaction signal onset served as the warning cue, offset as the phonate cue.
between responses.
The reaction signal consisted of the synthetic
vowel
/a/. Onset of the reaction signal served as the warning cue, and the offset served as the phonate cue. Subjects
were instructed
to “get ready” to
phonate when and only when they heard the warning cue. Duration
of
the reaction signal varied from 100 to 500 msec in IOO-msec increments, 700 to 1500 msec in 200-ms 500-mesc
increments,
increments,
and from 2000 to 3000 msec in
a total of 13 foreperiods.
A “terminate
phonation”
signal was presented 2 set after the phonate cue. The terminate consisted
of the synthetic
vowel
replicated five times, randomized, Haskins
Laboratories
/i/. Each of the 13 sequences
signal was
and output onto audiotape using the
Plus Code Modulation
(PCM) system.
Procedures Stimulus
sequences were presented simultaneously
seated in a soundproof recorder.
Subjects’
to the subject,
booth, and to track one of a two-track
responses
tape
were recorded on track two of the tape
recorder. Subjects were instructed to phonate the vowel /a/ immediately at the offset
of the reaction
signal
and to continue
phonation
until
presentation of the terminate signal. All subjects were allowed 21 training sequences,
including
long and short foreperiods.
required fewer than the maximum the relatively
Although
most subjects
number of training sequences to learn
simple task, all subjects were exposed to training sequences
Ben C. Watson and Peter I. Alfonso
190
containing
long and short foreperiods.
sented in two 7-min
Response
sequences were pre-
tests separated by an optional
3- to 5-min
rest
interval. Fluency Criteria We followed
the same procedures used in our original
to ensure that only fluent responses were analyzed. instructed
to identify
any production
Second, the experimenter dysfluent. Finally,
experiment
subjects were
that they thought was dysfluent.
noted any production
that he thought was
No responses were omitted on the basis of the first two criteria. productions
were excluded from the data set if the waveform
showed certain irregulatirites ing,
First,
such
as isolated
phonation.
As
that may be related to nonaudible stutter-
pitch
a result
pulses
before the onset
of the third
excluded from the mild stutterers’
criterion,
three
of continuous responses
were
data set, one response was excluded
from the severe stutterers’ data set, and no responses were excluded from the nonstutterers’ stutterers,
data set. Thus,
322 LRT values were measured for mild
324 values were measured for severe stutterers,
and 325 values
were measured for nonstutterers. Measurements Data were analyzed with the aid of a computer waveform-editing system
at Haskins
Laboratories.
Temporal
resolution
analyzer is accurate to one-tenth of a millisecond
of the waveform
(Nye et al., 1975). LRT
values were defined as the interval between the offset of the phonate cue and the onset of the first regular pitch pulse of the voiced vowel /a/. Statistical Analyses All data were subjected to several multiple
correlation
(MCR) analyses (Cohen & Cohen, 1975) for the following the procedure
permits
analysis
of interaction
regression
reasons. First,
effects between interval
(foreperiod) and nominal (stuttering severity) level independent variables, a capability
not provided
procedures.
Second,
MCR
specific group comparisons.
by traditional analysis Finally,
multiple
permits
analysis
experimenter
of variance selection
of
MCR analysis allows for the evalua-
ForeperiodlSeverity
Effects on Acoustic LRT
tion of nonlinear
relationships,
between foreperiod
such as the hypothesized
and LRT.
ment was a subjects within
191
relationship
The statistical
design used in this experi-
groups (normal,
mild, severe) by condition
(foreperiod) repeated measures MCR. This
design requires separate MCR
analyses to determine (1) the significance of the between-subject ing severity)
main effect and (2) the within-subject
effect and interaction MCR
analysis
(stuttering
severity
was conducted
coded to permit separate comparisons analysis
and between mild
foreperiod)
to determine
stuttering severity factor. For this analysis, stutterers
x
main
effect. The first
the significance
of the
the subject group variable was between nonstutterers
and severe stutterers.
was conducted to determine
(stutter-
(foreperiod)
The
the signifiance
and mild
second MCR
of the foreperiod
factor and the interaction betweeen stuttering severity and foreperiod. For this analysis, the subejct group variable was, once again, coded to permit comparisons mild
between normals
and severe stutterers.
determine
the
foreperiod
magnitude
and LRT
of
MCR analysis
the
nonlinear
effect. Finally,
values at each foreperiod
parametric Randomization the criteria data (Siegel,
as well as between was conducted to
relationship
between
for each group in order to determine whether there
was an optimal foreperiod mean LRT
and mild stutterers
The third
comparisons
between group
were conducted using the non-
Test for Independent Samples, since several of
required by parametric analyses were not fulfilled
by these
1956).
RESULTS Figure 3 displays
a summary
Each data point
in this
‘The
of LRT
figure
values for the complete data set.’
represents
present study reports results obtained
from statistical
data set. In so doing, it is consistent
with
stutterers.
are sometimes
However,
minimum
LRT
procedures
two procedures
values
is that
responses occurring
LRT
prior
to group
values
(1978)
most LRT
faster
of the complete nonstutterers
used to eliminate
the maximum
comparisons.
significantly
analysis
studies comparing The than
rationale
for
the mean
and lzdebski
either
reflect
before the phonate cue, whereas values significantly
mean reflect the subjects’ inattention and Shipp
the average of all analyzed
with and
of these
anticipatory
slower than the
to the task. As an example of one procedure, lzdebski
(1980)
used statistical
tests to eliminate
only significant
outliers.
As an example of the second procedure, Reich et al. (1981) omitted the fastest and
slowest
responses of each subject before group comparisons.
will discuss the effects of various data-reduction
In a forthcoming
procedures on the LRT
effect.
paper, we
Ben C. Watson and Peter 1. Alfonso
192
550
Nonstutterers
Mom 279.23
SD. 27.03
Mild Stutterers
340.35
46.26
Severe Stutterers
390.19
Qroup _ 500
f
1V 3 1:i I !i
460
48.00 T
T
7 6 400 :: I .S
..
‘.+.....”
. ..’ ..a.- ,
1;
5 350
J_L____-l ’ I I I
44
3 8 300
‘t
l
T
250
200
Fcrqmrbd Figure 3:
Acoustic
LRT
In Msoc.
values in group means and standard deviation disper-
sions for the 13 foreperiod
conditions.
Each data point represents the individual
subject averages pooled across the five subjects in each group.
responses
values
per subject
are expressed
dispersions Also
shown
pooled
in
across the five subjects in each group. LRT
group
means
and two
standard
for the three subject groups and 13 foreperiod are group means and standard deviations
the 13 foreperiod
conditions.
LRT
deviation conditions.
collapsed across
values for nonstutterers
are shown as
closed circles, for mild stutterers as open circles, and for severe stutterers as open triangles.
Note that this figure demonstrates that LRT
function of subject group and foreperiod.
varies
as a
ForeperiodlSeverity
Effects on Acoustic LRT
193
TABLE 1 Summary
of Main and Interaction
Effects F
Main Effects
2,12
f3.88a*b,c
Stuttering severity Foreperiod Interaction
ti
3.1 ahc
12,144
Effect
Stuttering severity by foreperiod
0.52
24.144
T.99 (2,12) = 6.93. bF.99 (12,144) = 2.31. ‘F.95 (24,144) = 1.59.
The
stuttering
first
two
severity
hypotheses
in this
effects on LRT.
main effects as well as the stuttering effect are summarized
in Table
study
The results severity
1. This
predicted foreperiod
and
of MCR a.nalyses of these by foreperiod
table shows
interaction
that both, the
stuttering severity and foreperiod factors are significant @I < 0.01). Partial regression MCR
are presented
coefficients in Table
obtained from the between-subjects
2. Coefficients
for both the nonstutterer
versus mild stutterer and mild versus severe stutterer group comparisons were significant LRT
fp
These
values were significantly
results
different
indicate that the three groups’ when collapsed
across the 13
foreperiod conditions. Table 3 shows
results
describing the relationship group. Second-order
of analyses of the power of the polynomial between foreperiod and LRT for each subject
polynomials
were found for the nonstutterers
TABLE 2 Partial
Regression
Coefficients
for Stuttering
Comparison Nonstutterers Mild stutterers
Severity
vs mild stutterers vs severe stutterers
V.99 (1,12)= 9.33.
Factor F
df
-57.36
14.1ga
1,12
-53.59
1 2.3ga
1,12
8
and
Ben C. Watson and Peter 1. Alfonso
194
TABLE 3 Summary
of Power
Polynomial
Analysis
Power Term Nonstutterers
Mild stutterers
Severe stutterers
of Foreperiod F
Inc R Square
df
Linear
0.27
4.17
1,ll
Quadratic
0.30
08.10”
1,lO
Cubic
0.06
1.45
I,9 1,ll
Linear
0.17
2.27
Quadratic
0.33
11.61b
1,lO
Cubic
0.16
4.64
1,9
Linear
0.11
1.39
Quadratic
0.36
13.33‘
Cubic
0.26
"F.95 (1,ll) =
4.84.
bF.95 (1,lO) =
4.96.
1,ll 1,lO
8.66d
1,9
‘F.99 (1,lo) = 10.04. dF.95 (1,9)=
mild
5.12.
stutterers.
minimum
That
is,
LRT
values
ship between foreperiod
and LRT
following
analysis
subjects,
a second-order
decrease to a
was also reported by lzdebski
(1980)
of a reduced data set. He found, using only normal relationship
However,
our
foreperiod
for severe stutterers
shows
for these subjects
and then increase as foreperiod increases. A nonlinear relation-
data indicate
that a third-order
between
foreperiod
that the relationship is different.
polynomial
and LRT.
between
LRT
For these subjects,
also becomes significant
and
Table
3
and ap-
proaches the second-order term in best describing the shape of the curve. This
implies
that LRT
values for severe stutterers
minimum,
then increase to a maximum,
foreperiod
increases.
These
results
tend to decrease to a
and then decrease again as
emphasize
the difference
severe stutterers versus mild stutterers and nonstutterers.
between
For example, the
data shown in Figure 3 for mild stutterers
and nonstutterers
show single
maximum
a single inflection
point in the
curve.
and minimum
A curve
deviations, Analysis tively.
of predicted
LRT
values,
representing
least-squared
was obtained by solving regression equations for each group.
of predicted
nonstutterers
values, yielding
curves
and mild stutterers
For severe stutterers,
indicates
that the inflection
occur at 2000
points
for
and 1500 msec, respec-
there is less difference
between maximum
ForeperiodlSeverity
and minimum and 2500
Effects on Acoustic LRT
LRT values
and the curve
of 2000
lzdebski
For the severe stutterers,
(1980).
foreperiod
for mild
stutterers
msec. Thus, minimum membership. maximum
for the
LRT values
severe
is, both the stuttering
severity
between
and severe
foreperiods.
stutterers
nonstutterers’
hypothesis
stated
stutterers’
of foreperiod.
Our original
between
nonstutterers
and 3-set
foreperiods.
Hence,
significant
differences
between
only at foreperiods
post-hoc
zation
Test for Independent
shown
below
and
difference
the
symbols
between
severe stutterers’ all
13 foreperiods
values
foreperiods,
revealed
that
M line
mild
nor-stutterers’
group
S refer
connecting means.
fp < 0.05). greater
at I-, 2-, to find
stutterers’
LRT
we expected
and severe stutterers’
These hypotheses
were
by using the Randomi-
Results of these comparisons to
mild
groups
Results
On the other less than
and
severe
of this
no
analysis
stutterers, significant reveal
greater than nonstutterers’ hand, 1100
are
N refers to nonstutter-
indicates
than nonstutterers’
is, at foreperiods
as a
nonsignificant
stutterers
and
group comparisons
and
and
between
study, we expected
nonstutterers’
LRT values are significantly
are significantly
across the 13
LRT values
and mild-moderate
Samples.
group
as between
LRT values.
the abscissa in Figure 3. The symbol A solid
significant
as well
severity)
in the present
to
coefficients
was a difference
experiment
between
the
effect on nonstutterers’
short and long foreperiods.
tested by conducting
respectively.
regression
less than 1100 msec. Conversely,
differences
at both
by
stutterers
of this study. That
are collapsed
there
(grouped
differences
to find significant
that
effect on the
To summarize,
main effect reflects
LRT values
1300
factor were shown
partial
has a greater
at a
of group
and mild
LRT values.
and mild stutterers
when
foreperiod
and
LRT values
of nonstutterers
LRT values than on severe stutterers’
The third
occurred
has a greater
factor and foreperiod severity
by
the fastest LRT values
but seems to be around
In addition,
900
occurred reported
fastest LRT values
the first two hypotheses
nonstutterers
Finally,
mild stutterers’
ers,
points,
the results
at which
stutterers’
significantly.
that the stuttering
differences
with
is less clear,
thus far support
affect LRT values
function
consistent
it appears that foreperiod
and minimum
results reported
values
inflection
LRT values also seem to vary as a function
Finally,
it does
revealed
msec,
of 500 msec. The foreperiod
occurred
mild
has two
msec. Note also that the fastest LRT for nonstutterers
at a foreperiod
than
195
mild
at only msec.
stutterers’
that at LRT
5 of the first 7
However,
we
Ben C. Watson and Peter /. Alfonso
196
unexpectedly
found significant
and mild stutterers msec. Thus,
LRT
results
differences
as a function
of foreperiod
differences between nonstutterers’
and
LRT values, but only partially support our hypothesized
between nonstutterers’
general, these results
and mild
demonstrate
proach those of nonstutterers stutterers’ LRTs
between nonstutterers
equal to and greater than 1100
of group comparisons
clearly support our hypothesized severe stutterers’
differences
at 4 of 6 foreperiods
stutterers’
LRT
that mild stutterers’
LRT
as foreperiod
remain significantly
increases,
values.
In
values ap-
whereas
greater than nonstutterers’
severe
throughout
the entire range of foreperiods. DISCUSSION The first
interesting
finding
of the present study is that of a significant
stuttering severity factor. This finding is consistent with reaction time data for complex Using
vocal and manual responses
the same stuttering
constant foreperiod differences
subjects
reported by Borden (1982).
used in the present study
and a
equal to 1 set, she also observed significant
group
between nonstutterers
and severe stutterers
of perceptually fluent counting and finger-tapping between nonstutterers reach significance. stuttering
and mild
Thus,
stutterers
for the execution
responses. Differences
for the same tasks failed to
results of this and the Borden study indicate that
severity affects group reaction time differences for both simple
and complex vocal responses as well as for manual responses.
Further-
more, these results suggest that the delayed LRT values demonstrated by the experimental
subjects may represent an underlying
motor control in stutterers
as a group. Finally,
these results suggest that
the magnitude of the delay, and correspondingly deficit,
is reflected in the stuttering
measurements
alone do not permit
cesses occurring discussion, motor-control
severity analysis
deficit in general
the magnitude of the
rating. Of course, acoustic of the motor-control
pro-
before the onset of the acoustic response. Later in this
we will
suggest
procedures
that may allow
analysis
of
processes during posturing and response onset.
Perhaps the most interesting
and important finding
the composite effect of the stuttering
severity
of this study is
and foreperiod factors on
the significance of group LRT differences between stutterers terers. Specifically,
and nonstut-
we observed that mild stutterers’ LRT values approach
Foreperiodheverity
normal
Effects on Acoustic LRT
values
as foreperiod
values are significantly of foreperiods. our original
increases,
whereas
severe stutterers’
greater than normal values throughout
LRT
the range
These results are in general agreement with the findings of
LRT experiment.
LRT differences stutterers
197
That study failed to show significant
between nonstutterers
for foreperiods
equal to 1, 2, and 3 sec. Although
study reports nonsignificant this range, it should
group
and a group of mild to moderate the present
differences at only two of six foreperiods
be pointed out that the results
in
of the present study
reflect fewer subjects per group, fewer responses per subject, and the use of nonparametric statistics. supports
our original
stutterers’
With these differences aside, the present study
experiment
and nonstutterers’
LRT
differences between nonstutterers’ Throughout
in that the differences
between mild
values are significantly
less than the
and severe stutterers’
LRT values.
this article, we have noted that long foreperiods
permit
subjects to complete activity required to posture the speech mechanism for the voiced response. LRT
values
approach normal
severe stutterers’ contribute mild
LRT
the finding
values as foreperiod
LRT
values
for
the two
with regard to the comparisons
stutterers,
our results
primary
generally
difficulty
vibration,
since their LRT
severe stutterers
Results
Following
this
example,
better understand stutterers’
delayed
How-
initiating
between nonstutterers
and
suggest that these stutterers deficits.
have been studied with (Botwinick
respect to their
and Thompson,
1966).
we have chosen
to study
the posture
of the reaction time
response
in an attempt to
the qualitative LRT
and
that mild
have some difficulty
of foreperiod
components
components
of stutterers.
values do not become identical with those of
as a function
and motor
groups
the hypothesis
of the comparisons
Reaction time responses
initiation
whereas
the speech mechanism.
may have both posturing and vibration-initiation premotor
increases,
between nonstutterers
support
is posturing
ever, it is also likely that our mild stutterers the nonstutterers.
that mild stutterers’
values do not, suggests that different deficits may
to delayed
Specifically, stutterers’
Consequently,
values.
differences These
in the deficits
components
and
underlying
are schematically
represented in Figure 4. The posture component is represented by a series of processes formulation
related to perception of the warning and transmission
speech mechanism,
of neuromotor
and/or phonate cue,
commands to posture the
posturing of the speech mechanism for the required
Figure 4:
Onset
Rk&t&
,
-
F&&E
Articulatory (V.T.Shape)
Laryngeal eaistancel
components
Posture Time
ExaZ~ion POSTURE cornrnands
mw3on and
n Trans -
Commands
-
b
Frmwlatiotl Respiratory (Psgl
Postural Processes
Processes
Newomotol r
Posture and initiation
lritaraction of Modality Y-l ana hltanaity Parcepts
Derceptual hocesses
POSTURE COMPONENT
of the laryngeal
MlJSCleS
reaction
time response.
COMPONENT
Initiation Processes
Aal (4Gbttal Raaiatarcr?)
#WI
Contraction of Raspkatory
INITIATION
+Muscle RT. 2 -Movement R.T. Y Acoustic R.T.
Transmission and Execution of INITIATION Commands
Neuromotor Processes
. Reaction Signal Offset
Formulation of HlTlATlON Commands
Neuromotor Processes
PhzFe
Foreperiodbeverity
response,
Effects on Acoustic LRT
and formulation
of neuromotor
response. The formulation occur
simultaneously
of neuromotor with
the
by processes commands
gestures. The initiation
the response.
The
(3) acoustic output.
which
foreperiod
duration
of
component is represented
consequences
of neuromotor
of executing
(2) articulator
Figure 4 demonstrates permits
the may
and transmission
and execution
commands are: (1)muscular adjustments, finally,
to initiate
Postural processes are also taken to
related to the transmission for
commands
commands for initiation
formulation
neuromotor commands for posturing. include prephonatory
199
completion
these
movement, and,
the special case in
of all postural
activity
prior to the presentation of the phonate cue. The interval required for perceptual processing of the warning and phonate cue will (Elliot,
1968;
conflicting
vary as a function
Murray,
1970;
stutterers
and nonstutterers
visual stimuli
Conversely,
not conclusive
whether
modality
experimenters
stimulus
time
stimulus
1970),
1982)
modality and intensity
may reflect
as a variable with
is determined
variables
that of
are presented at
to the subject and the experimental
of perceptual processes. interval
the failure
levels. In addition, cognitive and affec-
as well
may interact
it is
effect.
manner and, more importantly,
differences
equal intensity
Thus,
the LRT
to ensure that visual and auditory stimuli
(Murray,
stimulus-related
influences
has shown that stimulus
reaction
psychophysically
processing
(1982) failed to find significant
modality
tive factors, such as instructions
duration
but not for
differences for auditory or visual stimuli.
Kohfeld (1971)
Lehtonen,
is
modality on the LRT
have been reported for auditory
parameters interact in a complex
setting
There
group reaction time differences between
Watson and Alfonso
LRT
However,
and intensity
1982).
by McFarlane and Prins (1978) and McFarlane and Shipley
between-group
cross
modality
and Alfonso,
evidence regarding the effect of stimulus
effect. For example, significant
(1981).
of stimulus
Watson
stimulus
Thus,
foreperiod parameters
the duration
(Niemi
and
to alter the
of the perceptual The
effects of
may be reduced by maintaining
constant
modality and intensity
by several variables.
parameters for all subjects. Although
it is
not possible to measure the duration of perceptual processes in humans directly,
Wall
physiological trical
activity
et al. (1953)
provide an estimate of this interval based on
data obtained from anesthetized in pyramidal
tract neurons
animals.
Recording elec-
in the motor
cortex,
they
Ben C. Watson and Peter 1. Alfonso
200
observed a latency of approximately visual stimulus the contribution
of perceptual processing
may be relatively duration
small. To summarize,
of perceptual
stimulus
intensity interval
(i.e., formulation peripheral values
processes
and modality
may be held relatively The
30 msec between the onset of a
and the onset of neural activity.
These data suggest that
activity to overall
LRT
values
it is not possible to measure the
directly.
However,
by controlling
parameters, the duration of this interval
constant across subjects.
required for the completion and transmission
musculature)
in the simple
of neuromotor
processes
of appropriate neural commands to the
may also contribute
minimally
to overall
reaction time paradigm. Estimates
LRT
of formulation
latencies are not available for human subjects. However, the transmission velocity
of
neural
approximately
impulses
along
the recurrent
56 meters/second in nonstutterers
laryngeal
nerve
is
(Flisberg and Lindholm,
1970). This value, in addition to a residual latency of 1.5 to 2.5 msec due to synaptic junctions fibers
(Basmajian,
and the decreasing diameter of peripheral
1970),
latency in nonstutterers
yields
an estimated
of approximately
maximum
3.0 msec. Thus
although the duration of perceptual and neuromotor
nerve
transmission it appears that
processing compo-
nents in the LRT paradigm cannot be directly measured, it is likely that the contribution
of both these processes to group LRT differences is relatively
insignificant. Posturing the speech mechanism for the onset of an isolated, voiced vowel requires articulatory
muscular
systems.
the optimization
adjustments
in the respiratory,
In the respiratory
of thoracic
system, these adjustments
muscle
tension.
Optimal
levels, in turn, facilitate rapid generation of sufficient for phonation
initiation
muscular adjustments phonation. laryngeal
(Baken et al., 1979). adjustments postures
result
appropriate
these systems will occur simultaneously. nature of the posturing activity within the qualitative versus
interaction
in respiratory
voiceless
result in
muscle
tension
subglottal pressure
In the laryngeal
system,
in achievement
of supra-
for the required
response
(e.g., the isolated vowel /a/). We assume that posturing
differences
and
modify vocal-fold tension and position to facilitate
Articulatory vocal-tract
laryngeal,
vowels.
Furthermore,
activity within
it is likely that the
any system is, in part, a function of
between systems.
For example, there may be
and laryngeal coupling for the onset of voiced In addition,
articulatory
postures
may affect
Foreperiodheverity
Effects on Acoustic
LRT
201
laryngeal posturing (i.e., constricted open vocal-tract configurations). In the aerodynamic domain, respiratory respect to lung volume.
For example,
shown that a lung volume
posturing also occurs with
lzdebski
of approximately
and Shipp (1978)
50%
have
vital capacity yields
faster LRT values than do prephonatory lung volumes of 25 and 75% vital capacity. In addition,
Hoshiko
found that nonstutterers
(1965)
usually
initiate phonation from about 50% vital capacity. Thus, this value appears to represent
an optimal
lung volume
for the initiation
of vocal-fold
vibration. It is also true that LRT the initiation initiation
component.
neuromotor
values are affected by processes included in These
include transmission
commands,
movement of speech structures,
muscle
and execution
contraction,
and, finally,
of
coordinated
generation of the resultant
acoustic output. Reaction time measurements of the latter three processes can be obtained and are illustrated Lastly, would
we should
emphasize
delay initiation
vibration
or abnormally
may be prolonged
postured (i.e., simultaneous
in abnormally
in stutterers
the latency of
if the vocal folds
are
1978).
adduction and abduction, cf.
Hyperpostured
vocal folds would
likely
high levels of glottal resistance and, therefore, the
need for higher levels of subglottal
pressure,
whereas abnormally
tured vocal folds would prevent the accumulation pressure
deficits
For example,
that is, postured with excessive tension and adduction,
Freeman and Ushijima, result
that posturing
of the response.
onset for stutterers
“hyperpostured,”
in Figure 4.
to initiate vibration.
Finally,
markedly
of sufficient constricted
pos-
subglottal articulatory
postures increase supraglottal pressures and, thus, may prolong vibration onset latencies. The point we wish to make is that the delayed reaction time values in these instances would reflect postural rather than initiation deficits. We assume that the contribution study
to between-group
modality
and intensity
subjects.
In addition,
process stutterers’
to the
LRT
LRT
values
differences
of perceptual processes was
insignificant
since
parameters were held relatively it is likely effect was
constant for all
that the contribution insignificant.
approach those
The
in this stimulus
of neuromotor
finding
of nonstutterers
that
mild
as foreperiod
increases suggests that the primary difficulty
for this group of stutterers
related to posturing
However,
the speech mechanism.
is
since LRT values
202
Ben C. Watson
and Peter 1. Alfonso
for mild stutterers did not become identical with those of nonstutterers, is also
possible
initiating
that these
vibration
stutterers
as well. Conversely,
the finding that severe stutterers’
LRT values fail to approach those of nonstutterers
suggests that severe stutterers
it
have some degree of difficulty as foreperiod increases
may have difficulty
in both posturing the
speech mechanism and initiating vocal-fold vibration. is that the underlying
deficit may be qualitatively
and severe stutterers.
Unfortunately,
What is important,
different between mild
LRT measures obtained from acous-
tic analysis alone do not permit precise specification of the loci of deficits in phonation onset activity in these stutterers. that mild stutterers
For example, it is possible
have the same type of deficits as do severe stutterers
but to a lesser degree. Thus,
we feel that we have made the most of
acoustic measures of LRT. That is, we need to investigate those activities that occur before the onset of voicing. The advantage of obtaining simultaneous
measures in the acoustic,
movement, and EMG domains is discussed by Baer and Alfonso (in press). They suggest that simultaneous in LRT
experiments
measures may be particularly
because they provide information
prior to onset of the acoustic signal corresponding For example, processes
the combined
may be inferred
duration
from
regarding activity
to vocal-fold vibration.
of perceptual
EMG
signals
informative
and neuromotor
recorded from
intrinsic
laryngeal muscles. That is, the latency between the offset of the warning signal and the onset of the EMG signal in the laryngeal muscles may yield an estimate of the time required to complete perceptual and neuromotor processes. In addition, EMG measures may be useful in documenting the latency
of onset,
synergy,
and amount
of muscular
activity
during
prephonatory posturing of the speech system as well as during generation of subglottal chest-wall
pressure
by the respiratory
and vocal-fold
and transillumation information
movements,
instrumentation,
system.
Direct observation
respectively,
regarding the amount and coordination
laryngeal posturing activity as well as the interaction posturing and respiratory-system
of
via Respitrace (Cohn et al., 1977) may also
provide
of respiratory
and
between laryngeal
activity during the generation of subglot-
tal pressure. In conclusion, our original
the results of the present study support the results of
experiment
effect. Furthermore,
by demonstrating
the present results
a significant
stuttering
severity
support the notion that mild and
ForeperiodlSeverity
Effects on Acoustic
LRT
203
severe stutterers’ prolonged LRT values may reflect differential posturing and/or vibration
initiation.
tic analyses
alone will
contributing
to stutterers’
experiments
not specifically delayed
incorporating
movement,
reveal the nature of deficits
LRT values.
simultaneous
and EMG domains.
in initiating
Only through
A portion annual
phonation
of the data
convention
Los Angeles,
tion,
ported
California,
by NINCDS
gratefully
experimental assistance
awarded
the assistance
subjects.
We
a/so
in the preparation
described.
was first presented
798 7. This
November
at the Associa-
research
to Haskins
of Arlyne
wish
LRT
acoustic,
Speech-Language-Hearing
grant NSl3870
acknowledge
the
stutterers’ often reported
in this paper
American
in
the use of simultaneous
be systematically
reported
of the
We plan future
measures
measures can the nature of deficits underlying difficulty
deficits in
We recognize, however, that acous-
was
sup-
Laboratories.
We
Russo in classifying
the
to thank
Thomas
Baer
for
his
of this manuscript.
REFERENCES Adams,
M.R.,
and Hayden,
P. The ability
initiate and terminate Journal Baer, T.,
of Speech
and Hearing
and Alfonso,
PJ.
acoustic
measures
Science,
Vol. II, Normal
Gallagher, Baken,
On
S.A.,
1976,19,
and Weissman,
J. Muscles Alive.
to auditory stimuli.
290-296.
neuromuscular, In: Current
Speech. K.L.
and Hearing
Baltimore:
Williams
speakers to
of an isolated vowel. movement,
and
issues in Language
(D. Beasley, C. Prutting, T.
Eds.). San Diego: College Hill
of Speech
Birren, I.E., and Botwinick,
stutterers.
Research,
and Disordered
and normal
production
of speech articulation.
phonation.lournal
Borden, G.J. Initiation
of stutterers
during
simultaneous
and R.C. Daniloff,
R.J., Cavallo,
Basmajian,
phonation
Chest wall Research,
Press, in press.
movements
prior to
1979, 22, 862-872.
& Wilkins,
1970.
J. Age differences in finger, jaw, and foot reaction time
lournal
of Gerontology,
1955, 10, 429-432.
versus execution time during manual and oral counting by
Haskins
Laboratories
Status
Report
on Speech
Research,
1982,
SR-70, in press. Botwinick,
J., and Thompson,
time. journal
Cohen, J. and Cohen, P. Applied Behavioral
L.W.
of Experimental Sciences.
Premotor and motor components of reaction Psychology,
Multiple
Hillsdale,
1966, 71, 9- 15.
Regression/Correlation
Analysis
N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
for the 1975.
and Peter 1. Alfonso
Ben C. Watson
204
Cohn, M.A., Watson, for
International Cross,
H., Weisshaut,
Non-invasive
Symposium
D.E., and Luper,
children
R., Stott, F., and Sackner, M.A. A Transducer
Monitoring
on Ambulatory
H.L.
and adults. journal
Fluency Cullinan,
W.,
of Fluency
R.
Simple
23, 344visual
Psychonomical Flisberg,
K.,
of adult
1977.
59-77.
Effects of stimulus
stutterers
U.K.,
and nonstuttering
1979,4,
Disorders,
at the 2nd
Middlesex,
ear presentation on
and nonstutterers.
journal
of
45 -58. M.T.
and nonstuttering
1980,
search,
time 1979,4,
and ‘Springer,
stuttering Elliot,
reaction
Disorders,
Monitoring.
Voice reaction time of stuttering
Cross, D.E., Shadden, B.B., and Luper, H.L. the voice
Paper presented
of Respiration.
Voice
initiation
children.
journal
and termination of Speech and
times
in
Hearing
Re-
360.
and simple 1968,
Science,
and Lindholm,
T.
auditory
reaction
time:
A comparison.
10, 335-336.
Electrical
stimulation
of the human
laryngeal nerve during thyroid operation. Acta Otolaryngologica,
recurrent 1970, 263,
63-67. Freeman,
F.J.,
journal Hayden,
and Ushijima,
of Speech
T.
Laryngeal
and Hearing
Research,
P. The effects of masking
Hoshiko,
M.S.
Lung
Izdebski,
for
doctoral
stuttering.
and nonstutterers
dissertation,
and Hearing Simple
of phonation.
journal
interval on phonation initiation
and Hearing
Research,
Purdue
of Applied
1978,
21, 638-651.
reaction time as a function of stimulus
J., Ingham, R.].,
of Experimental
and Gervens,
intensity
Psychology,
and nonstutterers.
of the American
in decibels
1971,88,251-257.
A. The effect of stimulus
practice on voice reaction in stutterers the annual convention
reaction times.
1980, 23, 485-489.
reaction times for phonatory initiation. /ournal
Research,
of light and sound.lournal Lewis,
Unpublished initiation
K., and Shipp, T. Minimal D.L.
during
20, 480-482.
of Speech
of Speech Kohfeld,
volume
1965,
K. Effects of prestimulus
Journal
activity
21, 538-562.
1975.
Physiology, Izdebski,
1978,
and pacing on stutterers’
voice and speech onset time. University,
muscle
conditions
and
Paper presented at
Speech-Language-Hearing
Associa-
tion, Atlanta, Georgia, 1979. McFarlane,
S.C., and Prins,
D. Neural response time of stutterers and nonstutterers
in selected oral motor tasks. journal
of Speech
and Hearing
1978,
Research,
21,768-778. McFarlane,
S.C., and Shipley,
nonstutterers Speech Murphy,
under
and Hearing
M.,
K.G. Latency of vocalization
conditions Disorders,
and Baumgartner,
stuttering and nonstuttering
of auditory 1981,
cueing. journal
of
24, 307-312.
J.M. Voice children.
onset for stutterers and
and visual
initiation
lournal
and termination
of Fluency
Disorders,
time
in
1981,
6,
257-264. Murray,
H.G.
Stimulus
theory model. /ournal
intensity
and reaction time:
of Experimental
Psychology,
Evaluation
of a decision-
1970, 84, 383-391.
ForeperiodlSeverity
Niemi,
Effects on Acoustic
P., and Naatanen, R. Foreperiod 1981,
Bulletin, Niemi,
P., and Lehtonen, P.W.,
Reiss,
Montlick,
E. Foreperiod
L.J.,
1975,
Cooper,
SR-44,
A., Till,
Haskins
1981,24,
95-
Therapy
Nonparametric
McGraw-Hill, Starkweather,
Status
Mermelstein,
Report
P.,
and
and manipulation on Speech
of
Research,
and manual reaction times of
of Speech and Hearing
H.S.
nonstutterers.
instrument
for children
1972,37,
Research,
and adults.
lournal
of
314-322.
for Stuttering
in Children
and Adults.
Springfield,
1974. statistics
Hirschman,
Stutterers 1976,
Research,
Venkatagiri,
A reap-
for
the
Behavioral
Sciences.
York:
New
1956.
C.W.,
onset:
Venkatagiri,
R.M.,
H. Laryngeal
Research,
III.: Charles C Thomas,
tion
McGuire,
adults. /our&
severity
and Hearing
S.
intensity:
192-196.
Ryan, B. Programmed Siegel,
stimulus
107.
J., and Goldsmith,
G. A stuttering
Speech
F.S.
Laboratories
stuttering and nonstuttering Riley,
and visual
50, 73-82,
1982,
T. A digital pattern playback for the analysis
speech signals. Reich,
and simple reaction time. Psychological
89, 133-162.
praisal. Acta Psycho/o&a, Nye,
205
LRT
vs.
P., and Tannenbaum, nonstutterers.
journal
R.S. Latency of vocalizaof Speech
and
Hearing
19, 481-492.
Reaction time for voiced and whispered journal
of fluency
Disorders,
/a/ in stutterers
H.S. Reaction time for/s/ and lzl in stutterers and nonstutterers:
of discoordination
hypothesis.
/ournal
and
1981, 6, 265-271.
of Communication
Disorders,
A test 1982,
15, 55-62. Wall,
P.D.,
Remond,
action of visual raphy Watson,
and Clinical
A.G.,
and Dobson,
Neurophysiology,
B.C., and Alfonso, A.D.
journal
1953,
P.J. A comparison
stutterers and nonstutterers.journa/ Weiss,
R.L.
Studies
on the mechanism
afferents on motor cortex excitability. 5, 385-393. of LRT
of Fluency
and VOT
Disorders,
values between
1982, 7,219-241.
The locus of reaction time change with set, motivation, of Gerontology,
1965,
20, 60-64.
of the
Electroencephalog-
and age.